ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The controversy regarding the nature of posttraumatic growth includes two main competing claims: one which argues that posttraumatic growth reflects authentic positive changes and the other which argues that posttraumatic growth reflects illusory defenses. While the former might suggest that posttraumatic growth enhances intimacy and close relationships, the latter might imply that posttraumatic growth hinders interpersonal relations. The present study aimed to test these claims by investigating the association between posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment over time at both the individual and dyadic levels, and the potential role of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Former prisoners of war and comparable war veterans and their wives (n = 229) were assessed twice, 30–31 (T1) and 35–38 (T2) years after the 1973 Yom Kippur War in Israel, with regard to posttraumatic growth, posttraumatic stress symptoms and dyadic adjustment. Results indicated that posttraumatic growth was associated with both elevated posttraumatic stress symptoms and low dyadic adjustment among both husbands and wives. Posttraumatic stress symptoms at T1 and T2 mediated the association between posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment. Wives' posttraumatic growth at T1 predicted posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment of the husbands at T2. The higher the wives' posttraumatic growth, the higher the posttraumatic growth and the lower the dyadic adjustment of the husbands in the subsequent measure. The findings suggest that posttraumatic growth reflects defensive beliefs which undermine marital relationships and that posttraumatic growth might be transmitted between spouses and implicated in the deterioration of the marital relationship over time.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 30 June 2017
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01102
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Edited by:
Adam D. Brown,
Sarah Lawrence College, NYU School
of Medicine, United States
Reviewed by:
Sara Freedman,
Bar-Ilan University, Israel
Alexandre Luiz De Oliveira Serpa,
Hogrefe Publishing Group, Brazil
*Correspondence:
Yael Lahav
lahav.yael62@gmail.com
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Psychopathology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 17 November 2016
Accepted: 14 June 2017
Published: 30 June 2017
Citation:
Lahav Y, Kanat-Maymon Y and
Solomon Z (2017) Posttraumatic
Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
among War Veterans and their Wives.
Front. Psychol. 8:1102.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01102
Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic
Adjustment among War Veterans and
their Wives
Yael Lahav 1, 2*, Yaniv Kanat-Maymon 3and Zahava Solomon2, 4
1Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark, 2I-Core Research Center for Mass Trauma,
Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv, Israel, 3Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel, 4Bob Shapell School of Social Work,
Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv, Non-US/Non-Canadian, Israel
The controversy regarding the nature of posttraumatic growth includes two main
competing claims: one which argues that posttraumatic growth reflects authentic positive
changes and the other which argues that posttraumatic growth reflects illusory defenses.
While the former might suggest that posttraumatic growth enhances intimacy and close
relationships, the latter might imply that posttraumatic growth hinders interpersonal
relations. The present study aimed to test these claims by investigating the association
between posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment over time at both the individual
and dyadic levels, and the potential role of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Former
prisoners of war and comparable war veterans and their wives (n=229) were
assessed twice, 30–31 (T1) and 35–38 (T2) years after the 1973 Yom Kippur War
in Israel, with regard to posttraumatic growth, posttraumatic stress symptoms and
dyadic adjustment. Results indicated that posttraumatic growth was associated with
both elevated posttraumatic stress symptoms and low dyadic adjustment among both
husbands and wives. Posttraumatic stress symptoms at T1 and T2 mediated the
association between posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment. Wives’ posttraumatic
growth at T1 predicted posttraumatic growth and dyadic adjustment of the husbands at
T2. The higher the wives’ posttraumatic growth, the higher the posttraumatic growth
and the lower the dyadic adjustment of the husbands in the subsequent measure. The
findings suggest that posttraumatic growth reflects defensive beliefs which undermine
marital relationships and that posttraumatic growth might be transmitted between
spouses and implicated in the deterioration of the marital relationship over time.
Keywords: posttraumatic growth, dyadic adjustment, posttraumatic stress symptoms, war combat, prisoners of
war, secondary traumatization, trauma
INTRODUCTION
Combat and war captivity are highly traumatogenic experiences. Combat exposes the individual to
the threat of death and injury. War captivity includes, in addition to combat exposure, prolonged,
and deliberate psychological torture aimed at breaking the prisoner’ spirit (Herman, 1992). The
psychopathological implications of combat and war captivity are not limited to primary trauma
survivors, and may be transmitted to their significant others, a phenomenon known as secondary
traumatization (e.g., Figley, 1986).
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
Exposure to a traumatic event might lead to a posttraumatic
reaction, most commonly known as posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD). According to the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric
Association, 2000), which the current study relies upon, PTSD
symptoms (PTSS) include intrusion (e.g., flashbacks), avoidance
(e.g., numbness), and hyper-arousal (e.g., alertness). Research
has consistently documented PTSS to be the most common
psychiatric consequence of combat and war captivity (Engdahl
et al., 1997), and has indicated high rates of PTSS among
combatants, former prisoners of war (ex-POWs; Page, 1992), and
their spouses (e.g., Galovski and Lyons, 2004), even decades after
the traumatic event.
However, it has been suggested that exposure to traumatic
events can also result in positive gains or transformations. Most
notably, posttraumatic growth (PTG), is defined as the tendency
to report enhanced changes in the aftermath of traumatic events
in three domains: self-perception, interpersonal relationships,
and world view (e.g., Calhoun and Tedeschi, 2006).
Research has documented PTG among individuals who have
experienced various traumatic events, both first hand (for reviews
see Calhoun and Tedeschi, 2006) and indirectly through intimate
interactions with trauma survivors (e.g., Manne et al., 2004). PTG
has also been documented following exposure to combat and war
captivity in combat veterans (e.g., Aldwin et al., 1994), ex-POWs
(e.g., Ursano et al., 1986; Feder et al., 2008), and their wives (e.g.,
McCormack et al., 2011).
Although PTG has been documented in numerous
psychosocial studies, there remains a controversy regarding
its nature and long term implications. According to one
perspective, PTG is seen as a genuine transformation of basic
beliefs about the self and the world, which results from struggling
with the effects of trauma (e.g., Calhoun and Tedeschi, 2006).
However, according to an alternative perspective, PTG might
have an illusory quality that may be maladaptive and hinder
coping in the long term (e.g., McFarland and Alvaro, 2000;
Lahav et al., 2016). The Janus-face model (Maercker and
Zoellner, 2004), suggests that PTG simultaneously includes both
a constructive aspect and an illusory aspect. The constructive
aspect of PTG is the result of an active struggle with the
trauma, and is related to heightened adjustment and well-
being, both in the short and long term. However, the effects
of the illusory aspect depend on the timing of its usage and
the extent of denial involved. Specifically, it is claimed that
when the illusory aspect of PTG serves as an acute short term
palliative strategy, and co-exists with deliberate thinking about
the trauma and the coping effort, it has neither positive nor
negative long-term aftereffects. Yet, under conditions in which
PTG is solely illusory and serves in the long run as avoidance
strategy, strengthening efforts to evade the acknowledgment
of the traumatic event and the multiple losses it entails, it has
deleterious effects on adjustment. The present study explores
reports of PTG decades after the trauma has ended, and not
the usage of PTG occuring shortly after the exposure. Hence,
drawing upon the Janus-face model also indicates both positive
and negative potential implications of PTG, depending on the
dominance of the constructive side of PTG and the involvement
of denial.
The dispute about the nature of PTG is highlighted by mixed
results regarding the association between PTG and PTSS (e.g.,
Helgeson et al., 2006). While some studies have revealed a
negative association between PTG and PTSS (e.g., Frazier et al.,
2001), i.e., higher PTG associated with lower PTSS, others have
indicated a positive correlation (e.g., Dekel et al., 2012), or a
non-significant relationship between the two (e.g., Zoellner and
Maercker, 2006).
Although there is ample research on the implications of
PTG on the individual’s mental health, its consequences with
regard to the interpersonal domain, and specifically to the
marital relationship, have not been studied. The present study
investigates the implications of PTG reported many years after
the trauma on dyadic adjustment and the role of PTSS within
these associations. In the following sections we will first present
the literature regarding dyadic adjustment and PTSS among
ex-POWs, combatants and their wives. We will then present
perspectives with regard to PTG, dyadic adjustment and the role
of PTSS.
A considerable body of research has consistently
demonstrated that traumatic events have negative implications
on marital relationships. Empirical studies have indicated that
for couples in which one partner has experienced combat or
war captivity, there have been reports of lower relationship
satisfaction and dyadic adjustment (Neria et al., 2000), more
communication problems and conflicts (Cook et al., 2004), and
more impairments in intimacy and sexual relations (e.g., Zerach
et al., 2010).
PTSS is one of the most widely accepted mechanisms
underlying the low dyadic adjustment in the aftermath of
combat and war captivity (e.g., Cook et al., 2004). For
example, intrusive symptoms can amplify preoccupation with
the self at the expense of the relationship; avoidance symptoms
might lower self-disclosure; and hyperarousal symptoms might
amplify interpersonal conflict (e.g., Cook et al., 2004). Research
conducted among combat veterans or ex-POWs and their wives
indicated that the veterans’ PTSS was associated with lower
marital satisfaction for both partners (e.g., Cook et al., 2004)
and mediate the association between exposure to trauma and
low dyadic adjustment among ex-POWs (Dekel et al., 2008).
Furthermore, a longitudinal study conducted among fathers
in the Army National Guard from a Brigade Combat Team
indicated that increases in PTSS over time was associated with
poorer dyadic adjustment at a subsequent measurement (Gewirtz
et al., 2010).
Despite the fact that PTG refers to the interpersonal
domain and also assumes to reflect enhanced changes in
relationships, research regarding the implications of PTG on
marital relationships is limited. In fact, to the best of our
knowledge, former studies that investigated PTG with regard
to marital relationships have assessed dyadic adjustment as a
predictor of PTG and not as a consequence of growth (e.g.,
Manne et al., 2004; Weiss, 2004).
Given that the quality of a couple’s relationship affects both
partners’ psychological health (e.g., Proulx et al., 2007), it is
important to study the effects of PTG on dyadic adjustment.
Furthermore, as PTG by definition is assumed to be linked to
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
enhanced changes in relationships, examining the effects of PTG
on the quality of the marital relationship might illuminate the
nature of PTG. The present study aimed to fill this gap in the
knowledge by investigating the associations between PTG and
dyadic adjustment, and the role of PTSS within this association,
among war veterans and their wives.
As noted above, the literature implies two main alternatives
regarding the implications of PTG in dyadic adjustment, when
it is reported long after the trauma ended. The first assumes
that if PTG mainly reflects an authentic change it will be linked
to strengthened dyadic adjustment (e.g., Maercker and Zoellner,
2004). According to Calhoun and Tedeschi (2006), PTG reflects
positive schemas regarding interpersonal interactions. Trauma
survivors who experience growth become more emotionally
expressive, utilize social supports more than before, and show
efforts to improve relationships and greater sensitivity to others
(Calhoun and Tedeschi, 2006).
However, according to the alternate view, if, in the long
run, PTG serves mainly as an illusory defense that involves
denial, it could negatively affect coping with the trauma (e.g.,
Maercker and Zoellner, 2004) leading to subsequent difficulties in
adjustment. These adverse ramifications could, in turn, permeate
the individuals’ most intimate relationship, resulting in lower
dyadic adjustment.
A similar split in the literature arises regarding the potential
mediator role of PTSS in the association between PTG and dyadic
adjustment. The first alternative assumes that if PTG mainly
reflects an authentic transformation, it might ease the survivors’
distress and PTSS in the long term (e.g., Maercker and Zoellner,
2004), which, in turn, would enhance dyadic adjustment.
Alternatively, if PTG mainly reflects illusory defenses that
serve as a way to deny one’s adversity, it might affect PTSS
negatively, which in turn undermines dyadic adjustment. Beliefs
of growth might act as an avoidance strategy (e.g., Maercker and
Zoellner, 2004; Lahav et al., 2017) thereby inhibiting processing
the traumatic event and leading to elevated PTSS. This possible
negative effect, might, in turn, lead to low dyadic adjustment.
The inter-dependence between spouses is a well-established
notion as they affect each other in a multitude of dimensions.
Research has consistently demonstrated that veterans’ mental
health states are implicated in their spouses’ well-being (e.g.,
Renshaw et al., 2011) and the increased distress exhibited by
veterans is associated with lower marital satisfaction for both
veterans and their partners (e.g., Renshaw et al., 2008). Given that
the mental state of the husband and wife are interrelated, one
would assume that their PTG and dyadic outcomes would also
prove to be interrelated. However, to the best of our knowledge,
there has not been a study that has assessed whether PTG and
dyadic adjustment in one spouse affects the PTG and dyadic
adjustment in the other. The third aim of the present study is to
investigate the mutual and reciprocal effects between husbands’
and wives’ PTG and dyadic adjustment in a longitudinal design.
The present study will examine: (1) the associations between
PTG, PTSS, and dyadic adjustment among war veterans and
their wives, separately; (2) the mediating role of concurrent,
prospective and difference over time in PTSS in the relationships
between PTG and dyadic adjustment among war veterans
and their wives, separately and; (3) the associations between
husbands’ and wives PTG and dyadic adjustment over time.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Procedure and Participants
The present study used data from a longitudinal study on the
psychological implications of war among veterans and their wives
(Greene et al., 2014, for full details). A cohort of Israeli veterans
from the 1973 Yom Kippur War and their wives were followed
over time. The current study used data which were collected at
two points in time: 2003/2004 (T1) and 2008/2011 (T2), from
both veterans and their wives.
In order to locate veterans, we used Israel Defense Forces
(IDF) files. Wives of veterans were recruited via their spouses.
Both husbands and wives were contacted by telephone and asked
individually to take part in the study. A battery of questionnaires
were administered in their homes or in another location of their
choice. Before filling out the questionnaires, participants signed
an informed consent form. This study was approved by the Tel
Aviv University ethics committee.
War Veterans
According to records of the Israeli Ministry of Defense, 240
soldiers who served in the IDF land forces were taken prisoner
in the 1973 Yom Kippur War. In addition, 280 comparable
veterans were sampled from IDF computerized data banks.
These individuals participated in the same war, but were not
taken captive and were matched on military background and
sociodemographics. A total of 227 veterans participated in T1. Of
these, 121 were ex-POWs (53.3%) and 106 were combat veterans
(46.7%). In T2 there was a total of 294 participants. Of these, 176
were ex-POWs (59.9%) and 118 were combat veterans (40.1%).
For all veterans the mean age at T1 was 52.62 (SD =4.56),
mean years of schooling was 13.94 (SD =3.46); the majority were
secular (61.7%), with an over-average income (35.6%).
Veterans’ Wives
In T1, 213 veterans were married. A total of 165 wives
participated in T1. Of these, 90 were ex-POWs’ wives (54.5%) and
75 were controls’ wives (45.4%). In T2, the number of married
veterans increased to 250. Hence, the number of veterans’ wives
who participated also increased, to 171. Of these, 114 were ex-
POWs’ wives (66.6%) and 57 were controls wives (33.1%). The
mean age for wives of veterans at T1 was 50.70 (SD =6.36),
mean years of schooling was 14.18 (SD =3.18); the majority were
secular (57.9%), with an over-average income (35.0%).
In the present study, we used an anchor wherein couples were
included in the sample only if both partners participated in at
least one wave of measurement. The current sample consisted of
229 couples.
Handling Missing Data
Substantial attrition, and in several cases, addition, are very
common in longitudinal designs (Collins et al., 2001). In the
current study, both of these occurred from T1 to T2. Of the
current sample of 229 couples at T1 and T2, respectively: 179
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
(78.17%) and 187 husbands (81.66%), and 156 (68.12%) and 160
(69.87%) wives had data in regards to the study variables. In
addition, data were missing across variables, with some variables
having more missing data than others. Hence, we assessed the
differences between valid and missing data in each variable and
for each spouse and measurement. Overall 19.2–38.9% data were
missing across both waves and variables.
To decide whether the data had missing values in a pattern
that was random, we conducted analyses of differences between
these groups in all of the variables, using Little’s Missing
Completely at Random (MCAR) test (Collins et al., 2001). The
analysis revealed that the data were not missing completely at
random, χ2
(889) =1021.356, p=0.001. Supplementary analyses
revealed that the husbands of wives with missing data regarding
PTSS at T2, endorsed significantly lower PTSS symptoms at T1
(t=2.8, p=0.006) and lower PTG at T1 (t=2.1, p=0.04) than
husbands of wives without missing data.
As the mechanism of missingness was unknown and there
were indications that the missingness was related to the observed
data, we assumed that the data were missing at random (MAR).
Missing data were handled with maximum likelihood (ML) via
the SPSS 22 and AMOS 22 programs. Compared to conventional
methods, such as arithmetic mean, listwise or pairwise deletion,
ML method was recommended as an optimal method for both
attrition and addition of participants over time (Collins et al.,
2001). The final sample (after ML was implemented) comprised
of 229 Israeli couples (134 ex-POW couples and 95 control
couples).
Measures
PTSS (PTSD Inventory) (PTSD-I; Solomon et al., 1993)
Husbands’ and wives PTSS was assessed via the PTSD-I, a
well-validated, 17-item, self-report questionnaire. The items on
the PTSD-I correspond to the DSM-IV-TR diagnosis for PTSD
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Respondents rated the
symptoms experienced in the previous month on a scale ranging
from (0) not at all to (4) almost always. Wives’ PTSS scores were
obtained by asking wives to rate their symptoms related to their
husbands’ experiences of combat or captivity. The number of
positively endorsed symptoms was calculated by counting the
items in which the respondents answered “3” or “4.” The PTSD-I
has proven psychometric properties and convergent validity (e.g.,
Solomon et al., 1993). In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas
were 0.95, 0.96 for husbands and 0.91, 0.91 for wives, at T1 and
T2 respectively.
Post Traumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI; Tedeschi and
Calhoun, 1996)
The PTGI was used to assess the salutary impact of trauma for
both husbands and wives. The scale includes 21-items scored on a
4-point scale from (1) I didn’t experience this change at all, to (4) I
experienced this change to a very great degree. The PTGI includes
five subscales, however, in the current study only the total score
was calculated. The PTGI has good internal consistency as well as
good construct, convergent and discriminant validity (Tedeschi
and Calhoun, 1996). In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas were
0.94, 0.93 for husbands, and 0.96, 0.94 for wives, at T1 and T2
respectively.
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976)
Husbands’ and wives dyadic adjustment levels were assessed by
the DAS which consists of 32 items divided into four subscales:
satisfaction, cohesion, consensus, and affection expression. In
addition, the total score was computed by summing the ratings
on the 32 items. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to
which each item describes their current marital relationship. The
scale has good convergent and discriminant validity (Heyman
et al., 1994). In the current study, Cronbach’s alphas were 0.94–
0.96 for the total score and 0.90–0.96 for the subscales.
Data Analysis
To assess the associations between PTG, on the one hand,
and PTSS and dyadic adjustment, on the other hand, while
controlling for study group, partial correlations were conducted.
Next, in order to examine whether PTSS at T1 and T2 mediated
the link between T1 PTG and T2 dyadic adjustment, we
used multiple step mediation (Hayes et al., 2011). Specifically,
we examined: (a) whether T1 PTG directly affected dyadic
adjustment at T2, controlling for PTSS at T1 and T2; (b) whether
T1 PTG indirectly affected dyadic adjustment via PTSS at any
of the time points (i.e., T1 and T2, separately); and (c) whether
T1 PTG indirectly affected dyadic adjustment via a two-step
mediation process (i.e., via PTSS at T1-T2). We controlled for
study group as well as for T1 dyadic adjustment in order to
take into account the stability of dyadic adjustment over time.
To examine whether these indirect paths were significant, we
employed accelerated bias-corrected bootstrap analyses.
In order to examine the associations between husbands’
and wives’ PTG and dyadic adjustment over time, we used an
Actor-Partner Independence Model (APIM; Kashy and Kenny,
2000) via AMOS statistics, Version 22. The APIM, “is a model
of dyadic relationships that integrates a conceptual view of
interdependence in two person relationships with the appropriate
statistical techniques for measuring and testing it” (Cook and
Kenny, 2005, p. 101). A good model fit to the observed data is
suggested if the comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index
(NFI), and the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) are greater than 0.90
and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is
lower than 0.07.
RESULTS
Associations between PTG, PTSS, and
Dyadic Adjustment
Among both spouses, partial correlation analyses indicated that
PTG was positively correlated with PTSS beyond the effect of
study group. The higher the PTG, the more the PTSS. Among
husbands, PTG at T1 was negatively correlated with the levels
of dyadic adjustment measures at T2 beyond the effect of
study group. Among wives, PTG at T1 and T2 were negatively
correlated with the levels of dyadic adjustment measures at T1
and T2 beyond the effect of study group. The higher the PTG, the
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
lower the dyadic adjustment (see Table 1 for husbands’ results;
see Table 2 for wives’ results).
Multistep Mediation of PTSS in the Relation
between PTG and Dyadic Adjustment
Multiple step mediation analyses among the husbands (Table 3)
revealed that PTG had a non-significant direct effect for dyadic
adjustment, except for the cohesion subscale. PTG indirectly
predicted the husbands’ dyadic adjustment via PTSS at T2 as
well as via both PTSS at T1 and T2 for the dyadic adjustment
total score and all subscales (See Figure 1 for dyadic adjustment
total score). Higher PTG at T1 predicted higher PTSS at T1 (B=
1.71, SE =0.38, P<0.001), which, in turn, increased the levels
of PTSS between T1 and T2 (B=0.70, SE =0.06, P<0.001).
Next, higher levels of PTSS at T2 were associated with low dyadic
adjustment at T2 (B= 1.35, SE =0.47, P=0.004; B= 0.54,
SE =0.19, P=0.005; B= 0.23, SE =0.09, P=0.02; B=
0.53, SE =0.22, P=0.02; B= 0.13, SE =0.05, P=0.009, for
dyadic adjustment total score, satisfaction, cohesion, consensus,
and affection, respectively).
Analyses among the wives revealed (Table 3) that PTG had
a significant direct effect on dyadic adjustment, except for the
cohesion and consensus subscales. PTG did not have an indirect
effect on dyadic adjustment, apart from the affection subscale
(See, Figure 2). PTG indirectly predicted the wives’ affection via
PTSS at T2 as well as via both PTSS at T1 and T2. Higher PTG at
T1 predicted higher PTSS at T1 (B=2.22, SE =0.26, P<0.001),
which, in turn, increased the levels of PTSS between T1 and T2 (B
=0.69, SE =0.05, P<0.001). Next, higher levels of PTSS at T2
were associated with low affection at T2 (B= 0.21, SE =0.07,
P=0.003).
APIM Model for Wives’ and Husbands’ PTG
and Dyadic Adjustment
The fit indices of the APIM model indicated that the theoretical
model was a good representation of the data, χ2(6) =10.21,
p=0.116, χ2/df =1.701, CFI =0.98, NFI =0.97, TLI =
0.90, RMSEA =0.06. We wanted to estimate a simpler and
parsimonious model, containing only the significant paths found.
We compared its fit indices to the general model, arguing for
many paths of impact. Non-significant difference of the two
chi-squares suggest that the omission of the non-significant
parameters did not reduce model and indicate favors to the
simpler model (Ledermann et al., 2011).
No significant difference was found between the two chi-
squares, 2(9) =7.125, p=0.624. Hence, we proceeded with
the more parsimonious simple model omitting non-significant
paths. Fit indices of the simpler model indicated that the model
was an excellent representation of the data, χ2(15) =17.335,
p=0.299, χ2/df =1.155,CFI =0.99, NFI =0.94, TLI =0.98,
RMSEA =0.03.
Figure 3 and Table 4 display the standardized coefficients and
significant paths for the parsimonious nested model. The analysis
revealed high stability of PTG and dyadic adjustment over time
among both partners. Those with high levels of PTG and dyadic
adjustment at T1 tended to have high levels of PTG or dyadic
TABLE 1 | Partial correlations between the study measures controlling for study group among husbands.
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. PTG, T1
2. PTG, T2 0.60***
3. PTSS, T1 0.28*** 0.23***
4. PTSS, T2 0.26*** 0.28*** 0.72***
5. DAS total, T1 0.06 0.05 0.42*** 0.38***
6. DAS satisfaction, T1 0.09 0.07 0.39*** 0.30** 0.91***
7. DAS cohesion, T1 0.05 0.04 0.30*** 0.26** 0.79*** 0.65***
8. DAS consensus, T1 0.07 0.02 0.40*** 0.40*** 0.79*** 0.76*** 0.69***
9. DAS affection, T1 0.06 0.06 0.31*** 0.35*** 0.79*** 0.70*** 0.54*** 0.69***
10. DAS total, T2 0.12** 0.02 0.44*** 0.46*** 0.60*** 0.59*** 0.56*** 0.49*** 0.39***
11. DAS satisfaction, T2 0.17** 0.07 0.37*** 0.39*** 0.42*** 0.47*** 0.45*** 0.27*** 0.28*** 0.90***
12. DAS cohesion, T2 0.09 0.09 0.33*** 0.34*** 0.60*** 0.53*** 0.66*** 0.48*** 0.42*** 0.79*** 0.64***
13. DAS consensus, T2 0.12* 0 0.45*** .46*** 0.61*** 0.58*** 0.50*** 0.56*** 0.36*** 0.94*** 0.74*** 0.66***
14. DAS affection, T2 0.15* 0.09 0.31*** 0.37*** 0.38*** 0.38*** 0.26*** 0.32*** 0.36*** 0.76*** 0.64*** 0.48*** 0.73***
M(SD) 2.34 (0.67) 2.17 (0.66) 6.96 (5.59) 6.82 (5.69) 106.19 (22.60) 36.70 (7.50) 15.09 (5.45) 46.32 (10.07) 8.08 (2.47) 95.22 (29.57) 31.28 (10.90) 14.08 (5.88) 42.48 (13.57) 7.38 (2.58)
Range 2.76 3 17 20.25 125 40 28.65 57 11 142 51.92 29.72 67.5 12
PTG, posttraumatic growth; PTSS, posttraumatic symptoms; DAS, dyadic adjustment scale; T1, assessment in 2003; T2, assessment in 2008. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
TABLE 2 | Partial correlations between the study measures controlling for study group among wives.
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. PTG, T1
2. PTG, T2 0.78***
3. PTSS, T1 0.55*** 0.46***
4. PTSS, T2 0.57*** 0.52*** 0.78***
5. DAS total, T1 0.29*** 0.18** 0.31*** 0.30***
6. DAS satisfaction, T1 0.27*** 0.20** 0.29*** 0.30*** 0.90***
7. DAS cohesion, T1 0.13* 0.13* 0.20*** 0.17** 0.84*** 0.70***
8. DAS consensus, T1 0.30*** 0.16* 0.30*** 0.28*** 0.93*** 0.74*** 0.66***
9. DAS affection, T1 0.29*** 0.12 0.32*** 0.37*** 0.81*** 0.68*** 0.63*** 0.74***
10. DAS total, T2 0.35*** 0.17* 0.32** 0.33*** 0.60*** 0.48*** 0.36*** 0.66*** 0.52***
11. DAS satisfaction, T2 0.37*** 0.17* 0.33*** 0.27*** 0.62*** 0.62*** 0.32*** 0.66*** 0.45*** 0.83***
12. DAS cohesion, T2 0.18** 0.10 0.26*** 0.24*** 0.61*** 0.43*** 0.56*** 0.61*** 0.50*** 0.71*** 0.61***
13. DAS consensus, T2 0.30*** 0.15* 0.26*** 0.29*** 0.43*** 0.29** 0.22** 0.52*** 0.41*** 0.91*** 0.56*** 0.47***
14. DAS affection, T2 0.30*** 0.10 0.17** 0.31*** 0.31*** 0.26*** 0.19** 0.31** 0.41*** 0.78*** 0.54*** 0.43*** 0.74***
M (SD) 2.30 (0.82) 2.24 (0.79) 3.82 (3.80) 4.13 (4.83) 105.69 (24.08) 36.41 (7.39) 15.07 (5.95) 45.98 (11.25) 8.22 (2.49) 103.09 (24.82) 36.07 (7.95) 14.82 (5.33) 44.60 (13.33) 7.60 (2.75)
Range 12.00 68.79 25.00 40.00 136.99 12.40 65.00 35.00 45.50 141.50 18.82 16.72 3.95 3.20
PTG, posttraumatic growth; PTSS, posttraumatic symptoms; DAS, dyadic adjustment scale; T1, assessment in 2004; T2, assessment in 2011. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.
adjustment at T2. More importantly, the analysis revealed that
the initial level of wives’ PTG at T1 predicted husbands’ dyadic
adjustment at T2, above and beyond the stability of husbands’
dyadic adjustment. The higher the wives’ PTG at T1, the lower
the husbands’ dyadic adjustment at T2. The analysis also revealed
that the initial level of wives’ PTG at T1 predicted husbands’ PTG
at T2, above and beyond the stability of husbands’ PTG. The
higher the wives’ PTG at T1, the higher the husbands’ PTG at
T2. Lastly, the analysis revealed that the initial level of husbands’
dyadic adjustment at T1 predicted wives’ dyadic adjustment at
T2, above and beyond the stability of wives’ dyadic adjustment.
The higher the husbands’ dyadic adjustment at T1, the higher the
wives’ dyadic adjustment at T2. The other prediction axes were
non-significant.
We explored the possibility that the reported findings change
as a function of participants’ study group (i.e., ex-POWs and
their wives vs. war veterans and their wives). To that end, we
conducted multigroup APIM models that estimated the relation
between husbands’ and wives PTG and dyadic adjustment,
separately for each group. The multigroup model did not fit the
data well, χ2(29) =57.57, p=0.001, χ2/df =1.99, CFI =0.87,
NFI =0.81, TLI =0.69, RMSEA =0.07. Furthermore, the chi-
square difference test (Brown et al., 1990) yielded non-significant
values, 2(4) =3.411, p=0.492, suggesting that the results do
not change as a function of study group.
DISCUSSION
The current study explored PTG in the context of marital
relationships on both the individual and dyadic levels. The
present results revealed that among husbands who were exposed
directly to combat or war captivity, as well as their wives, who
were exposed indirectly to these traumatic events, PTG was
not only associated with elevated PTSS but also low dyadic
adjustment. PTSS mediated the association between PTG and
all of the dyadic adjustment subscales among husbands, and the
association between PTG and the dyadic adjustment affection
subscale among wives. Lastly, the wives’ PTG predicted higher
PTG and lower dyadic adjustment among the husbands over
time.
The current findings were inconsistent with previous findings
that indicated PTG to be related to enhanced adjustment,
manifested in lower PTSS (e.g., Frazier et al., 2001; Carver
and Antoni, 2004). It could be argued that the implications
of PTG depends upon the severity of the trauma; however, in
this study PTG was not found to alleviate subsequent distress
for either the ex-POW or control dyads. Alternatively, it might
be that the discrepancy between findings is rooted in the
measurement of PTG. While this study used the PTGI (Tedeschi
and Calhoun, 1996), a validated and widely-used questionnaire,
previous studies which indicated PTG to be adaptive, predicting
lower subsequent distress, often used unstandardized measures
of growth (Dekel et al., 2012).
The current findings were consistent with evidence indicating
a positive relationship between self-reported PTG and PTSS (e.g.,
Helgeson et al., 2006; Zalta et al., 2017). Moreover, our findings
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
TABLE 3 | Bootstrap 95% confidence intervals for predicting dyadic adjustment by PTG through PTSS in time 1 and time 2 among husbands and wives.
Measure DAS total DAS satisfaction DAS cohesion DAS consensus DAS affection
HUSBANDS
Direct {4.9985, 4.2098} {2.9297, 0.8421} {0.1477, 1.9502}* {2.2557, 2.1195} {0.7100, 0.2442}
Indirect through T1 PTSS {3.4385, 1.2416} {1.0888, 0.7898} {0.5781, 0.2043} {2.0401, 0.1158} {0.2873, 0.1876}
Indirect through T2 PTSS {1.9182, 0.0041}* {.7487, 0.0047}* {0.03053, 0.0071}* {0.8828, 0.0050}* {0.1910, 0.0007}*
Indirect through T1 and T2 PTSS {3.4463, 0.2788}* {1.4567, 0.1656}* {0.7128, 0.1098}* {1.5546, 0.0741}* {0.3794, 0.0382}*
WIVES
Direct {8.6758, 0.6303}* {3.3917, 0.8863}* {1.0249, 0.7738} {4.0650, 0.6634} {1.1532, 0.1497}*
Indirect through T1 PTSS {3.0300, 2.9537} {1.8815, 0.1247} {0.9924, 0.3929} {0.9851, 2.1684} {0.1602, 0.8889}
Indirect through T2 PTSS {1.9934, 0.7487} {0.1375, 0.7452} {0.4428, 0.2028} {1.4728, 0.1533} {0.4323, 0.0526}*
Indirect through T1 and T2 PTSS {3.0390, 1.2190} {0.2423, 1.1903} {0.7246, 0.3427} {2.4080, 0.2991} {0.6632, 0.0902}*
95% Confidence Intervals are presented in brackets. Confidence intervals that do not include 0 (null association) are significant. *Significant at 0.05. PTG, posttraumatic growth; PTSS,
posttraumatic symptoms; DAS, dyadic adjustment scale.
FIGURE 1 | Unstandardized coefficients b(SE) for the association between PTG and Dyadic Adjustment total score through the sequential mediation of PTSS at T1
and T2, among Husbands. Explained variance is located above all dependent variables. PTG, posttraumatic growth; PTSS, posttraumatic symptoms; DAS, dyadic
adjustment scale. **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
demonstrated, for the first time, that PTG is not only related to
the intensification of PTSS over time, but also to negative changes
in dyadic adjustment. This pattern of results might suggest that
PTG reflects illusory defenses (e.g., Lahav et al., 2016) and are in
line with former studies that indicated a gap between reports of
PTG and actual interpersonal behavior among trauma survivors
(Hobfoll et al., 2007). Hence, while trauma survivors report
growth, they may suffer from the deterioration of their most
intimate relationship.
Holding beliefs of growth might reflect the efforts of primary
and secondary trauma survivors to cope with trauma’s negative
repercussions. Individuals who have been exposed to severe
traumatic events experience a dismantling of basic beliefs about
the self and the world and, as a result, suffer from intense feelings
of horror and helplessness (Janoff-Bulman, 1989). While facing
extreme distress they might find possessing growth beliefs to be
comforting and adopt these beliefs as a coping strategy. However,
our findings suggest that in the long term this method of coping
could be maladaptive and might be associated with low dyadic
adjustment.
One possible explanation for the association between PTG
and lowered dyadic adjustment is that maintaining positive
beliefs of growth might limit the trauma survivors’ ability to
authentically express emotions and receive support within the
marital relationship. While trying to cling on to the growth
beliefs, war veterans and their wives might avoid expressing
their vulnerabilities and sharing their difficulties regarding their
trauma. Emotions such as helplessness, hopelessness, frustration,
anger, accusation, and blame regarding their traumatic past
are not pronounced within the relationship and the current
difficulties, resulting from the traumatic experience, are not
genuinely expressed. Moreover, in order to keep the defensive
beliefs of growth, war veterans and their wives might avoid asking
their spouses for help, further lowering the chance of receiving
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
FIGURE 2 | Unstandardized coefficients b(SE) for the association between PTG and Dyadic Adjustment affection subscale through the sequential mediation of PTSS
at Tl and T2, among Wives. Explained variance is located above all dependent variables. PTG, posttraumatic growth; PTSS, posttraumatic symptoms; DAS, dyadic
adjustment scale. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
FIGURE 3 | APIM nested Model of Husbands’ and Wives’ PTG and dyadic adjustment. Curved lines represent covariates between constructs. Solid lines represent
significant predictions. The other prediction axes were non-significant. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
the much needed social support from within their relationship.
These tendencies might impair intimacy and satisfaction in
marital relationships (Swann et al., 1994), thereby undermining
the dyadic adjustment. It should be noted that the current
explanation is speculative in nature, as the present study did
not assess the previously mentioned mechanisms, such as wives’
emotional authenticity or support within the marriage. Future
prospective studies should explore the processes underlying the
relations between PTG and dyadic adjustment.
At the same time, as the current results indicated, PTG might
negatively impact dyadic adjustment through increased PTSS.
Trauma survivors, such as ex-POWs, war veterans and their
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
TABLE 4 | APIM nested model for the associations between husbands’ and
wives’ PTG and DAS over time—standardized coefficients.
Path βSE
PTG T1 (H) PTG T2 (H) 0.52*** 0.07
DAS T1 (H) DAS T2 (H) 0.52*** 0.09
PTG T1 (W) PTG T2 (W) 0.69*** 0.07
DAS T1 (W) DAS T2 (W) 0.37*** 0.10
PTG T1 (W) PTG T2 (H) 0.18* 0.06
DAS T1 (H) DAS T2 (W) 0.29** 0.11
PTG T1 (W) DAS T1 (H) 0.20** 2.53
PTG, posttraumatic growth; DAS, dyadic adjustment scale, (H), husbands; (W), wives.
*p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.
wives, who maintain growth beliefs, may use it as a way to deny
the negative consequences of the trauma. Under these conditions,
PTG could serve as a way to avoid acknowledging their emotional
pain and losses resulting from the trauma, which prevents
working through the traumatic event (Maercker and Zoellner,
2004) and modifying the fear structure that is responsible for
posttraumatic reaction (Foa et al., 1989). This, in turn, enhances
distress and PTSS over time, which negatively affects marital
relations and thus lower dyadic adjustment (Cook et al., 2004).
The present results indicated, however, that the
aforementioned path, is more evident among husbands than
wives. While PTSS mediated the association between PTG and all
of the dyadic adjustment subscales among husbands, it mediated
only the association between PTG and the dyadic adjustment
affection subscale among wives. One possibility is that the results
reflect gender differences. Another possibility is that the results
are rooted in the type of exposure to trauma. While the negative
effect of PTG on dyadic adjustment is potentially explained
mainly by the amplification of PTSS among primary trauma
survivors, this might be apparent only in part among secondary
trauma survivors. Lastly, it might be that the differences in the
magnitude of posttraumatic reactions are responsible for the
results. Hence, PTSS has a broader role as a mechanism among
the husbands, who suffer from elevated PTSS, compared to the
wives, who reported lower levels. The present study does not
allow for differentiation between these alternatives, therefore we
offer only speculations.
Our results regarding the prediction of the husbands’ PTG
by the wives’ PTG are the first of their kind. The current
findings further the notion of transmission between spouses in
the aftermath of trauma. It seems that the transmission within
the marital dyad is not restricted to posttraumatic reaction, as
indicated in the contagion theory (Figley, 1986), but may also
occur in regards to posttraumatic growth beliefs.
Interestingly, growth cognitions were found to be transmitted
from the wives to the husbands, and not the other way around. It
could be that these findings reflect the important role of the wives
in shaping the way their traumatized husbands cope. As men tend
to rely primarily on their wives for intimacy (e.g., Hobfoll et al.,
1996), they might adopt their wives’ growth beliefs. Specifically,
one may speculate that interacting with a wife who maintains
growth beliefs could enhance the husband’s risk of implementing
defensive growth beliefs as an avoidant strategy.
Although the transmission of growth cognitions within the
marital dyad perhaps enhances the existence of a similar outlook
regarding the traumatic event, at the same time it could be
related to negative outcomes and specifically to lower dyadic
adjustment. Investigation of these associations between PTG and
dyadic adjustment among both spouses indicated that wives’ PTG
predicted a decrease in the husbands’ dyadic adjustment over
time, but not the other way around. One might offer two main
explanations for the present trend.
The directionality of the associations between spouses might
be rooted in gender roles. It is possible that both men
and women with high PTG not only avoid expressing their
own vulnerabilities, they are also defensive in the face of
others’ emotional distress, and experience it as a threat toward
their growth beliefs. However, while women often have close
relationships with other women, men tend to be more dependent
on their wives for emotional support (e.g., Hobfoll et al.,
1996). Therefore, men might feel more helpless in the face of
their spouses’ emotional defensiveness, compared to women,
and report lower dyadic adjustment as a consequence of their
wives’ PTG.
Alternatively, the present findings might be related to
the different types of exposure to trauma. Primary trauma
survivors, as the husbands in our study, exposed particularly
to manmade trauma, often suffer from elevated shame and
self-blame regarding the traumatic event (Leskela et al., 2002)
and are sensitive to others’ responses regarding their trauma
(Hong et al., 2011). Moreover, due to elevated emotional distress,
firsthand trauma survivors often tend to be more dependent
on their significant others for emotional support (Dekel, 2007).
Hence, it might be that trauma survivors who were exposed
directly to trauma, such as ex-POWs and war combatants, are
more vulnerable to their spouses’ growth beliefs, compared to
their wives, who were exposed indirectly. Ex-POWs and war
combatants might feel invalidated or ashamed of their difficulties
when facing their wives’ growth beliefs regarding the trauma, and
respond with heightened aloofness and emotional detachment
in the relationship. These, in turn, sabotage their relationship,
leading to low dyadic adjustment.
In our data, gender and type of trauma exposure are
intertwined. Hence, we were unable to distinguish between
these competing explanations regarding the directionality of the
associations between spouses. Moreover, the effects of gender
role and type of trauma exposure in association with PTG and
dyadic adjustment have not been studied. Therefore, no definite
conclusions can be reached.
Several limitations may have affected our findings. First, this
study was based on self-report measures, which may be subject to
response biases and shared method variance. Second, the present
study did not include data regarding PTG and dyadic adjustment
immediately after the trauma, rather only decades after the
traumatic event. This presented us with major constraints in
our ability to assess whether the ramifications of PTG on dyadic
adjustment depended on the amount of time it is in effect.
Third, the current study did not include data regarding PTG
or PTSS in relation to other potential traumatic events that
the participants might have experienced or data regarding the
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
suggested mechanism for the relations between PTG and dyadic
adjustment. This prevented us from controlling for the effects
of other important variables that may have shaped the present
results. Lastly, as was previously mentioned, gender and type
of trauma exposure were intertwined in the present data. This
prevented us from distinguishing between their different effects.
Future prospective studies should use PTG measures at different
time intervals to assess the temporal effects of PTG on dyadic
adjustment, and consider gender as well as type of trauma
exposure separately.
The present findings have important implications for theory
and treatment of direct and indirect survivors of trauma. Our
results call attention to the possible role of PTG with regard
to negative marital outcomes. War veterans, ex-POWs and
their wives who report PTG might be at-risk for low dyadic
adjustment. Moreover, this potential negative effect of PTG
on the marital realm may be transmitted from one spouse
to the other. This possibility suggests the need for caution
by the therapist when treating trauma survivors in regards to
encouraging reports of PTG—although in some cases these
reports might reflect true positive changes, in others they might
indicate efforts to deny the trauma and could have negative
implications. However, given the controversy regarding the
nature of PTG, we recommend that more studies be conducted
before any further conclusions can be drawn.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
YL and YK Made substantial contributions to the conception
and design of the work as well as the analysis and interpretation
of data for the work; Drafted the work and revised it critically
for content; Gave final approval of the version to be published;
Agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in
ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity
of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and
resolved. ZS Made substantial contributions to the conception
and design of the work as well as the analysis and interpretation
of data for the work; Drafted the work and revised it
critically for content; Gave final approval of the version to be
published.
REFERENCES
Aldwin, C. M., Levenson, M. R., and Spiro, A. (1994). Vulnerability and resilience
to combat exposure: can stress have lifelong effects? Psychol. Aging 9, 34–44.
doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.1.34
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-
Text Revision (DSM-IV-TRim, 2000). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., Veiel, H., and Andrews, B. (1990). Self-
esteem and depression. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 25, 225–234.
doi: 10.1007/BF00788643
Calhoun, L. G., and Tedeschi, R. G. (2006). Handbook of Posttraumatic Growth:
Research and Practice. New York, NY: Routledge.
Carver, C. S., and Antoni, M. H. (2004). Finding benefit in breast cancer during
the year after diagnosis predicts better adjustment 5 to 8 years after diagnosis.
Health Psychol. 23, 5950-598. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.6.595
Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., and Kam, C.-M. (2001). A comparison of inclusive
and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychol. Methods
6, 330–351. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.6.4.330
Cook, J. M., Riggs, D. S., Thompson, R., Coyne, J. C., and Sheikh, J. I.
(2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder and current relationship functioning
among World War II ex-prisoners of war. J. Fam. Psychol. 18, 36–45.
doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.36
Cook, W. L., and Kenny, D. A. (2005). The actor–partner interdependence model:
a model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29,
101–109. doi: 10.1080/01650250444000405
Dekel, R. (2007). Posttraumatic distress and growth among wives of
prisoners of war: the contribution of husbands’ posttraumatic stress
disorder and wives’ own attachment. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 77, 419–426.
doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.3.419
Dekel, R., Enoch, G., and Solomon, Z. (2008). The contribution of captivity and
post-traumatic stress disorder to marital adjustment of Israeli couples. J. Soc.
Pers. Relat. 25, 497–510. doi: 10.1177/0265407508090870
Dekel, S., Ein-Dor, T., and Solomon, Z. (2012). Posttraumatic growth and
posttraumatic distress: a longitudinal study. Psychol. Trauma Theor. Res. Pract.
Policy 4, 94–101. doi: 10.1037/a0021865
Engdahl, B., Dikel, T. N., Eberly, R., and Blank, A. Jr. (1997). Posttraumatic
stress disorder in a community group of former prisoners of war: a
normative response to severe trauma. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 1576–1581.
doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1576
Feder, A., Southwick, S. M., Goetz, R. R., Wang, Y., Alonso, A., Smith, B. W., et al.
(2008). Posttraumatic growth in former Vietnam prisoners of war. Psychiatry
71, 359–370. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2008.71.4.359
Figley, C. R. (1986). Trauma and Its Wake: Traumatic Stress Theory, Research and
Intervention. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel.
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., and Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive
conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav. Ther. 20, 155–176.
doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80067-X
Frazier, P., Conlon, A., and Glaser, T. (2001). Positive and negative life
changes following sexual assault. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69, 1048–1055.
doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.6.1048
Galovski, T., and Lyons, J. A. (2004). Psychological sequelae of combat violence: A
review of the impact of PTSD on the veteran’s family and possibleinter ventions.
Aggress. Violent Behav. 9, 477–501. doi: 10.1016/S1359-1789(03)0
0045-4
Gewirtz, A. H., Polusny, M. A., DeGarmo, D. S., Khaylis, A., and Erbes, C.
R. (2010). Posttraumatic stress symptoms among National Guard soldiers
deployed to Iraq: associations with parenting behaviors and couple adjustment.
J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 78, 599–610 doi: 10.1037/a0020571
Greene, T., Lahav, Y., Bronstein, I., and Solomon, Z. (2014). The role of ex-POWs
PTSD symptoms and trajectories in wives’ secondary traumatization. J. Fam.
Psychol. 28, 666–674. doi: 10.1037/a0037848
Hayes, A. F., Preacher, K. J., and Myers, T. A. (2011). “Mediation and
the estimation of indirect effects in political communication research, in
Sourcebook for Political Communication Research: Methods, Measures, and
Analytical Techniques, eds E. P. Bucy and R. Lance Holbert (New York, NY:
Routledge), 434–465.
Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., and Tomich, P. L. (2006). A meta-analytic
review of benefit finding and growth. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 74, 797–816.
doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.797
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and Recovery. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Heyman, R. E., Sayers, S. L., and Bellack, A. S. (1994). Global marital satisfaction
versus marital adjustment: an empirical comparison of three measures. J. Fam.
Psychol. 8, 432–446. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.8.4.432
Hobfoll, S. E., Cameron, R. P., Chapman, H. A., and Gallagher, R. W. (1996).
“Social support and social coping in couples, in Handbook of Social Support
and the Family, eds G. R. Pierce, B. R. Sarason, and I.G. Sarason (New York,
NY: Springer), 413–433.
Hobfoll, S. E., Hall, B. J., Canetti-Nisim, D., Galea, S., Johnson, R. J., and Palmieri,
P. A. (2007). Refining our understanding of traumatic growth in the face of
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
Lahav et al. Posttraumatic Growth and Dyadic Adjustment
terrorism: moving from meaning cognitions to doing what is meaningful. Appl.
Psychol. 56, 345–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00292.x
Hong, P. Y., Ilardi, S. S., and Lishner, D. A. (2011). The aftermath of trauma: the
impact of perceived and anticipated invalidation of childhood sexual abuse
on borderline symptomatology. Psychol. Trauma Theor. Res. Pract. Policy 3,
360–368. doi: 10.1037/a0021261
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic
events: applications of the schema construct. Soc. Cogn. 7, 113–136.
doi: 10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.113
Kashy, D. A., and Kenny, D. A. (2000). “The analysis of data from dyads and
groups, in Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology,
eds H. T. Reis and C. M. Judd (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press),
451–477.
Lahav, Y., Seligman, Z., and Solomon, Z. (2017). “Countertransference in the face
of growth: Reenactment of the trauma, in Countertransference in Perspective:
The Double-Edged Sword of the Patient-Therapist Emotional Relationship, eds
D. R. Aleksandrowicz and A. O. Aleksandrowicz (Sussex: Academic Publishing;
Academic Press), 57–79.
Lahav, Y., Solomon, Z., and Levin, Y. (2016). Posttraumatic growth and perceived
health: the role of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry. 86,
693–703. doi: 10.1037/ort0000155
Ledermann, T., Macho, S., and Kenny, D. A. (2011). Assessing mediation in dyadic
data using the actor-partner interdependence model. Struct. Equat. Model. 18,
595–612. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.607099
Leskela, J., Dieperink, M., and Thuras, P. (2002). Shame and posttraumatic stress
disorder. J. Trauma. Stress 15, 223–226. doi: 10.1023/A:1015255311837
Maercker, A., and Zoellner, T. (2004). The Janus face of self-perceived growth:
toward a two-component model of posttraumatic growth. Psychol. Inq. 15,
41–48.
Manne, S., Ostroff, J., Winkel, G., Goldstein, L., Fox, K., and Grana, G.
(2004). Posttraumatic growth after breast cancer: patient, partner, and couple
perspectives. Psychosom. Med. 66, 442–454.
McCormack, L., Hagger, M. S., and Joseph, S. (2011). Vicarious growth in wives
of vietnam veterans a phenomenological investigation into decades of “lived”
experience. J. Hum. Psychol. 51, 273–290. doi: 10.1177/0022167810377506
McFarland, C., and Alvaro, C. (2000). The impact of motivation on temporal
comparisons: coping with traumatic events by perceiving personal growth. J.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 327–343. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.327
Neria, Y., Solomon, Z., Ginzburg, K., Dekel, R., Enoch, D., and Ohry, A. (2000).
Posttraumatic residues of captivity: a follow-up of Israeli ex-prisoners of war. J.
Clin. Psychiatry 61, 39–46. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v61n0110
Page, W. F. (1992). The Health of Former Prisoners of War:: Results from the
Medical Examination Survey of Former POWs of World War II and the Korean
Conflict. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., and Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality
and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 576–593.
doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00393.x
Renshaw, K. D., Blais, R. K., and Caska, C. M. (2011). “Distress in spouses
of combat veterans with PTSD: The importance of interpersonally based
cognitions and behaviors, in Risk and Resilience in US Military Families, eds
S. MacDermid Wadsworth and D. Rigg (New York, NY: Springer), 69–84.
Renshaw, K. D., Rodrigues, C. S., and Jones, D. H. (2008). Psychological symptoms
and marital satisfaction in spouses of Operation Iraqi Freedom veterans:
relationships with spouses’ perceptions of veterans’ experiences and symptoms.
J. Fam. Psychol. 22, 586–594. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.586
Solomon, Z., Benbenishty, R., Neria, Y., Abramowitz, M., Ginzburg, K., and Ohry,
A. (1993). Assessment of PTSD: validation of the revised PTSD inventory. Isr.
J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 30, 110–115.
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing
the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38, 15–28.
doi: 10.2307/350547
Swann, W. B. Jr., De La Ronde, C., and Hixon, J. G. (1994). Authenticity and
positivity strivings in marriage and courtship. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 857–869.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.857
Tedeschi, R. G., and Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The Posttraumatic growth inventory:
measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J. Trauma. Stress 9, 455–471.
doi: 10.1002/jts.2490090305
Ursano, R. J., Wheatley, R., Sledge, W., Rahe, A., and Carlson, E. (1986). Coping
and recovery styles in the Vietnam era prisoner of war. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 174,
707–714. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198612000-00001
Weiss, T. (2004). Correlates of posttraumatic growth in husbands of breast cancer
survivors. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 733–746. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.5.733.50750
Zalta, A. K., Gerhart, J., Hall, B. J., Rajan, K. B., Vechiu, C., Canetti, D.,
et al. (2017). Self-reported posttraumatic growth predicts greater subsequent
posttraumatic stress amidst war and terrorism. Anxiety Stress Coping 30,
176–187. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2016.1229467
Zerach, G., Ben-David, A., Solomon, Z., and Heruti, R. (2010).
Posttraumatic symptoms, marital intimacy, dyadic adjustment, and
sexual satisfaction among ex-prisoners of war. J. Sex. Med. 7, 2739–2749.
doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.01784.x
Zoellner, T., and Maercker, A. (2006). Posttraumatic growth in clinical
psychology—A critical review and introduction of a two component model.
Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 626–653. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.008
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2017 Lahav, Kanat-Maymon and Solomon. This is an open-access
article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC
BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1102
... All studies included in the review were quantitative research studies. Two studies were longitudinal in design and both utilized two time points (Avidor et al., 2022;Lahav et al., 2017). All other studies were cross-sectional. ...
... Lastly, mixed results included partial or lacking results in the two longitudinal studies. However, one of these studies (Avidor et al., 2022) reported poor model fit and subsequent exclusion of PTSD variables from the model, and the other study (Lahav et al., 2017) did not test a direct path between couple functioning and secondary trauma survivor functioning. ...
... While treating the PTSD symptoms of the primary trauma survivor as a predictor among secondary trauma survivor functioning and relationship variables makes theoretical sense and has received due attention, longitudinal work to determine the directionality of effects between all parts of the couple system (primary trauma survivor, secondary trauma survivor, and couple functioning) is needed. This is further evident in the fact that most studies in this review were cross-sectional, and the two longitudinal studies (Avidor et al., 2022;Lahav et al., 2017) lacked a robust examination of the association of interest. Despite their mostly cross-sectional nature, it was interesting to see that eight studies assumed directionality from secondary trauma survivor measures to couple measures (Avidor et al., 2022;Bachem et al., 2020Bachem et al., , 2021Dirkzwager et al., 2005;Hamilton et al., 2009;Lahav et al., 2017Lahav et al., , 2019Riggs, 2014), whereas four studies assessed directionality from couple functioning to secondary trauma survivor distress (Brosseau et al., 2011;Dekel et al., 2016;Ein-Dor et al., 2010;Solomon et al., 1991), and three assessed directionality both ways (Campbell & Renshaw, 2012;Dekel, 2010;Lev-Wiesel & Amir, 2001). ...
Article
Systemic theories addressing posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in couples postulate associations between primary trauma survivor functioning, secondary trauma survivor functioning, and couple functioning. However, there is a lack of examination of the association between secondary trauma survivor functioning and couple functioning, which has clinical implications. Objectives of this study include informing clinicians of the evidence base for these associations and providing a synthesized review of research on PTSD in couples to inform future research. A systematic research synthesis screening 150 articles from three databases resulted in the inclusion of 15 quantitative articles to examine the quality of the available research addressing the association between secondary trauma survivor functioning and couple functioning. Correlation matrices in all studies and other partial evidence supported the current theory positing the relevance of secondary traumatic stress in interpersonal functioning for couples. Discussion includes the need for increased quality and diversity of systemic trauma research and treatment for couples.
... Thus, parents' secondary PTSS may shake their world assumptions (Janoff-Bulman, 2010) and ignite emotional distress (e.g., feeling of guilt, shame, and fear), that, after deliberate elaboration, may result in experience of growth. Moreover, both the severity of veterans' PTSS (Lahav et al., 2017) and their chronic and delayed PTSD trajectories were associated with spouses' highest SPTG (Greene et al., 2015). A question remains regarding the unique contribution of veterans' PTSS, compared with parents' own secondary PTSS, to parents' SPTG. ...
... Moreover, veterans' DT was associated with lower levels of their PTG but neither veterans' DT not parents' DT moderated the PTSS-PTG links. Although the concept of PTG still gives rise to scientific debate and controversy (e.g., Lahav et al., 2017), to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to report on SPTG among parents of veterans, some of whom are suffering from PTSD. It should be noted that we did not assess whether veterans and parents currently live together or how much time they spend together and the quality of their relationship. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Indirect exposure to traumatized combat veterans may facilitate positive transformations in the form of posttraumatic growth (PTG) among family members. We aimed to use a dyadic approach to explore the association between veterans’ and parents’ posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) and their PTG and that of their parents’ secondary PTG (SPTG), as well as to examine the moderating role of distress tolerance (DT) in these associations. Method: A volunteer sample of 102 dyads of Israeli combat veterans and their parents responded to online validated self-report questionnaires. Results: Veterans’ PTG was positively correlated with parents’ SPTG. Moreover, parents’ secondary PTSS was associated with higher levels of their own SPTG and their veteran offspring’s PTG. Furthermore, veterans’ DT contributed to lower levels of their own PTSS and their PTG, but the moderation effects of DT were not found. Conclusions: Parents’ experience of secondary PTSS, which refers to their offspring’s military service, may be also associated with their offspring higher levels of PTG.
... Researchers found that the most significant predictors of resilience were a more mature life, optimism and less expectancy of the time of confinement. Other research also confirms that extraordinary insight, self-confidence, optimism and greater capacity for growth are positive predictors of PTG (4,40) and demonstrated that the absence of active confrontation and weaker emotional control contributed to the adverse outcomes of traumatization (41). ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies have investigated the various consequences of traumatic experiences during the war. The most significant disorder that occurs as a result of war traumatization is posttraumatic stress disorder, in addition to which other psychological disorders often can occur. In the last few decades, the number of studies researching the occurrence of posttraumatic growth as a positive outcome of trauma, including that of war veterans, has been growing. Among war veterans, prisoners of war stand out for the intensity of their traumatic experience but also for the appearance and intensity of the pathological outcomes of the trauma. Studies show that in this group of veterans, posttraumatic stress disorder and comorbidity disorders often persist for decades after their release from captivity.There is not much research about the positive outcomes of trauma in these particularly vulnerable populations, and this paper is a review of several different studies, the results of which show that posttraumatic growth is possible even after challenging traumatic experiences such as war captivity.
... Understanding reports of PTG in light of dissociation (Lahav, Bellin, et al., 2016), might clarify the mixed findings in the literature (Helgeson et al., 2006;Linley & Joseph, 2004;Liu et al., 2017;Shakespeare-Finch & Lurie-Beck, 2014), including relations between PTG and lower levels of negative outcomes in some cases (Frazier et al., 2001;Lee et al., 2019;Lev-Wiesel & Amir, 2003) and relations between PTG and elevated levels of negative outcomes (Greene et al., 2015;Hamam et al., 2020;Lahav, Bellin, et al., 2016;Lahav, Kanat-Maymon, et al., 2017;, in others. The former findings might reflect an authentic positive transformation, which could be adaptive, whereas the latter might reflect dissociation-based beliefs of PTG, which could exacerbate survivors' distress (Lahav, Bellin, et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Childhood abuse puts individuals at risk for psychopathology and psychiatric symptoms such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and anxiety symptoms. At the same time, research has indicated that some survivors report positive transformations in the aftermath of their trauma, known as posttraumatic growth (PTG). Yet the essence of PTG reports is questionable, and some scholars claim that it may reflect maladaptive illusory qualities. Furthermore, according to a recent theoretical model, PTG might be dissociation-based and related to survivors' bonds with their perpetrators. This study aimed to explore these claims by assessing PTG, dissociation, and identification with the aggressor (IWA), as well as PTSD and anxiety symptoms. An online survey was conducted among 597 adult childhood abuse survivors. Study variables were assessed via self-report measures. Analyses indicated positive associations between PTG, dissociation, and IWA. Three distinct profiles were found, reflecting high, medium, and low scores on PTG, dissociation, and IWA. Profile type explained PTSD and anxiety symptoms above and beyond gender, age, and abuse severity. These findings suggest that whereas some childhood abuse survivors might experience a positive transformation subsequent to their trauma, others' PTG reports might reflect dissociative mechanisms and pathological attachments to their perpetrators , and thus might be maladaptive. ARTICLE HISTORY
... The coping style was determined by the median scores on the 2 scales. 16,24 Accordingly, patients were grouped to 4 different coping styles: participants with anxiety scores below the median and defensiveness scores above the median were classified as repressors. Participants with anxiety and defensiveness scores below median were classified as low-anxious; high-anxious individuals were those with anxiety score above median, and defensiveness score below median; and defensive individuals had anxiety and defensiveness scores both above median. ...
Article
Background Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) can be triggered by life-threatening medical emergencies, such as stroke. Data suggest that up to 25% of stroke survivors will develop PTSD symptomatology, but little is known about predisposing factors. We sought to examine whether neuroimaging measures and coping styles are related to PTSD symptoms after stroke. Methods Participants were survivors of first-ever, mild-moderate ischemic stroke, or transient ischemic attack from the TABASCO study (Tel Aviv Brain Acute Stroke Cohort). All participants underwent a 3T magnetic resonance imaging at baseline and were examined 6, 12, and 24 months thereafter, using neurological, neuropsychological, and functional evaluations. At baseline, coping styles were evaluated by a self-reported questionnaire. PTSD symptoms were assessed using the PTSD checklist. Data were available for 436 patients. Results Forty-eight participants (11%) developed probable PTSD (PTSD checklist ≥44) during the first year after the stroke/transient ischemic attack. Stroke was more likely to cause PTSD than transient ischemic attack. Stroke severity, larger white matter lesion volume, and worse hippocampal connectivity were associated with PTSD severity, while infarct volume or location was not. In a multivariate analysis, high-anxious and defensive coping styles were associated with a 6.66-fold higher risk of developing poststroke PTSD ([95% CI, 2.08–21.34]; P <0.01) compared with low-anxious and repressive coping styles, after adjusting for age, education, stroke severity, brain atrophy, and depression. Conclusions In our cohort, PTSD was a common sequela among stroke survivors. We suggest that risk factors for PTSD development include stroke severity, white matter damage, and premorbid coping styles. Early identification of at-risk patients is key to effective treatment.
Article
Full-text available
Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menguji dan memprediksi kepercayaan dan komitmen pada relasi dekat dalam dua tahap studi. Studi 1 dilakukan untuk menguji jalur antara penyesuaian, dependensi, kepercayaan, dan komitmen pada relasi dekat dengan menggunakan Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). Studi 2 dilakukan untuk memprediksi tingkat kepercayaan dan tingkat komitmen dengan menambah jenis kelamin dan jenis relasi sebagai prediktor dengan implementasi Jaringan Saraf Tiruan (JST) model Multilayer Perceptron (MLP), yang dilakukan dalam 27 percobaan (3 hidden layer x 3 learning rate x 3 epoch) untuk masing-masing tingkat kepercayaan dan tingkat komitmen. Studi 1 dan Studi 2 dilakukan terhadap 417 responden (103 pria, 314 wanita; 97 pacaran, 320 persahabatan). Pengumpulan data dilakukan dengan menggunakan kuesioner adaptasi Revised Dyadic Adjustment Scale (11 aitem; α = 0,78), Investment Model Scale (15 aitem; αdependensi = 0,886; αkomitmen = 0,933), dan Trust in Close Relationship Scale (13 aitem; α = 0,917). Hasil Studi 1 menunjukkan pengaruh penyesuaian dan dependensi terhadap kepercayaan; serta pengaruh dependensi dan kepercayaan terhadap komitmen pada relasi dekat. Hasil studi 2 menunjukkan model JST terbaik untuk memprediksi tingkat kepercayaan ialah dengan konfigurasi 4 hidden layer units, 0,75 learning rate, dan 1000 epochs dengan tingkat akurasi sebesar 75,85%; sementara, model JST terbaik untuk memprediksi tingkat komitmen ialah dengan konfigurasi 4 hidden layer units, 0,75 learning rate, dan 2500 epochs dengan tingkat akurasi sebesar 79,95%. Kata kunci: kepercayaan, komitmen, relasi dekat. Abstract This research aimed to examine and predict trust and commitment in close relationships under two studies. Study 1 examined the path of adjustment, dependence, trust, and commitment in close relationships using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). Study 2 was conducted to predict trust and commitment levels by adding gender and type of relationships as predictors by implementing Artificial Neural Network (ANN) with Multilayer Perceptron (MLP) model, which was conducted in 27 experiments (3 hidden layers x 3 learning levels x 3 epochs) for each both trust and commitment level. Study 1 and Study 2 were conducted on 417 respondents (103 male, 314 female, 97 dating, 320 friendships). Data collection was carried out using the adaptation questionnaire of the Revised Dyadic Adjustment Scale (11 items; α = 0.78), Investment Model Scale (15 items; αdependence = 0,886; αcommitment = 0,933), and Trust in Close Relationship Scale (13 items; α = 0.917). The result of Study 1 showed the effect of adjustment and dependence on trust; and the effect of dependence and trust on commitment in close relationships. The result of Study 2 showed that the best ANN model to predict trust level is by using the configuration of 4 hidden layer units, 0.75 learning rates, and 1000 epochs with an accuracy rate of 75.85%; while the best ANN model to predict commitment level is by the configuration of 4 hidden layer units, 0.75 learning rates, and 2500 epochs with an accuracy rate of 79.95%.
Article
Full-text available
Antecedentes: Los eventos altamente estresantes pueden tener efectos nocivos en el bienestar y salud mental en las personas. Objetivo: Evaluar la eficacia de un protocolo de intervención preventiva basado en la Terapia Sistémica Breve orientado a personas que vivenciaron de manera reciente un evento altamente estresante. Método: Participaron 75 personas, 39 conformaron el grupo experimental y 36 el grupo control. Para el análisis de los datos se siguió un diseño cuantitativo con dos grupos (experimental y control) y con dos puntos temporales (pre y post-intervención). Al grupo experimental se añadió un seguimiento a los seis meses. El protocolo estuvo estructurado en cuatro sesiones y apuntaba a incidir sobre los síntomas postraumáticos, el crecimiento postraumático, la respuesta rumiativa y las estrategias de afrontamiento. Resultados: Se observó un aumento del crecimiento postraumático, de la rumiación deliberada, del afrontamiento centrado en el problema y de la reinterpretación positiva en el grupo experimental. En cambio, solo hubo una tendencia no significativa en la reducción de la sintomatología postraumática y en la búsqueda de apoyo social. Conclusiones: Estos resultados contribuyen al desarrollo de la Terapia Sistémica Breve y apoyan su utilidad en la intervención preventiva con personas expuestas a un evento altamente estresante.
Article
Research emphasizing the negative psychosocial impacts of service-related post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) for military members, veterans, and their families is well established. Post-traumatic growth (PTG), positive psychological change resulting from managing adverse life events and situations, is an alternate outcome considered in research focusing on the impacts of serious illness and other life-altering circumstances on families. Little is known, however, about the processes that create and sustain PTG within military and veteran family systems. This paper will review conceptualizations of PTG and consider its relevance as a construct for analysis of outcomes related to the experiences of partners of military members and veterans living with PTSD and other operational stress injuries.
Article
This study examined the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation in the relationship between calling and posttraumatic growth (PTG) and the moderating role of transformational leadership among Air Force pilots. A total of 215 ROK Air Force pilots participated in this study twice with an interval of 4 weeks. The results of this study were as follows. First, calling, transformational leadership, adaptive emotion regulation, and PTG showed statistically significant correlations. Second, a mediating model showed that the relationship between calling and PTG was mediated by adaptive emotion regulation. Third, the moderation effect of transformational leadership in the relationship calling on adaptive emotion regulation was found. Finally, transformational leadership also moderated the mediating effect of calling on PTG through adaptive emotion regulation was identified. Implications, limitations, and future research suggestions were discussed.
Article
Full-text available
We proposed that married persons would want their spouses to see them as they saw themselves but that dating persons would want their relationship partners to evaluate them favorably. A survey of 176 married and dating couples tested these predictions. Just as married persons were most intimate with spouses whose evaluations verified their self-views, dating persons were most intimate with partners who evaluated them favorably. For married people with negative self-views, then, intimacy increased as their spouses evaluated them more negatively. Marriage apparently precipitates a shift from a desire for positive evaluations to a desire for self-verifying evaluations.
Article
Full-text available
Secondary traumatization describes the phenomenon whereby those in proximity to trauma survivors develop psychological symptoms similar to those experienced by the direct survivor. The current study examined secondary trauma (ST) and generalized distress symptoms (general psychiatric symptomatology, functional disability, and self-rated health) in wives of former prisoners of war (ex-POWs). The study compared wives of Israeli ex-POWs from the 1973 Yom Kippur War with wives of a matched control group of non-POW Yom Kippur War combat veterans (CVs). The wives also were divided into groups based on their husbands' current posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) status and PTSD trajectory (i.e., chronic, delayed), and their outcomes were compared with resilient CVs. We found that wives of ex-POWs with PTSD reported higher ST and generalized distress than wives of ex-POWs and non-POW CVs without PTSD. Wives of ex-POWs with chronic PTSD reported the highest levels of functional disability. We also found that the relationships between husbands' prior captivity, and wives' ST and general psychiatric symptomatology were fully mediated by the husbands' PTSD symptoms. These findings indicate that it is exposure to a partner with PTSD that leads to overall ST and other distress symptoms, and not simply to a trauma survivor. Furthermore, the more severe their husbands' PTSD, the more wives are at risk for ST and general psychiatric symptomatology. Wives of partners with PTSD should therefore be considered high-risk groups for ST and distress that may require targeted interventions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Chapter
Full-text available
Treatment of survivors of childhood sexual abuse (CSA) poses difficult challenges for therapists. This is, namely, because of the wide array of powerful countertransference (CT) reactions. A vast amount of literature exists which describes these patterns. However, discussion until now has predominantly focused on the reaction of therapists to the negative effects of the trauma. The present chapter addresses an aspect of CT that, to the best of the authors’ knowledge, has yet to receive any attention in the literature on trauma: countertransference in response to reports of gains or positive changes after trauma. This has been defined in the literature as posttraumatic growth (PTG). Exploring countertransferencial reactions to PTG and the mutual effects that occur between the two, will provide further insight into the phenomenon of PTG. It will also assist therapists in assuming responsibility for their part in the appearance of PTG and the ways it unfolds in therapy. Five distinct positions of countertransference will be presented: dissociated therapist; therapist as perpetrator; therapist as neglectful parent; therapist as rescuer; and mourning therapist. The authors argue that these positions should be considered as reenactments of dissociated self-states and relational dynamics, rooted in the original trauma, that must be worked through in psychotherapy in order to foster integration and healing.
Article
Full-text available
Background and objective: This study tested three alternative explanations for research indicating a positive, but heterogeneous relationship between self-reported posttraumatic growth (PTG) and posttraumatic stress symptoms (PSS): a) the third-variable hypothesis that the relationship between PTG and PSS is a spurious one driven by positive relationships with resource loss, b) the growth over time hypothesis that the relationship between PTG and PSS is initially a positive one, but becomes negative over time, and c) the moderator hypothesis that resource loss moderates the relationship between PTG and PSS such that PTG is associated with lower levels of PSS as loss increases. Design and method: A nationally representative sample (N = 1622) of Israelis was assessed at 3 time points during a period of ongoing violence. PTG, resource loss, and the interaction between PTG and loss were examined as lagged predictors of PSS to test the proposed hypotheses. Results: Results were inconsistent with all 3 hypotheses, showing that PTG positively predicted subsequent PSS when accounting for main and interactive effects of loss. Conclusions: Our results suggest that self-reported PTG is a meaningful but counterintuitive predictor of poorer mental health following trauma.
Article
Full-text available
The contested discourse regarding the nature of posttraumatic growth (PTG) includes 2 main competitive claims. The first argues that PTG reflects authentic positive transformation while the second posits that PTG reflects illusory defenses that could be maladaptive in the long run. The present study assesses these competing claims by investigating secondary PTG in relation to the somatic domain. Specifically, this study investigates: (a) the association between PTG, and perceived health (PH), as measured by 3 indices of somatic complaints, self-rated health (SRH) and a number of health problems; (b) the association between PTG, posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) and PH over time; and (c) the mediating role of PTSS between PTG and PH, among wives of former prisoners of war (ex-POWs) and wives of control veterans. Assessments were conducted 30 (T1) and 38 (T2) years after the Yom Kippur War. Results showed that wives of ex-POWs endorsed higher PTSS, higher PTG and poorer PH, compared to control wives. Higher PTG was associated with higher PTSS and poorer PH. PTG at T1 predicted an increase in PTSS between T1 and T2, which in turn was correlated with poorer PH. PTSS at T2 as well as changes in PTSS from T1 to T2 mediated the association between T1 PTG and T2 PH measures. The present findings imply that PTG might have negative implications on PH through the amplification of PTSS, among secondary trauma victims. It seems that although spouses of trauma victims describe benefits resulting from vicarious trauma exposures, their body indicates differently. (PsycINFO Database Record
Chapter
Full-text available
Traditionally, coping has been viewed as a process by which individuals confront and resolve (or fail to confront and resolve) challenges and obstacles. Rarely have the interpersonal context and consequences of coping been considered. As a result, the psychological literature on coping has demonstrated an individualistic, androcentric bias. The problem-solving coping strategies traditionally employed by men have been judged more efficacious than the emotion-focused and social-resource-based strategies traditionally employed by women. These gender differences have been used to explain women’s higher rates of certain mental illnesses, such as depression, and mental health care utilization, but often from the perspective of victim blaming (Rosario, Shinn, Morch, & Huckabee, 1988). The benefits of traditionally feminine coping behaviors and the costs of traditionally male coping behaviors to families and to society have been largely ignored (Solomon & Rothblum, 1986). Integration of the coping literature and the social support literature offers an opportunity to contextualize coping behavior as a multidimensional interpersonal phenomenon (Fondacaro & Moos, 1987).