ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Circadian Cortisol on the Development of a Health Habit

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objective: Given the impact of individuals' habits on health, it is important to study how behaviors can become habitual. Cortisol has been well documented to have a role in habit formation. This study aimed to elucidate the influence of the circadian rhythm of cortisol on habit formation in a real-life setting. Method: Forty-eight students were followed for 90 days during which they attempted to adopt a health behavior (psoas iliac stretch). They were randomly assigned to perform the stretch either upon waking in the morning, when cortisol concentrations are high, or before evening bedtime, when cortisol levels approach the nadir. A smartphone application was used to assess the Self-Report Behavioural Automaticity Index every day and to provide reminders for salivary measurements every 30 days. The speed of the health habit formation process was calculated by modeling the learning curves. Results: Extrapolation of the curves indicated that the morning group achieved automaticity at an earlier time point (105.95 days) than did the evening group (154.01 days). In addition, the cortisol level during the performance of the health behavior was identified as a significant mediator of the time point when the health behavior became habitual. Conclusion: The present findings suggest that the time course of the development of healthy habits depends on the time of the day and that the effect is mediated through diurnal variation in cortisol levels. Future studies are now needed to determine to what extent cortisol rhythmicity can help individuals to adopt new health behaviors. (PsycINFO Database Record
Content may be subject to copyright.
Health Psychology
Effects of Circadian Cortisol on the Development of a
Health Habit
Marion Fournier, Fabienne d’Arripe-Longueville, Carole Rovere, Christopher S. Easthope, Lars
Schwabe, Jonathan El Methni, and Rémi Radel
Online First Publication, June 26, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000510
CITATION
Fournier, M., d’Arripe-Longueville, F., Rovere, C., Easthope, C. S., Schwabe, L., El Methni, J., &
Radel, R. (2017, June 26). Effects of Circadian Cortisol on the Development of a Health Habit.
Health Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000510
BRIEF REPORT
Effects of Circadian Cortisol on the Development of a Health Habit
Marion Fournier and Fabienne d’Arripe-Longueville
Université Côte d’Azur
Carole Rovere
Université de Nice Sophia Antipolis
Christopher S. Easthope
Balgrist University Hospital
Lars Schwabe
University of Hamburg
Jonathan El Methni
Université Paris Descartes
Rémi Radel
Université Côte d’Azur
Objective: Given the impact of individuals’ habits on health, it is important to study how behaviors can
become habitual. Cortisol has been well documented to have a role in habit formation. This study aimed
to elucidate the influence of the circadian rhythm of cortisol on habit formation in a real-life setting.
Method: Forty-eight students were followed for 90 days during which they attempted to adopt a health
behavior (psoas iliac stretch). They were randomly assigned to perform the stretch either upon waking
in the morning, when cortisol concentrations are high, or before evening bedtime, when cortisol levels
approach the nadir. A smartphone application was used to assess the Self-Report Behavioural Automa-
ticity Index every day and to provide reminders for salivary measurements every 30 days. The speed of
the health habit formation process was calculated by modeling the learning curves. Results: Extrapolation
of the curves indicated that the morning group achieved automaticity at an earlier time point (105.95
days) than did the evening group (154.01 days). In addition, the cortisol level during the performance of
the health behavior was identified as a significant mediator of the time point when the health behavior
became habitual. Conclusion: The present findings suggest that the time course of the development of
healthy habits depends on the time of the day and that the effect is mediated through diurnal variation
in cortisol levels. Future studies are now needed to determine to what extent cortisol rhythmicity can help
individuals to adopt new health behaviors.
Keywords: health, habits, learning, stress, cortisol
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000510.supp
Attention should be paid to behavioral habits, given their important
role in health (Wood & Neal, 2016). A behavioral habit is a process
by which a stimulus automatically generates an impulse toward ac-
tion, based on learned stimulus–response associations (Gardner,
2015). Recent studies have pointed to the critical role of stress and the
stress hormones (mainly cortisol) in the development of habit behav-
ior. For instance, it has been shown that stress induces a shift in the
control of instrumental behavior from goal-directed toward habitual
responding (e.g., Schwabe, Schächinger, de Kloet, & Oitzl, 2010).
The endogenous level of cortisol varies according to a circadian
rhythm. In humans, cortisol levels are low at midnight and increase
overnight to a peak in the morning. Following this morning peak,
cortisol levels slowly decline throughout the day (Weitzman et al.,
1971). Although the impact of cortisol on habit behavior has been
well established through stress manipulation or cortisol injection (e.g.,
Fournier, d’Arripe-Longueville, & Radel, 2017; Quirarte et al., 2009),
the influence of cortisol’s circadian rhythm on habit formation re-
mains to be elucidated.
Many studies have demonstrated the predictive capacity of
habits on health behavior (e.g., de Bruijn & Rhodes, 2011), but
less is known about the process of habit formation in real-life
settings. A previous study followed participants over 84 days
while they adopted a new, daily health-promoting behavior such
as eating fruit or exercising (Lally, van Jaarsveld, Potts, &
Marion Fournier and Fabienne d’Arripe-Longueville, Faculty of Sport
Science, Université Côte d’Azur; Carole Rovere, Institut de Pharmacologie
Moléculaire et Cellulaire, Université de Nice Sophia Antipolis; Christopher
S. Easthope, Spinal Cord Injury Center, Balgrist University Hospital; Lars
Schwabe, Department of Cognitive Psychology, Institute for Psychology,
University of Hamburg; Jonathan El Methni, Department of Statistics,
Université Paris Descartes; Rémi Radel, Faculty of Sport Science, Univer-
sité Côte d’Azur.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marion
Fournier, Faculty of Sport Science, University Côte d’Azur, LAMHESS,
Nice, France. E-mail: fou2marion@gmail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Health Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 999, No. 999, 000 0278-6133/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000510
1
Wardle, 2010). Although this protocol was innovative in track-
ing the formation of habits by collecting daily reports of expe-
rienced automaticity, the collected data proved challenging to
model. In the present study, a similar protocol was used to
investigate the influence of diurnal variations in cortisol con-
centrations on the dynamics of habit formation using a real-
world application. To better fit the evolution of experienced
automaticity in time, we used logistic curves (Murre, 2014).
The level of circadian cortisol was manipulated by having two
groups of participants perform a new health behavior at differ-
ent times of the day. Given that cortisol levels are higher in the
morning than in the evening, we hypothesized that the new
behavior would become habitual more quickly if executed in
the morning than in the evening. The cortisol level was ex-
pected to mediate this effect.
Method
Participants
On average, diurnal variations of cortisol can lead to a 20%
difference in learning and memory (May, Hasher, & Stoltzfus,
1993; Petros, Beckwith, & Anderson, 1990; Wyatt, Ritz-De
Cecco, Czeisler, & Dijk, 1999), which suggests that at least 46
participants were needed to observe such an effect with a power
of .80. Forty-eight French students (28 male; age 21.7 1.78
years, range 20 –25) participated in exchange for course
credit. Participation was limited to healthy nonsmokers with
normal body mass index (22.4 2.17 kg/m
2
, range 18.2–
27.7 kg/m
2
) not under contraceptive medication according to
the recommendations for salivary cortisol measurements
(Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer, & Hellhammer,
1999). The study was approved by the local ethics committee
for the protection of individuals (Université de Nice Sophia-
Antipolis) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of
Helsinki (1964) ethical guidelines.
Procedure
Participants were invited to a first meeting with the experi-
menter. After providing written informed consent, they were
randomly assigned to a morning or evening group according to
a minimization algorithm, balancing for gender. Here, a chro-
notype questionnaire and a measure of intention to adopt the
behavior was completed. The intervention commenced within a
week of the first meeting. It consisted of performing a new
behavior once daily for 90 days. The behavior was a stretching
exercise that is highly recommended to maintain flexibility and
prevent low back pain (i.e., psoas iliac stretch; see the online
supplemental materials). Depending on group allocation, the
stretch was completed in bed upon waking or before sleeping.
Adherence and a visual analog scale version of the Self-Report
Behavioural Automaticity Index (SRBAI; Gardner, Abraham,
Lally, & de Bruijn, 2012) were recorded via a smartphone
reminder application on a daily basis. A salivary sample was
collected every 30 days. Details on the measures are provided in
the online supplemental materials.
Data Analysis
To evaluate the evolution of automaticity, we fitted a four-
parameter logistic function to participants’ daily responses to the
SRBAI (see the online supplemental materials). Although habit
formation has previously been modeled using a power function
(Lally et al., 2010), this method led to mixed results, providing a
moderate fit (R
2
.70) for only 48% of the participants. Because
a logistic function can outperform a power function in modeling
learning curves (Murre, 2014), this approach was adopted. We
considered that, in line with Lally et al. (2010), automaticity was
achieved at the time point at which 95% of the asymptote was
reached (x
.95
). This value served as the dependent variable.
To determine the group effect and the potential mediation effect
of cortisol on x
.95
, we employed a mediation model using the
PROCESS toolbox (Hayes, 2012). Group (morning vs. evening)
was applied as the independent variable, cortisol concentration as
a mediator, and sex and intention as covariates for the prediction
of the dependent variable. A bias-corrected bootstrapping method
with 2,000 samples was used to evaluate the effects. The total
effect of the independent variable and its indirect effect through
the mediator are presented. To control for the chronotype, we used
a moderation analysis to examine how the effect of group on x
.95
was influenced by the chronotype score. The mediation model was
also used in a further configuration with the independent variable
replaced by a Group Chronotype interaction term. The interac-
tion term adequately represented the hypothesis of a match be-
tween the moment of execution and the participants’ chronotype
score (Hasher, Goldstein, & May, 2005).
Results
Of the 48 recruited participants, 42 completed the experiment:
19 participants in the morning group and 23 in the evening group.
The intention to adopt the behavior was high (4.74 0.39), with
no difference between groups (p.24). The response rate to the
daily questionnaire was high (89.2%), with no difference between
groups (p.25). Adherence was high (94.9%), with no difference
between groups (p.25). The curve-fitting process was highly
successful. Fitting converged for each participant using the logistic
function (by comparison, only 31 participants could be fitted using
a power curve). A high adjustment quality index was obtained
(R
2
.945 .053). Figure 1 presents the mean traces of the
morning and evening groups obtained by averaging each of the
four function parameters for each condition.
Automaticity (x
.95
) was slightly positively skewed but did not
significantly deviate from normality (p.10). The results of the
first mediation model populated with group as the independent
variable are presented in Table 1. After controlling for covariates
(sex and intention), we observed a significant total effect of time
of day until habit development (effect ⫽⫺26.74, SE 10.25,
95% confidence interval [CI: 47.52, 5,96]), indicating that the
behavior became habitual more quickly when it was performed in
the morning (M105.95 46.72 days) as opposed to the evening
(M154.01 71.05 days). Group (morning vs. evening) had a
significant effect on the cortisol level, indicating that cortisol
levels differed between groups (morning: 2.16 .87 ng/ml; eve-
ning: 1.10 .86 ng/ml). When included in the model predicting
the time to form a habit (x
.95
), cortisol was a significant predictor
and the group effect was no longer significant, suggesting a me-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2FOURNIER ET AL.
diating role. Accordingly, the group effect had no significant direct
effect (effect ⫽⫺14.21, SE 11.21, 95% CI [36.93, 8.50]), but
a significant indirect effect on the dependent variable through the
effect of the cortisol (effect ⫽⫺11.89, SE 6.25, 95% CI
[30.39, 2.98]). Figure 2 illustrates the nature of the relation
between cortisol levels and x
.95
.
The second model, in which the independent variable group was
replaced by the Group Chronotype interaction, led to a similar
pattern (see Table 2). The Group Chronotype interaction had a
significant total effect on x
.95
(effect ⫽⫺.57, SE .22, 95% CI
[1.02, .12]). The total effect was decomposed into a nonsig-
nificant direct effect (effect ⫽⫺.29, SE .25, 95% CI [.79,
2.13]) and a significant indirect effect through cortisol (ef-
fect ⫽⫺.27, SE .15, 95% CI [.65, .04]).
Discussion
Because it has been shown that cortisol level has a strong
connection to habit memory formation and that cortisol concen-
trations vary markedly over the day, this study aimed to test the
influence of the circadian rhythm of cortisol on the development of
a healthy habit. The presented findings indicate that the stretching
behavior that participants intended to adopt became habitual more
quickly when it was performed in the morning than in the evening.
In addition, it seems that cortisol played a mediating role, because
there was a significant indirect effect of the time of day on
automatization speed through the level of cortisol displayed at the
moment of behavior execution. Cortisol concentrations in the
morning were higher than in the evening samples, and the cortisol
level was negatively associated with the time taken to make the
behavior habitual (see Figure 2). To our knowledge, this is the first
time that habit formation has been studied through the prism of
chronobiology. Our findings are consistent with those in other
studies that have manipulated glucocorticoides GC levels with
either pharmacological injection or stress induction, because they
have all indicated that cortisol level has an impact on habit for-
mation (e.g., Quirarte et al., 2009; Schwabe & Wolf, 2009).
Our results show that the indirect effect of time of day on habit
formation through the circulating cortisol level persisted, and was
even slightly stronger, when we took into account the match
between chronotype and the time of behavior execution. This
match effect contributes to the large body of literature on the
effects of chronobiology on behavior (e.g., Hasher et al., 2005)
showing that synchrony between individual preferences and the
time of the day has an important effect on performance and
particularly on tasks relying on attentional demands such as learn-
ing. It is thus not surprising that this individual trait further refines
Figure 1. Fitted logistic function representating the evolution of be-
havioral automaticity (Self-Reported Behavioural Automaticity Index,
SRBAI) in time in the morning and evening conditions. Shaded areas
represent standard error of the mean. Data from after the measurement
period (90 days) is extrapolated and indicated using light gray. The
moment when participants reach 95% of the maximal asymptote (dotted
line, x
.95
) represents the time taken to form the habit. Traces are
normalized for representation purposes.
Table 1
Regression Models Used to Determine the Mediating Role of Cortisol in the Effect of the
Condition on the Time Taken to Form a Behavioral Habit
Model and variable Coefficient SE t p
95% CI
LL UL
Model predicting cortisol: R
2
.28, F(1, 40) 14.89, p.0004
Constant 1.627 .138 11.831 .000 1.349 1.905
Condition .531 .138 3.859 .001 .252 .809
Model predicting x
.95
without inclusion of the mediator: R
2
.20, F(3, 38) 2.34, p.09
Constant 288.495 172.121 1.671 .102 59.950 636.941
Condition 26.601 10.169 2.616 .013 47.187 6.015
Intention 30.528 35.453 .861 .395 102.299 41.244
Sex 25.043 20.269 1.236 .224 66.075 15.990
Model predicting x
.95
with inclusion of the mediator: R
2
.29, F(4, 37) 2.58, p.053
Constant 290.459 150.281 1.933 .061 14.044 594.962
Cortisol 22.413 10.779 2.079 .045 44.254 .572
Condition 14.218 11.213 1.268 .213 36.937 8.502
Intention 23.578 31.366 .752 .457 87.133 39.976
Sex 22.316 19.782 1.128 .267 62.398 17.767
Note.CIconfidence interval; LL lower limit; ULCI upper limit.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
CORTISOL AND HEALTHY HABIT DEVELOPMENT
the effect of the moment of execution on the acquisition of behav-
ioral habits.
Our study was designed to test habit formation in real life and
therefore makes an original contribution to the literature by having
greater ecologic validity than do the laboratory tasks (i.e., instru-
mental tasks) that are generally used to study the habit formation
process. The short SRBAI and smartphone reminder application
made it possible for us to track the participants for 3 months.
However, self-reported habit measures have been criticized (Hag-
ger, Rebar, Mullan, Lipp, & Chatzisarantis, 2015) because people
may have limited conscious knowledge of automatic behaviors.
Nevertheless, some researchers have still contested the belief that
people can provide valid information on their habits using such
self-reports (Orbell & Verplanken, 2015). In addition, there are no
current alternatives for measuring behavioral habits and automa-
ticity, particularly for frequent repeated measurements. To suc-
cessfully analyze the evolution of automaticity we used a new type
of learning curve that seems to better represent the habit-formation
process. Unlike power learning curves, which show a substantial
gain at the beginning, the logistic function suggests that some
Figure 2. Relation between salivary cortisol values and the time necessary for behavior automatization (x
.95
)
for the morning and evening groups.
Table 2
Regression Models Used to Determine the Mediating Role of Cortisol on the Effect of the
Product Representing the Condition Chronotype Interaction on the Time Taken to Form a
Behavioral Habit
Model and
variable Coefficient SE t p
95% CI
LL UL
Model predicting Cortisol: R
2
.33, F(1, 40) 20.699, p.0001
Constant 1.634 .132 12.366 .000 1.367 1.901
Condition MEQ .012 .003 4.544 .000 .007 .018
Model predicting x
.95
without inclusion of the mediator: R
2
.20, F(3, 38) 2.18, p.10
Constant 281.140 171.910 1.635 .110 66.879 629.159
Condition MEQ .566 .219 2.591 .013 1.009 .124
Intention 28.987 35.409 .819 .418 100.671 42.696
Sex 25.203 20.360 1.238 .223 66.419 16.014
Model predicting x
.95
with inclusion of the mediator: R
2
.28, F(4, 37) 2.51, p.058
Constant 284.753 148.792 1.914 .063 16.733 586.239
Cortisol 22.404 11.129 2.013 .051 44.955 .146
Condition MEQ .283 .248 1.141 .261 .787 .220
Intention 22.369 31.232 .716 .478 85.652 40.914
Sex 22.369 19.934 1.122 .269 62.760 18.023
Note.CIconfidence interval; LL lower limit; ULCI upper limit; MEQ Morningness-Eveningness
Questionnaire.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4FOURNIER ET AL.
participants may actually start more slowly (see Murre, 2014),
which is often the case for complex learning such as the adoption
of a new healthy behavior that requires plenty of conscious effort
at the beginning to accommodate the behavior in the living context
and evaluate its costs and benefits (Wood & Neal, 2016). Only
when these obstacles have been overcome may the automatization
process begin.
A limitation of our work concerns the relatively small sample
size of this study due to a limited recruitment capacity and attrition
throughout the duration of the intervention. Although the signifi-
cant results indicate that sufficient evidence was obtained to reject
the null hypothesis despite the limited power, this study should be
replicated to ensure reproducibility. In future studies, other medi-
ators should also be tracked. We could not demonstrate a full
mediation effect, and it therefore seems likely that other factors
may come into play. As Schwabe, Tegenthoff, Höffken, and Wolf
(2012) indicated, cortisol alone does not affect habitual behavior,
and concurrent glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity may have
a role since it is known that this concurrent activity is needed to
shift learning from goal-directed to habitual control. Some psy-
chological factors might also explain the effect. For example, it is
possible that the behavior was perceived as less difficult, more
satisfying, or more easily cued in the morning than in the evening.
Moreover, people assigned to the morning group were given a
prior cue (i.e., do the stretch after waking up), but those in the
evening group were not (i.e., do the stretch before going to bed).
Because cueing is a critical aspect of habit formation (Gardner,
2015), it could have led to reinforcement of automatization in the
morning condition.
Conclusion
In this study, we demonstrated that a newly adopted stretching
behavior became habitual more quickly when it was performed in
the morning as opposed to the evening. This effect was mediated
by cortisol levels— higher cortisol levels in the morning resulted in
accelerated automatization. By matching the intervention time
with individuals’ chronobiology, faster automatization and there-
with higher success rates are likely. The extent to which these
results can be translated to applied settings and more complex
behaviors should be further investigated.
References
Courneya, K. S. (1994). Predicting repeated behavior from intention: The
issue of scale correspondence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24,
580 –594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb00601.x
de Bruijn, G. J., & Rhodes, R. E. (2011). Exploring exercise behavior,
intention and habit strength relationships. Scandinavian Journal of Med-
icine & Science in Sports, 21, 482– 491. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1600-0838.2009.01064.x
Declaration of Helsinki. (1964, June.). Recommendation physicians in
biomedical research involving human subjects. 18th World Medical
Assembly, Helsinki, Finland.
Fournier, M., d’Arripe-Longueville, F., & Radel, R. (2017). Effects of
psychosocial stress on the goal-directed and habit memory systems
during learning and later execution. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17,
275–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.008
Gardner, B. (2015). A review and analysis of the use of “habit” in
understanding, predicting and influencing health-related behaviour.
Health Psychology Review, 9, 277–295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
17437199.2013.876238
Gardner, B., Abraham, C., Lally, P., & de Bruijn, G.-J. (2012). Towards
parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive
validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9,
102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-102
Hagger, M. S., Rebar, A. L., Mullan, B., Lipp, O. V., & Chatzisarantis,
N. L. D. (2015). The subjective experience of habit captured by self-
report indexes may lead to inaccuracies in the measurement of habitual
action. Health Psychology Review, 9, 296 –302. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/17437199.2014.959728
Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & May, C. P. (2005). It’s about time: Circadian
rhythms, memory, and aging. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for ob-
served variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process model-
ing. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf
Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to
determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Inter-
national Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.
Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellham-
mer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral
contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal
axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 154 –162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
00006842-199903000-00006
Lally, P., van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Potts, H. W. W., & Wardle, J. (2010).
How are habits formed: Modelling habit formation in the real world.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 998 –1009. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/ejsp.674
MATLAB and Statistics Toolbox Release. (2012). The MathWorks, Inc.
Natick, MA.
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Stoltzfus, E. R. (1993). Optimal time of day and
the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychological Science, 4,
326 –330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00573.x
Murre, J. M. J. (2014). Learning and categorization in modular neural
networks. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2015). The strength of habit. Health Psy-
chology Review, 9, 311–317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2014
.992031
Petros, T. V., Beckwith, B. E., & Anderson, M. (1990). Individual differ-
ences in the effects of time of day and passage difficulty on prose
memory in adults. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 63–72. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02346.x
Quirarte, G. L., Ledesma de la Teja, I. S., Casillas, M., Serafín, N.,
Prado-Alcalá, R. A., & Roozendaal, B. (2009). Corticosterone infused
into the dorsal striatum selectively enhances memory consolidation of
cued water-maze training. Learning & Memory, 16, 586 –589. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1101/lm.1493609
Schwabe, L., Schächinger, H., de Kloet, E. R., & Oitzl, M. S. (2010).
Corticosteroids operate as a switch between memory systems. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1362–1372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn
.2009.21278
Schwabe, L., Tegenthoff, M., Höffken, O., & Wolf, O. T. (2012). Simul-
taneous glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity disrupts the neural
basis of goal-directed action in the human brain. Journal of Neurosci-
ence, 32, 10146 –10155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1304-
12.2012
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in
humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 7191–7198. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009
Taillard, J., Philip, P., Chastang, J.-F., & Bioulac, B. (2004). Validation of
Horne and Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire in a
middle-aged population of French workers. Journal of Biological
Rhythms, 19, 76 – 86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0748730403259849
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
CORTISOL AND HEALTHY HABIT DEVELOPMENT
Weitzman, E. D., Fukushima, D., Nogeire, C., Roffwarg, H., Gallagher,
T. F., & Hellman, L. (1971). Twenty-four hour pattern of the episodic
secretion of cortisol in normal subjects. Journal of Clinical Endocri-
nology and Metabolism, 33, 14 –22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/jcem-
33-1-14
Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2016). Healthy through habit: Interventions for
initiating & maintaining health behavior change. Behavioral Science &
Policy, 2, 71– 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/bsp.2016.0008
Wyatt, J. K., Ritz-De Cecco, A., Czeisler, C. A., & Dijk, D. J. (1999).
Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehav-
ioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. American Journal of
Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 277,
R1152–R1163.
Received September 7, 2016
Revision received February 24, 2017
Accepted March 3, 2017
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6FOURNIER ET AL.
... Across these studies, participants were instructed to establish a new habit by repeating a novel behavior (predominantly nutrition-or physical activity-related) once per day in response to a specified cue for approximately 3 months, where habit strength was recurringly assessed with the Self-Report Habit Index (SRHI) (Verplanken & Orbell, 2003) or its subscale, the Self-Report Behavioral Automaticity Index (SRBAI) (Gardner et al., 2012). Findings coherently suggest that successful habit formation may be described by a non-linear increasing trend, where the rate of change gradually slows down as the habit strength approaches an upper bound (Fournier et al., 2017;Keller et al., 2021;Lally et al., 2010;Van Der Weiden et al., 2020). Furthermore, findings suggest that the change in habit strength over time varies considerably between individuals, where consistent performance of the novel behavior in response to encountering the cue is key for habit formation to occur (Baretta et al., 2024;Keller et al., 2021;Lally et al., 2010). ...
... Furthermore, findings suggest that the change in habit strength over time varies considerably between individuals, where consistent performance of the novel behavior in response to encountering the cue is key for habit formation to occur (Baretta et al., 2024;Keller et al., 2021;Lally et al., 2010). Based on these studies, the time needed for habit formation to occur has been estimated to range from a matter of days to almost 1 year, with potentially only a minority of participants succeeding in forming a habit (Fournier et al., 2017;Keller et al., 2021;Lally et al., 2010). Noteworthily, estimates of long duration (e.g. ...
... In terms of modelling the process of change, important commonalities and differences across the studies can be identified. The change in habit strength has been modelled by participants' individual trajectories (Lally et al., 2010), by group-level modelling (Fournier et al., 2017;Van Der Weiden et al., 2020), or by combining both approaches (Baretta et al., 2024;Keller et al., 2021). Modelling person-specific trajectories has the advantage of highlighting the idiosyncratic nature of habit formation (Keller et al., 2021;Lally et al., 2010), but research questions related to group differences in habit change processes may be more appropriately addressed with group-level modelling (Fournier et al., 2017;Keller et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Habits are cue-behavior associations learned through repetition that are assumed to be relatively stable. Thereby, unhealthy habits can pose a health risk due to facilitating relapse. In the absence of research on habit decay in daily life, we aimed to investigate how habit decreases over time when trying to degrade a habit and whether this differs by four health-risk behaviors (sedentary behavior, unhealthy snacking, alcohol consumption, and smoking). This 91-day intensive longitudinal study included four parallel non-randomized groups (one per behavior; N = 194). Habit strength was measured daily with the Self-Report Behavioral Automaticity Index (11,805 observations) and modelled over time with constant, linear, quadratic, cubic, asymptotic, and logistic models. Person-specific modelling revealed asymptotic and logistic models as the most common best-fitting models (54% of the sample). The time for habit decay to stabilize ranged from 1 to 65 days. Multilevel modelling indicated substantial between-person heterogeneity and suggested initial habit strength but not the decay process to vary by behavioral group. Findings suggest that habit decay when trying to degrade a habit typically follows a decelerating negative trend but that it is a highly idiosyncratic process. Recommendations include emphasizing the role of person-specific modelling and data visualization in habit research.
... Prioritizing morning meditation has been observed in a previous trial, where participants who most successfully adhered to an anchoring strategy for meditation almost exclusively used morning anchors [31]. Furthermore, research in other domains shows that performing a target behavior in the morning (eg, taking diabetes medication [35] or engaging in a stretching routine [36]) leads to stronger habits. Similarly, specific action plans for fruit and vegetable intake were most effective when they focused on the morning [37]. ...
... One possible explanation for the success of morning meditations could be the influence of the circadian rhythm of cortisol. In attempting to understand the development of healthy habits at various points throughout a circadian cycle, Fournier et al [36] found that morning practitioners were the quickest to habituate a targeted behavior. Further, cortisol levels played a significant role in determining the time for the healthy behavior to become a habit, with cortisol levels being higher in the morning than in the evening. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The intensive data typically collected by mobile health (mHealth) apps allows factors associated with persistent use to be investigated, which is an important objective given users’ well-known struggles with sustaining healthy behavior. Objective Data from a commercial meditation app (n=14,879; 899,071 total app uses) were analyzed to assess the validity of commonly given habit formation advice to meditate at the same time every day, preferably in the morning. Methods First, the change in probability of meditating in 4 nonoverlapping time windows (morning, midday, evening, and late night) on a given day over the first 180 days after creating a meditation app account was calculated via generalized additive mixed models. Second, users’ time of day preferences were calculated as the percentage of all meditation sessions that occurred within each of the 4 time windows. Additionally, the temporal consistency of daily meditation behavior was calculated as the entropy of the timing of app usage sessions. Linear regression was used to examine the effect of time of day preference and temporal consistency on two outcomes: (1) short-term engagement, defined as the number of meditation sessions completed within the sixth and seventh month of a user’s account, and (2) long-term use, defined as the days until a user’s last observed meditation session. ResultsLarge reductions in the probability of meditation at any time of day were seen over the first 180 days after creating an account, but this effect was smallest for morning meditation sessions (63.4% reduction vs reductions ranging from 67.8% to 74.5% for other times). A greater proportion of meditation in the morning was also significantly associated with better short-term engagement (regression coefficient B=2.76, P
... For instance, a person may be eager to try out a new piece of software and use it frequently at first, but over time the habit strength may decrease as the novelty wears off. Further, habit formation is enabled by the frequency and consistency with which the desired behavior is performed, the rewarding nature of the behavior, the facilitating conditions, and the ease rather than the difficulty of performing a certain behavior [8,11,24]. In technology acceptance research, Venkatesh [26] observed that two distinct yet related concepts have been presented in the literature. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The UTAUT-2 offers the most comprehensive assessment of individual acceptance and use of technology to date. In particular, the theoretical additions of “hedonic motivation”, “price value”, and “habit”, made the model suitable for studying technology in a consumer context. However, a review of the literature revealed that the construct of habit has been dropped from a large number of studies. There are several reasons for this, including that the technologies examined were relatively new for the respondents to form a routine behavior. Therefore, this study aims to explore whether the construct can be used as a key predictor of future intention to use an innovation rather than an acquired practice among technology users. For this purpose, a conceptual model based on the theoretical additions to the UTAUT-2 is proposed and analyzed with structural equation modeling (SmartPLS). Our results showed significant relationships between the predictors and the behavioral intention to use battery electric vehicles (BEV) technology, and, in particular, depicted the construct of habit as the strongest factor in the decision to adopt the technology. In light of our findings, the construct of habit (HT) should be used in research together with the other UTAUT-2 predictors to assess individuals’ perceptions of possible future habitual behaviors.
... Social and personal factors such as family dynamics/ responsibilities might need to be considered when designing interventions for different racial groups to understand time allocation and management. Then, behavior-regulation strategies such as planning, and habit formation techniques, which are effective for securing protected time to exercise [33][34][35][36][37][38], can be facilitated in an education program. Specifically, experimental research has found clinical populations, such as those in cardiac rehabilitation such as acute coronary syndrome, are able to apply these behavior change techniques [39,40]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Heart Failure is a leading cause of mortality among older adults. Engaging in regular exercise at moderate-to-vigorous intensity has been shown to improve survival rates. Theory-informed methodologies have been recommended to promote exercise, but limited application of theoretical framework has been conducted for understanding racial disparities among older adults with heart failure. This study aimed to use the Health Belief Model to compare exercise behavior determinants between Black and White older adults diagnosed with heart failure. Methods The HF-ACTION Trial is a multi-site study designed to promote exercise among individuals with heart failure that randomized participants to an experimental (three months of group exercise sessions followed by home-based training) or control arm. The present study used structural equation modeling to test the change in Health Belief Model constructs and exercise behavior across 12 months among older adults. Results Participants (n = 671) were older adults, 72.28 (SD = 5.41) years old, (Black: n = 230; White, n = 441) diagnosed with heart failure and reduced ejection fraction. The model found perceived benefits, self-efficacy, perceived threats, and perceived barriers to predict exercise behavior among Black and White older adults. However, among these constructs, only perceived benefits and self-efficacy were facilitated via intervention for both races. Additionally, the intervention was effective for addressing perceived barriers to exercise only among White participants. Finally, the intervention did not result in a change of perceived threats for both races. Conclusions Among health belief model constructs, perceived threats and barriers were not facilitated for both races in the experimental arm, and the intervention did not resolve barriers among Black older adults. Racial differences need to be considered when designing interventions for clinical populations as future studies are warranted to address barriers to exercise among Black older adults with heart failure.
... In a seminal study (5), 96 undergraduates ate, drank, and exercised daily in the same context for 12 wk and selfreported habit strength every day. This study, and two similar ones (6) and (7), suggest that "habits typically develop asymptotically and idiosyncratically, potentially differing in rate across people, cues and behaviors" (8, pg. 220). ...
Article
Full-text available
We apply a machine learning technique to characterize habit formation in two large panel data sets with objective measures of 1) gym attendance (over 12 million observations) and 2) hospital handwashing (over 40 million observations). Our Predicting Context Sensitivity (PCS) approach identifies context variables that best predict behavior for each individual. This approach also creates a time series of overall predictability for each individual. These time series predictability values are used to trace a habit formation curve for each individual, operationalizing the time of habit formation as the asymptotic limit of when behavior becomes highly predictable. Contrary to the popular belief in a "magic number" of days to develop a habit, we find that it typically takes months to form the habit of going to the gym but weeks to develop the habit of handwashing in the hospital. Furthermore, we find that gymgoers who are more predictable are less responsive to an intervention designed to promote more gym attendance, consistent with past experiments showing that habit formation generates insensitivity to reward devaluation.
Article
Full-text available
The rapid shifts in society have altered human behavioural patterns, with increased evening activities, increased screen time and changed sleep schedules. As an explicit manifestation of circadian rhythms, chronotype is closely intertwined with physical and mental health. Night owls often exhibit unhealthier lifestyle habits, are more susceptible to mood disorders and have poorer physical fitness compared with early risers. Although individual differences in chronotype yield varying consequences, their neurobiological underpinnings remain elusive. Here we conducted a pattern-learning analysis with three brain-imaging modalities (grey matter volume, white-matter integrity and functional connectivity) and capitalized on 976 phenotypes in 27,030 UK Biobank participants. The resulting multilevel analysis reveals convergence on the basal ganglia, limbic system, hippocampus and cerebellum. The pattern derived from modelling actigraphy wearables data of daily movement further highlighted these key brain features. Overall, our population-level study comprehensively investigates chronotype, emphasizing its close connections with habit formation, reward processing and emotional regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Healthy lifestyles depend on forming crucial habits through the process of habit formation, emphasising the need to establish positive habits and break negative ones for lasting behaviour changes. This systematic review aims to explore the time required for developing health-related habits. Methods: Six databases (Scopus, PsychINFO, CINAHL, EMBASE, Medline and PubMed) were searched to identify experimental intervention studies assessing self-report habit or automaticity questionnaires (e.g., the self-report habit index (SRHI) or the self-report behavioural automaticity index (SRBAI)), or the duration to reach automaticity in health-related behaviours. Habit formation determinants were also evaluated. Meta-analysis was performed to assess the change in the SRHI or SRBAI habit scores between pre- and post-intervention, and the study quality was assessed using the PEDro scale. Results: A total of 20 studies involving 2601 participants (mean age range: 21.5–73.5 years) were included. Most studies had a high risk of bias rating (n = 11). Health behaviours included physical activity (n = 8), drinking water (n = 2), vitamin consumption (n = 1), flossing (n = 3), healthy diet (n = 8), microwaving a dishcloth (for foodborne disease reduction, n = 2) and sedentary behaviour reduction (n = 1). Four studies reported the median or mean times to reach habit formation, ranging from 59–66 days (median) and 106–154 days (means), with substantial individual variability (4–335 days). The meta-analysis showed significant improvements in habit scores pre- to post-intervention across different habits (standardised mean difference: 0.69, 95% CI: 0.49–0.88). Frequency, timing, type of habit, individual choice, affective judgements, behavioural regulation and preparatory habits significantly influence habit strength, with morning practices and self-selected habits generally exhibiting greater strength. Conclusions: Emerging evidence on health-related habit formation indicates that while habits can start forming within about two months, the time required varies significantly across individuals. A limitation of this meta-analysis is the relatively small number of studies included, with flossing and diet having the most evidence among the behaviours examined. Despite this, improvements in habit strength post-intervention are evident across various behaviours, suggesting that targeted interventions can be effective. Future research should aim to expand the evidence base with well-designed studies to better understand and enhance the process of establishing beneficial health habits.
Article
Full-text available
In early childhood, children are extremely susceptible to the acquisition of habits and the establishment of health-promoting habits. Therefore, the patterns, routines, and rules transmitted and expected by the adults surrounding the child are of paramount importance and can correlate with the level of their health literacy. Our cross-sectional, quantitative, exploratory study aimed to examine the relationships between parental health literacy and preschool children’s health-related habits, using simple, non-random sampling (n = 598). In addition to the sociodemographic characteristics, the measuring tool we compiled included the standardized European Health Literacy Survey Questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q16), as well as a set of questions containing 30 statements suitable for exploring children’s habit systems. The health literacy of the parents involved in our study proved to be more favorable than that of the general population. Regarding children’s habit systems, we found significant differences in several areas by age group (p < 0.05) and gender (p < 0.05). The levels of parental health literacy (0.003 ≤ p ≤ 0.048) and parents’ education (p < 0.05) show a correlation with the children’s health-related habit systems: the indicators of children with parents who have a higher level of health literacy and a higher level of education are more favorable in terms of established habits. In the long term, the formation of health-promoting habits may facilitate the internalization of favorable health behavior motives for the future, contributing to the establishment of positive physical, mental, and social health in adulthood.
Preprint
Full-text available
The rapid shifts of society have brought about changes in human behavioral patterns, with increased evening activities, increased screen time, and postponed sleep schedules. As an explicit manifestation of circadian rhythms, chronotype is closely intertwined with both physical and mental health. Night owls often exhibit more unhealthy lifestyle habits, are more susceptible to mood disorders, and have poorer physical fitness. Although individual differences in chronotype yield varying consequences, their neurobiological underpinnings remain elusive. Here we carry out a pattern-learning analysis, and capitalize on a vast array of ~ 1,000 phenome-wide phenotypes with three brain-imaging modalities (region volume of gray matter, whiter-matter fiber tracts, and functional connectivity) in 27,030 UK Biobank participants. The resulting multi-level depicts of brain images converge on the basal ganglia, limbic system, hippocampus, as well as cerebellum vermis, thus implicating key nodes in habit formation, emotional regulation and reward processing. Complementary by comprehensive investigations of in-deep phenotypic collections, our population study offers evidence of behavioral pattern disparities linked to distinct chronotype-related behavioral tendencies in our societies.
Article
Traditional models of behavior change emphasize knowledge, beliefs, and injunctive norms as targets of intervention. Emotion—a potent force guiding human behavior—is strikingly absent from most models and most behavioral interventions. This article reviews evidence that emotion is not only consequential for common targets of behavioral intervention, but can be activated strategically to facilitate behavior change. This article presents a new framework for classifying behavior change targets, along with specific emotion-leveraging intervention techniques matched to each category of behavior. Policy recommendations emphasize combining emotional tools with other evidence-based behavior-change techniques. Although more research is needed, emotion shows promise for helping to support people's behavioral goals.
Article
Full-text available
Background. The term ‘habit’ is widely used to predict and explain behaviour. This paper examines use of the term in the context of health-related behaviour, and explores how the concept might be made more useful. Method: A narrative review is presented, drawing on a scoping review of 136 empirical studies and eight literature reviews undertaken to document usage of the term ‘habit’, and methods to measure it. A coherent definition of ‘habit’, and proposals for improved methods for studying it, were derived from findings. Results: Definitions of ‘habit’ have varied in ways that are often implicit, and not coherently linked with an underlying theory. A definition is proposed whereby habit is a process by which a stimulus generates an impulse to act as a result of a learned stimulus-response association. Habit-generated impulses may compete or combine with impulses and inhibitions arising from other sources, including conscious decision-making, to influence responses, and need not generate behaviour. Most research on habit is based on correlational studies using self-report measures. Conclusion: Adopting a coherent definition of ‘habit’, and a wider range of paradigms, designs and measures to study it, may accelerate progress in habit theory and application.
Article
Full-text available
Background The twelve-item Self-Report Habit Index (SRHI) is the most popular measure of energy-balance related habits. This measure characterises habit by automatic activation, behavioural frequency, and relevance to self-identity. Previous empirical research suggests that the SRHI may be abbreviated with no losses in reliability or predictive utility. Drawing on recent theorising suggesting that automaticity is the ‘active ingredient’ of habit-behaviour relationships, we tested whether an automaticity-specific SRHI subscale could capture habit-based behaviour patterns in self-report data. Methods A content validity task was undertaken to identify a subset of automaticity indicators within the SRHI. The reliability, convergent validity and predictive validity of the automaticity item subset was subsequently tested in secondary analyses of all previous SRHI applications, identified via systematic review, and in primary analyses of four raw datasets relating to energy‐balance relevant behaviours (inactive travel, active travel, snacking, and alcohol consumption). Results A four-item automaticity subscale (the ‘Self-Report Behavioural Automaticity Index’; ‘SRBAI’) was found to be reliable and sensitive to two hypothesised effects of habit on behaviour: a habit-behaviour correlation, and a moderating effect of habit on the intention-behaviour relationship. Conclusion The SRBAI offers a parsimonious measure that adequately captures habitual behaviour patterns. The SRBAI may be of particular utility in predicting future behaviour and in studies tracking habit formation or disruption.
Article
The interaction of homeostatic and circadian processes in the regulation of waking neurobehavioral functions and sleep was studied in six healthy young subjects. Subjects were scheduled to 15–24 repetitions of a 20-h rest/activity cycle, resulting in desynchrony between the sleep-wake cycle and the circadian rhythms of body temperature and melatonin. The circadian components of cognitive throughput, short-term memory, alertness, psychomotor vigilance, and sleep disruption were at peak levels near the temperature maximum, shortly before melatonin secretion onset. These measures exhibited their circadian nadir at or shortly after the temperature minimum, which in turn was shortly after the melatonin maximum. Neurobehavioral measures showed impairment toward the end of the 13-h 20-min scheduled wake episodes. This wake-dependent deterioration of neurobehavioral functions can be offset by the circadian drive for wakefulness, which peaks in the latter half of the habitual waking day during entrainment. The data demonstrate the exquisite sensitivity of many neurobehavioral functions to circadian phase and the accumulation of homeostatic drive for sleep.
Article
Instrumental learning occurs through both goal-directed and habit memory systems, which are supported by anatomically distinct brain systems. Interestingly, stress may promote habits at the expense of goal-directed performance, since stress before training in an instrumental task was found to cause individuals to carry on with the learned association in spite of a devalued outcome. These findings nevertheless left pending questions, and it has been difficult to determine which system is primarily affected by stress (an improved habit system, an impaired goal-directed system, or both) and at what point the stress acts (at the moment of learning by making more resistant habits, or after devaluation by making individuals less sensitive to change in the outcome value). The present study (N=72 participants, 63 males and 9 females) aimed to answer these questions with (i) an instrumental task that dissociates the two memory systems and (ii) three conditions of psychosocial stress exposure (Trier Social Stress Test): stress induced before learning, before devaluation, and not induced for the control group. The study confirms that exposure to psychosocial stress leads to habitual performance. Moreover, it provides new insight into this effect by locating its origin as an impairment in the capacity of the goal-directed system rather than a reinforcement in habit learning. These results are discussed in light of recent neurobiological models of stress and memory.
Article
Interventions to change health behaviors have had limited success to date at establishing enduring healthy lifestyle habits. Despite successfully increasing people's knowledge and favorable intentions to adopt healthy behaviors, interventions typically induce only short-term behavior changes. Thus, most weight loss is temporary, and stepped-up exercise regimens soon fade. Few health behavior change interventions have been successful in the longer term. In this article, we unpack the behavioral science of health-habit interventions. We outline habit-forming approaches to promote the repetition of healthy behaviors, along with habit-breaking approaches to disrupt unhealthy patterns. We show that this two-pronged approach—breaking existing unhealthy habits while simultaneously promoting and establishing healthful ones—is best for long-term beneficial results. Through specific examples, we identify multiple intervention components for health policymakers to use as a framework to bring about lasting behavioral public health benefits.
Article
Across two studies comparing younger and older adults, age differences in optimal performance periods were identified (Study 1), and then shown to be an important determinant of memory differences (Study 2). A norming study showed that while most younger adults were Evening or Neutral types, as determined by a standard questionnaire, the vast majority of older adults were Morning types. A second study compared the recognition performance of younger and older adults tested in the morning or in the late afternoon. Substantial age differences were found in the late afternoon, when younger but not older adults were at their optimal times. However, no age differences in memory performance were found in the morning, when older but not younger adults were at their peak period. Thus, synchrony between optimal performance periods and the time at which testing is conducted may well be a critical variable in determining group differences in intellectual performance, particularly between older and younger adults.
Article
The study under report examined the effects of time of day on prose recall in morning- and evening-type individuals. Subjects listened to two easy and two difficult passages at either 09.00, 14.00 or 20.00. Immediately after listening to a taperecorded version of each story, subjects were asked to write their recalls. The results indicated that recall decreased across time of day for morning types but increased for evening types. The effects of importance level were similar for passages at both difficulty levels at all times of day; however, time-of-day effects were largest for highly important-idea units from difficult passages. The results demonstrated that time of day influences immediate recall of prose in adults, and the pattern of these effects depended upon whether the subject was a morning or evening type. It was suggested that subsequent examinations of time of day and prose memory should utilize concurrent measures of encoding effort to assess whether processing strategies change across time of day.