Content uploaded by Jordan Navarro
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jordan Navarro on Jul 17, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
IDENTIFICATION OF PRE-ACCIDENT SITUATIONS FACED BY
FRENCH CYCLISTS IN URBAN ENVIRONMENTS: AN ON-ROAD
STUDY
Jordan Navarro, Samuel Aupetit, Gaël Allain, Christophe Hilaire, Joceline Rogé
Presses Universitaires de France | « Le travail humain »
2017/2 Vol. 80 | pages 187 à 207
ISSN 0041-1868
ISBN 9782130788645
Article disponible en ligne à l'adresse :
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
http://www.cairn.info/revue-le-travail-humain-2017-2-page-187.htm
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Pour citer cet article :
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Jordan Navarro et al., « Identification of pre-accident situations faced by french
cyclists in urban environments: an on-road study », Le travail humain 2017/2 (Vol.
80), p. 187-207.
DOI 10.3917/th.802.0187
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Distribution électronique Cairn.info pour Presses Universitaires de France.
© Presses Universitaires de France. Tous droits réservés pour tous pays.
La reproduction ou représentation de cet article, notamment par photocopie, n'est autorisée que dans les
limites des conditions générales d'utilisation du site ou, le cas échéant, des conditions générales de la
licence souscrite par votre établissement. Toute autre reproduction ou représentation, en tout ou partie,
sous quelque forme et de quelque manière que ce soit, est interdite sauf accord préalable et écrit de
l'éditeur, en dehors des cas prévus par la législation en vigueur en France. Il est précisé que son stockage
dans une base de données est également interdit.
Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org)
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 186 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 187 / 240
- © PUF -
Le Travail Humain, tome 80, n°2/2017, 187-205
EMPIRICAL STUDIES
RECHERCHES EMPIRIQUES
IDENTIFICATION OF PRE-ACCIDENT SITUATIONS
FACED BY FRENCH CYCLISTS IN URBAN
ENVIRONMENTS: AN ON-ROAD STUDY
IDENTIFICATION ET CLASSIFICATION
DES SITUATIONS DE PRÉ-ACCIDENT RAPPORTÉES
PAR LES CYCLISTES FRANÇAIS EN ZONE
URBAINE : UNE ÉTUDE EN CONTEXTE NATUREL
by/par Jo r D A n nA v A r r o 1, sA M u e l Au p e t i t 2, gA ë l Al l A i n 3,
ch r i s t o p h e hi l A i r e 2 & Jo c e l i n e ro g é 4
SUMMARY
The increasing number of cyclists, and the introduction of new infrastructure
has created new ways for cyclists and other road users to interact, which may
impact road safety. Consequently, our aim was to contribute to a better under-
standing of accidents involving cyclists through an in-depth, pre-accident analy-
sis based on an activity-centred approach. Various pre-accident situations, judged
as risky by twenty cyclists, were collected and classied as a function of their rela-
tive frequency of occurrence. The analysis found that a third of these situations
involved interactions with pedestrians or other obstacles. Then, the most frequent
situations are related to: (a) interactions with motorists either entering or leav-
ing a parking space; (b) motorists who stepped in front of a cyclist when turning
right; (c) cars entering the road from the right at an intersection, and (d) motor-
ists squeezing cyclists off the road when overtaking. An analysis of situations
where cyclists’ visibility was impaired found similar results except for situations
(c) and (d) where cyclists’ visibility was very rarely engaged, and which both
appeared to be associated to deliberate motorist behaviours. These ndings point
out the most frequent pre-accident situations reported by cyclists, and highlight the
need for their better integration, as a new type of road user, in urban areas.
Keywords: cyclist, on-road studies, pre-accident.
1. Corresponding author:
Laboratoire d’étude des mécanismes cognitifs, Institut de psychologie, université Lyon-II,
5 avenue Pierre Mendès-France, 69676 Bron Cedex, France – email : jordan.navarro@univ-
lyon2.fr
2. Dédale, 21 rue de Fécamp, 75012 Paris, France – email : ergo-centre@hotmail.com
3. Mémoire et marketing, Le chanay, France – email : gallain@memoire-marketing.com
4. IFSTTAR-TS2-LESCOT, 25 avenue François Mitterrand, 69675 Bron Cedex, France
– email : joceline.roge@ifsttar.fr
Identification of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
Jordan Navarro
205
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
188 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 188 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 189 / 240
- © PUF -
RÉSUMÉ
Le nombre grandissant de cyclistes et les évolutions des aménagements routiers
associés induisent de nouvelles interactions entre cyclistes et autres usagers de la
route. Compte tenu des enjeux sécuritaires, l’objectif poursuivi ici était de mieux
comprendre les situations accidentogènes impliquant les cyclistes via une analyse
détaillée de ces situations avec une méthodologie ergonomique fondée sur l’analyse
de l’activité. Les différentes situations de pré-accident jugées comme risquées par
vingt cyclistes lors de leur pratique habituelle ont été collectées et classées selon leur
fréquence d’occurrence relative. Les résultats indiquent que près d’un tiers des situa-
tions de pré-accident rapportées résultent de l’interaction entre cyclistes et piétons.
Après quoi et par ordre de fréquence, les situations étaient liées à des interactions
avec des automobilistes (a) entrant ou quittant le stationnement, (b) interférant
avec les cyclistes en tournant à droite, (c) interférant avec les cyclistes en venant
de la droite à une intersection, (d) ou encore dépassant les cyclistes de trop près.
Lorsque l’intérêt s’est porté plus spéciquement sur les situations où la visibilité
des cyclistes était engagée, des résultats similaires ont été obtenus exception faite
des catégories (c) véhicule venant de la droite et coupant la trajectoire du cycliste
et (d) dépassant les cyclistes de trop près qui tendent à disparaître. Ces situations
(c & d) étaient majoritairement associées à des comportements délibérés des auto-
mobilistes. Dans leur ensemble, les résultats permettent de classer par fréquence
d’apparition les situations de pré-accident rapportées par les cyclistes et pointent la
nécessitée d’une meilleure intégration des usagers cyclistes en zone urbaine.
Mots-clés : Cycliste, contexte naturel, analyse située, pré-accident.
INTRODUCTION
Cyclists’ number on the roads of several French big cities such as Paris
and Lyon is increasing (Papon & De Solère, 2010). Public authorities keep
going on encouraging people to ride bikes for several reasons including
sustainable development, reducing trafc jams, reducing noise pollution in
urban areas, and ghting against sedentariness. Bike use in big urban areas
has been supported since the early 2000’s. Infrastructures improvements
in favour of cyclists such as bike paths, bike lanes, contraow bike lanes
and shared lanes (between public bus, taxis and cyclists) have been created
in several French cities including Paris and Lyon. If those improvements
aims to favour safe bike use and tend to do so (Forester, 1994), they
might also lead to a decrease of attention cyclists pay to other road users
(de Lapparent, 2005). The increasing number of cyclists and the related
infrastructure evolutions came with new interactions between cyclists and
other road users that might impact on road safety. Following this idea, a
high proportion of cyclists crashes seems to be related to drivers failure
to detect cyclists (Wood, Lacherez, Marszalek, & King, 2009). Even if
cyclists crashes are underreported (Amoros, Martin, & Laumon, 2006),
those implying another motorist are more severe (Amoros, Chiron, Thélot,
& Laumon, 2011). Cyclists are considered as vulnerable road users as they
are physically exposed directly in case of crash.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 188 / 240
- © PUF -
189Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 189 / 240
- © PUF -
Several studies focused on accidents records in order to estimate the risks
associated with bike riding (e.g. Aultman-Hall & Hall, 1998; Aultman-Hall
& Kaltenecker, 1999; Minikel, 2012). For instance, light conditions and
infrastructure are known to impact on risky situations. More than a third of
motorist/cyclist crashes occurred under low visibility conditions (at night,
twilight or daybreak). A disproportionate ratio taking into account the
few cyclists riding at those times of the day (Lacherez, Wood, Marszalek,
& King, 2013). Regarding infrastructures, it was observed that Australian
cyclists in Brisbane feel more fear than Danish cyclists in Copenhagen
(Chataway, Kaplan, Nielsen, & Prato, 2014). According to the authors, the
differences were due to cyclists’ more adapted infrastructures and a better
integration of cyclists as road users in Copenhagen. Those studies aimed at
describing crashes in a variety of situations but did not intend to understand
in details the reported crashes. If a detailed methodologies to do so exists
(e.g. Ferrandez, 1995), very few studies focused on detailed crashes analysis
implying cyclists and trying to understand what happened from road users
perspective. Two studies however pointed out that many cyclists/motorists
accidents occurred when the motorist turned right stepping into the cyclist
located to the right of the vehicle (Räsänen & Summala, 1998; Summala,
Pasanen, Räsänen, & Sievänen, 1996). While turning right motorists often
only look to the left (Summala et al., 1996), explaining why only 11 %
of motorists perceived the cyclist before the collision. Cyclists were more
aware of the motorist presence, as 68 % perceived the motorist before
the collision. Among the cyclists that saw the vehicle, 92 % declared they
thought that the driver would yield as prescribed in the Highway Code.
In 37 % of the crashes analysed, neither the motorist nor the cyclist had
realized the crash risk, in the remaining situations the motorist (27 %), the
cyclist (24 %) or both (12 %) tried to avoid the crash (Räsänen & Summala,
1998). Infrastructure changes at bicycle crossings were showed to modify
both cyclists and motorists behaviours even in situations where the formal
change does not apply (Räsänen, Koivisto, & Summala, 1999).
The rationale followed in the current study was to analyse cyclists
reported risky situations from their own perspective based on their usual
daily cycling activity. Hereafter those situations will be referred as pre-
accident situations. Pre-accidents situations correspond to the situations
faced prior an actual collision, with a breakdown of the situation followed
by an emergency situation (Ferrandez, 1995). The methodology to do so
refers to “activity-centered ergonomics” (Ombredane & Faverge, 1955;
Theureau, 2003). The approach was previously applied successfully to
road safety to analyse motorcycle riders’ (Aupetit, Riff, Buttelli, & Espié,
2013). The methodology stand on real word activities analyse with minimal
intervention of the experimenters. In the current study, cyclists were given
no other instructions than to ride their bike as they usually do. The analysis
is performed by means of face-to-face interviews where participants are
confronted to videos traces of their own cycling activity to explore their
behaviours in the context of appearance. Although time consuming, the
best interest of this methodology is to offer an in-deep analysis taking
into account technical, social and cultural environments. Following this
method pre-accident situations will not be explored objectively organizing
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
190 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 190 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 191 / 240
- © PUF -
crashes data according to the situation of occurrence but rather subjectively
based on cyclists’ comments on their own behaviours. In other words,
this methodological framework offers the possibility to analyse the same
cyclist safety concerns from another point of view. This new perspective
is not meant to be objective but to enrich accident analysis understanding
by exploring the main protagonists actions. This methodology is also
well suited to analyse variation in practices due to road users’ cultural
differences. In the current study, cyclists from two different French cities
were considered, the rational was that pre-accident situations and their
relative frequency might be different depending on the city considered as
suggested by previous studies (Chataway et al., 2014).
As suggested by previous results, crashes involving cyclists might be
related to a problem of cyclists visibility (Summala et al., 1996). A survey
leaded on 622 motorists and 838 cyclists showed that the main source of
accident between motorists and cyclists is linked to motorists not viewing
cyclists before the crash (Wood et al., 2009). Cyclists’ lack of visibility might
be explained by a different perception of the reality by motorists and cyclists.
Indeed, the distance at which cyclists estimate being visible (100.3m) is twice
bigger than the one estimated by drivers (48.3m). Additional data collected
on a test track indicated drivers difculties to detect cyclists, especially at
night (Wood et al., 2010). It also appears that drivers and cyclists do not
have the same representation of cyclists’ visibility. 88 % of cyclists judged
that drivers do not visually seek for potential cyclists’ presence but only 66 %
of drivers. Conversely, only 57 % of cyclists declared being hardly visible
in the trafc while 77 % of the drivers think so (Wood et al., 2009). All
together cyclists assume to be perceived earlier and more often than they
actually are by drivers. This gap between cyclists and drivers perception
may be attributed to four different factors: (a) the fact that drivers look in
the direction of the cyclist but do not perceive it (“looked-but-failed-to-
see phenomenon” ; Brown, 2005) and therefore related to a lack of cyclists
sensorial conspicuity (Engel, 1971, 1974; Wulf, Hancock, & Rahimic, 1989),
(b) a lack of cyclist appearance expectation from drivers perspective, because
biking in urban areas is relatively recent (Clarke, Ward, Bartle, & Truman,
2007) and therefore related to a problem of cognitive conspicuity (Hancock,
Wulf, Thom, & Fassnacht, 1990), (c) cyclists inaccurate expectations, such
as drivers yielding (Räsänen & Summala, 1998), (d) cyclists optimism
regarding the risk of accident. Cyclists are indeed optimistic regarding the
risk of being involved in an accident (King, Wood, Lacherez, & Marszalek,
2012). And for a given situation, cyclists perceive less risks than drivers
(Chaurand & Delhomme, 2013).
Within this context, our aim was to contribute to a better understanding
of accident involving cyclists through in deep pre-accident analysis based on
an activity-centred approach. The main objective of the current experiment
was to collect the different pre-accident situations judged as risky by cyclists
during their regular cycling activity and to class them according to their relative
frequency of occurrence. Another objective was to measure how cyclists’
perceived lack of visibility is a key factor in pre-accident situations appearance.
Finally, the type pavement and other road users implied in pre-accident
situations have been considered to guide future infrastructure development.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 190 / 240
- © PUF -
191Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 191 / 240
- © PUF -
I. METHOD
i.1. pA r t i c i p A n t s
20 cyclists (7 females and 13 males), aged from 24 to 63 years (mean
age = 38.32 years ± 9.7), took part in the experiment. Table 1 shows each
participant gender, residence city, age, declared frequency of cycle use,
riding distance and time per day. To those data, the actual distance tracked
during the experiment was added as well as the number of risky situations
reported. All cyclists had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Each
received a nancial compensation of 200 euros for their participation. The
local ethics committee approved the experiment.
i.2. nA t u r A l i s t i c o n -r o A D e x p e r i M e n t
The cyclists participating in the experiment were asked to ride their bike
as they usually do without any constraints apart from wearing the provided
helmet equipped with a video camera switched on. All cyclists’ travels
were therefore recorded by the video camera for the whole duration of the
experiment (i.e. four complete weeks). That way real life cycle journeys
data (including familiar journeys following regular routes at regular times)
with minimal intrusion from experimenters were collected.
The cyclists have been selected in the two biggest French urban areas:
Paris and Lyon urban agglomerations. Those two cities were the two rst
cities where large-scale public bike systems were proposed, in Lyon in 2005
and in Paris in 2007 and where cyclists are always more numerous (Papon
& De Solère, 2010).
So as to cover a large variety of weather and light conditions, the
experiment was spread on period going from March to December.
Five cyclists were experienced in March/April, ve in May/June, ve in
September/October and ve in November/December.
i.3. DA t A c o l l e c t i o n
Three types of data were collected: (1) video recordings of all cyclists’
travels, (2) logbooks to keep track of risky situations faced by cyclists’ day
by day and (3) self confrontation interviews to as to understand in details
the risky situations logged by cyclists.
I.3.1. Video recording
Participants were asked to record all bike trips using a video camera that
was offering a high quality recording and a wide-angle view (HD 1080p and
170° of eld of view, Garmin VIRB Elite®). In order to minimize interferences
between the video camera and the cycling activity helmets equipped with
camera xing device were provided to participants. The helmet and the
camera were adjusted to each participant so as to ensure both participants’
comfort and safety and a good video recording of the visual scene.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
192 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 192 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 193 / 240
- © PUF -
tA B l e 1: Participants’ main characteristics
tA B l e A u 1. Caractéristiques principales des participants à l'étude
Gen-
der City
Age
(years)
Declared
frequency
of cycle
use (days
per week)
Declared
riding
distance
per day
(km)
Declared
riding
time
per day
(minutes)
Distance
tracked
during the
experiment
(km)
Number
of risky
situations
reported
(n)
F Paris 24 Every day 10 40 330 13
M Paris 44 Every day 20 45 200 15
F Paris 33 Every day 10 30 180 16
M Paris 48 5 or 6 days 60 170 480 27
M Paris 49 5 or 6 days 46 150 960 18
M Paris 37 5 or 6 days 49 110 920 18
M Paris 26 Every day 25 90 510 13
M Paris 43 5 or 6 days 15 60 310 32
F Paris 42 5 or 6 days 26 90 520 15
M Paris 27 5 or 6 days 20 50 450 20
F Lyon 30 Every day 20 70 440 15
F Lyon 29 4 or 5 days 3 15 60 10
M Lyon 63 Every day 20 90 560 10
M Lyon 35 Every day 15 45 280 6
M Lyon 26 Every day 15 50 300 15
F Lyon 51 4 or 5 days 20 60 450 14
F Lyon 39 4 or 5 days 5 15 80 13
M Lyon 41 4 or 5 days 5 15 80 10
M Lyon 38 4 or 5 days 5 15 65 6
M Lyon 41 5 or 6 days 3 15 20 1
M 38.3 5.9 19.6 61.3 359.8 14.4
SD 9.7 1 15.3 43.3 256.9 6.8
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 192 / 240
- © PUF -
193Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 193 / 240
- © PUF -
I.3.2. Logbook
Participants were provided a logbook and instructed to note all the
situations judged as risky for them in it. Cyclists were also instructed to
complete the logbook at least once a day. For each situation reported on
the logbook cyclists had to detail a title, the date, time and travel starting
and end point. To that information, a drawing of the situation completed
with a short description was asked. The main objective of the logbook was
to identify the risky situations among all cyclists’ video recorded travels
and to guide the ensuing analysis.
I.3.3. Self-confrontation interviews
Self-confrontation interviews were run in face-to-face with every
participant individually at the end of each experimental week. All
participants faced four interviews of about half an hour each (depending
on the number of risky situations logged). Those interviews have focused
on (1) a detailed description of the risky situations reported in the logbook
and (2) self-confrontation based on the videos recordings of those risky
situations.
In practice, the cyclist and the experimenter looked at the computer
screen where the different risky situations reported by the cyclist were
played. The experimenter paused the recording as many times as required in
order to let the cyclist put words on the activity carried out in the presented
video. The principle of this method is to facilitate participants’ description
of their own activity by reconstructing step by step, the perceptive and
decision-making processes implied in the recorded situation (Theureau,
2003, 2010; Vermersch, 1994).
When cyclists commented videos, the experimenter encouraged them
to focus on the watched situation when necessary. The idea was to keep
the cyclist in the dynamic context of the situation previously experienced
so as to reach physical and cognitive actions actually performed during the
recorded situations. These techniques allow the exploration of both explicit
and implicit aspects of physical or mental actions. The interviewer guided
cyclists to put their experience into words. This method aimed at collecting
verbal description on feelings, sensations, internal states, and thoughts that
were experienced by the cyclist for each situation considered (Aupetit,
Gallier, Riff, Espié, & Delgehier, 2016; Aupetit et al., 2013; Vermersch,
1994).
i.4. pr o c e D u r e
Cyclists were recruited via advertisement on social networks and
cycling associations. Before inclusion in the experiment, all volunteers
completed a short questionnaire about cycling habits (see collected data
in Table 1). Only cyclists riding their bike 4 or 5 time a week or more
were included.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
194 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 194 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 195 / 240
- © PUF -
After inclusion, the experimenter met each cyclist face to face to present
the experiment and the material (i.e. video camera and logbook use). Each
cyclist was then ready to start the experiment itself for four consecutive
weeks.
An individual meeting was set every week so as to run self-confrontation
interviews. All participants took part to four self-confrontation interviews,
except the last participant presented in table 1 who stopped the experiment
after week 1.
i.5. DA t A A n A l y s i s
The type of pavement cyclists were riding on when the pre-accident
situations occurred was analysed in terms of frequency. The presence and
type of other road users implied in the pre-accident situations was also
analysed in terms of frequency.
Similar pre-accident situations were regrouped so as to offer a
categorization of the reported situations and their relative frequencies. Two
experimenters performed the categorization process independently. The
categorizations obtained were then compared and common decision made
in case of disagreement. This categorization process was conducted once
including all reported situations and once including only the situations
where the visibility of the cyclist was engaged based on videos and
interviews.
Pearson’s chi-squared tests were used for statistic analysis. The level of
signicance of p<0.05 was used.
II. RESULTS
All in all, 287 pre-accident situations were collected (187 in Paris and
100 in Lyon).
ii.1. ty p e o f p A v e M e n t i n t h e p r e -A c c i D e n t s i t u A t i o n s
The different types of pavement cyclists were riding on when pre-
accident situations occurred were regrouped in seven categories listed in
Figure 1. Those seven different types of pavement observed were not
equally distributed (Paris: c2 (6) = 449.92, p<0.001; Lyon: c2 (6) =208.42
p<0.001). The distributions observed in Paris and in Lyon were not
signicantly different (Pearson c2 (30) = 35, p=0.242). For both cities,
most of the pre-accident situations happened on regular shared pavement,
which represent more than half of the situations (M = 52%), much more
than off-road cycle paths (M = 15.5%) and bike lanes (M = 14%) and even
more than bus lanes (M = 8%), cycle path (M = 5.5%), contraow bike
lanes (M = 3.5%) and sidewalks (M = 1.5%).
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 194 / 240
- © PUF -
195Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 195 / 240
- © PUF -
Figure 1. Type of pavement cyclists were riding
on when pre-accident situations occurred in Paris and Lyon urban areas.
Percentage (%) of the number of situations reported.
Figure 1. Type de chaussée sur laquelle se trouvaient les cyclistes
lors des situations de pré-accident à Paris et Lyon.
Pourcentage (%) du nombre de situations reportées.
ii.2. ot h e r r o A D u s e r s i M p l i e D i n t h e p r e -A c c i D e n t s i t u A t i o n s
The other road users implied in pre-accident situations occurred were
regrouped in seven categories in
Figure 2. Other road users implied were not equally distributed (Paris:
c2 (6) = 318.33, p<0.001; Lyon: c2 (6) = 172.02, p<0.001). The distributions
observed in Paris and in Lyon were not signicantly different (Pearson
c2 (20) = 22.75, p=0.301). On the average car drivers were implied in more
than half of the pre-accident situations (M = 54.5%) and pedestrians in
about a quarter (M = 25.5%). The remaining fth of the situations was
spread between cyclists, truck and bus drivers, powered two-wheelers and
no other road user implication.
Figure 2. Other road users implied in the pre-accident situations reported in Paris and Lyon
urban areas. Percentage (%) of the number of situations reported.
Figure 2. Autres usagers de la route impliqués dans les situations de pré-accident à Paris et Lyon.
Pourcentage (%) du nombre de situations reportées.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
196 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 196 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 197 / 240
- © PUF -
ii.3. cA t e g o r i z A t i o n o f r e p o r t e D p r e -A c c i D e n t s i t u A t i o n s
II.3.1. All situations included
Fourteen different pre-accident categories were obtained by pooling
similar situations (see Table 2). Those fourteen categories were not equally
distributed (Paris: c2 (13) = 212.52, p<0.001; Lyon: c2 (13) = 37.07,
p<0.001). The distributions observed were not signicantly different
between Paris and Lyon (Pearson c2 (54) = 55.61, p=0.414). If all data are
available in Table 2, only the six categories that reached more than 5 % of
all reported situations on the average between the two urban areas will be
presented in detail. Among those six categories, very similar frequencies
were obtained for half of pre-accident situations and descriptive differences
must be mentioned for the other half.
tA B l e 2. Categorisation of all pre-accident situations reported ordered
by frequency.
tA B l e A u 2. Catégorisation de l'ensemble des situations de pré-accident rapportées
classées de la plus fréquente à la moins fréquente.
Category
number Category name Paris Lyon Mean
1
Pedestrian crossings or obstacles on bicycle facilities
32 % 33 % 32.5 %
2 Vehicle entering or leaving parking 3% 25% 14%
3 Vehicle turning right stepping into cyclists’ way 11% 5% 8%
4Vehicle coming from the right and stepping
into cyclists’ way 7% 8% 7.5%
5 Vehicle squeezing cyclists while overtaking 7% 8% 7.5%
6Other road users changing direction while
riding between lanes in trafc jams 11 % - 5.5 %
7 Conict with unpowered two-wheelers - 9 % 4.5 %
8 Opening doors of parked vehicles 4 % 4 % 4 %
9 Unadapted infrastructure - 8 % 4 %
10
Conict with other road users while crossings lanes
8 % - 4 %
11 Oncoming vehicle turning left stepping into
cyclists way 5 % - 2.5 %
12 Observed cyclist errors and violations 4% - 2 %
13 Conict with an oncoming road user in
contraow bike lane 4 % - 2 %
14 Conict in shared lane with bus or taxi 4% - 2 %
Categories 1, 4 and 5 appeared as similar between Paris and Lyon.
Pedestrian crossings or obstacles on bicycle facilities (category 1) were the
most frequently observed situations with about a third of the situations
reported. Categories 4 and 5 (i.e. vehicle coming from the right and
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 196 / 240
- © PUF -
197Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 197 / 240
- © PUF -
stepping into cyclists’ way and vehicle squeezing cyclists while overtaking
respectively) were much less frequent (7.5 % on the average). Even if
categories number 2, 3 and 6 descriptively appeared as quite different
between Paris and Lyon, no signicant differences were observed.
II.3.2. Situations with cyclists’ visibility engaged
151 (96 in Paris and 55 in Lyon) pre-accident situations among
the 287 collected were identied as related to cyclists’ visibility during
interviews. It is not an objective visibility measure, but rather cyclists’
point of view of their own visibility on the situations reported. When it
was explicit in the self-confrontation interviews that at least a contributing
factor to the pre-accident situation was due to a lack of cyclist visibility for
the other road user, the situation was classied as a “situation with cyclists’
visibility engaged”. The same category numbers were used.
Thirteen of the fourteen pre-accident categorises previously
described were found when focusing only on situations where cyclists’
visibility was engaged (see Table 3). Those thirteen categories were not
equally distributed (Paris: c2 (12) = 143.24, p<0.001; Lyon: c2 (12)
31.84, p<0.002) and the distribution observed was not signicantly
different between Paris and Lyon (Pearson c2 (45) = 55.25, p=0.141).
tA B l e 3. Categorisation of pre-accident situations reported with cyclists
visibility engaged ordered by frequency.
tA B l e A u 3. Catégorisation des situations de pré-accident rapportées associées à un défaut
de visibilité des cyclistes et classées de la plus fréquente à la moins fréquente.
Category
number
Category name Paris Lyon Mean
1
Pedestrian crossings or obstacles on bicycle facilities
37 % 42 % 39,5 %
2 Vehicle entering or leaving parking 4% 36% 20,0%
3Vehicle turning right stepping into cyclists’ way 18% 4% 11,0%
7 Conict with unpowered two-wheelers - 13 % 6,5 %
8 Opening doors of parked vehicles 6 % 5 % 5,5 %
6Other road users changing direction while
riding between lanes in trafc jams 10 % - 5,0 %
4Vehicle coming from the right and steeping into
cyclists’ way 8 % - 4 %
5 Vehicle squeezing cyclists while overtaking 4% - 2,0 %
10
Conict with other road users while crossings lanes
4 % - 2,0 %
13 Conict with an oncoming road user in
contraow bike lane 3 % - 1,5 %
14 Conict in shared lane with bus or taxi 3% - 1,5 %
11 Oncoming vehicle turning left stepping into
cyclists way 2 % - 1,0 %
12 Observed cyclist errors and violations 1% - 0,5 %
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
198 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 198 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 199 / 240
- © PUF -
Compared to all pre-accident situations, the same three rst categories in
term of average relative frequencies were found in the same order. If all
data are available in ween Paris and Lyon, no signicant differences were
observed.
Table 3, only the ve categories that reached more than 5 % of reported
situations on the average between the two urban areas will be presented in
detail. Among those ve categories, very similar frequencies were obtained
in Paris and Lyon for two of pre-accident situations and descriptive
differences must be mentioned for the three others.
Categories 1 and 8 appeared as very similar between Paris and Lyon.
Pedestrian crossings or obstacles on bicycle facilities (category 1) were
the most frequently observed situations with about 40 % of the situations
reported. Opening doors of parked vehicles (category 8) was also found in
both cities but much less frequently (5.5 %). Even if categories number 2,
3 and 7 appeared as descriptively quite different between Paris and Lyon,
no signicant differences were observed.
III. DISCUSSION
When looking at all pre-accident situations reported by cyclists’, it
appears that a third of the risky situations collected implied interactions
with pedestrians or obstacles located on bicycle facilities. The following
most frequent situations were related to interactions with motorists either
entering or leaving parking, stepping into cyclists’ way while turning right
or coming from the right at an intersection, or motorists squeezing cyclists
while overtaking. When focusing on situations where cyclists’ visibility was
engaged similar most frequent situations were observed except for vehicle
coming from the right at an intersection and steeping into cyclists’ way
and vehicle squeezing cyclists while overtaking that appeared related as
motorists deliberate behaviours. Instead, two other situations were found
quite frequent, conict between unpowered two-wheelers and opening
doors of parked vehicles. Whatever the agglomeration considered, more
than half of the pre-accident situations were located on regular shared
pavement and more than half of those situations implied a car driver, and
about a quarter pedestrians.
A more detailed presentation of the most frequently collected situations
will help to understand precisely what happened in those situations. The
main situation reported by cyclists in the current study was related to
pedestrian crossings, and not to other motorists. To our knowledge, no study
specically focused on cyclists/pedestrian conicts, possibly because their
number is under-estimated due to a lack of report by public forces and
because their physical and economical consequences are not as important as
with motorists/cyclists accidents. Another possible explanation is that many
pre-accident cyclists/pedestrians occur but only few accidents results from
these situations. This would be explained by relatively low displacement speed
compared to powered vehicles, as a consequence both cyclists and pedestrian
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 198 / 240
- © PUF -
199Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 199 / 240
- © PUF -
can slow down and change directions quickly. Pedestrians’ crossings were
regrouped with obstacles on bicycle facilities in the same category (category
number 1). Obstacles located on the bicycle facilities were added to that
category because they implied similar cyclists’ behaviours: cyclists had to
skirt and/or brake. Pedestrian crossings represent most of the situations
reported in this category (about 80 %). In many occasions the pedestrians
did not paid attention to cyclists’ presence because interacting with their
phone or seeking for noisy vehicles only for instance. Almost all the obstacles
were vehicles moving or parked on bicycle facilities, others obstacles consist
in objects on bicycle facilities (e.g. tree branches or trashes).
The second most frequent situation is related to a vehicle entering or
leaving parking. In most of those situations, the motorist did not perceive
the cyclist because his/her attention was focused on the parking manoeuver
or on its insertion in the trafc. Possibly due to a greater number of parking
possibilities in Lyon, the relative frequency of this category was higher in
Lyon than in Paris. The third most frequent situation occurred when the
motorist turned right stepping into cyclist way at the right of the vehicle,
a situation already analysed (Räsänen & Summala, 1998; Summala et al.,
1996). The relative frequency of this situation increased when looking
only to the pre-accident situations where cyclist visibility was engaged
(from 8 % to 11 %), indicating that visibility is indeed a key factor in that
context. However, in several occasions participants declared that motorists
perceived the cyclist presence but forced their way obliging the cyclist to
brake.
To provide a more detailed view on the sources of the pre-accident
situations the different situations were classied according to the different
causes identied in the literature (Table 4). As all the reported data, this
classication is based on cyclists’ subjective description of the considered
situations. A high proportion of cyclists crashes seems to be related to drivers
failure to detect cyclists (Wood et al., 2009). This failure might be due to:
(a) the fact that drivers or other road users look in the direction of the cyclist
but do not perceive it (“looked-but-failed-to-see phenomenon” ; Brown,
2005) and therefore related to a lack of cyclists sensorial conspicuity (Engel,
1971, 1974; Wulf et al., 1989) (b) a lack of cyclist appearance expectation
from drivers or other road user perspective, because biking in urban areas is
relatively recent (Clarke et al., 2007), and therefore related to a problem of
cognitive conspicuity (Hancock et al., 1990). Based on cyclists’ perspective
only it was not possible to differentiate sensorial and cognitive conspicuity,
consequently those two causes were regrouped under the same category
referred as other road users failure to detect cyclists (Table 4 second
column). To those other road users failures, pre-accident situations could
also be caused by: (c) cyclists inaccurate expectations, such as drivers yielding
(Räsänen & Summala, 1998) and (d) cyclists optimism regarding the risk
of accident. Based on cyclists’ self-confrontations analysis it appeared that
those two accident causes were linked and therefore hardly differentiable,
consequently they were regrouped (Table 4 third column). Finally in
addition to the previous four categories documented in the literature, a fth
cause appeared in the current study (e) motorists’ or pedestrians’ deliberate
behaviours (Table 4 fourth column). That is to say motorists or pedestrians
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
200 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 200 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 201 / 240
- © PUF -
that did perceive correctly the cyclist, should have given priority to the
cyclist according to the Highway Code and had the capacity to do so, but
deliberately engaged a displacement that put the cyclist at risk.
Slightly more than half of the pre-accident situations were linked
to cyclists not being correctly perceived (sensorial and or cognitive
conspicuity). Surprisingly, motorists’ and occasionally pedestrians’
deliberate behaviour caused about a third of the pre-accident situations. In
those situations, motorists’ break the rules and outing common courtesy
to gain way. The remaining situations were due to cyclists’ inaccurate
expectations and/or over-optimism. Our data are in line with previously
reported results (Wood et al., 2009). A high proportion (more than half in
the current study) of cyclists’ pre-accidents are related to motorists’ failure
to detect cyclists. Additionally our data point out motorists’ deliberate
behaviours to gain way over cyclists as another important source of pre-
accident situations. Along with cyclists’ inaccurate expectations and excess
of optimism, pre-accident situations seem to be related to difculties in
sharing the road.
tA B l e 4. Repartition of pre-accident situations reported related to:
motorists’ failure to detect cyclists (sensorial conspicuity (a) and/or
cognitive conspicuity (b)), cyclists inaccurate expectations (c) and over-
optimism (d) or motorists’ or pedestrians’ deliberate behaviours (e).
tA B l e A u 4. Répartition des situations de pré-accident rapportées selon : l'incapacité
des automobilistes à détecter les cyclistes (conspicuité sensorielle (a) et/ou conspicuité
cognitive (b)), les attentes erronées des cyclistes (c) et un excès d'optimisme (d), ou un
comportement délibéré des automobilistes ou piétons (e).
Location
Sensorial
conspicuity (a)
and/or cognitive
conspicuity (b)
Cyclist inaccurate
expectations (c) and/
or over-optimism (d)
Motorist’s or
pedestrians’
deliberate
behaviours (e)
Paris 52 % 15 % 33 %
Lyon 55 % 13 % 32 %
Mean 53,5 % 14 % 32,5 %
A more detailed category per category presentation of these results
offers more precise insights so as to improve safety depending on the
context. Only the pre-accident categories of 15 or more occurrences (more
that 5 %) are presented in Table 5. Those categories (from 1 to 6) represent
75 % of all reported pre-accident situations. All categories but number 6
present an unbalanced repartition with more than 60 % of pre-accident
situations related to (1) sensorial and/or cognitive conspicuity for cate-
gories 1, 2 and 3, or (2) motorists’/pedestrians’ behaviours for categories
4 and 5. Cyclists’ conspicuity is a critical issue involved in an average of
69 % of pre-accidents categories 1, 2 and 3. Those three categories repre-
senting themselves about 55 % of all pre-accident situations collected. So
as to improve noticeably cyclists’ safety in those situations, conspicuity
must be improved. Sensorial conspicuity may be improved with the use of
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 200 / 240
- © PUF -
201Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 201 / 240
- © PUF -
reecting gear for biking for instance. Besides cognitive conspicuity should
be improved by prevention campaigns to inform other road users in parti-
cular motorists’ while falling in categories 2 and 3 and pedestrians while
crossing roads. Some work should also be undertaken in order to change
motorists’ and pedestrians’ attitudes toward cyclists to reduce the amount
of deliberate behaviours that sometimes cause pre-accident situations in
particular while overtaking cyclists or by stepping into cyclists’ path (cate-
gories 4 and 5). A way to do so is to improve motorists’ road rule know-
ledge. Indeed low level of road rule knowledge is associated with poor
attitudes toward cyclists (Rissel, Campbell, Ashley, & Jackson, 2002). Pre-
accidents occurring while riding between lanes in trafc jam are not parti-
cularly related to a specic cause identied.
tA B l e 5. Category by category repartition of all pre-accident situations
reported related to: motorists’ failure to detect cyclists (sensorial and/
or cognitive conspicuity), cyclists inaccurate expectations and over-
optimism, or motorists’ or pedestrians’ deliberate behaviours.
tA B l e A u 5. Répartition catégorie par catégorie de l'ensemble des situations de pré-
accident rapportées selon : l'incapacité des automobilistes à détecter les cyclistes (conspi-
cuité sensorielle et/ou conspicuité cognitive), les attentes erronées des cyclistes et un excès
d'optimisme, ou un comportement délibéré des automobilistes ou piétons.
Category
number Category name
Sensorial
and/or
cognitive
conspicuity
Cyclist
inaccurate
expectations
and/or over-
optimism
Motorists’
or
pedestrians’
deliberate
behaviours
Category
weight /
all pre-
accident
situations
1
Pedestrian
crossings or
obstacles on
bicycle facilities
64 % 16.5 % 19.5 % 32.5 %
2Vehicle entering
or leaving parking 80% 0% 20% 14%
3Vehicle turning
right stepping
into cyclists’ way 62.5% 0% 37.5% 8%
4
Vehicle coming
from the right
and stepping into
cyclists’ way
28.5% 7% 64.5% 7.5%
5Vehicle squeezing
cyclists while
overtaking 15.5% 0% 84.5% 7.5%
6
Other road
users changing
direction while
riding between
lanes in trafc jams
45 % 30 % 25 % 5.5 %
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
202 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 202 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 203 / 240
- © PUF -
Using a survey, differences in perceived safety of mixed trafc infra-
structure layouts and fear of trafc were found between different cities
located in different countries with different cycling habits, namely
Brisbane and Copenhagen (Chataway et al., 2014). In the current
study, no signicant differences were observed between Paris and Lyon.
Possibly because of the relatively small number of participants involved
to compare the two French towns or because French cyclists, drivers,
their habits and the infrastructure available in the two French towns
are not different enough. Some descriptive differences in the variety of
the situations recorded, more important in Paris than in Lyon, and fre-
quencies in a few situations tend to indicate that differences might exists.
Further experiments focusing on that point particularly are required in
order to conclude.
About half of the pre-accident situations happened on regular shared
pavement and the other half was distributed across infrastructures dedica-
ted to unpowered road users: off-road cycle paths (15.5 %) and bike lanes
(14 %), bus lanes (8 %), cycle path (5.5 %), contraow bike lanes (3.5 %)
and sidewalks (1.5 %). Although less present on urban areas than regular
shared pavement, it appeared that specic cycle equipment are regularly
used by cyclists and are a location where pre-accident situations are almost
as frequent as regular pavement. As a consequence specic attention should
be paid to those relatively new and expanding road pavements.
Regarding the type of road users involved in reported pre-accident situa-
tions, car drivers were implied in more than half of the pre-accident
situations (54.5 %) and pedestrians in about a quarter (25.5 %). Those two
road users’ types were clearly identied as the main sources of pre-accident
situations. If a high proportion of situations with drivers was expected,
the proportion of situations implying pedestrians was unexpectedly high.
This might be due to the fact that during the last decades, pedestrians
were more and more protected and they have now priority in most of the
situations and location. In addition pedestrian often choose to walk on bike
lane or cycle paths increasing potential conicts with cyclists.
No crash was recorded during the experiment, and the results collec-
ted only took into account cyclists’ perception of the situations. Obviously
pre-accident situations are more numerous than actual accidents (Sanders,
2015). Identity pre-accidents situations is highly valuable because it may
help to improve safety in a different way that accident analysis. However
pre-accidents might not necessarily be representative of accidents. In addi-
tion, cyclists known the journeys were recorded by the video camera. As
a consequence, it can not be excluded that cyclists might have changed
some of their usual behaviours during the experiment. Another weakness
of the methodology used is that experimenters had no control on cyclists’
activity, the ways they choose, speed, time of the time and so on. The situa-
tions collected should now be tested in more controlled conditions so as
to understand precisely the underpinning mechanisms. The evaluation of
several other variables would also be of particular interest in order to better
characterise pre-accident situations. In particular, the inuence cyclists’
age, gender and expertise as well as the season of the year and weather of
the day might impact on pre-accident situation reported. Another inter-
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 202 / 240
- © PUF -
203Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 203 / 240
- © PUF -
esting perspective in order to complement the reported data will be to
consider pre-accident situations involving cyclists’ and their visibility from
motorist and/or pedestrian perspectives.
IV. CONCLUSION
The data collected offered a new perspective on cyclists’ safety on
French agglomerations. Cyclists’ increasing number and the development
of specic infrastructures comes with new challenges in terms of road
safety, including risky situations involving pedestrians and cyclists
visibility. About half of the situations analysed in the experiment were
related to a problem of cyclists’ visibility. This issue might be related to a
weak cyclists expectance from motorists’ and pedestrians’ perspectives. In
addition drivers-cyclists are known to be much more likely than drivers
only to report safe driving behaviours related to sharing the roads with
cyclists and to have a better knowledge of road rules related to cycling
infrastructure (Johnson, Oxley, Newstead, & Charlton, 2014). Therefore,
cyclists’ individual differences in the pre-accident situations should be
further analysed in future experiments. Another perspective to improve
cyclists’ safety would be to identify the most risky areas as performed in
Stockholm for instance (Gustafsson & Archer, 2013). More immediately,
the pre-accident scenarios identied in this naturalistic study would be
used to conduct simulated safe and well-controlled experiments in order to
understand even more precisely why the situations reported occurred.
AcknowleDgeMents
This study was supported by the French association “Fondation
Sécurité Routière” (VISIBLE project).
REFERENCES
Amoros, E., Chiron, M., Thélot, B., & Laumon, B. (2011). The injury epidemiology
of cyclists based on a road trauma registry. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 653.
Amoros, E., Martin, J.-L., & Laumon, B. (2006). Under-reporting of road crash
casualties in France. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(4), 627–635.
Aultman-Hall, L., & Hall, F. L. (1998). Ottawa-Carleton commuter cyclist on-and
off-road incident rates. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30(1), 29–43. http://doi.
org/10.1016/S0001-4575(97)00059-6
Aultman-Hall, L., & Kaltenecker, M. G. (1999). Toronto bicycle commuter safety
rates. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 31(6), 675–686. http://doi.org/10.1016/
S0001-4575(99)00028-7
Aupetit, S., Gallier, V., Riff, J., Espié, S., & Delgehier, F. (2016). Naturalistic study
of the risky situations faced by novice riders. Ergonomics, 59(8), 1109–1120.
http://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2015.1120887
Aupetit, S., Riff, J., Buttelli, O., & Espié, S. (2013). Naturalistic study of rider’s
behaviour in initial training in France: Evidence of limitations in the educational
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
204 Jordan Navarro et al.
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 204 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 205 / 240
- © PUF -
content. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 58, 206–217. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2012.09.036
Brown, I. D. (2005). Review of the “Looked but Failed to See” Accident Causation
Factor (No. 60). Department for transport, London, UK.
Chataway, E. S., Kaplan, S., Nielsen, T. A. S., & Prato, C. G. (2014). Safety
perceptions and reported behavior related to cycling in mixed traffic: A
comparison between Brisbane and Copenhagen. Transportation Research
Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 23, 32–43. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
trf.2013.12.021
Chaurand, N., & Delhomme, P. (2013). Cyclists and drivers in road interactions:
A comparison of perceived crash risk. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 50,
1176–84. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.09.005
Clarke, D. D., Ward, P., Bartle, C., & Truman, W. (2007). The role of motorcyclist
and other driver behaviour in two types of serious accident in the UK.
Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 39(5), 974–81. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2007.01.002
de Lapparent, M. (2005). Individual cyclists’ probability distributions of severe/
fatal crashes in large French urban areas. Accident; Analysis and Prevention,
37(6), 1086–92. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2005.06.006
Engel, F. (1971). Visual conspicuity, directed attention and retinal locus. Visison
Research, (11), 563–576.
Engel, F. (1974). Visual conspicuity and selective background interference in
eccentric vision. Vision Research, (14), 459–471.
Ferrandez, F. (1995). L’ étude détaillée d’accidents orientée vers la sécurité primaire:
méthodologie de recueil et de pré-analyse. Paris: Presses de L’E.N.C.T.
Forester, J. (1994). Bicycle Transportation: A Handbook for Cycling Transportation
Engineers. Cambridge, MA, 2nd ed., MIT Press.ISBN 978-0-262-56079-5.
Gustafsson, L., & Archer, J. (2013). A naturalistic study of commuter cyclists in the
greater Stockholm area. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 58, 286–98. http://
doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.06.004
Hancock, P., Wulf, G., Thom, D., & Fassnacht, P. (1990). Driver workload during
differing driving maneuvers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 22(3), 281–290.
Johnson, M., Oxley, J., Newstead, S., & Charlton, J. (2014). Safety in numbers?
Investigating Australian driver behaviour, knowledge and attitudes towards
cyclists. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 70, 148–54. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2014.02.010
King, M. J., Wood, J. M., Lacherez, P. F., & Marszalek, R. P. (2012). Optimism
about safety and group-serving interpretations of safety among pedestrians
and cyclists in relation to road use in general and under low light conditions.
Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 44(1), 154–9. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2010.12.003
Lacherez, P. F., Wood, J. M., Marszalek, R. P., & King, M. J. (2013). Visibility-related
characteristics of crashes involving bicyclists and motor vehicles – Responses
from an online questionnaire study. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic
Psychology and Behaviour, 20, 52–58. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2013.04.003
Minikel, E. (2012). Cyclist safety on bicycle boulevards and parallel arterial routes
in Berkeley, California. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 45, 241–7. http://doi.
org/10.1016/j.aap.2011.07.009
Ombredane, A., & Faverge, J. (1955). L’analyse du travail (PUF, Paris).
Papon, F., & De Solère, R. (2010). Les modes actifs: marche et vélo de retour
en ville. La Revue, Commissariat Général Au Développement Durable-Service de
L’observation et Des Statistiques.
Räsänen, M., Koivisto, I., & Summala, H. (1999). Car Driver and Bicyclist Behavior
at Bicycle Crossings Under Different Priority Regulations. Journal of Safety
Research, 30(1), 67–77. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4375(98)00062-0
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 204 / 240
- © PUF -
205Identication of pre-accident situations faced by cyclists
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 205 / 240
- © PUF -
Räsänen, M., & Summala, H. (1998). Attention and expectation problems in
bicycle-car collisions: an in-depth study. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 30(5),
657–66. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9678219
Rissel, C., Campbell, F., Ashley, B., & Jackson, L. (2002). Driver road rule
knowledge and attitudes towards cyclists. Australian Journal of Primary Health,
8(2), 66–69.
Sanders, R. L. (2015). Perceived traffic risk for cyclists: The impact of near miss
and collision experiences. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 75C, 26–34. http://
doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.11.004
Summala, H., Pasanen, E., Räsänen, M., & Sievänen, J. (1996). Bicycle accidents
and drivers’ visual search at left and right turns. Accident Analysis & Prevention,
28(2), 147–153. http://doi.org/10.1016/0001-4575(95)00041-0
Theureau, J. (2003). Course-of-action analysis and course-of-action centered
design. Handbook of Cognitive Task Design, 55–81.
Theureau, J. (2010). Les entretiens d’autoconfrontation et de remise en situation
par les traces matérielles et le programme de recherche «cours d’action». Revue
D’anthropologie Des Connaissances, 42(2), 287–322.
Vermersch, P. (1994). L’entretien d’explicitation. Paris : ESF.
Wood, J. M., Lacherez, P. F., Marszalek, R. P., & King, M. J. (2009). Drivers’ and
cyclists’ experiences of sharing the road: incidents, attitudes and perceptions of
visibility. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 41(4), 772–6. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aap.2009.03.014
Wood, J. M., Tyrrell, R., Marszalek, R. P., Lacherez, P. F., Carberry, T., Chu, B.,
& King, M. J. (2010). Cyclist visibility at night: Perceptions of visibility do not
necessarily match reality. Journal of the Australasian College of Road Safety, 21,
56–60. Retrieved from http://eprints.qut.edu.au/38338
Wulf, G., Hancock, P., & Rahimic, M. (1989). Motorcycle conspicuity: an evaluation
and synthesis of influential factors. Journal of Safety Research, (20), 153–176.
Manuscrit reçu : mars 2016.
Accepté après révision par F. Darses : décembre 2016.
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 206 / 240
- © PUF -
31 mai 2017 04:03 - Le travail humain n°2/2017 - Collectif - Le travail humain - 155 x 240 - page 207 / 240
- © PUF -
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France
Document téléchargé depuis www.cairn.info - Université Louis Lumière Lyon 2 - - 159.84.74.48 - 17/07/2017 12h00. © Presses Universitaires de France