ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

The Robust Running Ape: Unraveling the Deep Underpinnings of Coordinated Human Running Proficiency

Frontiers
Frontiers in Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In comparison to other mammals, humans are not especially strong, swift or supple. Nevertheless, despite these apparent physical limitations, we are among Natures most superbly well-adapted endurance runners. Paradoxically, however, notwithstanding this evolutionary-bestowed proficiency, running-related injuries, and Overuse syndromes in particular, are widely pervasive. The term ‘coordination’ is similarly ubiquitous within contemporary coaching, conditioning, and rehabilitation cultures. Various theoretical models of coordination exist within the academic literature. However, the specific neural and biological underpinnings of ‘running coordination,’ and the nature of their integration, remain poorly elaborated. Conventionally running is considered a mundane, readily mastered coordination skill. This illusion of coordinative simplicity, however, is founded upon a platform of immense neural and biological complexities. This extensive complexity presents extreme organizational difficulties yet, simultaneously, provides a multiplicity of viable pathways through which the computational and mechanical burden of running can be proficiently dispersed amongst expanded networks of conditioned neural and peripheral tissue collaborators. Learning to adequately harness this available complexity, however, is a painstakingly slowly emerging, practice-driven process, greatly facilitated by innate evolutionary organizing principles serving to constrain otherwise overwhelming complexity to manageable proportions. As we accumulate running experiences persistent plastic remodeling customizes networked neural connectivity and biological tissue properties to best fit our unique neural and architectural idiosyncrasies, and personal histories: thus neural and peripheral tissue plasticity embeds coordination habits. When, however, coordinative processes are compromised—under the integrated influence of fatigue and/or accumulative cycles of injury, overuse, misuse, and disuse—this spectrum of available ‘choice’ dysfunctionally contracts, and our capacity to safely disperse the mechanical ‘stress’ of running progressively diminishes. Now the running work burden falls increasingly on reduced populations of collaborating components. Accordingly our capacity to effectively manage, dissipate and accommodate running-imposed stress diminishes, and vulnerability to Overuse syndromes escalates. Awareness of the deep underpinnings of running coordination enhances conceptual clarity, thereby informing training and rehabilitation insights designed to offset the legacy of excessive or progressively accumulating exposure to running-imposed mechanical stress.
This content is subject to copyright.
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 1
MINI REVIEW
published: 12 June 2017
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00892
Edited by:
Duarte Araújo,
Universidade de Lisboa, Portugal
Reviewed by:
Robert Hristovski,
Saints Cyril and Methodius University
of Skopje, Macedonia
Jia Yi Chow,
National Institute of Education,
Nanyang Technological University,
Singapore
*Correspondence:
John Kiely
jkiely@uclan.ac.uk
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Movement Science and
Sport Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 22 February 2017
Accepted: 15 May 2017
Published: 12 June 2017
Citation:
Kiely J (2017) The Robust Running
Ape: Unraveling the Deep
Underpinnings of Coordinated Human
Running Proficiency.
Front. Psychol. 8:892.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00892
The Robust Running Ape: Unraveling
the Deep Underpinnings of
Coordinated Human Running
Proficiency
John Kiely*
Institute of Coaching and Performance, School of Sport and Wellbeing, University of Central Lancashire, Preston,
United Kingdom
In comparison to other mammals, humans are not especially strong, swift or supple.
Nevertheless, despite these apparent physical limitations, we are among Natures most
superbly well-adapted endurance runners. Paradoxically, however, notwithstanding this
evolutionary-bestowed proficiency, running-related injuries, and Overuse syndromes in
particular, are widely pervasive. The term ‘coordination’ is similarly ubiquitous within
contemporary coaching, conditioning, and rehabilitation cultures. Various theoretical
models of coordination exist within the academic literature. However, the specific
neural and biological underpinnings of running coordination,’ and the nature of their
integration, remain poorly elaborated. Conventionally running is considered a mundane,
readily mastered coordination skill. This illusion of coordinative simplicity, however,
is founded upon a platform of immense neural and biological complexities. This
extensive complexity presents extreme organizational difficulties yet, simultaneously,
provides a multiplicity of viable pathways through which the computational and
mechanical burden of running can be proficiently dispersed amongst expanded
networks of conditioned neural and peripheral tissue collaborators. Learning to
adequately harness this available complexity, however, is a painstakingly slowly
emerging, practice-driven process, greatly facilitated by innate evolutionary organizing
principles serving to constrain otherwise overwhelming complexity to manageable
proportions. As we accumulate running experiences persistent plastic remodeling
customizes networked neural connectivity and biological tissue properties to best
fit our unique neural and architectural idiosyncrasies, and personal histories: thus
neural and peripheral tissue plasticity embeds coordination habits. When, however,
coordinative processes are compromised—under the integrated influence of fatigue
and/or accumulative cycles of injury, overuse, misuse, and disuse—this spectrum of
available ‘choice’ dysfunctionally contracts, and our capacity to safely disperse the
mechanical ‘stress’ of running progressively diminishes. Now the running work burden
falls increasingly on reduced populations of collaborating components. Accordingly our
capacity to effectively manage, dissipate and accommodate running-imposed stress
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 2
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
diminishes, and vulnerability to Overuse syndromes escalates. Awareness of the deep
underpinnings of running coordination enhances conceptual clarity, thereby informing
training and rehabilitation insights designed to offset the legacy of excessive or
progressively accumulating exposure to running-imposed mechanical stress.
Keywords: running, rehabilitation, coordination, overuse, variability, plasticity
INTRODUCTION
Running is the most primitively ancient of athletic movements:
critical to competitive success in many sports and, in evolutionary
contexts, critical to survival. Uniquely amongst mammals
humans employ an upright bipedal bouncing gait when running.
A gait characterized by long flight times interspersed with brief
ground contacts during which the shock of impact, equating to
multiple times bodyweight, is absorbed, re-cycled, and steered
through the narrow stabilizing platform provided by a single
supporting foot. Nevertheless, despite these apparent limitations,
we are amongst Nature’s most supremely well-adapted runners
(Bramble and Lieberman, 2004).
The evolutionary innovations bestowing human running
proficiency do not, however, render us invulnerable to
breakdown and running-related injuries are common (van
der Worp et al., 2015). Runners seem particularly exposed to
Overuse injuries, with up to 70% suffering such injury each year
(Clarsen et al., 2013;Saragiotto et al., 2014;van der Worp et al.,
2015). Various definitions exist, amid some inconsistency, and
confusingly ‘Overuse’ describes both a ‘mechanism’ and ‘type of
injury (Clarsen et al., 2013). Although definitions vary, published
consensus agrees that Overuse syndromes arise consequent
to progressively mounting micro-trauma accumulated over a
protracted period, exacerbated by insufficient recovery leading
to increasing tissue sensitization in the absence of single
catastrophic events (Clarsen et al., 2013;Saragiotto et al., 2014).
Commonly cited risk factors include elevated running volumes,
prior injury, fatigue and background psychosocial stress (Clarsen
et al., 2013;van der Worp et al., 2015;Ivarsson et al., 2016). Yet
how these factors synergistically interact, leading to Overuse
injuries, has yet to be clarified (van der Worp et al., 2015).
A frequently overlooked distinction between running and
many other sporting movements is that running is one of a
limited sub-set of gaits—along with crawling and walking—that
are so evolutionary ancient as to have mutually co-evolved in
tandem with human neural and biological infrastructures (Kiely
and Collins, 2016). In short: how we run is shaped by, yet has also
contributed to shaping, modern human morphology, in ways that
other sporting movements—a golf swing; a tennis serve; rowing;
the butterfly stroke—, have not. An implication of this synergistic
co-evolution of form and function is that the adaptations
underpinning human running permeate every dimension of our
anatomical, biological, and neurological being. Our capacity to
withstand the extraordinary mechanical and stability challenges
imposed during our bouncing bipedal running gait is not
attributable to any single evolutionary adaptation. Instead human
running robustness emerges as a consequence of our slowly
developing capacity to seamlessly harness, orchestrate and
integrate the outputs of multiple biological and neurological sub-
systems to accomplish running objectives. In short: our ability to
coordinate the running action.
The core defining feature of coordination is that multiple
components work together to realize an objective (Diedrichsen
et al., 2010). Conventionally, within the Sports Sciences,
coordination is perceived through the lens of Dynamical Systems
Theory (DST). Recently, through the lens of Optimal Feedback
Control Theory (OFCT), conventional interpretations of DST
have been criticized for obscuring the fundamental priority of
sensory feedback in shaping effective movement coordination
(Todorov, 2004, 2009). The OFCT framework subsequently
claims to more prominently highlight the relationship between
high-level goals, and the real-time sensorimotor control strategies
most suitable for accomplishing those goals. Recent ecological
dynamics perspectives have similarly advocated the prominent
role of emerging sensory ‘information’ in regulating on-going
motor behavior (Seifert et al., 2013). As in other scientific
domains, however, debates and disagreements exist and the
need for on-going argument, skepticism and scrutiny remain
obvious. Various perspectives, accordingly, have been expertly
and extensively discussed within their respective motor control
and neuroscientific literatures (see for example: Davids and
Glazier, 2010;Nagengast et al., 2010;Kelso, 2012;Proske
and Gandevia, 2012). The problem, for the vast majority of
practical Sports Scientists, Sports medicine practitioners and
evidence-led Coaches is that while these academic debates
are essential, by necessity they are abstract, highly technical,
typically obscured by the in-house terminology of the specific
academic realm and often too narrowly focused to provide
practically implementable insight. Accordingly, any attempt to
construct a coherent overview of such a diverse and contentious
topic will, inevitably, be flawed and incomplete. Nevertheless,
the overarching objective of this review is to provide this
targeted group with an updated evidence-led synopsis of the
key linked dimensions of the running coordination phenomenon
deemed most relevant to performance, resilience and injury
rehabilitation.
THE EVOLUTIONARY UNDERCURRENTS
OF COORDINATED RUNNING
ROBUSTNESS
Evolutionary survival demands that biological systems, operating
in unpredictable environments using unreliable components and
finite energy sources, are robust to the challenges to which they
are most commonly exposed (Kitano, 2004). Accordingly, from
an evolutionary perspective, running coordination’s overriding
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 3
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
imperative is to deploy available resources to satisfactorily achieve
desired outcomes, while preserving an acceptable robustness
to any running-imposed ‘threat’ serving to reduce survival
probability.
This ‘threat’ takes many forms. If energy depletes; if
mechanical tissue tolerances are exceeded; if neural processes
are overloaded to the extent that movement precision
and/or cognitive clarity declines, then inevitably survival
probability diminishes. No single survival imperative necessarily
predominates. Instead the neurobiological system seeks to
satisfactorily resolve multiple partially overlapping, partially
competing organizational constraints (Skoyles, 2008;Hodges
and Tucker, 2011;Miller et al., 2012). In negotiating this complex
organizational problem, evolution has arrived at a typically
ingenious resource-sparing set of solutions.
Interpretation of Sensation Shapes
Movement
As running increases in severity we are made consciously
aware of mounting ‘threat’ through increasingly discomforting
interpretations of arising sensory information (Marcora et al.,
2009;Smirmaul, 2012). At the whole-body level growing
discomfort influences psycho-emotional state, amplifying
perceptions of anxiety, ‘pain’ and diminished attention which
in turn intensify the inner conflict between motivational drive
and perceived effort that, collectively, we interpret as mounting
‘fatigue (Marcora et al., 2009;Seay et al., 2011;Smirmaul,
2012). At the local level, muscle activation patterns are subtly
modulated to offload sensitized tissues, thereby moderating
regionalized discomfort and alleviating tissue irritation (Mizrahi
et al., 2000;Gerlach et al., 2005;Seay et al., 2011). Through
these mechanisms our interpretation of arising psychobiological
discomfort informs us of increasing risk—of impending tissue
damage, elevating metabolic costs, increasing neural processing
demands and cognitive effort—, thereby providing a direct
means through which the perceived relevance of changing
sensation directly influences running behavior (Marcora et al.,
2009;Wolpert et al., 2011).
Prompted by subtle, but persistent, sensory signals the CNS
continually searches for economic trade-offs between desired
outcomes, available resources and discomforting perceptions of
‘threat’ (Hodges and Tucker, 2011;Miller et al., 2012). As we
accumulate running experiences, we learn to more precisely
triangulate between sensory feedback, feedforward activation and
desired running outcomes and gravitate toward coordinative
solutions more satisfactorily resolving these multiple competing
constraints. Progressively, with practice, sensory information
and muscular activation strategies co-evolve into a seamlessly
integrated sensorimotor system: whereby changes in sensation
directly modulate muscular activations, and changes in activation
directly modify sensation (Wolpert et al., 2011; see Figure 1).
Through this elegantly efficient process, sensory feedback
information and feedforward activation instructions become
irrevocably mutually entangled: preserving running robustness
within acceptable limits through an integrated sensorimotor
process of ‘self-organizing optimality’ (Glazier and Davids, 2009).
Organizing Neuro-biological Complexity:
Modularity Facilitates Degeneracy
Biological lifeforms are reflectively characterized as complex
adaptive systems. Complex: as the behaviors of individual
components are inextricably linked to those of multiple others
through arrays of processes, cycles and regulatory feedback
loops. Adaptive: as the behaviors and collaborative outputs
of collections of components flexibly modify their concerted
contributions to best fit current context (Manor and Lipsitz,
2013).
Each individual entity within the complex organism is
linked, physically or functionally, to every other. Nevertheless
there remains an evident modularity, whereby collections of
elements are more densely networked to each other than to
elements within other modules (Whitacre, 2010;Mason, 2015).
All modules are inter-connected, yet are simultaneously partially
insulated and functionally semi-autonomous. Modularity,
accordingly, facilitates robustness as modules can evolve,
reshape, rewire, and repair in tandem, or independently, without
jeopardizing survivability of the entire organism (Maleszka et al.,
2014;Mason, 2015).
Modularity is a fundamental neuro-biological organizing
principle, greatly simplifying otherwise overwhelmingly
disordered complexity. Related modules exhibit extensive
functional overlap, such that alliances of neural networks and
peripheral tissues can spontaneously modify behaviors to achieve
equivalent ‘outputs’ through a multiplicity of pathways. This
functional agility is often conflated with redundancy, but is
perhaps more reflectively termed degeneracy (Glazier and
Davids, 2009;Mason, 2015;Seifert et al., 2016). Degeneracy
describes the ability of alternate structural pathways to achieve
similar functional outcomes in one context, or dissimilar
functional outcomes in divergent contexts (Seifert et al., 2016).
Degenerate systems are composed of diverse elements, capable
of alternately fulfilling similar or overlapping functions and
are fundamental facilitators of complexity, robustness, and
evolvability (Whitacre, 2010; Maleszka et al., 2014;Mason,
2015). Redundancies, in contrast, occur when sub-sets of
identical elements combine to achieve similar outcomes
and are subsequently rare, as there are few identical neural
and/or biological entities. Degeneracy describes a more flexibly
adaptive phenomenon, whereby collaborating communities of
fundamentally distinct components produce reliably consistent
outputs under fluctuating conditions (Mason, 2015;Seifert et al.,
2016).
The human runner represents a highly degenerate system.
Consider the phenomenon of leg stiffness during ground-
contact—the accurate calibration of which facilitates the
protective dampening and economic re-cycling of impact shocks.
Our highly degenerate neuro-biological design can produce
equivalent leg stiffness’s using diverse coordinative strategies:
muscle-tendon units (MTU’s) vary individual contributions
whilst, collectively, whole-leg power outputs remain consistent;
individual MTU’s achieve similar force outputs by summating
different muscle and tendon contributions; individual muscles
vary activated motor unit populations under differing contractile
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 4
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
FIGURE 1 | Mechanisms through which pain leads to re-distribution of activity within, and between, muscles. Used by permission from Hodges and Tucker (2011).
conditions to produce identical tensions; multiple combinations
of torso, leg and foot postural orientations and pre-set
tensionings deliver equivalent propulsive and stabilization-
enabling contributions (Wickham and Brown, 1998;Roberts
and Azizi, 2011;Turvey and Fonseca, 2014). This option-rich,
highly degenerate movement landscape provides a multiplicity
of avenues through which collaborating modular alliances
combine, and re-combine, to flexibly satisfy dynamically shifting
demands.
This degenerate design offers multiple means to accomplish
running objectives. Historically, the apparently overwhelming
complexity presented by this proliferation of movement ‘options’
was famously interpreted as a control ‘problem’ (Bernstein,
1967). This potentially complex problem, however, is reduced
by the gradual construction of synergies—coordinative structures
comprised of highly context-specific, context-sensitive functional
linkages serving to temporarily constrain collaborating elements
such that they act as single coherent units (Latash et al., 2007;
Wu and Latash, 2014). Through the formation of synergies
the control ‘problem’ is greatly simplified, while simultaneously
retaining the benefits of complexity and degeneracy. As such,
more recently, the apparent problem of excessive choice has
been reframed as the ‘bliss’ of motor abundance (Latash, 2012).
When running, this abundance of potentially over-whelming
movement choice’ can be, through effective coordination,
productively deployed to disperse the running work-burden
among networks of collaborating tissues: thereby promoting
efficiency and robustness.
Fractal Variation: Deploying Coordinative
Abundance
Conventionally, we equate skilful running with metronomic
regularity. As proficient runners achieve reliably consistent stride
outcomes, it seems sensible to assume experts precisely replicate
running stride characteristics. In recent years, however, close
scrutiny of running behaviors illustrates that, even when experts
run at steady paces, movement parameters persistently vary
(Stergiou and Decker, 2011). Through the lens of traditional
motor control paradigms such variability was initially interpreted
as ‘noise’—meaningless error arising from the intricacies of the
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 5
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
engineering challenge, measurement inaccuracies and fallible
biological components. Intriguingly, however, more recent
investigations reveal the structure of gait variability to be neither
randomly erratic, nor independent of prior events. Instead, the
architecture of past, current and future stride variability’s appear
statistically linked through, as yet incompletely understood, long-
range correlations (Hausdorff, 2007;Stergiou and Decker, 2011;
Hamill et al., 2012).
Structured Non-random Variability
Mandelbrot’s classic work, The Fractal Geometry of Nature
(Mandelbrot, 1982), first popularized the term ‘fractal’ to describe
the phenomenon, pervasive in Nature, of recurrent structural
self-similarity. The unifying characteristic of fractals is scale-free
structural replication: whereby individual entities are composed
of sub-units of a shared structure, while themselves forming
super-ordinate entities conforming to a similarly patterned
design. Examples include the branching networks of the vascular
system and convoluted folding surfaces of the neo-cortex: both
fractally replicating architectures exponentially increasing tissue
surface area.
Fractal self-similarity is not, however, confined to physical
architectures and also manifests as time-series or organizational
replications. Thus sub-regions may be exact or distorted
copies of the all-encompassing over-arching structure, or may
simply share quantitative, qualitative, or statistical properties
(Goldberger et al., 2002;Newell et al., 2005;West, 2010;
Vázquez et al., 2016). Fractal signatures are ubiquitous in
neurophysiology, with multiple phenomena exhibiting self-
similarity across observational scales. Famously, the time series
of inter-heartbeat intervals—heart-rate variability—is a fractal
phenomenon. Although each beat is unique, its uniqueness is not
random but shaped by an innate, neurally embedded background
algorithm blending the organism’s unique idiosyncrasies with
past experiences, current status, and transient momentary
demands, to collectively shape the time-series architecture of
the emergent heartbeat (Goldberger et al., 2002). Accordingly
the beat-to-beat ‘solution’ to the circulation ‘problem’ is neither
tightly prescribed, nor loosely erratic.
Expert running coordination is similarly characterized by
the tuned inter-play between predictability and responsiveness
bestowed by the fractally fluctuating deployment of option-rich,
functionally overlapping degenerate networks. Together, these
networks provide the diverse repertoire of behavioral responses
essential for survival in chaotic, unpredictable environments (Van
Orden, 2007;Nakayama et al., 2010;Stergiou and Decker, 2011;
Vázquez et al., 2016).
RUNNING VARIABILITY: SHARING THE
RUNNING WORK-BURDEN
As with other neuro-biological processes running dynamics
exhibit robust fractal characteristics: suggesting stride-to-stride
variability is neither random, nor dictated by the fluctuating
idiosyncrasies of current conditions. Instead on-going stride
variations are meaningfully related—in a decaying Power law
FIGURE 2 | Inter-relationships between complexity and injury resilience.
fashion—to past variations stretching back over thousands
of strides (Meardon et al., 2011;Hamill et al., 2012). This
pervasive fractal variation ensures the mechanical stress of
running is distributed in ever varying, yet non-randomly
organized, patterns: patterns tuned, through practice, to the
runner’s unique architectural and experiential peculiarities.
This structured variability enables the well-trained runner
to disperse the running ‘work burden’ amongst expanded
networks of biological tissues, whilst simultaneously retaining
the agility to spontaneously respond to emerging challenge
(Figure 2). Healthy running, accordingly, is characterized by
an optimal bandwidth of movement variability: neither too
much, nor too little (Meardon et al., 2011;Hamill et al.,
2012).
Accordingly proficient running coordination is not the
capacity to monotonously replicate an idealized stride pattern,
but the ability to continuously recombine expansive, yet
conditioned, populations of collaborating neural and biological
components. Thereby, enabling the achievement of reliably
consistent running outcomes through a diversity of subtly
shifting movement permutations.
Diminishing Complexity, Drives
Dysfunctional Variability
As we move through a running life, accumulative cycles of ‘wear
and tear’—of injury, overuse, misuse, and disuse—gradually
degrade both the material integrity of biological components
and the networked richness of neural connectivity (Elbert and
Rockstroh, 2004;Taubert et al., 2010;Hoppeler et al., 2011).
As neuro-biological complexity contracts, the landscape of
viable degenerate permutations, capable of satisfying running
demands, deteriorates. Now, the mechanical stress of running
must be distributed amongst shrinking networks of collaborating
components (Pelletier et al., 2015).
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 6
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
FIGURE 3 | Relationships between running gait variability, risk and efficiency.
Reductions in viable degeneracies do not, however, inevitably
decrease running variability. Instead, as the neuro-biological
systems struggles to proficiently manage imposed loadings,
mechanical stress becomes either more tightly focused on
restricted populations of working tissues, or is erratically
dispersed amongst expanded webs of unconditioned tissues (Van
Orden, 2007;Hamill et al., 2012). As illustration, ACL-deficient
knees typically exhibit reduced, whereas ACL-reconstructed
knees exhibit dramatically expanded, inter-stride variability
(Stergiou and Decker, 2011;Hamill et al., 2012). Such deviations
from habituated variability ranges, oscillating between overly
formulaic constancy and wild randomness, signify an impaired
capacity to absorb, disperse, and purposefully recycle and re-
direct impact momentums (Nakayama et al., 2010;Figure 3).
As coordinative fluency deteriorates, vulnerability to Overuse
syndromes and unexpected perturbations escalates.
Global Accommodation of Local
Perturbation
The entangled nature of complex neurobiology ensures that
when variability changes at any discrete location, accommodating
compensatory behaviors occur elsewhere in the system. As
illustration: active injuries typically reduce habitual running
variability in the injured leg—constraining control to protect
sensitized tissues—, while simultaneously inducing expansions
of variability in the non-injured leg (Hamill et al., 2012).
Such evidence illustrates that, although running injury is a
site-specific event, the accommodation of injury is a system-
wide phenomenon occasioning system-wide coordinative
adjustments. Importantly, these behavioral modifications,
although temporarily functional, inevitably expose compensating
tissues to unhabituated loadings. What has not been discussed
within the running-related literature, however, are the neural and
biological mechanisms which structurally embed running habits
and which must be micro-architecturally altered to support
coordinative change.
PERVASIVE BIO-PLASTICITY: THE
EMBEDDED LEGACY OF PRIOR EVENTS
A fundamental dimension of human neuro-biology is life-
long experience-dependent plasticity: the capacity within the
CNS and tissues of the periphery to lastingly respond—
structurally, chemically, electrically and materially—to repeat
experience (Elbert and Rockstroh, 2004;Taubert et al., 2010).
Throughout supra-spinal and spinal branches of the CNS
persistent patterns of neural activation induce experience-
dependent plastic re-configurations, micro-architecturally
embedding relationships between regularly co-operating
neural components and associated motor units. Plasticity in
the CNS is mirrored in the periphery, as tissues re-model in
response to habitual loading patterns (Hoppeler et al., 2011).
Experience-dependent plasticity refines and economizes
communications linkages between collaborating neural
networks, and conditions peripheral tissue structures to
better cope with regularly encountered movement contexts
(Pelletier et al., 2015).
As we converge on individually unique running styles,
pervasive neuro-biological plasticity embeds movement habits:
thereby constraining the landscape of degenerate movement
options to manageable proportions and increasing the probability
previously successful ‘solutions will be recycled in the future.
Plasticity, accordingly, drives the physical embodiment of
coordinative change: thereby sculpting the micro-architectural
basis of coordinative synergies, linkages, and attractors.
Inevitably, however, plasticity is both blessing and curse,
and the engraining of new habits inevitably degrades old
habits.
The Plasticity of Over-Specialization
As running experience accumulates, the sensorimotor apparatus
becomes ever-more efficient at executing the running task. But
neural resources are evolutionarily precious and fundamentally
limited commodities and, as such, are persistently re-deployed to
fulfill varying roles within diverse tasks. Such conflicting usage
patterns drive competitive plasticity processes, as neural networks
strive to persistently re-model neurological ‘form’ to best fit
currently prioritized ‘function.’
Consequently, as we progress from novice to ‘skilful,
a by-product of on-going neuroplastic refinement is that
fewer networked collaborators are required to manage
the evermore highly practiced running pattern (Coq and
Barbe, 2011;Avanzino et al., 2014;Pelletier et al., 2015).
Subsequently it becomes evolutionarily wasteful to continually
dedicate expanded sensorimotor networks to task execution.
Accordingly, when the range of running behaviors to which
we are regularly exposed becomes monotonously stereotypical,
evolutionary pressure to economize resource uptake ensures the
landscape of conditioned neural and biological collaborators,
dedicated to executing highly practiced running patterns,
progressively diminishes as under-utilized resources are re-
allocated elsewhere (Elbert and Rockstroh, 2004;Avanzino
et al., 2014). A drawback, therefore, of engaging in only a
narrow band of overly stereotypical running tasks, is that we
become hyper-efficient at deploying reduced populations of
degeneracies to execute a narrowing band of self-similar running
patterns. As direct consequence, we become increasingly
vulnerable to both overuse syndromes, and unhabituated
challenges.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 7
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
The Plasticity of Disuse
Prolonged abstinence from running drives a progressive loss
of physiological conditioning, and also dims the regular flow
of running-related sensorimotor information. Consequently, the
cortical circuitry normally maintained by consistently processing
running-related sensorimotor information is eroded as voracious
competitive plasticity re-models neural connectivity to best fit
current usage patterns (Elbert and Rockstroh, 2004;Pelletier
et al., 2015). Subsequently when we return to regular running
coordinative control is slightly less proficient and slightly less
resilient.
The Plasticity of Misuse
When we run in injured or irritated states we subtly alter
coordination patterns to divert discomforting mechanical stress
away from sensitized tissues: alleviating negative sensation,
tempering structural damage, and facilitating healing. If,
however, we continue to run in compromised patterns for
prolonged periods, newly adapted remedial strategies become
progressively more plastically engrained within CNS and tissue
architectures (Engineer et al., 2012;Avanzino et al., 2014).
The dynamic inter-play between experience-driven and
competitive plasticity processes ensure the traces of temporarily
functional coordinative compensations typically remain
plastically embedded within neuro-biological structures:
thereby becoming the new ‘normal, and exerting a legacy not
easily erased within the abbreviated timeframes offered by
conventional rehabilitation paradigms (Pelletier et al., 2015).
Promoting Positive Plasticity
Ultimately plastic re-modeling, as it consumes precious material
and energetic resources, is evolutionarily expensive (Merzenich
et al., 2014;Clark et al., 2015). Within the adult-brain it
is not evolutionarily economical to plastically adapt to all
stimulation—valuable neural reserves would be immediately
depleted. Accordingly, structures in the mature cortex plastically
remodel only when specific criteria—regulated by modulatory
neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine, dopamine, serotonin,
and norepinephrine—are satisfied (Merzenich et al., 2014;Clark
et al., 2015). Operating collectively these neuro-modulatory
enablers act as “on–off switches, engaging excitatory and
inhibitory processes and temporarily opening plasticity-enabling
‘windows of opportunity’ within which sensorimotor inputs
contributing to ‘success are selectively amplified; while signals
from competing inputs, uncorrelated with that success, are
selectively dampened (Merzenich et al., 2014).
Over time, the continued amplification of relevant
sensorimotor inputs provides a competitive advantage;
greatly enhancing the representational detail embedded in
the cortical territory dedicated to processing running-related
sensorimotor information (Wolpert et al., 2011;Engineer et al.,
2012). Crucially, a core finding emanating from this research
domain is that repetitively non-varying, non-challenging
‘mindless’ movements—those not demanding focused attention
for satisfactory execution—are insufficiently stimulating to
reliably release the cocktail of neuromodulating chemical
catalysts necessary for plastic re-modeling within the mature
motor cortex (Merzenich et al., 2014;Clark et al., 2015). In
contrast, positive plastic re-modeling is optimized in response
to behaviorally relevant intense practice, executed at the limits
of current abilities and therefore demanding high attentional
and motivational drives (Avanzino et al., 2014;Merzenich
et al., 2014). Thus positive neural re-modeling is promoted
only when tasks are neither so easy that they fail to stimulate
focused attention, nor so difficult that continuous failure
undermines motivation. In essence coordination improves
through engaging challenge, not mindless routine. A rationale
perhaps explaining why rehabilitation processes employing non-
challenging coordinative tasks typically fail to generate optimal
recovery (Elbert and Rockstroh, 2004;Merzenich et al., 2014;
Clark et al., 2015).
CONCLUSION
As we accumulate running experiences, sensory feedback biases
us toward personalized styles more satisfactorily resolving
achievement of the running objective against an acceptable
investment of survival-relevant resources. Guided by innate
evolutionary influences, individualized coordinative habits
progressively shape around our unique anatomical, biological,
neurological, and experiential idiosyncrasies.
Subsequently, as we progress from “novice” to “skilled
runners we more sensitively and smoothly respond to small
perturbations, thereby offsetting the need to periodically
and clumsily respond to larger challenges as minor errors
accumulate. We adjust activation patterns to navigate away from
discomforting sensation, thereby moderating tissue aggravations.
We gravitate toward activations more proficiently poising bio-
composite tissue structures to productively absorb and re-cycle
impact momentums, thereby reducing energetic investment and
dampening shock decelerations. We learn to exploit our layered
landscape of degenerate movement options by fractally varying
stride parameters under the integrated influence of historical
events and current context, thereby dispersing running work-
burdens amongst expanded webs of conditioned tissues. As we
accumulate running experience plasticity-processes progressively
embed working relationships between regularly collaborating
neural components, and embed the tissue features most
adaptive to running-specific loadings. As such plasticity is the
mechanism that engrains synergies and linkages, and embeds
the attractor states underpinning running coordination habits.
A key observation, accordingly, is that running coordination
change is supported on a platform of neuro-biological plastic
modification.
The evolutionary neuro-economics that embed efficient
habits, however, eventually encase us within limiting constraints.
Plasticity facilitates learning by engraining efficient habits,
yet also retains the residues of past traumas and prolonged
sensitivities: subsequently ensuring injuries are rarely transient
peripheral events, but long-lasting insults etched into cortical
tissues of the CNS (Elbert and Rockstroh, 2004;Coq and Barbe,
2011;Pelletier et al., 2015). Similarly the enduring traces of
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 8
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
repeated cycles of over-specialization, disuse, and misuse, impose
plastic re-configurations not automatically reverting to original
conditions once discomfort diminishes and pain-free running is
resumed.
As we progress through our running lives, the sensorimotor
landscape is in perpetual plastic flux as the integrated influences
of health, training, and injury subtly re-configure neural
connectivity and biological tissue architectures. Chronically, the
progressive accumulation of plastic mal-adaptations drives the
creeping decay of networked neural connectivity: compromising
sensorimotor information flow, blurring cortical representations
of peripheral structures, prompting mal-adaptations in neuronal
excitability, and driving disorder within the primary motor cortex
(Coq and Barbe, 2011;Avanzino et al., 2014). As a consequence,
coordinative control degrades.
When other lifestyle and training considerations—
background psycho-emotional stress, monotonous running
volumes, generalized and localized fatigue—, are overlaid on
already compromised operating conditions, access to expansive
populations of viable movement degeneracies further diminishes.
As this self-perpetuating cycle escalates, coordinative proficiency
decays, susceptibility to tissue irritations grows and we become
increasingly fragile to Overuse syndromes and non-formulaic
perturbations.
Practical Insights and Relevance
Deeper appreciation of the various phenomena underpinning
running coordination potentially informs many aspects of
conventional theory and practice. The topics below are offered
as tentative examples:
Overuse Injury
Documented incidence rates suggest running-related Overuse
injury is neither a ‘solved, nor perhaps clearly articulated,
problem. Contextualizing Overuse as a direct consequence
of chronically compromised coordination emphasizes the
necessity of balancing the monotonous stagnation, often
implicit in conventional endurance running programs, with
the unhabituated challenging stimulation essential to promoting
positive neuro-plastic re-modeling. Furthermore, this rationale
suggests that introducing coordinative diversity into high-
volume running programs may be an effective prophylactic
against Overuse occurrence.
Enforcing Technical Change
A deeper appreciation of the embedded undercurrents that
shape running coordination also questions the long-standing
practice of attempting to change technique simply by instructing
the runner to consciously re-configure established coordination
patterns so as to better conform to an aesthetic ideal.
Suddenly altering engrained running habits diverts mechanical
stress along unhabituated pathways: thereby inevitably exposing
unconditioned tissue to unaccustomed loadings and elevating
injury risk. And although empirical evidence remains scarce,
there is a suggestion of rising injury rates following short-term
technical interventions (see Tucker, 2007).
Driving Neuro-plastic Change
Crucially, the perspective presented here suggests we should
perhaps pay less attention to how running styles look, and more
attention to designing interventions that provide the coordinative
challenge necessary to sufficiently stimulate the neuro-plastic
re-modeling necessary to persistently refine communicative
clarity between CNS and the peripheral musculature. Although
such interventions typically fall outside the scope of conventional
run-training dogma, many coaches, past and present, have
intuitively designed training practices fulfilling the criteria
for optimally stimulating neuro-modulatory processes (see for
example: Kiely, 2013; Pfaff, personal communication; Smith,
personal communication). What emerging scientific insight
does add, however, is a growing appreciation of the value of
regularly challenging running coordination through the design
and implementation of appropriately constructed practices.
Visual Evaluation of Running Technique
Conventionally, we associate running coordination with running
technique—the visual evaluation of running style evaluated
against an aesthetic ideal. This pervasive assumption, however,
has never been satisfactorily demonstrated, and no empirical
evidence supports a direct relationship between looking ‘better,
and actually being ‘better.’
When we visually assess a runner’s technique, and extrapolate
these observations to running efficiency and injury risk
conclusions, we make judgments based on very superficial
information. Typically we fail to acknowledge the unseen
underlying terrain—the idiosyncratic neurology; the embedded
fractal signatures; the unique anatomical architectures and tissue
structures; the plastically personalized legacy of historical habits
and traumas—upon which coordinative habits are founded.
And while it is feasible that, to the highly practiced eye,
visual evaluation may provide clues, generally how these
clues are interpreted is rooted in assumptions currently
lacking an evidence base. Certainly, visual assessments of
running proficiency seem unavoidably subjectively biased and
previous investigations demonstrate differences in technical
ratings between coaches, and even when the same coach
evaluates the same footage at different times (Norris et al.,
2014).
Would performances improve if running form more closely
conformed to perceived technical ideals? Are more aesthetically
pleasing runners less injury prone; more economical? While
opinions are plentiful, evidence is scarce. Anecdotally, renowned
coach and physiologist, Dr. Jack Daniels, once sent video of 20
physiologically evaluated competitive runners to a selection of
coaches and exercise scientists, asking them to—on the basis of
visual inspection—rank athletes in order of running economy but,
“they couldn’t tell, no way at all” (Kolata, 2007).
Finally. . .
The perspective offered within this review is that coordination
is the overarching super-capacity ultimately orchestrating how
proficiently neural, muscular, cardiovascular, and metabolic
reserves are purposefully harnessed, or wastefully squandered.
In relation to running: coordination is the learned deployment
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 9
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
of available neuro-biological resources to satisfactorily realize
running objectives for an acceptable cost’—in terms of depletion
of energetic and neural reserves, and exposure to risk. It is the
physical expression of a confluence of psychological, emotional,
neural, and biological constraints emerging in response to the on-
going interplay between intention, motivation, and perception
of risk; informed by emerging sensory feedback; modulated by
prior experiences and expectations; biased toward repeatedly
re-employing plastically embedded coordinative solutions to
current running ‘problems.’
Ultimately, running performance is underpinned by
a conglomeration of assorted capacities—cardiovascular,
neurological, psychological, physiological, anatomical, muscular,
and biomechanical. Yet it is the super-capacity of coordination
that regulates how proficiently these overlapping performance
contributors are collaboratively expressed to generate propulsive
power, promote efficiency, preserve robustness, and accomplish
running objectives for an acceptable exposure to discomfort and
risk. A deeper appreciation of the underpinnings of the running
coordination phenomenon will hopefully enable practitioners to
more judiciously design interventions to promote, nurture, and
preserve coordinative proficiency in the face of the inevitably
accumulating ‘wear and tear’ endured over the course of a
running lifetime.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and
approved it for publication.
REFERENCES
Avanzino, L., Pelosin, E., Abbruzzese, G., Bassolino, M., Pozzo, T., and Bove, M.
(2014). Shaping motor cortex plasticity through proprioception. Cereb. Cortex
24, 2807–2814. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht139
Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The Co-Ordination and Regulation of Movements. Oxford:
Pergamon Press.
Bramble, D. M., and Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and
the evolution of Homo. Nature 432, 345–352. doi: 10.1038/nature
03052
Clark, D., Schumann, F., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Mindful movement and
skilled attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:297. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00297
Clarsen, B., Myklebust, G., and Bahr, R. (2013). Development and validation
of a new method for the registration of overuse injuries in sports injury
epidemiology: the Oslo Sports Trauma Research Centre (OSTRC) overuse
injury questionnaire. Br. J. Sports Med. 47, 495–502. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-
2012-091524
Coq, J. O., and Barbe, M. F. (2011). “Peripheral and central changes combined
induce movement disorders on the basis of disuse or overuse, in Movement
Disorders: Causes, Diagnoses and Treatments, ed. B. J. Larsen (Hauppauge, NY:
Nova Science Publishers Inc), 2–14.
Davids, K., and Glazier, P. (2010). Deconstructing neurobiological coordination:
the role of the biomechanics-motor control nexus. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 38,
86–90. doi: 10.1097/JES.0b013e3181d4968b
Diedrichsen, J., Shadmehr, R., and Ivry, R. B. (2010). The coordination of
movement: optimal feedback control, and beyond. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 31–39.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004
Elbert, T., and Rockstroh, B. (2004). Reorganization of human cerebral cortex:
the range of changes following use and injury. Neuroscientist 10, 129–141.
doi: 10.1177/1073858403262111
Engineer, N. D., Engineer, C. T., Reed, A. C., Pandya, P. K., Jakkamsetti, V.,
Moucha, R., et al. (2012). Inverted-U function relating cortical plasticity
and task difficulty. Neuroscience 205, 81–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.
12.056
Gerlach, K. E., White, S. C., Burton, H. W., Dorn, J. M., Leddy, J. J., and Horvath,
P. J. (2005). Kinetic changes with fatigue and relationship to injury in female
runners. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 37, 657–663. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000158994.
29358.71
Glazier, P. S., and Davids, K. (2009). Constraints on the complete optimization
of human motion. Sports Med. 39, 15–28. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200939010-
00002
Goldberger, A. L., Amaral, L. A., Hausdorff, J. M., Ivanov, P. C., Peng, C. K.,
and Stanley, H. E. (2002). Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with
disease and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99(Suppl. 1), 2466–2472.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.012579499
Hamill, J., Palmer, C., and Van Emmerik, R. E. (2012). Coordinative variability
and overuse injury. BMC Sports Sci. Med. Rehabil. 4:45. doi: 10.1186/1758-
2555-4- 45
Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the
stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 555–589.
doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.05.003
Hodges, P. W., and Tucker, K. (2011). Moving differently in pain: a new theory
to explain the adaptation to pain. Pain 152, S90–S98. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.
10.020
Hoppeler, H., Baum, O., Lurman, G., and Mueller, M. (2011). Molecular
mechanisms of muscle plasticity with exercise. Compr. Physiol. 1, 1383–1412.
doi: 10.1002/cphy.c100042
Ivarsson, A., Johnson, U., Andersen, M. B., Tranaeus, U., Stenling, A., and
Lindwall, M. (2016). Psychosocial factors and sport injuries: meta-analyses for
prediction and prevention. Sports Med. 47, 353–365. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-
0578-x
Kelso, J. S. (2012). Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic
coordination in the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 906–918.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351
Kiely, J. (2013). The Running Machine Myth. The Running Times. Available at:
http://www.runnersworld.com/race-training/the- running-machine- myth
[accessed September 4, 2013].
Kiely, J., and Collins, D. J. (2016). Uniqueness of human running coordination:
the integration of modern and ancient evolutionary innovations. Front. Psychol.
7:262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00262
Kitano, H. (2004). Biological robustness. Nat. Rev. Genet. 5, 826–837. doi: 10.1038/
nrg1471
Kolata, G. (2007). Running Efficiency: It’s Good, but How Do You Get It? New York
Times. Available at: http://www.nytimes.com/2007/10/11/fashion/11Best.html?
pagewanted=all&_r=1& [accessed October 11, 2007].
Latash, M. L. (2012). The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance
(not redundancy). Exp. Brain Res. 217, 1–5. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-
3000-4
Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., and Schöner, G. (2007). Toward a new theory of motor
synergies. Mot. Control 11, 276–308. doi: 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276
Maleszka, R., Mason, P. H., and Barron, A. B. (2014). Epigenomics and the
concept of degeneracy in biological systems. Brief. Funct. Genomics 13, 191–202.
doi: 10.1093/bfgp/elt050
Mandelbrot, B. B. (1982). The Fractal Geometry of Nature. New York, NY: Henry
Holt and Company.
Manor, B., and Lipsitz, L. A. (2013). Physiologic complexityand aging: implications
for physical function and rehabilitation. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol.
Psychiatry 45, 287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.020
Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., and Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs
physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 857–864. doi: 10.1152/
japplphysiol.91324.2008
Mason, P. H. (2015). Degeneracy: demystifying and destigmatizing a core concept
in systems biology. Complexity 20, 12–21. doi: 10.1002/cplx.21534
Meardon, S. A., Hamill, J., and Derrick, T. R. (2011). Running injury and stride
time variability over a prolonged run. Gait Posture 33, 36–40. doi: 10.1016/j.
gaitpost.2010.09.020
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
fpsyg-08-00892 June 8, 2017 Time: 15:36 # 10
Kiely The Robust Running Ape
Merzenich, M. M., Van Vleet, T. M., and Nahum, M. (2014). Brain plasticity-based
therapeutics. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:385. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00385
Miller, R. H., Umberger, B. R., Hamill, J., and Caldwell, G. E. (2012). Evaluation
of the minimum energy hypothesis and other potential optimality criteria for
human running. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 1498–1505. doi: 10.1098/rspb.
2011.2015
Mizrahi, J., Verbitsky, O., Isakov, E., and Daily, D. (2000). Effect of fatigue on leg
kinematics and impact acceleration in long distance running. Hum. Mov. Sci.
19, 139–151. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(00)13- 10
Nagengast, A. J., Braun, D. A., and Wolpert, D. M. (2010). Risk-sensitive optimal
feedback control accounts for sensorimotor behavior under uncertainty. PLoS
Comput. Biol. 6:e1000857. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000857
Nakayama, Y., Kudo, K., and Ohtsuki, T. (2010). Variability and fluctuation
in running gait cycle of trained runners and non-runners. Gait Posture 31,
331–335. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.12.003
Newell, K. M., Liu, Y. T., and Mayer-Kress, G. (2005). Learning in the brain–
computer interface: insights about degrees of freedom and degeneracy from
a landscape model of motor learning. Cogn. Process. 6, 37–47. doi: 10.1007/
s10339-004- 0047-6
Norris, M., Anderson, R., and Kenny, I. C. (2014). Method analysis of
accelerometers and gyroscopes in running gait: a systematic review. Proc. Inst.
Mech. Eng. P J. Sports Eng. Technol. 228, 3–15.
Pelletier, R., Higgins, J., and Bourbonnais, D. (2015). Is neuroplasticity in the
central nervous system the missing link to our understanding of chronic
musculoskeletal disorders? BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 16:25. doi: 10.1186/
s12891-015- 0480-y
Proske, U., and Gandevia, S. C. (2012). The proprioceptive senses: their roles in
signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol.
Rev. 92, 1651–1697. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011
Roberts, T. J., and Azizi, E. (2011). Flexible mechanisms: the diverse roles
of biological springs in vertebrate movement. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 353–361.
doi: 10.1242/jeb.038588
Saragiotto, B. T., Yamato, T. P., Junior, L. C. H., Rainbow, M. J., Davis, I. S., and
Lopes, A. D. (2014). What are the main risk factors for running-related injuries?
Sports Med. 44, 1153–1163. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014- 0194-6
Seay, J. F., Van Emmerik, R. E., and Hamill, J. (2011). Low back pain status affects
pelvis-trunk coordination and variability during walking and running. Clin.
Biomech. 26, 572–578. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2010.11.012
Seifert, L., Button, C., and Davids, K. (2013). Key properties of expert movement
systems in sport. Sports Med. 43, 167–178. doi: 10.1007/s40279-012- 0011-z
Seifert, L., Komar, J., Araújo, D., and Davids, K. (2016). Neurobiological
degeneracy: a key property for functional adaptations of perception and action
to constraints. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.
2016.08.006
Skoyles, J. (2008). Respiratory, postural and spatio-kinetic motor stabilization,
internal models, top-down timed motor coordination and expanded cerebello-
cerebral circuitry: a review. Nat. Preced. 1–65. doi: 10.1038/npre.2008.
2092.1
Smirmaul, B. D. P. C. (2012). Sense of effort and other unpleasant sensations during
exercise: clarifying concepts and mechanisms. Br. J. Sports Med. 46, 308–311.
doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2010.071407
Stergiou, N., and Decker, L. M. (2011). Human movement variability, nonlinear
dynamics, and pathology: is there a connection? Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 869–888.
doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.06.002
Taubert,M., Draganski, B., Anwander, A., Müller, K., Horstmann, A., Villringer, A.,
et al. (2010). Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related
changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J. Neurosci. 30,
11670–11677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010
Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat. Neurosci.
7, 907–915. doi: 10.1038/nn1309
Todorov, E. (2009). Efficient computation of optimal actions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A. 106, 11478–11483. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710743106
Tucker, R. (2007). Pose Running Reduces Running Economy: The Missing
Study. Available at:http://sportss cientists.com/2007/10/pose-running-reduces-
running-economy/ [accessed October 3, 2007].
Turvey, M. T., and Fonseca, S. T. (2014). The medium of haptic perception: a
tensegrity hypothesis. J. Mot. Behav. 46, 143–187. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2013.
798252
van der Worp, M. P., Ten Haaf, D. S., van Cingel, R., de Wijer, A., Nijhuis-van
der Sanden, M. W., and Staal, J. B. (2015). Injuries in runners; a systematic
review on risk factors and sex differences. PLoS ONE 10:e0114937. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0114937
Van Orden, G. C. (2007). The fractal picture of health and wellbeing. Psychol. Sci.
Agenda 21, 1–5.
Vázquez, P., Hristovski, R., and Balagué, N. (2016). The path to exhaustion:
time-variability properties of coordinative variables during continuous exercise.
Front. Physiol. 7:37. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00037
West, B. J. (2010). Fractal physiology and the fractional calculus: a perspective.
Front. Physiol. 1:12. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2010.00012
Whitacre, J. M. (2010). Degeneracy: a link between evolvability, robustness and
complexity in biological systems. Theor. Biol. Med. Model. 7:6. doi: 10.1186/
1742-4682- 7-6
Wickham, J. B., and Brown, J. M. M. (1998). Muscles within muscles: the
neuromotor control of intra-muscular segments. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup.
Physiol. 78, 219–225. doi: 10.1007/s004210050410
Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., and Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of
sensorimotor learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn
3112
Wu, Y. H., and Latash, M. L. (2014). The effects of practice on coordination.
Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 42, 37–42. doi: 10.1249/JES.00000000000
00002
Conflict of Interest Statement: The author declares that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2017 Kiely. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or
reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor
are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance
with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 June 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 892
... On the contrary, non-linear analyses address these temporal behaviours [12]. The healthy runner is a complex, highly degenerate system with a large repertoire of behaviours to achieve similar running outcomes in a single context [12,14]. To address a part of the complexity of this system, non-linear analyses could consider the complementary notions of complexity and stability of running parameters within a given time series [12,15]. ...
... However, at these speeds we could reliably estimate CV and SD for contact time and Kleg, which was not possible at 12 km/h. Variability reflects the biomechanical adaptation of a runner to a specific condition like running speed [12,14]. We believe this could explain the differences in reliability level [12,14]. ...
... Variability reflects the biomechanical adaptation of a runner to a specific condition like running speed [12,14]. We believe this could explain the differences in reliability level [12,14]. For example, when running speed decreases, mean contact time and relative reliability level of CV and SD of contact time increase (supplementary material). ...
Article
Background Running spatio-temporal parameters (STp) and their variability allow determining the individual running style. STp are use in the field of injury, training load monitoring or rehabilitation. However, there are no recommendations about how many steps are required for their reliable assessment while this is needed to determine if a change is due to natural fluctuation or a condition like injury. Research question What is the optimal number of steps needed to measure different STp and their variability at different running speeds for recreational runners? Methods Twenty-five recreational runners performed three experimental sessions of treadmill running at 8, 10 and 12 km/h separated by 24–96h. Measured STp were stride time, step time, contact time, flight time and cadence, and duty factor and the leg stiffness index (Kleg) were then calculated. Mean, linear (coefficient of variation and standard deviation) and non-linear STp (Higuchi fractal index, α1 coefficient of detrend fluctuation analysis) were computed for different number of steps. Relative reliability was determined using intraclass coefficient correlation. The minimal number of steps who achieved good level of reliability was considered as the optimal number of steps for STp measurement (nSteps). Absolute reliability was assessed by calculating standard error measurement and minimal detectable change. Results nSteps is obtained for all mean STp measured and ranged between 16 and 150 steps. nSteps were not achieved for cadence, stride and step time variability across all speeds. Only Kleg and contact time linear variability achieved nSteps (around 350 steps). Non-linear variability STp measurement required between 350 and 540 steps. Significance Researchers and clinicians should optimise experimental conditions (steps number measured, running speed imposed) in function of STp or STp variability targeted. Future studies must use absolute reliability metrics to report significant modifications to better determine whether changes are associated with error measurements or different conditions studied (fatigue, footwear, injury, etc…).
... In addition to enhancing athletic capabilities in youth athletes, the efficacy of NMT programmes may also relate to the variability of movement patterns presented within such programmes. Accordingly, the performance of varied movement patterns reduces the persistent mechanical stress on the same soft-tissue structures through repeated exposure to SSSs, while concomitantly developing a greater breadth of FMS [29,30]. Indeed, owing to their high levels of neural plasticity-especially during pre-pubescence, youth athletes who are exposed to NMT stimuli may develop motor control more readily [28,31]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of a parkour-based warm-up to a conventional neuromuscular training (NMT) warm-up on the athletic capabilities of youth basketball players. This was examined through two arms: In Investigation 1, the aims were to measure the effects of the two warm-ups on physical measures of athletic performance in prepubescent basketball players. Using post-intervention semi-structured interviews, Investigation 2 aimed to gain insights from the players in relation to the perceived benefits of the two warm-ups. Pre-adolescent children were recruited from two youth level basketball teams. Participants from one club were randomly assigned to either a conventional NMT warm-up group or a parkour warm-up group, while a control group was formed of participants from the second club. Participants of both experimental groups were required to complete a 15-minute warm-up once per week before their regular basketball practice across 8-weeks. For both groups, the coach adopted the same pedagogical approach, utilising a guided discovery strategy. Pre-post test measures of overhead squat performance, countermovement jump, and 10-metre sprint speed were recorded in all three groups. Additionally, pre-post measures were recorded for a timed parkour-based obstacle course for the two experimental groups. No significant between-group differences were found between pre- and post-test measures. However, analysis using Cohen’s d effect sizes revealed improvements in both intervention groups versus the control. Moreover, between group effect size differences were observed between the two experimental groups. Following the intervention, participants from both experimental groups were also invited to take part in a post-intervention semi-structured interview to discuss their experiences. The thematic analysis of these semi-structured interviews revealed three higher order themes: Enjoyment; Physical literacy; and Docility; of which the two former themes appear to align to constructs relating to the wider concept of physical literacy. In summary, warm-ups designed to improve athleticism can include less structured and more diverse movement skills than are typical of conventional NMT warm-ups. Specifically, we provide evidence that advocates for warm ups that include parkour-related activities alongside conventional NMT exercises to preserve physical fitness qualities and to simultaneously evoke a sense of enjoyment, fun, and purpose. The benefit of such activities may extend beyond athletic development and, more broadly, contribute to the development of physical literacy.
... 18,19 We herkennen variabiliteit niet alleen op het niveau van bewegingsvaardigheden, maar ook elders in het menselijk lichaam, bijvoorbeeld hartslagvariabiliteit. 20,21 Daarin is een sterke variatie in de tijd tussen de opeenvolgende hartslagen een indicatie van een gezond systeem, in tegenstelling tot een minder variabele hartslag die een indicatie kan zijn dat er iets niet in orde is. 20 De ideeën over motorische variabiliteit vinden hun oorsprong in het werk van Bernstein. ...
Article
Full-text available
In het eerste deel van deze reeks hebben wij het theoretisch kader rondom Ecological Dynamics beschreven. In dit tweede deel staan we stil bij twee trainingsmethodes die zelforganisatie om tot de gunstigste individuele oplossingsruimte te komen bevorderen, namelijk differentieel leren (DL) en de constraints-led approach (CLA). Ook zetten we uiteen wat de rol is van variabiliteit in op (sport)prestaties en het ontstaan of onderhouden van blessures.
... Periodization principles were developed to improve neuromuscular or cardiopulmonary system endurance, strength or power [43,48,52]. These principles, however, do not adequately address individualized youth or adolescent athlete growth, recovery and remodeling variability, cognitive development, epiphyseal region maturation, emotional development or hormonal changes [43,47,52]. The homeostasis recovery requirements of lower metabolic rate ligaments and tendons are likewise not considered. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Sports injuries among youth and adolescent athletes are a growing concern, particularly at the knee. Based on our current understanding of microtrauma and anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) healing characteristics, this clinical commentary describes a comprehensive plan to better manage ACL microtrauma and mitigate the likelihood of progression to a non-contact macrotraumatic ACL rupture. Methods Medical literature related to non-contact ACL injuries among youth and adolescent athletes, collagen and ACL extracellular matrix metabolism, ACL microtrauma and sudden failure, and concerns related to current sports training were reviewed and synthesized into a comprehensive intervention plan. Results With consideration for biopsychosocial model health factors, proper nutrition and modified sports training with increased recovery time, a comprehensive primary ACL injury prevention plan is described for the purpose of better managing ACL microtrauma, thereby reducing the incidence of non-contact macrotraumatic ACL rupture among youth and adolescent athletes. Conclusion Preventing non-contact ACL injuries may require greater consideration for reducing accumulated ACL microtrauma. Proper nutrition including glycine-rich collagen peptides, or gelatin-vitamin C supplementation in combination with healthy sleep, and adjusted sports training periodization with increased recovery time may improve ACL extracellular matrix collagen deposition homeostasis, decreasing sudden non-contact ACL rupture incidence likelihood in youth and adolescent athletes. Successful implementation will require compliance from athletes, parents, coaches, the sports medicine healthcare team, and event organizers. Studies are needed to confirm the efficacy of these concepts. Level of evidence V
... Each individual moves differently; variability in movement is normal and is becoming increasingly recognised as an important marker of the health of movement (Hamill et al., 2012, Kiely, 2017. ...
Thesis
Changes in movement quality, specifically how people coordinate movement, have been identified in people with pain, history of pain and linked to risk of injury, changes in performance and quality of life. The health of movement is a balance between how an individual uses their body to engage with life and an ability to display choices in movement coordination strategies (MCS). The aim of this thesis is to explore the concept that assessing and retraining MCS improves the health of movement. Five core publications are included: two theoretical papers detailing the concept for assessing and retraining MCS; one reliability study establishing robustness of an assessment tool; a case report demonstrating validity and proof-of-concept of assessment and retaining of MCS; and a morphological study of the serratus anterior muscle illustrating knowledge of anatomical architecture can shape retraining strategies. The commentary includes the following topics: i) theoretical concept for assessing and restoring the health of movement (Chapter 2); ii) aspects of anatomy and neurophysiological function to support methods of assessment and retraining (Chapter 3); iii) assessment of loss of movement choices (LMC) using cognitive movement control tests to inform retraining (Chapter 4); iv) cognitive movement retraining/movement coaching, a person-centred clinical reasoning framework to design individual tailored programmes to restore LMC (Chapter 5); v) General discussion - significance, implementation and impact, illustrated over 25 years (Chapter 6). Results have demonstrated: i) good inter-rater and excellent intra-rater reliability for the assessment tool; ii) testing for LMC can inform retraining and cognitive movement retraining can change biomechanical and neurophysiological measures; and iii) novel findings of morphologically distinct subdivisions of serratus anterior. This thesis recommends the assessment of MCS to guide retraining to improve the health of movement. Theoretical concepts presented and research conducted have provided evidence for proof-of-concept and validity and reliability of assessment procedures<br/
... Motoric factors relate to structures involved during the execution process and, although there is justification for peripheral consideration (e.g., the hard-wired ability of the motor system to adjust movements under stress or fatigue; see Kiely, 2017), we focus here on centralized control structures. Specifically, we focus on representations that underlie cognitive processes. ...
... On this understanding, instead of asking patients to repeat the same goal-orientated movement to achieve some form of consistency that may be considered 'normal', practitioners should encourage individuals to perform a variety of movement competencies to challenge coordination pattern availability [44][45][46][47]. Since coordination patterns are unique to each individual an essential part of evidenced-based rehabilitation is attention towards individual patients [20,44]. ...
Article
Objective Vector coding is a non-linear data analysis technique that quantifies inter-segmental coordination and coordination variability. The traditional approach of reporting time-series data from vector coding can be problematic when overlaying multiple trials on the same illustration. The objective of this study was to describe and present novel data visualisations for displaying the coordination pattern, segmental dominancy, range of motion on an angle-angle diagram, and coordination variability. This allows for a comparison of data across multiple participants with a focus on single subject analysis. Methods Novel data visualisation techniques that involve the use of colour and data bars to map and profile coordination pattern and coordination variability data.The introduction and profiling of inter-data point range of motion quantifies range of motion of the dominant segment on an angle-angle plot and illustrates patterns of movement control. As an example, the dataset used the Istituto Ortopedico Rizzoli foot model to describe rearfoot-forefoot and shank-foot coordination during stance. Results The use of colour mapping provides the option to inspect an entire dataset and to compare data across multiple participants, groups, and segment couplings. Combining coupling angle mapping with segmental dominancy profiling offers an intuitive and instant summary on coupling angle distribution. The novel inclusion of inter-data point range of motion profiling provides meaning to the interpretation of segmental dominancy data and demonstrates distinct patterns of movement control. Conclusions The use of colour mapping and profiling techniques highlighted differences in coordination pattern and coordination variability data across several participants that questions the interpretation and relevance of reporting group data. Colour mapping and profiling techniques are ideal reporting methods to compliment prospective multiple single-subject design studies and to classify commonalities and differences in patterns of coordination and patterns of control between individuals or trials. The data visualisation approaches in the current study may provide further insight on overuse injuries, exercise prescription and rehabilitation interventions.
... • mindful movement and requires cognitive retraining • tasks are not so easy that they fail to stimulate focused attention, nor so difficult that continuous failure undermines motivation (Kiely, 2017) Progression Challenging the ability to display choice in movement is essential in a progressive programme and can be facilitated by: ...
Article
Introduction Exploring characteristics of human movement has long been the focus of clinicians and researchers. Changes in movement coordination strategies have been identified in the presence of pain highlighting the need for assessment in clinical practice. A major development in the understanding of movement related disorders is recognition of individual differences in presentation and consequently the need to tailor interventions based on assessment. Purpose The purpose of this masterclass is to build a rationale for the clinical assessment of movement coordination strategies, exploring loss of movement choices, coordination variability, and to present a clinical framework for individualised management, including the use of cognitive movement control tests and retraining interventions. An approach for the qualitative rating of movement coordination strategies is presented. A compromised movement system may be one characterised by a lack of ability to access motor abundance and display choice in the use of movement coordination strategies. The identification of lost movement choices revealed during the assessment of movement coordination strategies is proposed as a marker of movement health. Implications for practice The health of the movement system may be informed by the ability to display choice in movement coordination strategies. There is evidence that restoring these choices has clinical utility and an influence on pain and improved function. This approach seeks to provide individuals with more flexible problem solving, enabled through a movement system that is robust to each unique challenge of function. This assessment framework sits within a bigger clinical reasoning picture for sustained quality of life.
Preprint
Full-text available
Hip arthroplasty or hip replacement surgery for total osteoarthritis aims to restore mobility, not just in the replaced joint but also in reintegration into whole-body movement. While traditional biomechanical approaches provide insights into joint-specific outcomes, they often overlook broader neuromuscular adaptations emerging before and after surgery. Using network analysis, we examine how movement coordination reorganizes in unilateral total hip osteoarthritis (HOA) patients before and after total hip arthroplasty (THA) surgery. By analyzing local, global, and integrative properties of movement networks, we assess how osteoarthritis disrupts coordination and the extent of post-surgical recovery. Pre-surgery movement patterns exhibit severely compromised whole-body coordination by various network metrics: localized compensations, fragmented coordination, asymmetrical movements, altered synergies between key segments, and increased reliance on the unaffected limb. These asymmetries persist post-surgery, particularly in the contralateral limb, suggesting a potential neural reorganization of motor control compensating for pre-surgery injury that extends beyond surgery and slows full restoration of bilateral coordination. Following THA, local connectivity becomes more balanced, and whole-body integration improves, yet the persistence of elevated participation coefficients in both the affected and contralateral limbs indicates lingering motor adaptations and altered control strategies. These findings underscore the rehabilitative value of a network analytical framework that might track functional recovery, highlighting the need for whole-body reintegration and inter-limb coordination beyond isolated joint function. By quantifying the balance between local adaptations, global movement coordination, and asymmetry, this approach provides critical insights into post-surgical motor reorganization, with implications for clinical decision-making and personalized rehabilitation planning.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this scoping review was to evaluate research approaches that quantify changes to non-linear movement dynamics during running in response to fatigue, different speeds, and fitness levels. PubMed and Scopus were used to identify appropriate research articles. After the selection of eligible studies, study details and participant characteristics were extracted and tabulated to identify methodologies and findings. Twenty-seven articles were included in the final analysis. To evaluate non-linearities in the time series, a range of approaches were identified including motion capture, accelerometery, and foot switches. Common methods of analysis included measures of fractal scaling, entropy, and local dynamic stability. Conflicting findings were evident when studies examined non-linear features in fatigued states when compared to non-fatigued. More pronounced alterations to movement dynamics are evident when running speed is changed markedly. Greater fitness levels resulted in more stable and predictable running patterns. The mechanisms by which these changes are underpinned require further examination. These could include the physiological demand of running, biomechanical constraints of the runner, and the attentional demands of the task. Moreover, the practical implications are yet to be elucidated. This review has identified gaps in the literature which should be addressed for further understanding of the field.
Article
Full-text available
Background Several studies have suggested that psychosocial variables can increase the risk of becoming injured during sport participation. Objectives The main objectives of these meta-analyses were to examine (i) the effect sizes of relationships between the psychosocial variables (suggested as injury predictors in the model of stress and athletic injury) and injury rates, and (ii) the effects of psychological interventions aimed at reducing injury occurrence (prevention). Methods Electronic databases as well as specific sport and exercise psychology journals were searched. The literature review resulted in 48 published studies containing 161 effect sizes for injury prediction and seven effect sizes for injury prevention. Results The results showed that stress responses (r = 0.27, 80 % CI [0.20, 0.33]) and history of stressors (r = 0.13, 80 % CI [0.11, 0.15]) had the strongest associations with injury rates. Also, the results from the path analysis showed that the stress response mediated the relationship between history of stressors and injury rates. For injury prevention studies, all studies included (N = 7) showed decreased injury rates in the treatment groups compared to control groups. Conclusion The results support the model’s suggestion that psychosocial variables, as well as psychologically, based interventions, can influence injury risk among athletes.
Article
Full-text available
Running is a pervasive activity across human cultures and a cornerstone of contemporary health, fitness, and sporting activities. Yet for the overwhelming predominance of human existence running was an essential prerequisite for survival. A means to hunt, and a means to escape when hunted. In a very real sense humans have evolved to run. Yet curiously, perhaps due to running's cultural ubiquity and the natural ease with which we learn to run, we rarely consider the uniqueness of human bipedal running within the animal kingdom. Our unique upright, single stance, bouncing running gait imposes a unique set of coordinative difficulties. Challenges demanding we precariously balance our fragile brains in the very position where they are most vulnerable to falling injury while simultaneously retaining stability, steering direction of travel, and powering the upcoming stride: all within the abbreviated time -frames afforded by short, violent ground contacts separated by long flight times. These running coordination challenges are solved through the tightly-integrated blending of primitive evolutionary legacies, conserved from reptilian and vertebrate lineages, and comparatively modern, more exclusively human, innovations. The integrated unification of these top-down and bottom-up control processes bestows humans with an agile control system, enabling us to readily modulate speeds, change direction, negotiate varied terrains and to instantaneously adapt to changing surface conditions. The seamless integration of these evolutionary processes is facilitated by pervasive, neural and biological, activity-dependent adaptive plasticity. Over time, and with progressive exposure, this adaptive plasticity shapes neural and biological structures to best cope with regularly imposed movement challenges. This pervasive plasticity enables the gradual construction of a robust system of distributed coordinated control, comprised of processes that are so deeply collectively entwined that describing their functionality in isolation obscures their true irrevocably entangled nature. Although other species rely on a similar set of coordinated processes to run, the bouncing bipedal nature of human running presents a specific set of coordination challenges, solved using a customized blend of evolved solutions. A deeper appreciation of the foundations of the running coordination phenomenon promotes conceptual clarity, potentially informing future advances in running training and running-injury rehabilitation interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Running is a pervasive activity across human cultures and a cornerstone of contemporary health, fitness, and sporting activities. Yet for the overwhelming predominance of human existence running was an essential prerequisite for survival. A means to hunt, and a means to escape when hunted. In a very real sense humans have evolved to run. Yet curiously, perhaps due to running's cultural ubiquity and the natural ease with which we learn to run, we rarely consider the uniqueness of human bipedal running within the animal kingdom. Our unique upright, single stance, bouncing running gait imposes a unique set of coordinative difficulties. Challenges demanding we precariously balance our fragile brains in the very position where they are most vulnerable to falling injury while simultaneously retaining stability, steering direction of travel, and powering the upcoming stride: all within the abbreviated time-frames afforded by short, violent ground contacts separated by long flight times. These running coordination challenges are solved through the tightly-integrated blending of primitive evolutionary legacies, conserved from reptilian and vertebrate lineages, and comparatively modern, more exclusively human, innovations. The integrated unification of these top-down and bottom-up control processes bestows humans with an agile control system, enabling us to readily modulate speeds, change direction, negotiate varied terrains and to instantaneously adapt to changing surface conditions. The seamless integration of these evolutionary processes is facilitated by pervasive, neural and biological, activity-dependent adaptive plasticity. Over time, and with progressive exposure, this adaptive plasticity shapes neural and biological structures to best cope with regularly imposed movement challenges. This pervasive plasticity enables the gradual construction of a robust system of distributed coordinated control, comprised of processes that are so deeply collectively entwined that describing their functionality in isolation obscures their true irrevocably entangled nature. Although other species rely on a similar set of coordinated processes to run, the bouncing bipedal nature of human running presents a specific set of coordination challenges, solved using a customized blend of evolved solutions. A deeper appreciation of the foundations of the running coordination phenomenon promotes conceptual clarity, potentially informing future advances in running training and running-injury rehabilitation interventions.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to detect qualitative changes in the structure of coordinative variable (elbow angle) fluctuations during a quasi-isometric exercise performed until exhaustion. Seven physical education students performed a quasi-isometric arm-curl exercise holding an Olympic bar (weight: 80% 1RM) with an initial elbow flexion of 90° three times over a period of 4 weeks. They were encouraged to persist, even if the elbow angle was lost, until the fatigue-induced spontaneous termination point (FISTP). Changes in both elbow angles were registered during the task through an electrogoniometer. Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) was conducted on the initial and final 1024 data points of the series and the associated Hurst exponents were obtained. Multi-way RM ANOVA analyses revealed a significant main effect of the Time on task on the Hurst exponent values but also revealed a significant Trial × Time on task interaction. In the initial (non-fatigue) condition participants tended to produce anti-persistent fBm fluctuations. In the final part before exhaustion a tendency toward persistent fBm was dominant. The trial to trial differences in time-variability structure points to an existence of a long-term variability in control strategies during exercise. The changes in the temporal structure of the elbow angle variability as effort accumulated reflected an increase in low-frequency fluctuations signifying a change in psychobiological mechanisms used to negotiate the task demands. The variability properties of the coordinative variable during exercise may provide information about the dynamic mechanisms that lead to exhaustion.
Article
Full-text available
Bodily movement has long been employed as a foundation for cultivating mental skills such as attention, self-control or mindfulness, with recent studies documenting the positive impacts of mindful movement training, such as yoga and tai chi. A parallel “mind-body connection” has also been observed in many developmental disorders. We elaborate a spectrum of mindfulness by considering ADHD, in which deficient motor control correlates with impaired (disinhibited) behavioral control contributing to defining features of excessive distractibility and impulsivity. These data provide evidence for an important axis of variation for wellbeing, in which skillful cognitive control covaries with a capacity for skillful movement. We review empirical and theoretical literature on attention, cognitive control, mind wandering, mindfulness and skill learning, endorsing a model of skilled attention in which motor plans, attention, and executive goals are seen as mutually co-defining aspects of skilled behavior that are linked by reciprocal inhibitory and excitatory connections. Thus, any movement training should engage “higher-order” inhibition and selection and develop a repertoire of rehearsed procedures that coordinate goals, attention and motor plans. However, we propose that mindful movement practice may improve the functional quality of rehearsed procedures, cultivating a transferrable skill of attention. We adopt Langer’s spectrum of mindful learning that spans from “mindlessness” to engagement with the details of the present task and contrast this with the mental attitudes cultivated in standard mindfulness meditation. We particularly follow Feldenkrais’ suggestion that mindful learning of skills for organizing the body in movement might transfer to other forms of mental activity. The results of mindful movement training should be observed in multiple complementary measures, and may have tremendous potential benefit for individuals with ADHD and other populations.
Article
Full-text available
The popularity of running continues to increase, which means that the incidence of running-related injuries will probably also continue to increase. Little is known about risk factors for running injuries and whether they are sex-specific. The aim of this study was to review information about risk factors and sex-specific differences for running-induced injuries in adults. The databases PubMed, EMBASE, CINAHL and Psych-INFO were searched for relevant articles. Longitudinal cohort studies with a minimal follow-up of 1 month that investigated the association between risk factors (personal factors, running/training factors and/or health and lifestyle factors) and the occurrence of lower limb injuries in runners were included. Two reviewers' independently selected relevant articles from those identified by the systematic search and assessed the risk of bias of the included studies. The strength of the evidence was determined using a best-evidence rating system. Sex differences in risk were determined by calculating the sex ratio for risk factors (the risk factor for women divided by the risk factor for men). Of 400 articles retrieved, 15 longitudinal studies were included, of which 11 were considered high-quality studies and 4 moderate-quality studies. Overall, women were at lower risk than men for sustaining running-related injuries. Strong and moderate evidence was found that a history of previous injury and of having used orthotics/inserts was associated with an increased risk of running injuries. Age, previous sports activity, running on a concrete surface, participating in a marathon, weekly running distance (30-39 miles) and wearing running shoes for 4 to 6 months were associated with a greater risk of injury in women than in men. A history of previous injuries, having a running experience of 0-2 years, restarting running, weekly running distance (20-29 miles) and having a running distance of more than 40 miles per week were associated with a greater risk of running-related injury in men than in women. Previous injury and use of orthotic/inserts are risk factors for running injuries. There appeared to be differences in the risk profile of men and women, but as few studies presented results for men and women separately, the results should be interpreted with caution. Further research should attempt to minimize methodological bias by paying attention to recall bias for running injuries, follow-up time, and the participation rate of the identified target group.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Musculoskeletal rehabilitative care and research have traditionally been guided by a structural pathology paradigm and directed their resources towards the structural, functional, and biological abnormalities located locally within the musculoskeletal system to understand and treat Musculoskeletal Disorders (MSD). However the structural pathology model does not adequately explain many of the clinical and experimental findings in subjects with chronic MSD and, more importantly, treatment guided by this paradigm fails to effectively treat many of these conditions. Discussion: Increasing evidence reveals structural and functional changes within the Central Nervous System (CNS) of people with chronic MSD that appear to play a prominent role in the pathophysiology of these disorders. These neuroplastic changes are reflective of adaptive neurophysiological processes occurring as the result of altered afferent stimuli including nociceptive and neuropathic transmission to spinal, subcortical and cortical areas with MSD that are initially beneficial but may persist in a chronic state, may be part and parcel in the pathophysiology of the condition and the development and maintenance of chronic signs and symptoms. Neuroplastic changes within different areas of the CNS may help to explain the transition from acute to chronic conditions, sensory-motor findings, perceptual disturbances, why some individuals continue to experience pain when no structural cause can be discerned, and why some fail to respond to conservative interventions in subjects with chronic MSD. We argue that a change in paradigm is necessary that integrates CNS changes associated with chronic MSD and that these findings are highly relevant for the design and implementation of rehabilitative interventions for this population. Recent findings suggest that a change in model and approach is required in the rehabilitation of chronic MSD that integrate the findings of neuroplastic changes across the CNS and are targeted by rehabilitative interventions. Effects of current interventions may be mediated through peripheral and central changes but may not specifically address all underlying neuroplastic changes in the CNS potentially associated with chronic MSD. Novel approaches to address these neuroplastic changes show promise and require further investigation to improve efficacy of currents approaches.
Article
A crucial aspect of understanding human behavior relates to how perception and action sub-systems are integrated during coordinated and controlled movement in goal-directed activity. Here we discuss how a neurobiological system property, degeneracy (i.e., many coordinative structures to achieve one function), can help us understand how skilled individuals functionally adapt perception and action to interacting constraints during performance. Since most research investigating degeneracy has been conducted in neuroanatomy, genetics and theoretical neurobiology, here we clarify how degeneracy is exhibited in perceptual-motor systems. Using an ecological dynamics framework, we highlight how degeneracy underpins the functional role of movement coordination variability in performance of multi-articular tasks. Following that, we discuss how degenerate neurobiological systems are able to exploit system stability and flexibility in their movement coordination. Third, we show how better coupling of information and movement could lead individuals to explore functionally degenerate behaviors. Last, we explore how degeneracy can support pluri-potentiality (i.e., one coordinative structure for many perceptual-motor functions) as a way toward innovation or refinement in performance.