ArticlePDF Available

Defining Borderline Personality Disorder Impulsivity: Review of Neuropsychological Data and Challenges that Face Researchers

Authors:
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 154
Journal of Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Disorders
Volume 1, Issue 3 Review Article
Defining Borderline Personality Disorder Impulsivity: Review
of Neuropsychological Data and Challenges that Face
Researchers
Jean Gagnon1,2,3
1Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Quebec, Canada
2Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation of Greater Montreal, Canada
3Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognition (CERNEC), Canada
*Corresponding Author: Jean Gagnon, Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Centre-ville station,
Montreal, Quebec, Canada, Fax: 514 343-2285; E-mail : jean.gagnon@umontreal.ca
Received: 31 May 2017; Accepted: 05 June 2017; Published: 12 June 2017
Abstract
It has been pointed out that the definition of BPD impulsivity would be improved by incorporating neurobehavioral
models in order to bridge the research and the DSM behavioral criterion. Moeller et al. [1] have proposed three
neuropsychological diagnostic criterions related to impulsivity in psychiatric disorders: (1) rapid, unplanned
reactions to stimuli before complete processing of information; (2) lack of regard for long-term consequences and;
(3) decreased sensitivity to negative consequences of behavior. The goal of this paper was to review the
neuropsychological literature of BPD impulsivity in line with these neuropsychological diagnostic criterions to
verify if the evidence from neuropsychological data and measurements is sufficiently strong to be integrated into the
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 155
DSM definition of BPD impulsivity. Results of the review highlight some evidence regarding neuropsychological
deficits in BPD patients that may be underlying their impulsive self-damaging behaviors. However, at least five
methodological challenges are pointed out and need to be addressed before these deficits can be successfully
integrated into a definition of BPD impulsivity. Some solutions are proposed to face the main challenges in studying
impulsivity in BPD.
Keywords: Borderline personality disorder; Impulsivity; Neuropsychology; Inhibition; Delay discounting;
Decision making
1. Introduction
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a complex disorder characterized by several symptoms. But these patients
are particularly known for their wide range of impulsive and risky behaviors which are a target priority in cognitive
behavioral therapy in order to protect the patient as well as to ensure the continuation of therapy [2]. Impulsivity is
one of the main factors underlying the symptomatology of the disorder and is considered as a key component of
neurobehavioral models of BPD [3]. Moreover, the rate of prevalence of impulsive behaviors is very high in BPD,
as can be seen in a study showing that the prevalence of various impulsive actions ranges from 43% to 99% [4].
Longitudinal studies have shown that impulsivity predicts BPD psychopathology at follow-up [5] and some
impulsive patterns such as self-mutilation have a quick declining rate over time but some others such as substance
abuse or excessive spending seem to be relatively stable over a period of 7 years [6].
Neuropsychology is a promising approach to develop diagnostic measures and etiological models of BPD
impulsivity. In fact, it has been more than 25 years that neuropsychology has focused its attention on borderline
personality disorder with the aim of describing its cognitive dysfunctions. Over the years, neuropsychological
approach and methods evolve rapidly and neuropsychological deficits in several functional domains have been
identified in BPD. Consequently, BPD self-damaging behaviors can be seen as the reflection of underlying cognitive
impairments, notably deficits of inhibition. Moreover, several authors [1, 7, 8] have pointed out that the definition of
BPD impulsivity should incorporate neurobehavioral models in order to bridge the research and the DSM. The goal
of this paper is to review the neuropsychological literature of BPD impulsivity in order to verify if the evidence from
neuropsychological data and measurements is sufficiently strong to be integrated into the DSM definition of BPD
impulsivity and to propose solutions to the main challenges that face researchers in studying impulsivity in BPD.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 156
2. Defining Borderline Impulsivity
The DSM-IV [9] and DSM-5 [10] define borderline impulsivity as potentially self-damaging behaviors in at least
two areas such as spending, sex, substance abuse, reckless driving or binge eating. This definition is strictly based
on the expression of behavior regardless of the mechanisms explaining why a person acts impulsively. According to
several studies [11], one of the mechanisms that is likely to explain borderline impulsivity is a lack of inhibition. If
we choose to define impulsivity as a disinhibition of mood and behavior, four other criteria could be related to
impulsivity that is unstable interpersonal relationships, recurrent suicidal or self-mutilating behavior, affective
instability and inappropriate anger and problems in controlling it. The opposition between the DSM behavioral
criterion and a lack of inhibition to define impulsivity illustrates two very different ways of defining BPD
impulsivity. The first one is based on the negative outcome of the action, whereas the second one is based on a
putative underlying mechanism.
The DSM behavioral criterion to define BPD impulsivity has a number of limitations. Although behaviors such as
alcohol abuse or excessive spending may occur impulsively on some occasions, individuals may also engage in
these behaviors in a nonimpulsive manner on other occasions [12]. Moreover, even when the behavior is engaged in
an impulsive manner, it could be the product of a predisposition or the result of an emotional or intoxication state
[13]. Also, the temporal instability of impulsive-like behaviors that has been described in longitudinal studies can
hinder a more temporal stability of their underlying psychological processes [14]. Indeed, symptomatic behaviors
can reflect attempts at adapting to or coping with pathological impulsivity trait. Otherwise, important aspects of
impulsivity such as lack of planning or perseverance are not captured in a pure behavioral approach [1]. Finally, the
DSM definition of impulsivity can benefit from various findings of biological and psychological studies to
determine who is impulsive, why he is impulsive and how to treat his impulsivity [15].
Moeller and collaborators [1] have proposed a definition that can be used to bridge the research and the DSM:
Predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli without regard to the negative
consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individuals or to others. The key features of the definition are
predisposition which refer to a pattern of behavior rather than a single act; rapid unplanned reactions refer to actions
that occur before the opportunity to consciously weigh the consequences of an act and this feature separates
impulsivity from impaired judgment or compulsive behaviors in which planning occurs before behavior; and finally
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 157
without regard to the consequences of these actions refers to decision-making style of impulsive individuals and to
the social aspect of impulsivity. According to the authors, a multidimensional approach combining self-report
measures, behavioral laboratory measures and event-related potentials would permit to distinguish impulsive
patients from non-impulsive patients, and identity psychological mechanisms underlying impulsive behaviors in
order to guide treatment. In an effort to incorporate biological and behavioral models into a definition of impulsivity,
these authors have proposed to include three neuropsychological diagnostic criterions: (1) rapid, unplanned reactions
to stimuli before complete processing of information; (2) lack of regard for long-term consequences and; (3)
decreased sensitivity to negative consequences of behavior.
These three criterions are based on finding from neuropsychological tasks measuring inhibition functions, delay
discounting and decision making. The next section will review the performance of BPD patients on these tasks
related to impulsivity.
3. Inhibition-related Functions
Even tough inhibition is an old concept introduced in psychology and physiology in the beginning of the 19th
century [16], the interest in the concept of inhibition in the explanation of impulsivity-related disorders is relatively
recent and has increased during the past 20 years with the neurosciences [17]. At the behavioral level, disinhibition
refers to an ongoing pattern of failing to inhibit, or continuing to engage in, certain appetitive behaviors that have
previously led to aversive consequences [18]. For example, the individual with alcohol dependence continues to
drink to excess despite repeatedly experiencing such negative outcomes as loss of friends, spouse, health, and job.
At the cognitive level, it refers to any mechanism that reduces or dampens neuronal, mental, or behavioral activity
[19]. A number of tasks have been developed and used to tap these processes. As it is the case with impulsivity,
inhibition is conceptualized as a multidimensional construct rather than a single concept. With the use of statistical
techniques, it has been possible to extract the common variance among multiple tasks and identity as least three
different dimensions of inhibition [17]: (1) prepotent response inhibition; (2) resistance to distractor interference; (3)
resistance to proactive interference.
3.1 Prepotent response inhibition
Prepotent response inhibition is the first and most investigated inhibition-related function in BPD. It refers to the
ability to deliberately withhold or suppress dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses [20]. The two most common
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 158
paradigms to assess these processes are the Go/no-go task [21] and the Stop-signal Task [22]. Both tasks are
composed of Go trials during which the participant is requested to answer. The Go/no-go task adds No-go trials
during which a distinct stimulus from the Go is presented. Here, the participant is requested to not answer, to
withhold his response. An inhibition failure happens when the participant answers during a No-go trail, which is
called error of commission. The Stop-signal task consists of Go and No-go trials. During No-go trials, first a Go
stimulus is presented, followed by a stop-signal after a certain delay. Even though a stimulus Go is presented, the
Stop-signal requires a response suppression. An inhibition failure happens when the participant answers during a
Stop-signal trial. Inhibition is an important executive function in our daily life when we have to withhold an
inappropriate action or when there is a change in the environment and an ongoing action becomes suddenly
inappropriate.
In the first study on prepotent response inhibition in BPD by Nigg and collaborators [11], the results obtained in a
group composed of BPD and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) patients showed that BPD
symptoms were correlated with poor performance on several executive functions tasks. Also, the poor response
inhibition as measured by the Stop-signal task was specific to BPD symptoms even when other personality disorder
symptoms were controlled for. Moreover, poor response inhibition was a unique predictor of BPD symptoms when
other executive functions problems were controlled. However, when ADHD symptoms were entered as a predictor
in the statistical model, response inhibition ceased to be significantly associated with BPD symptoms, suggesting
that BPD and ADHD symptoms probably overlap in the explanation of response inhibition deficit.
Studies that followed shown that BPD patients made more errors of commission, revealing problems to inhibit a
prepotent response, and that poor performance was inconsistently correlated with a trait impulsivity questionnaire
across studies [23, 24]. Recent studies have called upon event-related potential technique to investigate response
inhibition in BPD. In an event-related potential study [25] where the participants performed a Go/no-go task, the
authors focused on the Nogo-P3, which is a positive waveform with a peak latency around 300 ms that can be
observed after a No-go stimulus is presented. The Nogo-P3 is related to response inhibition. BPD patients showed
reduced Nogo-P3 amplitudes compared to controls, and this event-related potential component was negatively
correlated with the score on a trait impulsivity questionnaire for the entire group of participants.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 159
Three remarks can be made about response inhibition in BPD. First, BPD patients usually scored higher than
controls on trait impulsivity questionnaires [26-32]. In contrast, they often show similar performances as controls on
inhibition response tasks [33, 34]. Secondly, most studies did not compare BPD patients with ADHD patients and
the ones that did so had obtained negative results [35, 36]. Finally, event-related potential measures proved to be
useful to complement behavioral measures by showing positive results where such results were impossible to obtain
with behavioral measures alone [25].
3.2 Resistance to distractor interference
The second inhibition-related function is resistance to distractor interference. This function refers to the ability to
resist or resolve interference from information in the external environment that is irrelevant to the task at hand [11,
17, 37]. It has been shown that this inhibition-related function is closely related to the prepotent response inhibition
[17]. Indeed, both types require focusing on the task goal in order not to get distracted by external stimuli and not to
initiate an automatic response that is inappropriate in the situation. For example, while driving, one has to focus on
the road and the directions and not to be distracted by the cell phone and not miss a red light. This inhibition-related
function is commonly measured by the Stroop task.
In the Stroop task [38], there are at least two conditions. In the first condition, the neutral condition, examinees name
the ink color of a series of rectangles arranged in rows and columns. In the second condition, the interference
condition, examinees must name the ink color and ignore the content of the word, which is the name of another
color. This condition is more demanding because it requires the inhibition of the strong tendency to read the word.
The increased reaction time in the interference condition compared to the neutral condition is called the interference
effect. Resistance to distractor interference does not seem to be problematic in BPD as three studies failed to show
group differences between BPD patients and controls on the interference effect [34, 36, 39].
3.3 Resistance to proactive interference
The third inhibition-related function is resistance to proactive interference. This function refers to the ability to resist
memory intrusions from information that was previously relevant to the task but has since become irrelevant [11,
17]. Friedman and Miyake [17] have shown that this inhibition-related function is not related to either prepotent
response inhibition or resistance to distractor interference. Among the existing tasks to assess resistance to proactive
interference, is the Directed forgetting task.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 160
The Directed forgetting task [40] requires the participants to view words, some of which they are instructed to
remember and some they are instructed to forget. They are then tested on both the to-be-remembered words, the R-
cues, and the to-be-forgotten words, the F-cues. Among healthy controls, the directed forgetting effect refers to the
fact that participants tend to remember words they were instructed to remember and forget words they were
instructed to forget.
Korfine and Hooley [41] called upon this paradigm to investigate memory bias in BPD patients, and used three types
of words: neutrally valenced, positively valenced and borderline-related words. Borderline-related words were
words presumably salient to individuals with BPD, and included themes of abandonment, rejection, anger and rage,
self-harm, and others being uncaring. It was expected that BPD patients would show a memory bias for borderline
words. Results indicated that BPD patients recalled significantly more of the borderline words from the forget
condition than did controls, suggesting the continuation of encoding the words even after they saw the forget
instruction. This continuation to encode could reflect a deficit to inhibit proactive interference in working memory.
Moreover, the number of recalled words in the forget condition were correlated with BPD symptomatology.
According to the authors, cognitive distortions and intrusion of affect into information processing that are observed
in BPD patients might be related to a difficulty to inhibit negative thoughts about social rejection for example. This
finding of an enhanced recall of negative information in BPD participants, despite the instruction to forget it, has
been replicated by another study [27].
3.4 Effect of emotion on inhibition in BPD
Given that self-destructive behaviors usually happen in the context of negative affect [42] and that affective
dysregulation is, with impulsivity, a central symptom in BPD [43], recent studies investigated the interaction
between emotion and inhibition-related functions. More specifically, the researchers were interested to know if
inhibition capacities of BPD patients were diminished in an emotional condition compared to a neutral condition.
The assumption behind this question was that inhibition of emotion would be one of the main mechanisms in
affective regulation, and that it enables us to regulate emotions and express them at the moment and in a manner that
we want to [44]. Silbersweig and collaborators [45] called upon the fMRI technique to answer this question. The
authors have investigated the functioning of prefrontal control mechanisms in the setting of negative emotional
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 161
states. They developed an emotional Go/no-go task with verbal stimuli containing themes salient for individuals
with BPD and for which the affective valence could be positive, negative or neutral. Under conditions of behavioral
inhibition in the context of negative emotion, BPD patients, compared with controls, made more errors of
commission, suggesting a deficit in response inhibition. Also, in the same conditions, BPD patients showed
decreased ventromedial prefrontal activity and increased activity of the amygdala and associated structures.
Moreover, these neurodynamic changes were correlated with a questionnaire measuring negative emotional and lack
of constraint temperamental traits. According to the authors, the results suggest a reciprocal functional relationship
between a ventromedial prefrontal cortex playing a top-down inhibitory role, and an amygdala and associated
structures playing a bottom-up emotional interfering role.
Alternatively, researchers made use of the Emotional Stroop task to investigate the effect of emotion on resistance to
distractor interference [46]. In this version of the test, the participants must name the color of the stimuli as they do
in the standard version, but this time the words have a positive, negative or neutral valence. Also, the words can be
personally relevant or not relevant to the participants. It is assumed that the interference effect on the emotional
Stroop task is related to a difficulty to inhibit emotional words, resulting in attentional bias. Studies that have
manipulated the valence of stimuli only, for example comparing neutral versus negative stimuli, did not find group
differences between BPD patients and controls on the interference effect [27, 47]. In contrast, studies that have
manipulated not only the valence of the words but also their relevance for the participants obtained positive results
more often. For example, Arntz and collaborators [48] found that BPD patients had difficulty to inhibit interference
from words associated with BPD danger signals, for example rejection from others, childhood traumas and negative
self-views as being bad or helpless. However, this effect was not specific to BPD patients as cluster C patients did
show the same difficulty. In Sieswerda and collaborators’ study [49], it was possible to obtain an emotional Stroop
effect specific to BPD patients compared to cluster C patients. In this study, schema-related negative words were
used. Finally, a last study [47] showed that group differences between BPD patients and controls were found but
only for words related to personal stressful situations with current relevance for the participants in comparison to
words with less current importance. However, in this last study, only BPD patients with post-traumatic stress
disorder showed reduced inhibitory functioning compared to BPD without post-traumatic stress disorder.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 162
Other studies investigated the effect of stress or emotion on state of impulsivity. State of impulsivity refers to the
tendency to act impulsively in certain specific situations but not all the time [13]. For example, state-dependant
impulsivity in individuals with BPD may be related to emotional states. State of impulsivity can be measured by
questionnaire with items such as “I am in the sort of mood in which I can say whatever comes into my head”. Two
studies have shown that BPD patients with or without ADHD reported that their state of impulsivity was increased
after an experimental stress induction [35, 50]. In a series of two studies with non-clinical participants, Chapman
and collaborators [51, 52] were interested in verifying if a negative emotional state would increase impulsive
responses in a passive avoidance learning task. In a passive avoidance learning task, the participants must learn to
inhibit a response that was previously punished, for example with the withdrawal of money. In the first study [51],
the current emotional state was assessed by a questionnaire whereas, in the second study [52], the emotional state
was manipulated by a fear induction. The fear induction consisted of watching a horror movie’s extract. Contrasting
results were obtained between the two studies. In the first one, participants who were high in BPD features and were
in a current negative emotional state committed fewer errors of inhibition than individuals high in BPD features but
who were not in a negative emotional state. In the second study using a mood induction, participants who were high
in BPD features committed a greater number of impulsive responses than low-BPD participants and these group
differences were observed in the fear condition but not in the neutral condition. According to the authors, the results
of those two studies suggest that incidental emotional states that accompany a situation are not strong enough to
impact on impulsivity. In contrast, an acute emotional stressor may result in disruptions in behavioral control.
According to the authors, impulsivity among persons with BPD features my not be a trait-like tendency but depends
largely on emotional context.
The previous studies have shown that deficits of inhibition in BPD patients would be partially attributed to their
negative emotional state. Some researchers interpret this as the result of a reciprocal influence between the
inhibitory capacity and the intensity of emotion [45]. Other researchers question the very existence of trait
impulsivity in BPD and would rather put forward the idea that impulsive behaviors of BPD patients are the result of
a state of impulsivity or a by-product of their affective dysregulation [3]. Independently of the interpretation, two
critiques can be said about studies that made use of emotional and personal stimuli to study the effect of emotion in
inhibition. Firstly, there is a methodological bias in the selection of stimuli. Indeed, the emotionally relevant words
used in some studies were based on schemas that are relevant to the BPD group only, and they were not based on
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 163
schemas that could be relevant to Cluster C patients or even control participants. Consequently, it is possible that
group differences found in those studies would disappear if the words were chosen in respect with schemas of each
group. This would increase the probability of getting a stronger emotional Stroop effect across all participants. The
second limitation relates to the fact that most of those studies did not investigate the relationship between the
interference effect and a measurement of impulsivity or BPD symptoms, and the only study that had included such a
measurement did not find a significant correlation [27].
4. Delay Discounting
The performance tasks used in neuropsychological studies to assess inhibition-related functions formed one group of
behavioral paradigms of impulse control. There exists a second group of impulse control paradigms focusing on
more complex behavioral patterns, such as tasks assessing delay discounting. The delay discounting principle refers
to the fact that the value of a reward is discounted as a function of the delay before receiving it. According to this
principle, impulsive individuals prefer smaller immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards. In return, they prefer
delayed larger negative consequences over immediate smaller negative consequences. In BPD patients, this
preference could explain why they choose to engage in self-harm behaviors because of the immediate benefits of
temporary emotional distraction and relief from intense negative affect, despite potential long-term negative
consequences. The motivational impulse control is commonly assessed with the Delay discounting task.
In the Delay Discounting task, the participant is given two choices. In the delayed choice, there is a fixed larger
reward that can be received after a certain delay, for example 1000 $ in one month. In the immediate choice, the
amount of the reward is manipulated over successive trials, for example going up from 600 $ to 900 $. The goal of
the task is to estimate the value of a delayed reward which is called the indifference point where there is a switch
from a larger later reward to a smaller sooner reward. In other words, the immediate reward generates the same
value as the delayed reward.
In a first study [53], the participants were given a choice between a smaller immediate real monetary reward, which
was 5 cents, or a larger but progressively delayed reward, which was 15 cents. During the delayed choice, the
participants must actually wait for a maximum delay of 2 minutes. Results showed that BPD patients did not select
the short-delay more frequently than the controls, but their averaged longest delay before reward dropped by 50%
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 164
from the first to the second session of the task. Controls endured identical maximum delays of reinforcement across
the two sessions. This pattern of results reflected a change in the BPD patients’ strategy to avoid long delays in
large-reward reinforcement in the second session of the task. Indeed, the BPD subjects tended to alternate between
the short and long-delayed response options. However, the interpretation of the results remains ambiguous as the
change in strategy may not indicate an impulsive style in choice preferences but may simply reflect the fact that
BPD patients were less tolerant than controls toward the experimental condition where they had to wait for more
than a minute in an isolated booth. The absence of correlation between performance on the task and a trait
impulsivity questionnaire is consistent with this interpretation.
In a standard delay discounting task study [54], it was possible to confirm a preference for immediate gratification
and discounting for delayed rewards. Lawrence and collaborators administered the task before and after a mood
induction with the intention to verify if a change in mood would exacerbate the preference for an immediate reward.
The results indicated that the BPD patients had a greater preference for immediate gratification and a higher rate of
discounting the delayed reward than the controls. Also, the rate of discounting did not change for the BPD group
after mood induction whereas there was a change for controls, indicating less discounting of the delayed rewards
following mood induction. In other words, controls valued the delayed reward more after their mood change,
suggesting a shift toward greater self-control. For BPD patients, even though they experienced a mood change as the
controls did, this did not alter their reward-based decision-making, suggesting a failure to reduce impulsivity after
rejection. Finally, the rate of discounting was correlated with non-planning impulsivity trait in BPD group,
suggesting that the tendency to be present-oriented with limited regard for future planning leads to an immediate
gratification decision-making style.
5. Decision Making
According to the somatic marker hypothesis [55], decision making is a process that depends both on conscious and
unconscious processes, and it is influenced by our emotions. In fact, when facing choices or situations when the
future is uncertain, our emotions through somatic signals help us to make advantageous decisions by telling us that
some behaviors will be associated with positive consequences whereas other behaviors will be associated with
negative consequences. Several impulsive behaviors exhibited by BPD patients such as self-destructiveness, self-
harm behaviors and suicidal attempts are consistent with difficulties in emotionally guided decision-making. The
most known paradigm to assess these processes is the Iowa Gambling task.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 165
The Iowa Gambling task is designed to study the integration of emotion and cognition in decision processes [56]. It
simulates real-life decision making with uncertainty concerning premises and outcomes as well as rewards and
punishments. The participants are given play money and have to make a series of card selections out of four decks,
with the goal of maximizing profit. Immediately after every choice, the participants receive a financial reward,
although in some cases they also receive a financial punishment. Two of the decks, A and B, are considered
disadvantageous and result in immediate large rewards, but also result in higher punishments at unpredictable points.
The other two decks, C and D, are considered advantageous and result in immediate modest rewards, but lower
punishments as well. In the long run, choosing from the advantageous decks would result in a net gain, while
choosing from the disadvantageous decks would result in a net loss. The score is defined as the number of choices
from the advantageous decks minus the number of choices from the disadvantageous decks over 100 trials. Through
somatic markers, normal subjects are capable of anticipating the consequences associated with each deck and end up
by making the right decision. In contrast, patients with a damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex show a
persistence of disadvantageous choices, a deficit interpreted as the consequence of insensitivity to future
consequences, positive or negative [57].
Several studies have shown that BPD patients showed less advantageous choices on the Iowa Gambling task than
did the healthy comparison subjects [39, 58]. For example, BPD patients without a substance abuse disorder made
fewer advantageous choices than controls [39]. However, BPD with a substance abuse disorder made fewer
advantageous choices than BPD without such a disorder, a result that is not surprising given that reduced
performance has been found in patients with substance dependency [59]. However, other studies showed that BPD
patients have normal performance in the Iowa Gambling task [60, 61].
In order to better understand the reason why BPD patients make disadvantageous choices, some studies used
modified versions of the Gambling task to investigate the cognitive processes underlying their poor performance, for
example, the feedback processing. The Iowa Gambling task simulates real-life situations in which the outcome of a
decision is unpredictable. However, Svaldi and collaborators [62] reasoned that, in everyday life, individuals with
BPD are most often explicitly aware of the deleterious long-term consequences of their impulsive behaviors. So they
used a modified version of the Gambling task called the Game of Dice Task in which there are explicit rules for
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 166
gains and losses and where winning probabilities are obvious, stable and predictable. The results showed that BPD
patients still displayed significantly more risky decision-making behaviors and these deficits in decision-making
were associated with BPD symptom severity. Moreover, BPD patients continued to make risky decisions following
negative feedback after a risky choice. They also continued to select advantageous choices less often after positive
feedback. These results suggest a difficulty to process the feedback information. Another study [63] showed that
BPD patients exhibited altered processing of information about potential losses but only when the probability of
gains was simultaneously high. This result suggests a failure to properly process negative consequences in
circumstances when there is a high probability of reward. For example, BPD patients would not process the
probability of getting hurt after an impulsive action when they expect that this behavior will result in the reduction
of intense negative affect. Finally, event-related potentials studies have identified three components in decision-
making: (1) error-related negativity or ERN, a waveform that arises following the execution of erroneous responses
[64]; (2) feedback-related negativity or FRN, elicited by negative performance feedback when outcomes are worse
than expected [65] and (3) the feedback-related P300 associated with the significance or the value attributed to the
feedback [66]. Several studies have shown that BPD patients, in contrast with controls, exhibited smaller ERN and
FRN amplitudes, suggesting that BPD patients would exhibit a reduction in the monitoring of their actions and do
not learn from their errors [67-70]. Also, these components were negatively correlated with a trait impulsivity
questionnaire. Alternatively, the P300 amplitudes following a negative feedback were increased in BPD patients,
suggesting that they would attribute more meaning to negative feedback information than positive feedback [69].
To summarize delay discounting and decision making data in BPD, there is clear evidence that BPD patients exhibit
deficits in impulse control when motivational and decisional processes are involved. These deficits seem associated
with their impulsivity trait and their symptoms. These deficits might be associated with a failure to properly process
feedback information and to monitor action. However many questions remain open to more investigation such as the
role played by a comorbidity with a substance abuse disorder in the rate of delay discounting in BPD patients, and
the influence of the reinforcement valence in their poor decision-making.
6. Challenges that Need to be Addressed
The critical review of neuropsychological studies highlighted some evidence regarding neuropsychological deficits
in BPD patients that may be underlying their impulsive self-damaging behaviors. These deficits relate to prepotent
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 167
response inhibition and resistance to proactive interference in the context of emotion, a preference for immediate
gratification and discounting for delayed rewards, and a failure to properly process feedback information and to
monitor action in decision making. However, at least five methodological challenges have to be resolved before
these deficits can be integrated into a definition of BPD impulsivity.
The first challenge relates to the heterogeneous findings on neuropsychological deficits found among BPD patients.
The high proportion of BPD patients with a neurological history suggested to some researchers the existence of a
subgroup of BPD patients [32, 71]. Also, various degrees of severity and areas of dysfunction could simply reflect
the normal variation in cognitive functioning between individuals or the co-occurrence of other mental disorders
[72]. However, it is possible that one of the most problematic sources of variation could origin in the diagnostic
criteria of BPD itself. Indeed, as a result of the polythetic diagnosis system, 151 possible diagnostic presentations
can be found among BPD patients [73]. As recommended by Paris [74], one possible solution to the complex nature
of BPD would be to diagnose it with a narrower set of criteria that cover all of its domains: affective instability,
impulsivity, unstable relationships and cognitive defects. In keeping with this recommendation, future
neuropsychological research should call upon diagnostic instruments in which the BPD diagnosis requires symptoms
among all of these domains, such as the Revised Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines [75]. Efforts toward a more
homogeneous diagnostic profile seem to be a prerequisite to all future neuropsychological studies.
A second challenge relates to the clinical relevance of neuropsychological performance found among BPD.
Although studies found group differences, cognitive impairments associated with BPD are described as subtle [72].
Moreover, the individual test scores of BPD patients are generally in the average range [72]. Thus, even though
deficits stand out in statistical group differences and that the performances of BPD patients are correlated with their
symptoms, a question remains: Are they substantial enough to be seen as deficits that justify a neuropsychological
intervention? To answer this question, single case studies describing patients with different levels of performance
could be useful to draw particular profile in terms of cognitive impairment.
A third challenge relates to the causal link between neuropsychological deficits and BPD symptoms, including
behavioral impulsivity. Indeed, studies that have shown relationships between cognitive deficits and BPD symptoms
were correlational. Thus, longitudinal studies are needed to make progress on the issue. Furthermore, longitudinal
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 168
studies would benefit from using the event-related potential technique in order to obtain objective data on cognitive
deficits among BPD patients and verify if they can predict the emergence of symptoms over time.
How to handle comorbidity with substance abuse disorder and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder is
certainly another challenge to researchers. Regarding the first comorbidity, it seems difficult to believe that the
exclusion of BPD patients with a substance abuse disorder in research sampling could result in representative
samples of BPD patients. This is because substance abuse may represent a maladaptive strategy to regulate intense
negative affect in BPD [7]. Also, there exists an overlap between BPD and substance abuse disorder because the
diagnostic criterion for impulsivity in BPD can be met with impulsive substance abuse. Moreover, levels of
impulsivity and negative affect may enter into a reciprocal relationship during the development of one disorder and
may contribute to the development of the second disorder. Alternatively, if BPD patients with a substance abuse
disorder are included in samples, it would be difficult to disentangle the respective effect of each disorder on
impulsivity. One possible solution would be to determine if the substance abuse is primary or secondary to BPD
[76]. For example, in contrast with substance abuse disorder in which behaviors are caused by substance-seeking,
the substance use by an individual with BPD can be a craving to soothe himself or herself in the context of
dysphoric feeling states. Also, contrary to substance abuse disorder, the type of substance is not very important
among BPD patients and their abuse tends to be episodic and impulsive. The use of an instrument such as the
Revised Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines [75] can help to establish the differential diagnosis. Regarding the
ADHD, given that almost 50% of adult BPD patients report childhood ADHD [77, 78] and that childhood ADHD
symptoms predict higher rates of BPD during young adulthood [79, 80], it seems appropriate to systematically
include a subgroup of BPD patients with ADHD and a subgroup of BPD patients without ADHD in future
neuropsychological studies. This is consistent with the fact that some researchers argue that BPD might be
subdivided in two subgroups according to the presence or absence of comorbid ADHD [81].
One last challenge would be to better understand the role of emotional, situational and personality variables in
impulse control deficits in BPD. Indeed, deficits of inhibition are modulated by emotional states or the current
relevance of negative stimuli. Given that inhibition processes are composed of several functions, future studies
should try to better differentiate which emotion contributes to disrupt which component of inhibition in which
situation. For example, it has been shown that fear can hamper the inhibition of a previously punished response [52].
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 169
What can be said about other borderline-related feelings such as anger, abandonment, rage, sadness and anxiety?
What can be said about the other components of inhibition? Otherwise, state impulsivity should be investigated in
several contexts, not only in the state of negative emotions. Indeed, one could study the differences between a social
versus a non-social situation. For example, what about social rejection versus a non-social frustration? Would they
have the same effect on impulse control processes? Finally, personality-related variables such as personal schemas,
attributional bias or defensive styles can also interact with inhibition processes in the emergence of impulsive
behaviors. It would be important to include such personality variables in the future studies on inhibition, delay
discounting and decision making in BPD.
7. Conclusion
The DSM behavioral criterion to define BPD impulsivity has a number of limitations. The definition of BPD
impulsivity would be improved by incorporating neurobehavioral models in order to bridge the research and the
DSM. Specifically, a multidimensional approach combining self-report measures, behavioral laboratory measures
and event-related potentials would permit to distinguish impulsive patients from non-impulsive patients, and identity
psychological mechanisms underlying impulsive behaviors in order to guide treatment. In an effort to incorporate
biological and behavioral models into a definition of impulsivity, Moeller and collaborators [1] have proposed to
include three neuropsychological diagnostic criterions: (1) rapid, unplanned reactions to stimuli before complete
processing of information; (2) lack of regard for long-term consequences and; (3) decreased sensitivity to negative
consequences of behavior. The goal of this paper was to review the neuropsychological literature of BPD
impulsivity in line with these neuropsychological diagnostic criterions to verify if the evidence from
neuropsychological data and measurements is sufficiently strong to be integrated into the DSM definition of BPD
impulsivity.
The critical review of neuropsychological studies highlighted some evidence regarding neuropsychological deficits
in BPD patients that may be underlying their impulsive self-damaging behaviors. These deficits relate to prepotent
response inhibition and resistance to proactive interference in the context of emotion, a preference for immediate
gratification and discounting for delayed rewards, and a failure to properly process feedback information and to
monitor action in decision making. However, at least five methodological challenges have been pointed out and
need to be addressed before these deficits can be successfully integrated into a definition of BPD impulsivity and
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 170
some solutions have been proposed to face the main challenges in studying impulsivity in BPD. It is wished that
future studies will take into account these methodological issues considering the importance of better defining BPD
impulsivity given its dramatic consequences and of finding interventions that aim the underlying cognitive and
psychological mechanisms.
References
1. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, et al.Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. The American Journal of
Psychiatry 158 (2001): 1783-1793.
2. Linehan MM. Dialectic behavioral therapy: a cognitive behavioral approach to parasuicide. Journal of
Personality Disorders 1 (1987): 328-333.
3. Sebastian A, Jacob G, Lieb K, et al. Impulsivity in Borderline Personality Disorder: A matter of disturbed
impulse control or a facet of emotional dysregulation? Current Psychiatry Reports 15 (2013): 339-346.
4. Mary C. Discriminating borderline personality disorder from other axis II disorders. The American Journal
of Psychiatry 147 (1990): 161-167.
5. Links PS, Heslegrave R, Reekum RV. Impulsivity: Core aspect of borderline personality disorder. Journal
of Personality Disorders 13 (1999): 1-9.
6. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, et al. The longitudinal course of borderline psychopathology: 6-
year prospective follow-up of the phenomenology of borderline personality disorder. American Journal of
Psychiatry 160 (2003): 274-283.
7. Bornovalova MA, Lejuez CW, Daughters SB, et al. Impulsivity as a common process across borderline
personality and substance use disorders. Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005): 790-812.
8. Lenzenweger MF, Clarkin JF, Fertuck EA, et al. Executive neurocognitive functioning and neurobehavioral
systems indicators in borderline personality disorder: A preliminary study. Journal of Personality Disorders
18 (2004): 421-438.
9. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disordersIVText
Revision (4th Edn.) Washington, DC (2000).
10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th Edn.)
Washington, DC (2013).
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 171
11. Nigg JT, Silk KR, Stavro G, et al. Disinhibition and borderline personality disorder. Development and
Psychopathology 17 (2005): 1129-1149.
12. Tomko RL, Solhan MB, Carpenter RW, et al. Measuring Impulsivity in Daily Life: The Momentary
Impulsivity Scale. Psychological Assessment 26 (2014): 339-349.
13. Wingrove J, Bond AJ. Impulsivity: a state as well as trait variable. Does mood awareness explain low
correlations between trait and behavioral measures of impulsivity? personality and Individual Differences
22 (1997): 333-339.
14. Verheul R. Impulsivity in the diagnosis of borderline personality disorder, in Phenotype Conference of the
Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation. New York, NY (1996).
15. Barratt ES. Impulsivity: Integrating cognitive, behavioral, biological, and environmental data (1993).
16. Macmillan M. The concept of inhibition in some nineteenth century theories of thinking. Brain &
Cognition 30 (1996): 4-19.
17. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference controle functions: a latent-
variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133 (2004): 101-135.
18. Endres MJ, Rickert ME, Bogg T, et al. Externalizing Psychopathology and Behavioral Disinhibition:
Working Memory Mediates Signal Discriminability and Reinforcement Moderates Response Bias in
Approach-Avoidance Learning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120 (2011): 336-351.
19. Clark JM. Contributions of inhibitory mechanisms to unified theory in neuroscience and psychology. Brain
& Cognition 30 (1996): 127-152.
20. Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there
overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clinical Psychology Review 31
(2011): 965-982.
21. Casey BJ, Trainor RJ, Orendi JL, et al. A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation
during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9 (1997): 835-847.
22. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users' guide to the stop signal paradigm (1994).
23. Coffey SF, Schumacher JA, Baschnagel JS, et al. Impulsivity and risk-taking in borderline personality
disorder with and without substance use disorders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment
2 (2011): 128-141.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 172
24. Rentrop M, Backenstrass M, Jaentsch B, et al. Response inhibition in borderline personality disorder:
performance in a Go/Nogo task. Psychopathology 41 (2008): 50-57.
25. Ruchsow M, Groen G, Kiefer M, et al. Response inhibition in borderline personality disorder: event-related
potentials in a Go/Nogo task. Journal of Neural Transmission 115 (2008): 127-133.
26. Berlin HA, Rolls ET, Iversen SD. Borderline personality disorder, impulsivity, and the orbitofrontal cortex.
American Journal of Psychiatry 162 (2005): 2360-2373.
27. Domes G, Winter B, Schnell K, et al. The influence of emotions on inhibitory functioning in borderline
personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 36 (2006): 1163-1172.
28. Henry C, Mitropoulou V, New AS, et al. Affective instability and impulsivity in borderline personality and
bipolar II disorders: similarities and differences. Journal of Psychiatric Research 35 (2001): 307-312.
29. Kunert HJ, Druecke HW, Sass H, et al. Frontal lobe dysfunctions in borderline personality disorder?
Neuropsychological findings. Journal of Personality Disorders 17 (2003): 497-509.
30. Lemelin S, Villeneuve E. Impulsivity associated with the borderline personality disorder. Revue
Quebecoise de Psychologie 24 (2003): 195-210.
31. van Reekum R, Links PS, Fedorov C. Impulsivity in borderline personality disorder, in Biological and
neurobehavioral studies of borderline personality disorder, Silk KR, American Psychiatric Association:
Washington, DC (1994) 1-22.
32. van Reekum R. Repeat neurobehavioral study of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatry &
Neuroscience 21 (1996): 13-20.
33. Ferraz L, Vállez M, Navarro JB, et al. Dimensional assessment of personality and impulsiveness in
borderline personality disorder. Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009): 140-146.
34. Jacob GA, Gutz L, Bader K, et al. Impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: impairment in self-report
measures, but not behavioral inhibition. Psychopathology 43 (2010): 180-188.
35. Krause-Utz A, Sobanski E, Alm B, et al. Impulsivity in relation to stress in patients with borderline
personality disorder with and without co-occurring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an exploratory
study. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease 201 (2013): 116-123.
36. Lampe K, Konrad K, Kroener S, et al. Neuropsychological and behavioural disinhibition in adult ADHD
compared to borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 37 (2007): 1717-1729.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 173
37. Stahl C, Voss A, Schmitz F, et al. Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General 143 (2014): 850-886.
38. Golden CJ, Freshwater SM. Stroop color and word test. Age 15 (1978): 90.
39. LeGris J, Links PS, van Reekum R, et al. Executive function and suicidal risk in women with Borderline
Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Research 196 (2012): 101-108.
40. Johnson HM. Processes of successful intentional forgetting. Psychological Bulletin 116 (1994): 274-292.
41. Korfine L, Hooley JM. Directed forgetting of emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology 109 (2000): 214-221.
42. Linehan MM. Cognitivebehavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press
(1993).
43. Lieb K. Borderline personality disorder. Lancet 364 (2004): 453-461.
44. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39 (2002):
281-291.
45. Silbersweig D, Clarkin JF, Goldstein M, et al. Failure of frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of
negative emotion in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 164 (2007): 1832-
1841.
46. Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C. The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological
Bulletin 120 (1996): 3-24.
47. Wingenfeld K, Mensebach C, Rullkoetter N, et al. Attentional bias to personally relevant words in
borderline personality disorder is strongly related to comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
Personality Disorders 23 (2009): 141-155.
48. Arntz A, Appels C, Sieswerda S. Hypervigilance in borderline personality disorder: A test with the
emotional Stroop paradigm. Journal of Personality Disorders 14 (2000): 366-373.
49. Sieswerda S, Arntz A, Mertens I, et al. Hypervigilance in patients with borderline personality disorder:
specificity, automaticity, and predictors. Behaviour Research & Therapy 45 (2006): 1011-1024.
50. Cackowski S, Reitz AC, Ende G, et al. Impact of stress on different components of impulsivity in
borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 44 (2014): 3329-3340.
51. Chapman AL, Leung DW, Lynch TR. Impulsivity and emotion dysregulation in borderline personality
disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 22 (2008): 148-164.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 174
52. Chapman AL, Dixon-Gordon KL, Layden BK, et al. Borderline Personality Features Moderate the Effect
of a Fear Induction on Impulsivity. Personality Disorders-Theory Research and Treatment 1 (2010): 139-
152.
53. Dougherty DM, Bjork JM, Huckabee HC, et al. Laboratory measures of aggression and impulsivity in
women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 85 (1999): 315-326.
54. Lawrence KA, Allen JS, Chanen AM. Impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: reward-based
decision-making and its relationship to emotional distress. Journal of Personality Disorders 24 (2010): 786-
799.
55. Damasio AR, Everitt BJ, Bishop D. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the
prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London - Series B: Biological
Sciences 351 (1996): 1413-1420.
56. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, et al. The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some
questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (2005): 159-162.
57. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, et al. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to
human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50 (1994): 7-15.
58. Maurex L, Zaboli G, Wiens S, et al. Emotionally controlled decision-making and a gene variant related to
serotonin synthesis in women with borderline personality disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 50
(2009): 5-10.
59. Haaland VO, Landrø NI. Decision making as measured with the Iowa Gambling Task in patients with
borderline personality disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 13 (2007): 699-
703.
60. Dowson J, Bazanis E, Rogers R, et al. Impulsivity in patients with borderline personality disorder.
Comprehensive Psychiatry 45 (2004): 29-36.
61. McCloskey MS, New AS, Siever LJ, et al. Evaluation of behavioral impulsivity and aggression tasks as
endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 43 (2009): 1036-1048.
62. Svaldi J, Philipsen A, Matthies S. Risky decision-making in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry
Research 197 (2012): 112-118.
63. Kirkpatrick T, Joyce E, Milton J, et al. Altered emotional decision-making in prisoners with borderline
personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 21 (2007): 243-261.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 175
64. Gehring WJ. The error-related negativity: an event related brain potential accompanying errors.
Psychophysiology 27 (1990): S34.
65. Miltner WH, Braun CH, Coles MG. Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-
estimation task: evidence for a 'generic' neural system for error detection. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 9 (1997): 788-798.
66. de Bruijn ER, Mars RB, Hulstijn W. It wasn't me... or was it? How false feedback affects performance, in
Errors, conflicts, and the brain. Current opinions on performance monitoring, M. Ullsperger and M.
Falkenstein, MPI of Cognitive Neuroscience: Leipzig (2004): 118-124.
67. de Bruijn ER, Grootens KP, Verkes RJ, et al. Neural correlates of impulsive responding in borderline
personality disorder: ERP evidence for reduced action monitoring. Journal of Psychiatric Research 40
(2006): 428-437.
68. Ruchsow M, Walter H, Buchheim A, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of error processing in borderline
personality disorder. Biological Psychology 72 (2006): 133-140.
69. Schuermann B, Kathmann N, Stiglmayr C, et al. Impaired decision making and feedback evaluation in
borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 41 (2011): 1917-1927.
70. Vega D, Soto À, Amengual JL, et al., Negative reward expectations in Borderline Personality Disorder
patients: neurophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology 94 (2013): 388-396.
71. Andrulonis PA, Glueck BC, Stroebel CF, et al. Organic brain dysfunction and the borderline syndrome.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America 4 (1981): 47-66.
72. Judd PH. Neurocognitive impairment as a moderator in the development of borderline personality disorder.
Development & Psychopathology 17 (2005): 1173-1196.
73. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Pfohl B, et al. The borderline diagnosis I: psychopathology, comorbidity, and
personality structure. Biological Psychiatry 51 (2002): 936-950.
74. Paris J. The nature of borderline personality disorder: multiple dimensions, multiple symptoms, but one
category. Journal of Personality Disorders 21 (2007): 457-473.
75. Zanarini MC, Gunderson JG, Frankenburg FR, et al. The revised diagnostic interview for borderlines:
discriminating BPD from other Axis II disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders 3 (1989): 10-18.
76. Gunderson JG. Borderline personality disorder: A clinical guide. American Psychiatric Pub (2009).
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2017; 1 (3): 154-176 176
77. Fossati A. History of childhood attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and borderline
personality disorder: a controlled study. Comprehensive Psychiatry 43 (2002): 369-377.
78. Philipsen A, Limberger MF, Lieb K, et al. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as a potentially
aggravating factor in borderline personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 192 (2008): 118-123.
79. Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, et al. Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: self-reported
psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of childhood conduct problems and teen CD. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology 30 (2002): 463-475.
80. Miller CJ, Flory JD, Miller SR, et al. Childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the emergence
of personality disorders in adolescence: a prospective follow-up study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69
(2008): 1477-1484.
81. Ferrer M, Andión Ó, Matalí J, et al. Comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in borderline patients
defines an impulsive subtype of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 24 (2010):
812-822.
This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC-BY) license 4.0
... working memory, response inhibition, interference control, cognitive flexibility, problem solving, planning, delay discounting and decision making). In BPD individuals, neuropsychological impairments are conceived as a global dysexecutive syndrome [49,69,71], reflecting a wide frontotemporal dysfunction more strongly lateralized to the right hemisphere [49,69], affecting both cold (attention, working memory, visuospatial abilities, cognitive flexibility, response inhibition, planning, and speeded processing) [61,69,71,72] and hot facets of EFs (affective inhibition, delay discounting, and decision-making) [61,[72][73][74]. It is important to note that, while neuropsychological impairments could moderate BPD development [48,75], being associated with worse clinical outcomes including suicidal behaviors [76], at therapeutic level there is no definitive approach to improve cognitive impairments in BPD. ...
... working memory, response inhibition, interference control, cognitive flexibility, problem solving, planning, delay discounting and decision making). In BPD individuals, neuropsychological impairments are conceived as a global dysexecutive syndrome [49,69,71], reflecting a wide frontotemporal dysfunction more strongly lateralized to the right hemisphere [49,69], affecting both cold (attention, working memory, visuospatial abilities, cognitive flexibility, response inhibition, planning, and speeded processing) [61,69,71,72] and hot facets of EFs (affective inhibition, delay discounting, and decision-making) [61,[72][73][74]. It is important to note that, while neuropsychological impairments could moderate BPD development [48,75], being associated with worse clinical outcomes including suicidal behaviors [76], at therapeutic level there is no definitive approach to improve cognitive impairments in BPD. ...
... On the other hand, only few NIBS studies investigated the effects of such interventions on cold and hot cognitive functioning with specific cognitive tasks. Considering response inhibition in BPD patients [61,72], Sverak et al. 2021 evaluated whether HF-rTMS to the right DLPFC could affect the performance in a non-affective Go/No-Go task [30]: however, no significant changes were observed. As previously reported [21], a suggestion is to modulate response inhibition in BPD patients by targeting other brain areas with NIBS interventions, particularly the Inferior Frontal Gyrus (IFG) and the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA). ...
Article
Introduction: Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe mental disorder characterized by emotion dysregulation, impulsivity, neuropsychological impairment, and interpersonal instability, presenting with multiple psychiatric comorbidities, functional disability and reduced life expectancy due suicidal behaviors. Areas covered: In this perspective, the authors explore the application of noninvasive brain stimulation (NIBS) (rTMS, tDCS, and MST) in BPD individuals by considering a symptom-based approach, focusing on general BPD psychopathology, impulsivity and neuropsychological impairments, suicidality and depressive/anxious symptoms, and emotion dysregulation. Expert opinion: According to a symptoms-based approach, NIBS interventions (particularly rTMS and tDCS) are promising treatment options for BPD individuals improving core symptoms such as emotional and behavioral dysregulation, neuropsychological impairments and depressive symptoms. However, the heterogeneity of stimulation protocols and of assessment tools used to detect these changes limits the possibility to provide definitive recommendations according to a symptom-based approach. To implement such armamentarium in clinical practice, future NIIBS studies should further consider a lifespan perspective due to clinical variability over time, the role of psychiatric comorbidities affecting BPD individuals and the need to combine NIBS with specialized psychotherapeutic approaches for BPD patients and with functional neuroimaging studies. ARTICLE HISTORY
... 27 Studies have shown that individuals with BPD exhibit impairments in a range of cognitive processes, including attention, memory, and executive functions, which are thought to underlie their impulsivity and emotional dysregulation. 28 Despite this, there is still a great deal of uncertainty about the precise relationships between impulsivity, cognitive impairments, and BPD. This highlights the need for further research to fully understand the underlying mechanisms and to develop effective treatments for this debilitating disorder. ...
... 26 This prior finding is aligned with the current results. Gagnon 28 reviewed the literature on impulsivity in BPD and emphasized the importance of considering impulsivity as a multidimensional construct that encompasses both behavioral and cognitive aspects. Impulsive behavior in BPD can be seen as an expression of underlying cognitive impairments, such as difficulties with impulse control and decision-making. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a common and disabling mental health disorder and has detrimental effects on affected individuals across multiple domains. We aimed to investigate whether individuals with BPD differ from control subjects in terms of cognitive functions, and to see if there is a relationship between cognitive functions, impulsivity, and BPD symptom severity. Methods: BPD individuals (n = 26; mean age = 26.7; 69.2% female) and controls (n = 58; mean age = 25.3; 51.7% female) were enrolled. Intra/Extra-Dimensional Set Shift (IED) and One Touch Stockings of Cambridge (OTS) tasks from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) were used to assess cognitive functions. Barratt Impulsivity Scale-version 11 (BIS-11) was administered to measure impulsivity and both the Zanarini Scale for Borderline Personality Disorder self-report and the clinician-administered versions were used to assess BPD symptom severity. Results: BPD group showed significantly impaired cognitive performance on the IED task versus controls, but there was not a significant difference in the OTS task. BPD symptom severity was positively correlated with trait (BIS-11) impulsivity and no correlation was found between BPD symptom severity and cognitive functions. Conclusions: This study suggests people with BPD experience impaired cognitive flexibility and heightened impulsivity. Only impulsivity appeared to be directly related to symptom severity, perhaps indicating that cognitive inflexibility could be a vulnerability marker. Future research should focus on a longitudinal approach to extend clinical and theoretical knowledge in this area.
... Firstly, as several impulsivity facets (e.g., response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, decision-making, planning, problem-solving, delay discounting and proactive interference) have been found altered in BPD population (Gagnon, 2017) (Turner et al., 2017) (Koudys et al., 2018), a suggestion is to modulate via NIBS those dysexecutive functioning features clinically related to impulsivity. As several reviews and metanalysis emphasized NIBS applications targeting the prefrontal cortex to enhance cognitive control and peculiar impulsivity features (Brevet-Aeby et al., 2016) (Schroeder et al., 2020) (Teti Mayer et al., 2020 (Friehs et al., 2021), our review is in line with these findings advising to stimulate predominantly the DLPFC also in the case of BPD. ...
Article
Treating Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a major challenge for psychiatrists. As Brain Stimulation represents an alternative approach to treat psychiatric disorders, our systematic review is the first to focus on both invasive and Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation (NIBS) interventions in people living with BPD, examining clinical effects over core features and comorbid conditions. Following PRISMA guidelines, out of 422 original records, 24 papers were included regarding Deep Brain Stimulation (n = 1), Electroconvulsive therapy (n = 5), Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (n = 13) and transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (n = 5). According to impulsivity and emotional dysregulated domain improvements, NIBS in BPD appears to restore frontolimbic network deficiencies. NIBS seems also to modulate depressive features. Safety and tolerability profiles for each technique are discussed. Despite encouraging results, definitive recommendations on Brain Stimulation in BPD are mitigated by protocols heterogeneity, lack of randomized controlled trials and poor quality of included studies, including high risk of methodological biases. To serve as guide for future systematic investigations, protocols optimization proposals are provided, focusing on alternative stimulation sites and suggesting a NIBS symptom-based approach.
... Firstly, as several impulsivity facets (e.g., response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, decision-making, planning, problem-solving, delay discounting and proactive interference) have been found altered in BPD population (Gagnon, 2017) (Turner et al., 2017) (Koudys et al., 2018), a suggestion is to modulate via NIBS those dysexecutive functioning features clinically related to impulsivity. As several reviews and metanalysis emphasized NIBS applications targeting the prefrontal cortex to enhance cognitive control and peculiar impulsivity features (Brevet-Aeby et al., 2016) (Schroeder et al., 2020) (Teti Mayer et al., 2020 (Friehs et al., 2021), our review is in line with these findings advising to stimulate predominantly the DLPFC also in the case of BPD. ...
Article
Treating Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is a major challenge for psychiatrists. As Brain Stimulation represents an alternative approach to treat psychiatric disorders, our systematic review is the first to focus on both invasive and Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation (NIBS) interventions in people living with BPD, examining clinical effects over core features and comorbid conditions. Following PRISMA guidelines, out of 422 original records, 24 papers were included regarding Deep Brain Stimulation (n = 1), Electroconvulsive therapy (n = 5), Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (n = 13) and transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (n = 5). According to impulsivity and emotional dysregulated domain improvements, NIBS in BPD appears to restore frontolimbic network deficiencies. NIBS seems also to modulate depressive features. Safety and tolerability profiles for each technique are discussed. Despite encouraging results, definitive recommendations on Brain Stimulation in BPD are mitigated by protocols heterogeneity, lack of randomized controlled trials and poor quality of included studies, including high risk of methodological biases. To serve as guide for future systematic investigations, protocols optimization proposals are provided, focusing on alternative stimulation sites and suggesting a NIBS symptom-based approach.
... Пограничное расстройство личности (ПРЛ): при нейропсихологическом обследовании лиц с ПРЛ выявлен дефицит в широком спектре когнитивных областей, включая внимание, когнитивную гибкость, память, управляющие функции, планирование, обработку информации и зрительно-пространственные функции, слухоречевую память и беглость речи, а также снижение скорости психомоторной реакции и координации, когда на первый план выходят трудности подавления непосредственных импульсивных реакций, что проявляется не только при прохождении тестов, но и в поведении [20]. Нарушения управляющих функций, связанные с импульсивностью при психических расстройствах пограничного уровня, характеризуются быстрой незапланированной реакцией на стимул до полной обработки воспринимаемой информации, отсутствием учета долгосрочных последствий, снижением чувствительности к негативным последствиям поведения. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective of the Review: To justify possible use of neuropsychological assessment of cognitive impairments in some most common mental disorders encountered in forensic psychiatry, using analysis of the classic and modern empiric studies conducted with the help of psychometric and brain imaging methods. Key Points. The biopsychological and social nature of disorders associated with a mental pathology can form the methodological basis to choose various diagnostic procedures to study mental disorders. Analysis of biological and psychological aspects within such an approach can be associated with diagnostics of neurocognitive impairments. The term “neurocognitive impairments”, meaning cognitive disorders causing impaired fulfilment of professional, social and community functions, is now commonly used by psychiatrists and psychologists, and soon it will be officially included into diagnosable disorders of the International Classification of Diseases, revision 11. The article discusses justification of introduction of operational methods to assess cognitive dysfunction, in particular of neuropsychological assessment, into forensic psychiatry to ensure that expert conclusions made during borderline mental disorder assessment are valid. Conclusion. The use of neuropsychological methods for cognitive disorder assessment in psychiatry, especially in forensic examinations, is a promising tool for identification of actual cognitive impairments with the possibility to locally diagnose dysfunctions and dimensionally assess the intensity of neurocognitive disorders. Keywords: neuropsychology, cognitive functions, organic mental disorders, expert assessment outlooks.
... But the effects of the reward system's anomalies in BPD are not restricted to attachment and social behavior (Gagnon, 2017). Anhedonia and feelings of emptiness may be an expression of reduced activity of the EOS. ...
Chapter
Depression and personality disorder, in particular borderline personality disorder as defined by DSM and ICD classifications, are characterized by great phenomenological heterogeneity, and high comorbidity with each other and with other psychiatric disorders. These characteristics suggest that several domains of mental functioning are differentially affected, to give rise to one or another diagnosis and their comorbidities. This chapter reviews and links the evidence related to the impairments in functioning of the self-other domain, particularly in adult depression, through advancing a model based on three of its main component systems: stress regulation (negative valence and arousal/regulatory systems), reward (positive valence systems), and mentalizing (system for social processes or social cognition) systems, which we see as interconnected. For each of these systems, we review and link the evidence arising from genetic, neurophysiological and behavioral domains. The chapter follows a developmental psychopathology perspective, which highlights the developmental cascades that give rise to such psychopathology. Finally, we propose an understanding of comorbidity and heterogeneity, future lines for research and for the development of evidence-based interventions.
... Relatedly, the absence of a symbolic capacity for mental states causes individuals to be particularly vulnerable to a teleological self and to exist in a "teleological mode of functioning" with consequent concretisation of inner life. Living in pre-mentalistic states of mind further causes individuals to be governed by outer forces, impulses, and/or simple stimulus responses, a common complaint of BPD impulsive traits (Bateman & Fonagy, 2004, 2006Gagnon, 2017). ...
Article
In the last fifty years the thinking around borderline personality disorder (BPD) shifted from an intra-psychic to a psychoanalytically oriented relational model. The latter described the difficulties associated with this presentation as arising from a disorganisation of the "self" structure in the context of an early caregiving relationship. The concept of inaccurate, or inconsistent "social biofeedback parental affect mirroring" has been pivotal to explain the characteristic failure to mentalize and the interpersonal difficulties associated with a diagnosis of BPD. Nevertheless, far from being the result of a sole linear relationship, these difficulties appear linked to communicative and emotional feedback loops that are reminiscent of cybernetic principles. Furthermore, recent claims have suggested that the communication feedback loops characteristic of the carer-child attachment style represent nothing less that the communication styles of the wider social environment in which the dyad is located. These claims have recently prompted a further shift from men-talizing to epistemic trust and epistemic vigilance, hence departing from a relational model towards a more systemic one. The difficulties associated with BPD are now suggested to be linked to a disorder of social learning, impacted by the rigid nature of the person's information-processing systems. This article reviews this journey.
... (American Occupational Therapy Association, 2014). Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a complex diagnosis that lacks a simple definition (Gagnon, 2017), but has been characterized by several symptoms that disrupt, alter, or interfere with occupational participation, such as fear of abandonment, unstable relationships, suicidal thoughts and behavior, poor self-image and self-esteem, and marked impulsivity (National Alliance on Mental Illness, 2017). Symptoms can begin early in life, and continue to manifest over time (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized by intense emotions, self-harm, unstable self-image, and risky behaviors, which impede wellness and interfere with occupational participation. However, literature on occupational participation of people with BPD is scarce and has mostly focused on women. This study explores and elucidates intersections of occupational participation and BPD in a sample of mostly male veterans in order to identify potential ways occupational therapists and other health professionals can support wellbeing for this population. Grounded theory analysis was conducted on data collected using the Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview (IPII), a semi-structured interview designed to elicit illness personal narratives. Analysis yielded three main themes—influencing environment, internal experience, and occupation—and several subthemes including being abused, arising problems, feeling neglected, feeling victimized, escape, self-segregating, positive change, participating/engaging, and substance abuse. Occupations both influenced and were influenced by the environment and internal experiences. Environments appeared to influence internal experience, but internal experiences did not influence environments directly. Rather, internal experiences impacted a person’s occupations which, in turn, impacted their environments. Participants’ occupational lives revealed, as expected, several subthemes depicting negative and/or isolating experiences. However, participants’ occupations directly impacted both their environmental contexts and internal experiences, suggesting occupational performance may be a powerful mechanism of change for this population. Findings offer promise that occupational therapists could facilitate health-promoting occupational participation which, in turn, may result in more positive and health-promoting environments and internal experiences.
Article
A research protocol was developed to test a theoretical model regarding impulsivity in borderline personality (BP) disorder. It was hypothesized that the impact of identity disturbance of individuals with BP features on their response-inhibition functions could be explained by the disposition of their self-concept to increase the intensity of negative emotions. Participants with different levels of BP features were assigned to a self-description condition (N = 29) that had the potential to manipulate the identity coherence, or a control condition (N = 27) prior to a response inhibition task with high and low arousal emotional stimuli. We also explored the relationship between participants’ self-description and their performance on the inhibition task. The results showed a significant interaction between condition, level of BP features, valence, and stimulus intensity on commission errors. Post-hoc analysis did not reveal significant differences. In addition, a moderate correlation was found between a lesser differentiated description of the self and a higher mean of errors of commission. This preliminary study highlights the relevance of studying the relationship between the self-concept and inhibition regarding borderline impulsivity. The findings should be replicated with a larger sample and with individuals who meet the diagnostic criteria.
Article
Full-text available
Impulsivity is a core feature of borderline personality disorder (BPD) and is most frequently measured using self-rating scales. There is a need to find objective, valid and reliable measures of impulsivity. This study aimed to examine performance of participants with BPD compared with healthy controls on delay and probabilistic discounting tasks and the stop-signal task (SST), which are objective measures of choice and motor impulsivity, respectively. A total of 20 participants with BPD and 21 healthy control participants completed delay and probabilistic discounting tasks and the SST. They also completed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS), a self-rating measure of impulsivity. Participants with BPD showed significantly greater delay discounting than controls, manifest as a greater tendency to accept the immediately available lesser reward rather than waiting longer for a greater reward. Similarly they showed significantly greater discounting of rewards by the probability of payout, which correlated with past childhood trauma. Participants with BPD were found to choose the more certain and/or immediate rewards, irrespective of the value. On the SST the BPD and control groups did not differ significantly, demonstrating no difference in motor impulsivity. There was no significant difference between groups on self-reported impulsivity as measured by the BIS. Measures of impulsivity show that while motor impulsivity was not significantly different in participants with BPD compared with controls, choice or reward-related impulsivity was significantly affected in those with BPD. This suggests that choice impulsivity but not motor impulsivity is a core feature of BPD.
Article
Parasuicide, is particularly prevalent among individuals meeting criteria for borderline personality disorder (Clarkin, Widiger, Frances. Hurt, & Gil-more, 1983). To date almost all published treatments for these individuals have been psychoanalytic (Adler, 1985; Kernberg, 1984; Masterson, 1976). Behavior therapy methods are also useful. Over the past several years, we have developed and evaluated a comprehensive, behavioral treatment Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) for chronically parasuicidal individuals. DBT is based on a biosocial theory that views parasuicide as problem-solving behavior emitted to cope with or ameliorate psychic distress brought on by negative environmental events, self-generated dysfunctional behaviors, and individual temperamental characteristics. Three factors keep parasuicide high in the individual's hierarchy of problem-solving responses: (1) low distress tolerance, (2) inadequate functional coping resources, and (3) parasuicidogenic expectancies. Low distress tolerance provi...
Article
The Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines (DIB) was revised to sharpen its ability to discriminate between clinically diagnosed borderline patients and patients with other types of Axis II clinical diagnoses. The discriminant power of both the revised DIB (DIB-R) and the DIB itself was then tested in a sample of 237 inpatients and outpatients given an Axis II diagnosis by their therapists. The DIB-R was administered blind to clinical diagnosis, while a DIB score was independently derived from DIB-R and other data using a predetermined scoring algorithm. At a cutoff of 8, the DIB-R had a sensitivity of .82, a specificity of .80, a positive predictive power of .74, and a negative predictive power of .87. Overall, these conditional probabilities compare favorably to those obtained for the DIB at its standard cutoff of 7: sensitivity = .97, specificity = .27, positive predictive power = .47, and negative predictive power = .93. They also compare favorably with those obtained in studies that used semistructured or self-report instruments based on DSM-III or DSM-III-R criteria for BPD.