ArticlePDF Available

Conscious-Nonconscious Processing Explains Why Some People Exercise but Most Don't

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychology
Conscious-Nonconscious Processing Explains Why Some
People Exercise but Most DRQ¶W
Seppo E. Iso-Ahola
Department of Kinesiology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
Although it is well established by physiologists that exercise is the single best thing individuals can do for their health, most
people are not regular exercisers. This failure poses an interesting but important question and challenge for psychological
science. Why lack of success? Clearly, exercise is more a cognitive than physical battle. This paper reviews research from
cognitive neuroscience to social psychology and proposes a theory in the form of a 3-stage model to explain why some succeed
but most fail to become regular and habitual exercisers. The model elucidates how beginners, if successful, will progress on a
continuum from fully conscious processing and little exercise (First Stage) to largely nonconscious processing and regular
exercise (Third Stage). However, most beginners cannot get past the Second Stage, conscious-nonconscious-conscious
(occasional exercise),and therefore fail to reach the third stage where this behavior is mainly driven by situational and contextual
cues. This failure is reflected in findings that most beginners cannot get through five weeks without a lapse. The major obstacle in
the second stage emanates from the combination of activated interdependent psychological processes: the human tendency to
follow the law of least effort, especially after self-control depletion from daily work, and threats to personal freedom. Exercise can
only be understood in the temporal and social contexts associated with leisure time when most people are most likely to be able
to engage in exercise activities. It is in this context that the interconnected conscious and nonconscious processes are activated.
The ensuing battle within and between these processes prevents most beginners from moving to the third stage and regular
exercise.
Conscious-nonconscious processing | Exercise | Self-control | Freedom
INTRODUCTION
Lifestyle is the most important determinant of human health. For example, of the ten leading causes for years of potential life lost before
age 65, lifestyle is estimated to account for 53%, followed by environment (21.8%), human biology (16.4%), and the health care system
(9.8%) (1). For most people, lifestyle is controllable and centers on four key health behaviors: regular exercise, non-smoking, healthy
diet, and moderate alcohol use. If people followed these health behaviors throughout their lives, they would live, on average, seven years
longer (2). Statistically, physical activity contributes five years to this expanded lifespan (e.g., 3-4). In contrast, if cancer were eliminated
altogether the lifespan would be extended only by two years (2). A recent review of over 30 years of physiological research (5) concluded
that exercise is ³the single most important thing people can do to improve or maintain their health´. Regular exercise cuts premature
mortality and heart disease in half, linearly decreases the likelihood of stroke with increased frequency and intensity of participation,
reduces the probability of various forms of cancer, and helps eliminate diabetes altogether (6-9). Exercise has also been found to be at
least as effective as pharmacological medications for reducing depression (e.g., 10). These health outcomes are possible because of the
strong effects of exercise on the human body at cellular and molecular levels (11).
Against this background, assuming that health is important to people, one would think that everyone would be a regular exerciser.
Yet, only 22% of the U.S. population exercise regularly, with 78% being either non-H[HUFLVHUVRU³RFFDVLRQDO´ exercisers
(6); exercise rates have remained stagnant for decades (12-13). This pattern presents an interesting and important problem for science,
psychological science in particular. Why does the majority not engage in this health behavior? Although the role of environmental factors
has been investigated, observed effects have been marginal at best (e.g., 14). Similarly, knowledge of exercise and its benefits make little
or no difference in initiation or maintenance of exercise behavior (15-17). However, none of this is surprising to psychologists given that
enactment of behavior always emanates from the processes of human mind. Even if people knew of the benefits of exercise and were
surrounded by alluring outdoor environments, they would not exercise unless they could bring themselves (their conscious and
nonconscious minds) to do so.
The purpose of the present paper is to review relevant research in an effort to help us understand why some succeed but most fail to
regularly engage in this important health behavior. In doing so, the paper first provides a general theoretical framework (Figure 1) and
then a more specific theoretical model (Figure 2) to account for psychological processes in initiation and maintenance of exercise
behavior. Although this theory-building is based on empirical research, much work remains to be done to verify the offered explanations
and models. For example, while we know the dominance of conscious control attempts and their neural basis in early exercisers, it is
empirically unclear why most beginners fail to get past the first five weeks without a lapse. Is this a failure in conscious self-regulation or
neurological processing, or both? This paper provides a theoretical explanation for this and other unanswered questions to stimulate
future research. In doing so, it affirms the general argument that science advances not by the statistical testing of the null hypothesis but
by theory construction and model expansion (154).
For clarity, nonconscious processing refers to mental operations (e.g., feelings and thoughts) of which a person is unaware. They are
fast, automatic, associative and effortless (18WKHVH³UHIOHFWLYHUHDFWLRQV´19) can affect all psychological processes with respect to
perception, motivation and behavior. In conscious processing, in turn, attended information enters cognitive awareness and is reportable
to others; it can be pondered and reoriented (20). When behaviors become routine and automatic with little or no conscious awareness,
WKH\JURZKDELWXDO(PHUJLQJKDELWVDUH³FKXQNV´RIQHXUDODFWLYLW\ORFDWHGLQWKHVSHFLILFUHJLRQVRIWKHbrain (21).
Conflict of interest: No conflicts declared. Corresponding Author: Seppo E. Iso-Ahola. Email: isoahol@umd.edu.
© 2017 by the Authors | Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI).
Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 1 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
Figure 1. Conscious ±nonconscious processing and exercise.
According to the general theoretical framework, when faced with the question whether to exercise today (Figure 1), 22% of the
population does not have to think about it as the positive answer is provided by situational cues and associated nonconscious processing.
To be sure, they can lapse occasionally, EXWWKHLU³SULPDU\´UHVSRQVHLVWRH[HUFLVHWRGD\,QFRQWUDVWWKHTXHVWLRQHOLFLWVconscious
thinking among the 78% group and activates formidable obstacles, of which threats to a sense of personal freedom and the general human
WHQGHQF\WRIROORZWKH³ODZRIOHDVWHIIRUW´DUHIRUHPRVW$VDUHVXOWWKHLU³SULPDU\´UHVSonse is not to exercise, although they may
occasionally be able to overcome these barriers and thus get engaged in physical activity.
The paper proceeds to elucidate this overall process by presenting a 3-stage model to explain how the initiation and maintenance of
exercise participation moves from early conscious deliberations to later nonconscious processing, and shows why most people cannot get
beyond the second stage. In so doing, it proposes a psychological mechanism that sheds light on how nonconscious processing activates
health- and exercise-related goals and resultant continued participation among regular exercisers, but not until the third stage. In contrast,
conscious processing is fully responsible for initial engagement and experimentation in exercise activities during the first stage. However,
this conscious processing likely activates feelings of threat to SHUVRQDOIUHHGRP³H[HUFLVH-or-HOVH´DQGthe human tendency to follow
³WKHODZRI OHDVWHIIRUW´ in occasional/non-exercisers. The second stage becomes the battleground where both conscious and
nonconscious processes face the formidable task of facilitating regularity of participation. Unfortunately, research indicates, most people
lose the battle and therefore fail to move to the third stage of sustained engagement.
MADE TO BE ACTIVE
Human mind and body were made to be actively used. The associated SULQFLSOHRI³XVH-it-or-lose-LW´PHDQVWKDWmany aspects of the
human machinery deteriorate with lack of use, or in reverse, grow with use, as evidenced by various scientific findings. For one, recent
research has shown that lipoprotein lipase (LPL) plummets in the blood stream when people do not move at all but sit all day long (22).
As a consequence, the reduced LPL cannot break cholesterol and thus increases the likelihood of heart disease by 54%, with sitting
becoming an independent risk factor for it (23). For another, there have been several case studies reported according to which young
people have developed deep venous thrombosis (i.e., blood clots) after playing videogames nonstop for 7-8 hours, and as a result, some
have died from pulmonary embolism (24).
A different line of research has indicated that bone density is increased with exercise and decreased in its absence (25), and it is well
known that astronauts experience skeletal muscle atrophy if unable to engage in weight-bearing exercises in weightlessness (26). Perhaps
the most visible consequence of the ³XVH-it-or-lose-LW´SULQFLSOH is WRGD\¶V obesity crisis (use-it-or-gain-it), which has much to do with the
³epidemic of physical inactivity´(7, 27). Finally, to all of this evidence, one should add a long line of research on the effects of stimulus
deprivation on the human brain, mind and the central nervous system (28-29). For example, even as early as the 1950s, it was reported
that orphanage children who had no environmental stimuli (but were given adequate nutrition and fluids to survive) suffered from
dramatic locomotor retardation. It took two years for these children to be able to sit by themselves and four years before they were able to
walk (30-31).
Research supporting the positive impact of the use-it-or-lose-it principle has shown that physical activity and active use RIRQH¶VPLQG
confer important neural and cognitive benefits (i.e., enhanced neuroplasticity and neurogenesis). Learning new motor skills or rehearsing
RQH¶VVNLOOVLQWKHPLQFUHDVHVWKHEUDLQ¶VJUD\PDWWHU32), while effortful and successful learning keeps new neurons alive (33). Physical
activity increases the size of hippocampus and thereby improves memory function (34), prevents impairment of executive function (35),
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 2 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
facilitates creativity (36), and mitigates the negative effects of certain genes on cognitive performance (37). Furthermore, cognitive
functioning has been shown to improve through regular participation in cognitive (38-39), physical (40-42), and social (43) activities.
Regular participation in these activities builds the ³QHXURJHQLFUHVHUYH´WKDWSURWHFWVDJDLQVWEHKDYLRUDOPDQLIHVWDWLRQVRf the age-related
cognitive decline (44). The net result is that regular participation in challenging activities is associated with reduced risk of dementia
(45,QVKRUW³neuronal adapWDWLRQVUHVXOWIURPEHLQJDFWLYHGXULQJOHLVXUHWLPH´(32, p. 12448) and are manifested in enhanced
neuroplasticity and neurogenesis, all of which translates into a more efficiently running and better performing brain. But does this
biological and psychological imperative for active lifestyle translate into rational thinking and doing what is the best for us?
Temporal and Social Contexts of Exercise
Before attempting to understand whether people do what is best for their health, it is important to keep in mind that exercise behavior can
only be understood if it is analyzed as a leisure activity, in its temporal and social contexts (46-47). For most people, daily exercise has to
be undertaken after work. It therefore becomes another leisure activity that competes with other activities for time and social resources,
leading to a cognitive battle in the choice of activities. Herein is a major problem for would-be exercisers. Given that a sense of freedom
is the defining characteristic of leisure (48-49), any violation of it leads to psychological reactance (50) and avoidance of any behavior
that threatens that sense of freedom.
A problem with exercise is that it is promoted DVD³GR-it-or-HOVH´activity, giving non-exercisers and occasional exercisers little or no
sense of choice. Importantly, it makes them conscious thinkers who have to make hard choices, which is cognitively straining and
generally avoided (18). Typically, when people come home from work, it is the first time during the day when they feel, ³LWLVP\time to
do whatever I want´DQGWKHUHIRUH do not want to be told what to do (i.e., you have to go for a run) and engage in making difficult
decisions. To a considerable extent, free time, of course, is an illusion because there are many compulsory activities that have to be done
in leisure time, such as household chores and child care. Nevertheless, there is plenty of free time available in leisure for exercise as an
average American finds five hours a day to watch TV (51). A critical psychological point, however, is that for occasional exercisers and
non-exercisers, choosing to exercise is cognitively straining and undermines their sense of freedom, while other common leisure
activities (e.g., TV watching) do not; many of them are triggered by situational cues and run by nonconscious processing (47), and are
therefore not cognitively straining. Empirical evidence supports the idea that the exertion of cognitive (executive) control is
neurologically costly or aversive (52). The result is that the law of least effort is followed while fulfilling the fundamental need for
autonomy (53), making this principle a formidable obstacle for would-be exercisers.
This means that exercise, if perceived as a choice among other leisure activities, faces an uphill battle to become an integral part of
one¶Vleisure repertoire. Recent research from other contexts (54-56) suggests that important health-promoting behaviors such as
exercise cannot be perceived and presented as choices to be selected from a host of leisure activities; rather, they are to be seenas
activities that have to be undertaken regardless of conditions (46-47). A high commitmenWWRRQH¶VH[HUFLVHJRDOVIDFLOLWDWHVWKHVH
perceptions as it suppresses the desire to restore a sense of personal freedom (57). In this view, then, exercise becomes a forced "choice",
and self-identity becomes associated with the mere doing of the activity, but not with a sense of freedom or any of the benefits to be
obtained from the exercise activity (58). Yet, such a forced but accepted choice to exercise allows people to maintain their general
tendency to overestimate the role of consciousness and experience themselves as agents making individual decisions (59).
To further elucidate the role of freedom of choice, it is important to distinguish between temporary and permanent choices. Beginning
exercisers see exercise as one activity (choice) among many other leisure activities and therefore continue to make a temporary choice to
exercise or not to exercise on a daily basis (46). Conscious processing, therefore, is an essential part of their decision making. However,
later with a sufficient number of repeats (60), some of these exercisers advance to the point where a permanent choice is made to exercise
regardless of daily situations. Subsequently, this permanent choice eliminates temporary choices about exercise, as well as the permanent
choice itself; exercise is simply done no matter what. Thus, the importance of personal freedom is significantly reduced, if not eliminated
altogether, and the role of conscious processing diminished. Conscious processing is, of course, decreased with reduced perceived
freedom because people do not think about the consequences of the elimination of freedom. In general, reduced conscious processing
gives way to increased nonconscious processing, and therefore, to a greater likelihood that behavior will be repeated (19, 61-62). This
repeating of nonconsciously triggered behaviors is manifested in the dominance of sedentary leisure activities, especially TV watching
(47). It should be noted that although freedom generally is important to individuals, there are considerable cultural differences in the need
for self-expression and choice (63-64). These differences suggest that in Asian cultures, for example, it would be easier to overcome the
freedom problem associated with health behaviors, especially exercise.
Rational Thinking and Decision Making?
If people were rational thinkers and decision makers regarding their activity involvement, everyone would be a regular exerciser
(assuming that health is important to them). They would reason, ³,KDYHWLPHIRUD-40 min. run/walk and still four hours left to watch
79´ They might also weigh the benefits and costs of their behavioral choices and would select a behavior with the most favorable cost-
benefit ratio (65). If they were rational, they would also respond to perceived health risk by exercising. However, people do not make
rational choices regarding health behaviors as reflected in the finding that the perception of risk is generally a poor predictor of
behavioral change to reduce risk (66). If would-be exercisers were rational about their personal time use and health risk, they would
become ³VHOI-as-GRHUV´ (58) concerning their exercise participation. People with this predisposition have been shown to engage in health-
related behaviors regardless of daily circumstances and obstacles (58); they are not concerned with immediate rewards either. Compared
to non-H[HUFLVHUVH[HUFLVHUVGRQRWHQJDJHLQ³WHPSRUDO GLVFRXQWLQJ´RUDGHFLVLRQ-making process according to which they would seek
immediate gains; instead, they view long term healthy rewards as more important (153). The fact that 78% of the population remains
sedentary suggests an absence of such rational reasoning, resulting in people not doing what is good for them (67-68). This apparent
irrationality seems to be reflected in research findings suggesting that people rate TV watching among their worst leisure experiences, yet
on average spend almost five hours a day doing it (69). Why would anyone do something for five hours a day if it is experienced so
negatively?
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 3 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
Research VXJJHVWVWKDW³SHRSOHDUHXQDEOHWRUHVLVWVSHQGLQJPRUHWLPHLQWKLVDFWLYLW\79ZDWFKLQJWKDQWKH\ZRXOGFRQVLGHU
KHDOWK\RUGHVLUDEOH´(70, p. 49). :RXOGQ¶WUDWLRQDOGHFLVLRQ-makers be able to resist it? The fact that they do not may reflect the
overriding influence of situational cues (e.g., the sight of TV itself) and the resultant strength of nonconscious over conscious processing
(62, 71-72). Consistent with this, it has been argued that leisure is an incubator for automatic processes and a difficult setting for the
conscious mind to override the influence of situational cues (47). In other words, because leisure is their personal time, people tend to
avoid difficult conscious decisions (e.g., Should I exercise today?), but instead, answer easier ones (e.g., Will I watch my favorite
program on TV tonight?) without conscious awareness. Giving up on conscious thinking and letting the nonconscious mind run
unopposed poses a formidable challenge for non-exercisers and occasional exercisers during their leisure time, but helps us understand
why people often do not do what is good for them. More generDOO\³WKHHDVHZLWKZKLFKSHRSOHare satisfied enough to stop thinking is
UDWKHUWURXEOLQJ´18, p. 46).
7KHWHQGHQF\QRWWRGRZKDWLVLQRQH¶VEest interest is not limited to just health behaviors. It has been demonstrated (73) that
emotions can have powerful effects on financial decisions. Experimentally induced emotions carried over from a prior, irrelevant
situation to another situation and negatively affected study SDUWLFLSDQWV¶ selling and buying decisions. In a similar vein, wild swings in the
stock market are known to make people emotional and irrational, leading them to ³EX\KLJK´DQG³VHOOORZ´74). ³/RVVDYHUVLRQ´PHDQV
that losing evokes stronger negative feelings than winning elicits positive emotions (18). Furthermore, the median retirement savings of a
family between 50 and 55 years old is just $8,000. Saving, of course, denotes delaying immediate gratification for greater future gains but
is difficult in an environment where mere (nonconscious) exposure to symbols of the instant society activates impatience and leads to
inferior financial decisions (i.e., non-saving) (75). These kinds of ILQGLQJVFDVWVHULRXVGRXEWRQWKH³UDWLRQDO-DJHQW´ model of human
financial behavior espoused by traditional schools of economics. According to this model, people make rational decisions as long as the
relevant information is provided.
Behavioral economists (e.g., 76), however, have challenged this model in a fundamental way by showing that although people do not
generally make rational financial decisions, they can be helped, guided and even protected in the process. For example, if employees are
³QXGJHG´WRMRLQDSHQVLRQSODQDVD default option or if they allow their employers to tie a fixed proportion of their salary raises to a
saving plan by default, the savings rate improves dramatically (76). It is interesting to note that such an automatic enrollment in a pension
or savings plan takes the focus away from freedom to choose, akin to taking freedom of choice out of exercise behavior by designating it
as a necessary activity that has to be done like teeth brushing in the morning. All of the above, however, raises a bigger question about
whether people, when left to their own devices, can make rational choices about their health, finances and other matters, and whether
others (and society) should intervene LQWKHSURFHVVDQG³QXGJH´WKHPWRward better decisions. The fact that 78% do not exercise
regularly and 68% are overweight or obese suggests that they do not seem to be able to make conscious decisions that are in their best
interest. Alternatively, situational cues and the associated nonconscious processing are so powerful that they override rational decision
making.
CONSCIOUS-NONCONSCIOUS PROCESSING: 3-STAGE MODEL
Is it conscious or nonconscious?
Abundant empirical evidence accumulated during the last 20 years on the role of conscious-nonconscious processes (for reviews of
research, see 18-19, 61-62, 77) provides a clear theoretical framework for answering fundamental questions about human behavior, such
as, why do we choose to engage in certain activities but not in others (47)? Baumeister et al.¶V61) comprehensive and insightful review
RIUHVHDUFKOHDYHVOLWWOHGRXEWDERXWFRQVFLRXVWKRXJKWV¶³SURIRXQG´DQG³HPSLULFDOO\VWURQJ´HIIHFWVRQFRJQLWions and behaviors. At the
same time, numerous experiments have shown that goal pursuit can run outside of cognitive awareness (62). Many prominent researchers
(e.g., 18-19KDYHDUJXHGWKDWLQHYHU\GD\OLIHSHRSOHDUH³IDVW´WKLQNHUVPRVWRIWKHWLPH; that is, their responses and decisions are
mostly based on intuitive, impulsive, associative and automatic (nonconscious) thinking. Accordingly, they operate on a simple cognitive
level, answering such questions as, what is 2+2?, but avoid the strain of ³VORZ´WKLQNLQJ["They are predisposed to stay away
from cognitively demanding tasks and protect their pleasant affect by avoiding excessive mental effort (78).
It is not surprising that dual processing of cognitive activity (nonconscious vs conscious; System 1 vs System 2; Type 1 vs Type 2)
has been discussed and argued at length (79-80). A recent vigorous debate for and against unconscious vs. conscious influences on
cognitions and behaviors has divided researchers into two camps (81). This debate, however, misses the mark, because conscious vs.
nonconscious processing is not an either-or-proposition. Both are present and needed, even if to varying degrees, for initiation and
maintenance of human behaviors. There are no purely consciously driven activities and no purely nonconsciously guided activities. For
example, even though it has been shown that watching library pictures triggers nonconscious processing resulting in people speaking
more softly (82), it does not mean that they do this always and under all conditions. Even if situational cues become so powerful that they
initiate certain behaviors through nonconscious processing, the conscious can readily interrupt habitual and automatic responses (e.g., 61,
83).
Laboratory research has provided strong support for the existence of nonconscious processing that can result in the automatic
sedentary activity choice and participation noted above. However, when thinking of behaviors outside of laboratory (e.g., exercise) that
do not involve millisecond responses on computer keyboards, does it really matter, as has been found, that nonconscious processing
begins 1 second or more before movement when the conscious intention to act also starts before movement, even 206 milliseconds before
the onset of muscle activity (84)? Such findings, aside from major methodological concerns (85)LQGLFDWHWKDWWKHEUDLQ¶VSUHSDUDWRU\
activity could begin a few hundred milliseconds before the conscious intention; however, these findings do not mean that intentions
cannot intervene right before or after the onset of movement. In fact, there is plenty of evidence that conscious processing can override
(86) and ³YHWR´87) nonconscious impulses, alter responses assumed to be immune to conscious control (88), and reduce the power of
situational cues (89). In short, nonconscious automatic processes can be suppressed and altered by conscious intentions, which is
important when considering everyday behaviors such as exercise.
Furthermore, there is no clear starting point or location for neural processing for motor movements, with the processing beginning in
several places in the brain simultaneously and involving distinct cortical circuits (20). These circuits consist of loops rather than linear
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 4 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
chains (90). The basal ganglia-presupplementary motor area circuit and the parietal-premotor circuit seem to play a key role in the
process. There is also evidence that decision making and the motor preparation in the brain proceed in parallel (91). As the preparatory
neural activity must itself be caused, the cascading neural activity does not begin randomly or appear magically from thin air, but instead,
has its basis in social learningSDVWH[SHULHQFHVDQGRWKHUV¶EHKDYLRUVin similar situations (92). It is clear, then, that both processes
(conscious and nonconscious) are responsible for human behavior. An important unanswered question remains about the conditions (e.g.,
starting an exercise program vs. continuing an existing one) which give rise to and dominance over one another. $QDUJXPHQWWKDW³WKHUH
LVQRUROHIRUFRQVFLRXVQHVV´LQKXPDQEHKDYLRU72) is, needless to say, misleading and theoretically or empirically unjustified. While no
clear consensus exists on the dominance of conscious over nonconscious processing, or vice versa, it is indisputable that conscious
mental events cause and drive behaviors (93). Baumeister et al.¶V(61, p. 334) conclusion perhaps best summarizes the current state of
knowledge: ³,WLVSODXVLEOHWKDWLPSXOVHVWRDFWJHQHUDOO\RULJLQDWHLQWKHQRQFRQVFLRXVEXWEHKDYLRUDORXWFRPHGHSHQGVFUXFLDOO\RQ
ZKDWKDSSHQVZKHQWKH\DUHFRQWHPSODWHGFRQVFLRXVO\´ In light of recent experimental evidence (94), this is especially true for complex
behaviors such as exercise.
What does all of this mean for exercise behavior? First, it suggests that both the conscious and nonconscious minds have to be
involved in making people regular exercisers. ³1HFHVVLW\IRUWKHQRQFRQVFLRXVDQGconscious mind to work together and complement
HDFKRWKHUXQGHUVFRUHVWKHIDFWWKDWH[HUFLVHLVDVPXFKDFRJQLWLYHDVLWLVDSK\VLFDOFKDOOHQJH´46, p. 104). Second, initiation and
maintenance of any complex and demanding behavior operates on the continuum of conscious-nonconscious processing such that in the
beginning, conscious processing dominates behavioral engagement but after countless repeats, nonconscious processing takes over and
makes behavior habitual, thereby sustaining it in the long run. Based upon this general model, I propose a more specific model according
to which there are three stages of exercise behavior (Figure 2) characterized by a varying degree of conscious vs. nonconscious
processing, proceeding from fully conscious processing (First Stage) to predominantly nonconscious processing (Third Stage).
Figure 2. Three (3) Stages of Exercise.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 5 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
FIRST STAGE: CONSCIOUS PROCESSING
In the first stage, conscious-cognitive, people for a variety of reasons become aware of the importance of exercise for human health in
general and their own health in particular. Subsequently, they may take initial steps to find out how,when,where,and with whom they
could exercise, thereby starting to build exercise infrastructure for their physical activity. Sooner or later, this information search leads to
the first attempts to engage in selected forms of exercise. It is also likely to result in the establishment of goals for exercise participation.
Research has shown that proximal, specific, difficult EXWDWWDLQDEOHJRDOVSURGXFHEHWWHUUHVXOWVWKDQYDJXH³GR-your-EHVW´JRDOV95).
6LPLODUO\SHUVRQDOO\PHDQLQJIXOJRDOVWKDW³VHOI-DIILUP´DQGDUHDOLJQHGZLWKRQH¶VFRUHYDOXHV enhance self-control (96); such goals are
linked with maintenance of health behaviors and yield significant health gains from them (97). The monitoring and elimination of goal
conflicts, however, becomes important given the evidence that when desires for leisure activities (e.g., mass media) conflict with other
activity goals, they bring about the most self-control failure (983K\VLFDODFWLYLW\JRDOSURJUHVVFDQEHLQFUHDVHGE\³DFWLRQSODQQLQJ´
but only if exercise goals do not conflict with other goals (99). Thus, monitoring discrepancies between goals and current behaviors is
essential for self-control and goal achievement, and may explain why mindfulness improves self-control (100). All of this suggests that in
the first stage, continuing to exercise is more a matter of cognitive effort and control, as well as conflict resolution, than actual
competence with the muscle movements. Cognitive effort, in turn, is closely connected to engagement of the executive control network
(101).
Research further suggests that goal conflicts can be pre-empted and managed by the systematic construction of exercise
infrastructure. Gollwitzer (102) reported that compliance with participation in a vigorous exercise program was dramatically greater
(39% vs. 91%) when specifiF³LI-WKHQ´SODQVZHUHHPSOR\HGFRPSDUHGWREHLQJH[SRVHGWRDJHQHUDOmotivational package (promoting
self-efficacy, providing information about vulnerability to disease etc.). Such plans constitute the essence of exercise infrastructure and
specify when, where, how, and with whom exercise will be undertaken; they in part determine whether beginners are able to move from
dabbling in exercise to sustained attempts to benefit from physical activity. RegardlessHYHU\RQH¶V engagement during this stage is
driven and characterized by heavy conscious involvement and processing. Common experience tells us that people embarking on their
initial exercise programs cannot do so without thinking hard about how, when, where and with whom to participate.
Conscious self-regulatory efforts, either by their presence or absence, play a key role in the first stage. Research has shown that self-
regulation has a much greater effect on physical activity than other social-cognitive factors such as self-efficacy (e.g., 103). However,
most beginners appear to fail in self-regulation as the proportion of people surviving five weeks without an exercise lapse is as low as
18.7% (60). This failure most likely reflects lack of SDUWLFLSDQWV¶self-regulatory skills, such as goal setting, conflict PRQLWRULQJDQG³VHOI-
DIILUPDWLRQ´)XUWKHUSeople may have general intentions WRH[HUFLVHEXWQRVSHFLILF³DFWLRQSODQV´IRUHQJDJHPHQW,n fact, general
intention is a relatively poor predictor of physical activity (104-105) and considerably worse than specific intentions in goal achievement
(106). The role of intentions in exercise diminishes with increased habitualness, but grows with changes in stable environments and when
habits are weak (83, 107). Given that habitualness is virtually nonexistent in the first stage, these findings support the idea that exercisers
in the first stage rely heavily on conscious cognitive control to regulate their participation. Evidence suggests that the better self-
regulation capacity, the more successful people are in converting intentions into physical activity (108).
In the first stage, conscious processing centers on two cognitive control tasks: (1) self-regulation of exercise behavior and associated
cognitions (e.g., monitoring, self-affirming, evaluating) and (2) resistance or inhibition of competing behaviors and temptations.
Evidence suggests that limited self-regulatory skills undermine efforts to consciously and cognitively control achievement behavior
(109). Deficient self-regulatory skills also correlate with reduced capacity to control or inhibit temptations and impulses (110-111).
Attempts to control temptations and competing habits are also undermined when self-control resources have been exhausted (112). It has
been suggested that such depletion often occurs at work and subsequently spills over to leisure, weakening efforts to resist temptations
(e.g., TV watching) in leisure when exercise is typically undertaken (47).
5HVHDUFKVXJJHVWVWKDWZKHQSHRSOHWU\WR³EDODQFH´DFKLHYHPHQWJRDOVDQGWHPSWDWLRQVWKH\WHQGWRFKRRVH³ILUVWWHPSWDWLRQ then
WKHJRDO´UHVXOWLQJLQthe postponement of goal pursuit (exercise) in favor of instant gratification (113, 153). This research has shown
WKDWLQVWHDGRI³EDODQFLQJ´³KLJKOLJKWLQJ´ (or focusing on the primary goal) leads to employment of self-control strategies that block
temptations. 6LPLODUO\UHVHDUFKRQ³JRDOVKLHOGLQJ´KDVVKRZQthat when people are highly committed to their primary goal, the mere
DFWLYDWLRQRIWKLVJRDO³VKLHOGV´WKHPIURPFRQIOLFWLQJJRDOVDQGWKXVKHOSVLQFRQWUolling temptations (114). Such a strategy would seem
to be important to exercisers in the first stage when they need to resist the power of temptations and the influence of nonconsciously
processed cues for inactivity during leisure.
Cognitive control attempts for goal pursuit in general (115) and with respect to physical activity in particular (116) are underpinned
by neural processes, specifically executive functions and working memory operations (101). It is unclear, however, which neuron
networks underlie self-regulation of exercise behavior. But it has been established that the prefrontal regions and subcortical limbic
structures of the brain are involved in reward detection in goal pursuit (117) and may therefore energize exercise behavior for those who
perceive exercise as rewarding. The role these brain regions play in reward processing and effort recruitment for exercise remains to be
determined.
In the first stage, an important determinant of self-regulatory success or failure LVLQGLYLGXDOV¶ initial motivation for entering their
exercise programs. Do people begin out of pure interest or felt obligation? It is well established that intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation
influences cognition, affect and behavior (53). Other things being equal, the self-determination research suggests that intrinsic motivation
leads to self-regulatory success and extrinsic motivation to self-regulatory failure. Recent research supports this general conclusion and
indicates that initial motivation affects how temptations are experienced and resisted. More specifically, Milyavskaya et al. (118) found
that SHRSOHZLWK³ZDQW-WR´ (intrinsic) motivation experienced fewer and weaker goal-interfering temptations, perceived fewer obstacles to
goal pursuit, and exhibited stronger resistance to conflicting desires. By contrastSHRSOHZLWK³KDYH-WR´ (extrinsic) motivation perceived
stronger temptations, greater obstacles, and lower control for goal-thwarting stimuli. Such individuals are also likely to engage in
³WHPSRUDOGLVFRXQWLQJ´RUPDNLQJGHFLVLRQVWKDWHPSKDVL]HLPPHGLDWHJDLQVDQGUHZDUGVThese findings are consistent with
research indicating that self-control is most successful when attention is fully available to devote to the self-relevant task or activity
(119³:DQW-WR´PRWLYDWLRQOLNHO\HQKDQFHVWKLVDWWHQWLRQWRWKHDFWLYLW\
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 6 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
These results suggest that motivation for starting exercise programs sets the stage for success or failure at subsequently maintaining
physical activity. If people initiate participation wLWKWKHLGHDRID³PXVW´,RU³,VKRXOGGRLW´, they experience more and stronger
temptations and are less able to resist them, with the net result of an increased likelihood of lapsing and quitting. In support of this idea, it
has been found that framing exercise goals in an intrinsically motivating and autonomy-supportive way enhances long-term persistence in
exercise activities (120). Self-determined motives also build habit strength (121-122) because they lead to more frequent repeats of the
behavior. Growing habit strength, in turn, helps exercisers move onward to the second and third stages, where behavior becomes
increasingly determined by nonconscious processing.
However, although conscious cognitive control processing is heavily involved in early exercise participation, conscious rational
thinking in and of itself does not guarantee that most people become regular exercisers. Instead, success of continued engagement in the
first stage seems to crucially depend on whether conscious thoughts are directed at building self-determined motivation, developing self-
regulation and self-control skills and strategies, eliminating goal conflicts, and reducing the influence of temptations and competing
habits.
SECOND STAGE: CONSCIOUS-NONCONSCIOUS-CONSCIOUS PROCESSING
The second stage, conscious-nonconscious-conscious, is the most important of all three. If participants can get past this stage to the third
one, the battle for uninterrupted engagement has been won. Unfortunately, most people can never get past the second stage, as reflected
by the repeated finding that about RIH[HUFLVHUVDUH³RFFDVLRQDO´ and 24% non-exercisers. If the first stage is characterized as putting
RQH¶Vtoes in water, the second stage is equivalent to jumping in it. It is suggested that at this stage, participants experiment with different
types of exercise, as well as frequency, duration and intensity of exercise. Accordingly, they practice new forms, repeat temporal
patterns, and search for personally meaningful engagement. Through trial and error, new skills are learned and rehearsed in various ways
and settings. Such experimentation is a reflection of cognitive control attempts and the heavy involvement of conscious processing.
At this stage, feedback plays an important role, whether in the form of the ERG\¶s physiological response to various work-out
programs or SDUWQHUV¶DQGSHUVRQDOWUDLQHUV¶advice and social reinforcement. Competence feedback (i.e., success at exercise) enhances
intrinsic motivation (123), and feedback on goal progress increases self-efficacy, with elevated self-efficacy in turn sustaining motivation
and behavior (124-125). In the second stage, exercise infrastructure is further constructed and solidified. In other words, the
infrastructure is formulated for when (time management), how (format management), where (place management), and with whom (social
management) exercise is undertaken and performed. Without such a solid infrastructure, achievement of the third stage is difficult, if not
insurmountable, because the foundation for the repeating of the behavior is otherwise absent.
Although no definite and precise starting and ending points exist for the second stage, this stage can begin in the first few weeksafter
initiation of an exercise program and end about six months later. In general, the third stage commences and the second one ends when
exercise has become largely, or completely, habitual. Some studies (e.g., 60) suggest that it can happen as early as five weeks after the
beginning of a program, depending on frequency and success of early exercise. If exercise does not become habitual by the end of six
months, it is unlikely to become so later. By the end of the sixth month, at least one half of the participants have quit (126). The 50%
drop-out rate suggests that these participants still struggle with conscious thoughts (e.g., ³6KRXOG,H[HUFLVHWRGD\´?). Ironically, they still
need conscious thoughts for participation but these thoughts lead to quitting, because they prevent situational cues and nonconscious
processing from becoming the main generator and sustainer of behavior. For them, conscious thoughts become an impediment for rather
than a facilitator of continued participation.
The high rate of lapsing and quitting causes additional problems for conscious processing in the first two stages. Individuals who
lapse or quit altogether after starting a program experience dissonant thoughts (46). Obviously, they could eliminate such thoughts by
continuing their participation, but most people are unable to do it on their own; only about 19% survive the first 5 weeks without a lapse
in participation (60). Unfortunately, from a behavioral perspective, the problem is that people can easily resolve dissonance by cognitive
means or rationalizations. There are, however, ways to facilitate the behavioral resolution. Bator and Bryan (127) reported that when
people were reminded about not living up to their own preaching (i.e., they were hypocrites), they subsequently used the fitness center far
more frequently than participants who were not made feel hypocritical. By living up to their commitments and pledges, participants
avoided feeling dissonant and hypocritical, thereby resolving dissonance by behavioral means. Similarly, making a commitment to
exercise through a signed contract has been shown to have positive long-term effects on workHUV¶H[HUFLVHEHKDYLRU128). Such
commitments allow people to pre-empt or eliminate dissonant thoughts about exercise, which consequently helps them maintain the
activity in a long run. Nevertheless, since lapsing or quitting occurs among most participants (60), dissonance is inevitable and reflects
the difficulties cognitive processing faces in efforts to relegate exercise to nonconscious processing.
The second stage is characterized by both conscious and nonconscious processing. Although conscious processing is still dominant,
especially in early phases of this stage, theoretically, nonconscious processing becomes stronger with time and more frequent repeats of
exercise behavior. Exercise grows more habitual with each repeat because it can increasingly be primed by situational cues, such as the
time of the day, the sight of exercise gear or other exercisers. The role of conscious and nonconscious processing in this stage can be
likened to a person learning to ride a bicycle. Although the conscious is heavily involved in early phases of learning the skill, increased
success at pedaling and balancing the bike reduces the role of conscious processing and enhances that of the nonconscious. However,
wobbling movements when biking quickly activate the conscious mind to become involved in the process to take over the execution of
minute parts of the skill. The back-and-forth between conscious and nonconscious processing goes on till the skill is firmly entrenched
neurologically (91) and behaviorally (61, 83). When this level is achieved, behavior and skill execution become primarily driven by the
nonconscious. At that point, attention and conscious processing are freed up to attend to other internal thoughts or feelings, or other
incidental happenings in the external world when biking.
So it is with exercisers in the second stage. There is back-and-forth between conscious and nonconscious processing. The critical
difference, KRZHYHULVWKDWSDUWLFLSDQWV¶ decisions regarding exercise continue to be strongly influenced by conscious processing (e.g.,
³6KRXOG,H[HUFLVHWRGD\´"HYHQLIthey are making progress toward nonconscious processing. Nonconscious thinking cannot become
dominant if the behavior is not repeated frequently enough; situational cues have to become strong enough to elicit actual behavior, not
just relevant goals (94). 7KLVLVHYLGHQWLQFDVHRI³RFFDVLRQDO´H[HUFLVHUV. When they occasionally H[HUFLVHWKHVHH[HUFLVHUV¶HQJDJHPHQW
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 7 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
has to be initiated and maintained by conscious processing each time. Paradoxically, conscious processing, which is supposed to promote
rational thinking and active participationEULQJVWRRQH¶VPLQGGLIILFXOWTXHVWLRQVHJ³:K\VKRXOG,VDFULILFHP\SHUVRQDOWLPHDQG
freedom for yet another obligatory activity´", and in doing so, blocks frequent participation and the nonconscious from becoming
dominant; exercise habituation does not grow as long as conscious processing is in charge. Evidence indicates that on days when
intentions are strong, habit strength does not predict physical activity (107), suggesting that conscious thoughts undermine habit
development and associated exercise behavior. )RU³RFFDVLRQDO´H[HUFLVHUVQonconscious priming of health-related goals is not enough
to trigger actual behavior on a regular basis (46-47, 94). Using the bicycle analogy, they have not learned the skill sufficiently well and
therefore cannot relegate it to the nonconscious; the net result is that they remain ³RFFDVLRQDO´H[HUFLVHUs. Only 22% of the population has
been able to get through the second stage to the third one and thereby become habitual exercisers (as described below).
Three Psychological Processes
The second stage is the battleground mainly within rather than between conscious and nonconscious processing. While both are deployed
to promote exercise participation, paradoxically they also resist exercise from becoming a regular behavior. This resistance stems from
the interdependent influence of three psychological processes: (1) the general human tendency to try to achieve goals with the least
amount of effort (18, p. 35), (2) self-control GHSOHWLRQGXULQJDGD\¶VZRUNand/or other activities (129-130), and (3) reactance to possible
loss of a sense of personal freedom (50). The first leads to the avoidance of demanding cognitive and physical activities in general, and
thus the selection of the least demanding course of action to achieve a goal. Tasks and activities that require either cognitive or physical
effort are shunned because people expend effort only when they need to (131). The search for the path of least resistance is also seen in
SHRSOH¶VWHQGHQF\WRVXEVWLWXWHYLWDPLQSLOOVIRUH[HUFLVHDn obvious attempt at a shortcut for health (132).
Second, coinciding with the tendency to follow the law of least effort, depletion of self-control resources (129) GXULQJDGD\¶s
stressful work leaves people with diminished mental strength to be harnessed for engagement in demanding physical activity during their
leisure on one hand and to resist temptations for involvement in non-demanding activities (e.g., TV watching) on the other (46-47). The
empirical evidence suggests that the more recently and frequently people have resisted their earlier desires, the less successful they are in
their subsequent attempts to resist or control other desires (98). ³Ego depletion´also leads to reduced pain tolerance, less persistence, and
lowered delay of gratification (96), all of which means it is difficult for conscious processing to make demanding exercise activities
become regular daily behaviors.
It should be noted that experimental studies on self-control depletion have recently been criticized for a lack of consistent findings
across laboratory tasks (133). The validity of this criticism, however, is questionable for several reasons. First, the fact that ego depletion
does not appear to occur in all kinds of laboratory tasks may have more to do with artificial tasks performed in artificial ways (e.g., quick
sequential performance of a second task) in artificial settings, rather than a failure to find support for the existence of the phenomenon
itself. Second, like any other psychological phenomenon, self-control failure (due to previous use of self-control) cannot be expected to
be a universal phenomenon that occurs everywhere all the time; that is, there are conditions under which it is more likely to materialize. It
is the task of empirical research to discover such conditions (154). Third, as Baumeister (129) and others have indicated, self-control can
also be strengthened with judicious use, similar to working on different muscle groups on different days. Fourth, and most importantly,
when thinking of everyday work-related behaviors and leisure activities (e.g., exercise) in a longer-term sequence, self-control depletion
is more likely to be a real phenomenonDVLQGLFDWHGE\+RIPDQQHWDO¶s (98) findings. For example, compared to a quick sequential task
performance in a laboratory, spending 8-9 hours at work, and engaging in self-regulation and self-control during most of this time, is
much more likely to produce the depletion effect subsequently in leisure, especially in regard to such demanding leisure activities as
exercise (47).
But even such depleting conditions of work are not likely to lead to self-control failure in leisure among everyone. Iso-Ahola (47)
theorized that 22% of the population (regular exercisers) may actually use self-control depletion at work to their advantage by
³FRPSHQVDWLQJ´IRULWLQOHLVXUHWKURXJKH[HUFLVH6XFKFRPSHQVDWLRQLVlikely to be easier for regular exercisers than occasional
exercisers, because the former notice more readily cues for compensating activities. A golfer, for example, may notice sunny weather late
in the afternoon while at work, which is then a reminder of the rewards associated with hitting balls or playing.
/HLVXUH¶VFDSDFLW\IRUFRPSHQVDWLRQ becomes more evident upon examination of the characteristics of work and leisure. Work is
typically a conscious, straining and freedom-undermining activity, whereas leisure is more a non-straining and freedom-restoring activity
(48, 134). Although the freedom associated with leisure is generally seen as positive, it is also conducive to the dominating influence of
nonconscious processing (47); unfortunately, nonconscious processing often leads people to seek out WKHOHDVWGHPDQGLQJDFWLYLW\³ODZ
RIOHDVWHIIRUW´) for compensation. The result is that most people end up compensating through such readily cued and non-demanding
activities as TV watching in their leisure.
Third, when faced with choosing to exercise in leisure, the problem of self-control depletion is further compounded by the triggered
psychological reactance and the resultant activation of a sense of freedom or choice. This reactance and the tendency to choose non-
exercise forms of leisure is in part a result of physical activity participation generally being promoted as a ³do-it-or-HOVH´health-
promoting activity and not as fun experience. Thus, participation in any activity perceived as obligatory LQRQH¶VIUHHWLPHLVYLJRURXVO\
resisted unless a restriction on personal freedom is perceived as definite (55, 58), a non-choice option is highlighted (56), persistence is
emphasized over a desirable choice (54), and high commitment to exercise goals has been made (57). In other words, certain restrictions
on personal freedom are seen as everyday facts of life in which non-choice options are accepted and other characteristics (e.g.,
persistence) are instead embraced. Such perceptions can be helpful not RQO\IRUPLWLJDWLQJWKH³QHJDWLYH´HIIHFWV of freedom on choosing
to exercise but reducing the role of conscious processing and enhancing that of the nonconscious. However, overcoming the importance
of freedom in leisure through these and other ways in the second stage is difficult, because freedom is the defining characteristic of
leisure (48, 134). Counterintuitively, the importance of freedom cannot be overcome by increasing choices within an obligatory activity,
as it has been found that people choose sedentary activities over exercise even if they have multiple options for physical activity (135).
Thus, threats to freedom, in combination with the tendency to follow the law of least effort, make it harder for conscious processing to
promote continuity of exercise (Figure 1).
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 8 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
Nonconscious processing is inherently inclined to support the general human tendency to follow the law of least effort by making
people more susceptible to noticing cues and excuses for not engaging in demanding activities, and cues for engaging in non-demanding
activities (47). Recent experimental evidence showed that contextual nonconscious priming of anti-exercise goals decreased exercise, but
nonconscious priming of exercise goals did not increase exercise (94).Thus, nonconscious processing operates to decrease the likelihood
of choosing to exercise; it is easier to motivate people not to be active than to motivate them to become active (46, p. 103). Situational
cues for saving time and for not being active abound in social environments (75, 136). Saving time, of course, is anti-exercise because
exercise takes time, at the minimum 30 min. five times a week.
The above evidence highlights the nature and difficulty of the second stage. Both conscious and nonconscious processing work for
and against exercise. On one hand, conscious processing lays the foundation (e.g., motivation and resolution of goal conflicts) for
nonconscious processing to take over later in the third stage. This foundational work is necessary for the facilitation of regular
engagement so that participation increasingly becomes reliant on situational cues and nonconscious processing. On the other hand,
conscious processing resists continued exercise involvement through the activation of the need for personal freedom and the law of least
effort. In a similar vein, nonconscious processing resists regular participation because it readily responds to cues for non-demanding
DFWLYLW\DQGUHOD[DWLRQLQRQH¶s free time. However, it also works for the advancement of regularity in participation through the
strengthened cue-behavior relationship when exercise activity is repeated frequently. These conflicting tendencies within both conscious
and nonconscious processing are a central reason why most people struggle in the second stage and fail to progress to the third stage of
automaticity of engagement.
Conflicts between and among internal (e.g., motivation) and external (e.g., situational cues, temptations) factors, whether processed
consciously or nonconsciously, reflect the cognitive nature and difficulty of engaging in regular exercise over and above the physical
demands involved. This is begiQQHUV¶DQGRFFDVLRQDOH[HUFLVHUV¶GLOHPPDIf the conflict is between a non-demanding (e.g., TV
watching) and demanding (exercise) activity, the latter has great difficulties in defeating the former. Research (137) has shown that
simply forming a counterhabitual implementation intention to exercise without a strong goal intention will not break the old and
competing habit (TV watching). Extrapolating IURPWKLVVWXG\¶V data, even when a habitual response (watching TV) and an alternative
response (exHUFLVHKDYHDQHTXDOFKDQFHRIEHFRPLQJDQHQDFWHGEHKDYLRUWKHODWWHUORVHVWKLV³FRJQLWLYHKRUVHUDFH´ZLWKRXWDVWURQJ
goal intention or commitment to exercise. Therefore, stronger measures of cognitive control have to be employed in such situations.
Research suggests that visual monitoring WHOOLQJRQHVHOI³GRQ¶WGRLW´LHwatch TV) can aid habit control by enhancing conscious
control processing (138). Simultaneously, inhibitory control training (e.g., brief mindfulness-training) could be employed as it has been
shown to weaken implicit cognition (attentional bias) to competing or unhealthy stimuli (139), that is, TV watching in the above
example.
Individuals who have not yet been able to make exercise habitual in the second stage can benefit from the mindfulness mindset.
Evidence has shown the intention-exercise relationship is greater among mindful individuals and non-habitual exercisers than less
mindful and habitual exercisers (140). Thus, the mindfulness mindset seems to facilitate the effectiveness of intentions by increasing self-
control of and focus on action plans. In contrast, habitual exercisers do not need conscious intentions for their engagement as situational
FXHVDUHVXIILFLHQWWRWULJJHUWKHEHKDYLRU$VKDELWXDOQHVVJURZV)LJXUHSHRSOHEHFRPHPRUHUHOLDQWRQ³FRJQLWLYHVKRUWKDQG´RU
affective associations to drive their engagement (141). Chatzisarantis DQG+DJJHU¶V140) data also demonstrate that mindfulness protects
against the negative influence of counterintentional habits (e.g., habitual binge-drinking) on exercise behavior.
The above findings illustrate the essence of the second stage where individuals strive to move from conscious (mindfulness and
intentions) to nonconscious processing triggered by situational cues. The mindfulness mindset and other cognitive strategies are needed
to enhance conscious control until exercise is repeated frequently enough to be driven by situational cues. This progression is not linear
as people slip in their participation (60), as reflected in the recursive relationships between the three stages (Fig. 2). The back-and-forth
between conscious and nonconscious processing becomes more evident when conscious thoughts for one activity (exercise) and a
nonconscious habit for another (e.g., TV watching) collide. Is it then any wonder why people encounter difficulties in attempts to
progress from conscious to nonconscious processing in the second stage, and why the majority of the population remains permanently
³RFFDVLRQDO´H[HUFLVHUV"A challenge for empirical research is to determine how the two processes can complement one another to help
people move to the third stage.
THIRD STAGE: NONCONSCIOUS PROCESSING AND HABITUAL BEHAVIOR
The third stage, nonconscious processing, is where behavior has become largely driven by nonconscious processing and situational cues.
7KHUHVXOWDQW³IDVWWKLQNLQJ´RU6\VWHP18) is in charge. It is automatic, associative, intuitive, and impulsive, requiring little or no
mental effort and deliberate thought. This level of nonconscious processing can be achieved fairly quickly in simple tasks (e.g., for
reviews of research, see 19, 62), because responses can be repeated easily and frequently. However, it is a different matter with complex
behaviors such as exercise.
It has been shown experimentally that exercise-related contextual (nonconscious) cues do not increase actual exercise participation (94),
even if these cues PDNHH[HUFLVHUV¶UHDFWion time to exercise stimuli faster than that of non-exercisers (142-143). This is not surprising
given that there is a monumental difference between reaction time to a stimulus on a computer screen and going for a 30-minute run or
walk. Nonconscious processing can become the dominant driver of goal pursuit only if behavior has been repeated frequently and
regularly enough, and many more repeats are needed for complex than simple behaviors for the same level of automaticity. As pointed
out earlier, most people are unable to achieve this automaticity (only 19% of beginning exercisers last five weeks without a lapse; 60). In
contrast, however, those who exercise regularly (5 times a week or more) are continuously and gradually building the strength of
situational cues for this behavior. It is these individuals (22% of the population) who have advanced to the third stage, because their
exercise is effectively powered and maintained by situational and contextual cues without conscious deliberation and effort. This is not to
suggest that the third stage is totally nonconscious in regard to exercise. Even regular exercisers have to exert cognitive control from time
to time. But as explained later, such brief detours into conscious thoughts are in service of the dominant tendency of nonconscious
processing to direct exercise behavior at this stage.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 9 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
There is, of course, a multitude of situational cues for exercise in social environments such as the sight of joggers in neighborhoods.
These cues are so ubiquitous that it is impossible not to encounter them. But if these cues and the associated nonconscious processing
were powerful, everyone would be a regular exerciser. What, then, do these cues do to exercisers and non-exercisers? For the former,
they can readily trigger this behavior (if they have not already exercised that day). For the latter group, the effects are binary: either no
effect or arousal of cognitive dissonance (46-47). For non-exercisers, seeing a jogger in a neighborhood is just another environmental cue
lost among many of them because it does not have any personal relevance and meaning. Even if joggers are seen every day and many
times during a day, these cues do not build the strength of the cue-behavior relationship in observers, despite strengthening this particular
cue in and of itself; the problem is that the cue (a jogger) is not associated with a relevant personal behavior. Cues alone do not have the
power to trigger behaviors unless they are linked to specific behaviors through continuous repetition.
It is important to note that the cue-behavior relationship is also bidirectional. Besides cues triggering behaviors, behaviors also cause
changes in cue strength. This phenomenon is particularly important in case of beginning exercisers. As would be expected, a previous
episode of a behavior has an effect on a subsequent episode of the behavior, that is, prior exercise participation is a significant predictor
of subsequent exercise engagement, but only up to a point. Armitage (60) showed that this effect occurred only during the first five
weeks, after which the dropout rate leveled off, suggesting that participation had become habitual after five weeks. What these findings
further, and perhaps more importantly, suggest is that the time (e.g., 5 vs. 12 weeks) when drop-out rates level off is not essential but
rather, that this leveling off indicates the point where previous behavior no longer significantly builds cue strength. In a way, cue strength
has reached maturity and the cue is ready to direct behavior, which has become habitual. Although the influence of previous exercise
participation on cue strength is never-ending, it is clear that the repetition of a behavior is most important during the early phases of
exercise engagement. Literally step-by-step, repeated behavior builds cue strength and finally, the point of no return to non-participation
is achieved. At that juncture, cues and nonconscious processing have taken over the process. For some individuals, this construction work
by previous behavior ceases by the end of the fifth week, for others much later. But it is unclear where that juncture is, how it is achieved,
and what differences exist between various groups in this respect.
A situational cue can also have a temporary negative effect on non-exercisers, especially occasional exercisers. The sight of a jogger
reminds them of the importance of exercise and simultaneously, of personal failure to exercise. The resultant dissonant thoughts have to
be reconciled either behaviorally or cognitively. Since the cue-behavior relationship is weak among these individuals, they are likely to
resolve dissonance by cognitive rationalizations and excuses (46). As people generally are good at rationalizing their behaviors,
dissonance-causing cues can quickly be dismissed, rendering their negative effect short-lived among occasional and non-exercisers. This
dismissal is further facilitated if the cue also activates threats to personal freedom. These threats can be eliminated by indulging in
temptations (144) such as TV watching.
Sometimes, though, a cue and attendant dissonance can have a positive effect on occasional exercisers by making them resolve
dissonance by behavioral means (i.e., going for a run/walk), especially if dissonance is accompanied by feelings of regret (145) and guilt
(146-147). It follows that as the cue-behavior link grows stronger, actual exercise becomes a more likely way for resolving dissonance,
depending on the severity of the dissonance. This topic remains a fruitful area for future research.
Nonconscious processing seeks simplicity
A central characteristic of nonconscious processing is to seek simplicity and concomitantly, to strive to reduce complexity in human
cognitions and behaviors. The nonconscious lives in simplicity (2+2?) because it cannot handle complex cognitive operations (18, 61).
As noted earlier, humans generally shun complexity and demanding cognitive and physical activities. As a result of these tendencies,
complex behaviors (e.g., exercise) can become a part of activity repertoire only if they are reduced in complexity. This reduction in
complexity is achieved through increased habituation (Figure 2). Consequently, exercise becomes increasingly simple both cognitively
and physically. In general, the more frequently a behavior is repeated (and thus the stronger the cue-behavior relationship), the more
readily it occurs in response to its situational and contextual cues, thus becoming more habitual (21). In short, reduced complexity leads
to a greater likelihood of regularity in behavior, other things held constant. All of this means that exercise will not become a regular
activity unless it is reduced in complexity to the level where it is driven by situational cues processed nonconsciously. Reduced
complexity, of course, means simplicity or non-existence of conscious thoughts.
To be sure, even regular exercisers have to juggle mundane living demands (e.g., child care), thus being forced to take short detours
into conscious thoughts, but such temporary barriers are likely to only briefly delay the start of physical activity. The underlying
nonconscious processing for the behavior has already been triggered before a mundane demand surfaces. Theoretically, attempting to
perform a behavior out of its normal sequence can prevent the action from being cued by the preceding action in the sequence (148) and
therefore prevent it from being executed. Such an effect, however, is more likely to occur in simple tasks (e.g., reactions on computer
keyboards) requiring quick sequential responses, whereas the effect is unlikely among regular exercisers due to the high level of
habitualness of the behavior and because of the longer-lasting effect of the triggered cue. That is, DUHJXODUH[HUFLVHU¶VHQJDJHPHQWFDQEH
nonconsciously triggered by the sight of sneakers hours before actually going for a run, and this nonconscious effect does not disappear
with brief detours into unexpected behaviors because the resultant plan to exercise can be held in the anterior medial prefrontal cortex
during a delay period (90). Evidence suggests that people automatically adjust the selection and execution of their behaviors as long as
the relevant goal has been primed (62). Nevertheless, it remains to be determined empirically how long the effect of the earlier-triggered
nonconscious processing is maintained before it has to be reset by contextual cues.
Some forms of physical activity are simple to begin with, such as WKH³walking to the stairs´YHUVXV ³elevator´point of choice.
Although these simple behaviors are not exercise per se, they are useful everyday activities for burning calories in the long run. Placing
VLJQV³7DNHVWDLUV´RU³:LOO\RXWDNHWKHVWDLUV"´QHDUWKHSRLQWRIFKRLFHFDQEHHIIHFWLYHGHSHQGLQJRQSURFHVVLQJWLPHFonstraints
(1497KH³WDNH-stairs´signs become heuristic nonconscious ³UXOHVRIWKXPE´XQGHUWLPHFRQVWUDLQWVDQGlead to greater use of stairs.
Extrapolating from the previously cited research, with repeated choice of stairs, this behavior grows into an automatic action and
becomes cued by the context without the sign. Complexity can, of course, be reduced much faster with these kinds of simple physical
activities than demanding forms of exercise. Nevertheless, the psychological process is the same in both and suggests that even complex
exercise can be reduced into simple forms of action with frequent repeats.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 10 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
Once formed, habits are stubborn. We perform habitual behaviors even when we do not want to engage in them. Graybiel and Smith
(21) reported that rats would run to a reward (food) even when it had become distasteful. Similarly, people eat popcorn in movie theaters
out of habit, regardless of its freshness (150). This level of stubbornness is possible only because ingrained habits are supported by the
EUDLQ¶VPXOWLSOHFLUFXLWVWKDWLQWHUFRQQHFWWKHQHRFRUWH[DQGWKHVWULDWXPZLWKKDELWXDOEHKDYLRUVEHFRPLQJ³FKXQNV´VLQJOHXQLWVRI
brain activity (21, 151). All the while, brain circuits monitor habitual behaviors without our conscious awareness. These findings suggest
that frequent repeats of exercise develop the chunking neural pattern in the brain, which then supports continued participation. To reach
this level of habitualness in exercise, however, requires a long period of time and a great number of repetitions of the behavior. It remains
for future research to determine what the magic number of repetition is for different groups of participants and how it is achieved.
Achieving a high level of habitualness in exercise equates to building a psychological firewall against distractions. It is difficult to
break a strong habit. Automatized behavior patterns are highly resistant to change. Laboratory research suggests that if a newly
developed habit (e.g. going out for drinks after work) that competes with an older habit (e.g., exercise) is blocked, the old one instantly
reappears (21). This is good news for regular exercisers living hectic lifestyles that can give rise to new and interfering habits. Even if
they get side-tracked sometimes, the old habit of exercising quickly resurfaces and gets them back to doing the usual activity. However,
before reaching this level of automaticity and habitualness in exercise, many pairs of shoes probably have to be worn out. But, one by
one, each walking and running step builds automaticity and ultimately makes exercising as easy and mindless as brushing RQH¶Vteeth in
the morning.
It should be noted that even the most avid and regular exercisers occasionally have to rely on conscious processing and cognitive
control because of situational demands. But sudden detours into conscious thoughts do not derail the influence of the dominant
nonconscious processing because the goal of exercising had already been triggered or will quickly be re-triggered after unexpected
behaviors. Furthermore, evidence suggests that when a goal has been primed, people nonconsciously adjust the behaviors available in
their repertoire based on the perceived situation (62). This adjustment becomes easier with the reduced simplicity of exercise achieved
through frequent repeats. The findings that complex everyday behaviors can reliably be primed (for a review of research, see 152) further
support this adjustment process. Coupled with the evidence that habits are stubborn (21), it is clear that conscious thoughts and cognitive
FRQWUROSOD\DUHODWLYHO\PLQRUUROHLQUHJXODUH[HUFLVHUV¶DFWLRQVLQWKHWKLUGVWDJH
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
The scientific evidence accumulated in various areas of human behavior leaves little doubt about the need to be cognitively and
physically active to maintain health. Research supports the use-it-or-lose-it principle, or its variant use-it-and-gain-it, and indicates that
regular physical activity not only reduces the likelihood of major illnesses but also enhances neuroplasticity and neurogenesis (32-34),
thus promoting a better performing and more efficiently running brain. In short, based upon research over the last 30 years, regular
physical activity is the best thing people can do for their health (5). Does this biological and psychological imperative for an active
lifestyle then WUDQVODWHLQWRUDWLRQDOGHFLVLRQPDNLQJDQGGRLQJZKDWLVEHVWIRURQH¶VKHDOWK"The fact that 68% of the population is
overweight or obese and 78% fails to take advantage of the ³IUHHPHGLFLQH´ of exercise suggests that people do not make conscious
decisions that are in their best interest. This is not surprising given the evidence that people are not rational about other matters either,
such as their finances (18, 73, 75).
How do we explain most people¶Vfailure to exercise regularly? More fundamentally: Why do people choose to engage in some
activities but not others? Why do some individuals spend 5 hours daily watching TV while others are active in their leisure? Given that
enactment of behavior always emanates from the operations of the human mind, the answer can only be found in understanding
conscious and nonconscious processing of human affect, cognition and behavior.
The role of conscious versus nonconscious processes in the initiation of action and movement has recently been debated at length, to
the extent that it has divided researchers into two competing camps (81). Although empirical evidence seems to suggest that
nonconscious processing starts a few hundred milliseconds before conscious processing, conscious intentions nevertheless are expressed
before the onset of movement (84). However, there is no clear evidence about the starting point or location for neural processing for
motor movements, with the processing likely beginning in several places simultaneously and involving distinct cortical circuits (20,90).
Evidence further indicates that nonconscious and automatic processing can be suppressed and altered by conscious cognitions (61). The
debate, therefore, about which comes first seems to miss the point, especially because there is plenty of evidence that, to a varying
degree, both processes are jointly responsible for human behavior. The question, then, becomes one of determining how the two
processes work together to influence complex daily behaviors.
In general, the initiation and maintenance of any complex and demanding behavior operates on a continuum of conscious-
nonconscious processing such that when starting a new activity program (e.g., exercise), conscious processing dominates behavioral
engagement. But after countless repeats, the nonconscious takes over and behavior becomes habitual, thereby being sustained in the long
run. The present analysis goes beyond this general idea and proposes a 3-stage model according to which exercise behavior proceeds
from the fully conscious processing in the beginning (1st Stage) to predominantly nonconscious processing and automatic behavior (3rd
Stage). This theory and supporting empirical evidence suggest that most people fail to get past the second stage, conscious-nonconscious-
conscious processing, therefore failing to become regular exercisers. In the third stage, even complex behaviors come to be driven by
situational and contextual cues.
Building exercise infrastructure is an important part of the first stage. Participants discover when, where, how, and with whom to
exercise. Evidence suggests that such an infrastructure has the potential to dramatically increase compliance with vigorous exercise
programs (102). The infrastructure also means that participants set goals for engagement and ensuing outcomes. Exercise goals, however,
cannot conflict with other activity goals and desires for leisure activities (e.g., media), because such a goal conflict brings about a self-
control failure (98). Initial motivation for starting an exercise program appears to be a key for prevention of goal conflicts and
maintenance of activity involvement. Research has shown that SHRSOHZLWK³ZDQW-WR´LQWULQVLFPRWLYDWLRQH[SHULHQFHIHZer and weaker
goal-interfering temptations, perceive fewer obstacles to goal pursuit, and exhibit stronger resistance to conflicting desires (118). In
general, this self-determination motivation leads to self-regulatory success. As conscious self-regulation plays a central role in the first
stage, the acquisition of self-regulatory skills becomes essential. For example, monitoring discrepancies between goals and current
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 11 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
behaviors is important for self-control and goal achievement (100). Unfortunately, most people seem to fail in self-regulation as the
proportion of people surviving the first five weeks of an exercise program without a lapse is less than 20% (60). All of this suggests that
in the first stage, exercise has more to do with conscious self-regulation and conflict resolution than muscle movements.
Although no definite starting and ending points exist for the second stage, the third stage commences and the second stage ends when
exercise has become largely or completely automatic and habitual. Research suggests that this can happen as early as in the fifth week
(60), but if automaticity has not been achieved in six months, exercise is unlikely to ever become habitual. A key issue in the second
stage is whether conscious processing increasingly gives way to nonconscious processing or whether the process continues to be
dominated with such TXHVWLRQVDV³'R,KDYHWLPHIRULW´"³&DQ,GRLW´" These kinds of questions become impediments to attempts to
repeat the behavior, and therefore, to the dominance of nonconscious processing in driving behavioral engagement in response to
situational and contextual cues. Nonconscious thoughts cannot become dominant if behavior is not repeated frequently enough to make
situational cues sufficiently strong to elicit actual behavior (61).
The second stage is paradoxical LQWKDWERWKFRQVFLRXVDQGQRQFRQVFLRXVSURFHVVLQJZRUNIRUDQGDJDLQVWSDUWLFLSDQWV¶DWWHPSWsto
grow into regular exercisers. On one hand, conscious processing lays the foundation (e.g., exercise infrastructure) for the facilitation of
later regular engagement driven by situational cues and nonconscious processing. On the other hand, it resists this progression to
nonconscious processing largely due to the DFWLYDWLRQRI³WKHODZRIOHDVWHIIRUW´(18) and of the need for personal freedom (50). Along
with the general tendency to achieve a goal with the least amount of effort, the depletion of self-control resources (130) during a stressful
working day leaves people with little mental strength for engagement in demanding activities in leisure (e.g., exercise), as well as to resist
temptations for involvement in non-demanding activities, most notably TV watching (47). The problem is compounded by the activation
of a need of a sense of freedom because exercise is promoted and perceived to be a ³GR-it-or-HOVH´DFWLYLW\ When personal choice and
freedom are made cognitively accessible, any activity perceived as obligatory LQRQH¶VIUHHWLPHis vigorously resisted--unless a
restriction on personal freedom is perceived as definite (58, 55) and as a necessary everyday fact of life in which a non-choice option is
accepted and other characteristics (e.g., persistence) instead embraced (54).
Nonconscious processing also operates in the service of the law of least effort by making it more likely that cues will automatically
trigger the adoption of excuses for non-engagement in exercise and trigger participation in less demanding activities. Experimental
evidence (94) has shown that the contextual nonconscious priming of anti-exercise goals decreases exercise, whereas such priming of
exercise-related goals does not increase exercise participation. This supports the idea that nonconscious processing is more likely to
inhibit than facilitate exercise and that it is easier to motivate people not to be active than to become active (46). Nevertheless,
nonconscious processing also works to advance continued participation by strengthening the cue-behavior relationship every time
exercise behavior is repeated. As habitualness grows with repetitionSHRSOHEHFRPHPRUHUHOLDQWRQ³FRJQLWLYHVKRUWKDQGs´RUDIIHFWLYH
associations with exercise (141) to drive the behavior. As a whole, however, the problem remains in that both conscious and
nonconscious processing support and resist exercise. This conflicting tendency within and between the two processes is the key reason
why people struggle in the second stage and are not able to move onward to the third stage.
In the third stage, nonconscious processing drives human behavior. But, tREHFRPHDQDXWRPDWLFDQG³IDVWWKLQNHU´DERXWH[HUFLVH is
difficult, albeit possible, as indicated by the fact that 22% of the population are regular exercisers and their behavior is powered by
situational and contextual cues (46). With their continuous and frequent repeats, these exercisers have built exercise into a powerful and
stubborn habit that is not easily shaken (21). However, regular exercisers occasionally have to rely on conscious thoughts and cognitive
control because of unexpected situational demands. But such brief detours into conscious thoughts do not derail the influence of the
dominant nonconscious processing; if anything, they help nonconscious processing stay on track. Evidence suggests that goals for
complex everyday behaviors (e.g., exercise), and thus these behaviors themselves, can reliably be primed (152) and that people
automatically adjust their behaviors from their available repertoire based upon the perceived situation (62). This adjustment is easier
when exercise is reduced in complexity via frequent repeats. But nonconscious processing can become the dominant driver of goal
pursuit only if behavior has been repeated frequently and regularly enough; in general, many more repeats are needed for complex than
simple behaviors for the same level of automaticity. Continuous exercise builds cue-behavior strength, ultimately leading to the point
where the mere sight of a cue is sufficient to trigger exercise without any conscious deliberations.
Although the influence of previous participation on cue strength is never-ending, research (60) suggests that prior exercise has its
greatest effect on subsequent exercise during the early phases of participation (i.e., the first five week); afterwards, the effect becomes
nonsignificant and levels off. While the leveling-off point can vary individually, it signals that the cue-behavior relationship is now
strong enough for nonconscious processing to be in charge of the behavior. Occasional exercisers, however, are not able to attain this
cue-behavior strength in spite of continuously being exposed to a multitude of exercise cues in social environments (e.g., sight of
joggers); for them, it is easy to dismiss or rationalize these cues away and thereby avoid the cognitive dissonance resulting from believing
in the importance of exercise but not engaging in it. Occasionally, though, exercise cues can spur infrequent exercisers, through
conscious deliberations, to go for a walk or run. A big challenge for future research is to determine how the cue-behavior strength can be
built among occasional exercisers.
Nonconscious processing seeks simplicity because it cannot handle complex cognitive operations (61). It therefore strives to reduce
the cognitive and physical complexity of exercise, and achieves it through increased habituation. When complex behaviors are reduced to
the level of simple behaviors, they are much easier to repeat. This reduction in complexity is an important reason why habituation works
in general and why people can habituate to complex behaviors (e.g., exercise). Research (21, 151) indicates that once a high level of
habitualness has been attained, it is difficult to shake off habits. In short, habits are stubborn. This is good for regular exercisers but bad
for avid TV watchers; if the latter want to become regular exercisers their task is exceedingly difficult. Research has shown that the
stubbornness of habits is rooted in the multiple circuits in the EUDLQ¶VQHRFRUWH[DQGLVUHIOHFWHGE\ the fact that if a new and competing
habit is blocked neurologically, the old habit instantly reappears (21).
But even the most avid exercisers are sometimes faced with everyday demands that disrupt their exercise routine. Although mundane
living arrangements occasionally force regular exercisers into short detours of conscious thinking, these temporary barriers are unlikely
to derail their habitual behavior. This is particularly true if a situational cue has triggered nonconscious processing in regard to exercise
before an unexpected event occurred. It is unclear, though, how long the triggered nonconscious processing will last or can be suppressed
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 12 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
by unexpected events before it has to be reset by another situational cue, or if out-of-sequence exercise has to be temporarily restarted by
conscious thoughts. The way in which nonconscious and conscious processing puts the old habit of exercising back on the track after
disruptions promises to be an interesting area for future research.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
The author thanks Roger C. Mannell and Matthew W. Miller for their insightful comments and suggestions.
1. Powell, K. 1988. Habitual exercise and public health: An epidemiological view. In R. Dishman (Ed.), Exerc adher (pp.15-59). Champaign, Il:
Human Kinetics.
2. Ornstein, R. and Erlich, P. 1989. New world-new mind: Moving toward conscious evolution. New York, NY: Doubleday.
3. Farahmand, B., Broman, G., de Faire, U., Vagero, D., and Ahlbom, A. 2009. Golf: A game of life and death²reduced mortality in Swedish golf
players. Scand J Med Sci Sports 19: 419-424.
4. Lee, I-M. , Hsieh, C., and Paffenbarger, R. 1995. Exercise intensity and longevity in men. The Harvard alumni health study. J Amer Med Assoc 273:
1179-1184.
5. Bassuk, S., Church, T., and Manson, J. 2013. Why exercise works magic. Sci Amer 309: 74-79.
6. Blair, S. 1993. Physical activity, physical fitness, and health. Res Quart Exerc Sport 64: 365-374.
7. Kohl, H., Craig, C., Lambert, E., Inoue, S., Alkandari, J., Leetongin, G., and Kahlmeier, S. 2012. The pandemic of physical inactivity: Global action
for public health. The Lancet 380: 294-305.
8. Lee, I-M., Shiroma, E., Lobelo, F., Puska, P., Blair, S., and Katzmarzyk, P. 2012. Effect of physical inactivity on major noncommunicable diseases
worldwide: An analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. The Lancet 380: 219-229.
9. Moore, S., Lee, I-0:HLGHUSDVV(&DPSEHOO36DPSVRQ-.LWDKD&«3DWHO$$VVRFLDWLRQRIOHLVXUH-time physical activity with risk
of 26 types of cancer in 1.44 million adults. JAMA Internal Med 176: 816-825.
10. %OXPHQWKDO-%DE\DN0'RUDLVZDP\3:DWNLQV/+RIIPDQ%%DUERXU.«6KHUZRRG$([HUFLVHDQGSKDUPDFRWherapy in the
treatment of major depressive disorder. Psych Med 69: 587-596.
11. Hawley, J., Hargreaves, M., Joyner, M., and Zierath, J. 2014. Integrative biology of exercise. Cell 159: 738-749.
12. Talbot, L., Fleg, J., and Metter, J. 2003. Secular trends in leisure-time physical activity in men and women across four decades. Prev Med 37: 52-60.
13. Westerterp, K., and Speakman, J. 2008. Physical activity energy expenditure has not declined since the 1980s and matches energy expenditure of
wild mammals. Int J Obesity 32: 1256-1263.
14. Gebel, K., Bauman, A., and Petticrew, M. 2007. The physical environment and the physical activity, a critical appraisal of review articles. Amer J
Prev Med 32: 361-369.
15. Beaudoin, C. Fernandez, C., Wall, J., and Farley, T. 2007. Promoting healthy eating and physical activity: Short-term effects of a mass media
campaign. Amer J Prev Med 32: 217-223.
16. Knapp, D. 1988. Behavioral management techniques and exercise promotion. In R. Dishman (Ed.), Exerc adher (pp. 203-235). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
17. Tai-Seale, T. 2003. Stage of change specific triggers and barriers to moderate physical activity. Amer J Health Beh 27: 219-227.
18. Kahneman, D. 2011. Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
19. Bargh, J. 2014a Our unconscious mind. Sci Amer 310: 30-37.
20. Dehaene, S. 2014. Consciousness and the brain. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
21. Graybiel, A., and Smith, K. 2014. Good habits, bad habits. Sci Amer 310: 39-43.
22. Hamilton, M., Hamilton, D., and Zderic, T. 2004. Exercise physiology versus inactivity physiology: An essential concept for understanding
lipoprotein lipase regulation. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 32: 161-166.
23. Katzmarzyk, P., Church, T., Craig, C., and Bouchard, C. 2009. Sitting time and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer. Med
Sci Sport Exerc 41: 998-1005.
24. Chang, H-C., Burbridge, H., DQG:RQJ&([WHQVLYHGHHSYHLQWKURPERVLVIROORZLQJSURORQJHGJDPLQJµJDPHU¶VWKURPERVLV¶$FDVH
report. J Med Case Rep 7: 235.
25. Nelson, M., Fiatarone, M, Morganti, C., Trice, I., Greenberg, R., and Evans, W. 1994. Effects of high-intensity strength training on multiple risk
factors for osteoporotic fractures, a randomized controlled trial. J Amer Med Assoc 272: 1909-1914.
26. Fitts, R., Riley, and Widrick, J. 2000. Physiology of a microgravity environment. Invited review: Microgravity and skeletal muscle. J App Physiol
89: 823-839.
27. Freedman, D. 2011. How to fix the obesity crisis. Sci Amer 305: 40-47.
28. Harlow, H., and Harlow, M. 1962. Social deprivation in monkeys. Sci Amer 207: 136-146.
29. Hunt, J. Mc. V. 1969. The challenge of incompetence and poverty. Urbana, Ill: University of Illinois Press.
30. Dennis, W. 1960. Causes of retardation among institutionalized children: Iran. J Genetic Psychol 96: 47-59.
31. Dennis, W., and Najarian, P. 1957. Infant development under environmental handicap. Psychol Monogr 71: No. 7.
32. Bezzola, L., Merillat, S., Gaser, C., and Jancke, L. 2011. Training-induced neural plasticity in golf novices. J Neurosci 31: 12444-12448.
33. Shors, T. 2014. The adult brain makes new neurons, and effortful learning keeps them alive. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 23: 311-318.
34. (ULFNVRQ.9RVV03UDNDVK5%DVDN&6]DER$&KDGGRFN/«Kramer, A. 2011. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and
improves memory. Proc Nat Acad USA 108: 3017-3022.
35. Hoang, T., Koyama, A., Barnes, D., Sidney, S., Jacobs, D., Zhu, N.,«Yaffe, K. 2013. Long-term patterns of low physical activity and cognitive
function in mid-life: the CARDIA study. Alzh Dem 9: 134-135.
36. Oppezzo, M., and Schwartz, D. 2014. Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking. J Exp Psychol: Learn, Mem,
and Cogn 40: 1142-1152.
37. Erickson, K., Banducci, S., Weisntein, A., MacDonald, A., Ferrell, R., Halder,I., Flory, J., and Manuck, S. 2013. The brain-derived neurotrophic
factor V66Met polymorphism moderates an effect of physical activity on working memory performance. Psychol Sci 24: 1770-1779.
38. Verghese, J., Lipton, R., .DW]0+DOO&'HUE\&.XVODQVN\*«%XVFKNH+/HLVXUHDFWLYLWLHVDQGWKHULVNRIGHPHQWLDLQWKHHOGHUO\
New Engl J Med 348: 2508-2516.
39. Wirth, M., Haase, C., Villeneuve, S., Vogel, J., and Jagust, W. 2014. Neuroprotective pathways: Lifestyle activity, brain pathology, and cognition in
cognitively normal older adults. Neurobiol Aging 35: 1873-1882.
40. Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. 2003. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 14: 125-130.
41. Rovio, S. Kareholt, I., Helkala, E-/9LLWDQHQ0:LQEODG%7XRPLOHKWR-«.LYLSHOWR0/HLVXUH-time physical activity at midlife and
WKHULVNRIGHPHQWLDDQG$O]KHLPHU¶VGLVHDVHThe Lancet Neurol 4: 705-711.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 13 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
42. 5RYLR66SXOEHU*1LHPLQHQ/1LVNDQHQ(:LQEODG%7XRPLOHKWR-«.LYLSHOWR07KHHIIHFWRIPLGOLIHShysical activity on
structural brain changes in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 31: 1927-1936.
43. Ybarra, O., Burnstein,, E., Winkielman, P., Keller, M., Manis, M., Chan, E., and Rodriquez, J. 2008. Mental exercising through simple socializing:
Social interaction promotes general cognitive functioning. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34: 248-259.
44. Hertzog, C., Kramer, A., Wilson, R., and Lindenberger, U. 2009. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of
older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Publ Interest 9: 1-65.
45. Andel, R., Crowe, M., Pedersen, N., Fratiglioni,, L., Johansson, B., and Gatz, M. 2008. Physical exercise at midlife and risk of dementia three
decades later: A population-based study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol: Med Sci 63A: 62-66.
46. Iso-Ahola, S. 2013. Exercise: Why it is a challenge for both the nonconscious and conscious mind. Rev Gen Psychol 17: 93-110.
47. Iso-Ahola, S. 2015. Conscious versus nonconscious mind and leisure. Leisure Sci 37: 289-310.
48. Iso-Ahola, S. 1980. The social psychology of leisure and recreation. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
49. 0DQQHOO5=X]DQHN-DQG/DUVRQ5/HLVXUHVWDWHVDQG³IORZ´H[SHULHQFHV7HVWLQJIUHHGRPDQGLQWULQVLFPRWLYDWLRn hypotheses. J
Leisure Res 20: 289-304.
50. Brehm, S. and Brehm, J. 1981. Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. London, UK: Academic Press.
51. Grontved, A., and Hu, F. 2011. Television viewing and risk of type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and all-cause mortaliy. J Amer Med Assoc
305: 2448-2455.
52. McGuire, J., and Botvinick, M. 2010. Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. PNAS Proc Nat Acad Sci USA, 107:
7922-7926.
53. 'HFL(DQG5\DQ57KH³ZKDW´DQG³ZK\´RIJRDOSXUVXLWV+XPDQQHHGVDQGWKHVHOI-determination of behavior. Psychol Inquiry 11: 227-
268.
54. Halkjelsvik, T., and Rise, J. 2014. Persistence motives in irrational decisions to complete a boring task. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 41: 90-102.
55. Laurin, K., Kay, A., and Fitzsimons, G. 2012. Reactance versus rationalization: Divergent responses to policies that constrain freedom. Psychol Sci
23: 205-209.
56. Schrift, R., and Parker, J. 2014. Staying the course: The option of doing nothing and its impact on postchoice persistence. Psychol Sci 25: 772-780.
57. Fishbach, A. and Dhar, R. 2005. Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. J Consum. Res. 32: 370-377.
58. Houser-Marko, L. and Sheldon, K. 2006 Motivating behavioral persistence: The self-as-doer construct. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 32: 1037-1049.
59. Bear, A., and Bloom, P. 2016. A simple task uncovers a postdictive illusion of choice. Psychol Sci 27: 914-922.
60. Armitage, C. 2005. Can the theory of planned behavior predict the maintenance of physical activity? Health Psychol 24: 235-245.
61. Baumeister, R., Masicampo, E., and Vohs, K. 2011. Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annual Rev Psychol 62: 331-361.
62. Custers, R., and Aarts, H. 2010. The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science 329: 47-50.
63. .LP+DQG6KHUPDQ'³([SUHVV\RXUVHOI´&XOWXUHDQGWKHHIIHFWRIVHOI-expression on choice. J Pers Soc Psychol 92: 1-11.
64. Savani, K., Markus, H., Naidu, N., Kumar, S., and Berlia, N. 2010. What counts as a choice? U. S. Americans are more likely than Indians to
construe actions as choices. Psychol Sci 21: 391-398.
65. Weinstein, N. 1993. Testing four competing theories of health protective behavior. Health Psychol 12: 324-333.
66. Schwarzer, R. 2001. Social-cognitive factors in changing health-related behaviors. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10: 47-51.
67. Mannell, R. , and Loucks-$WNLQVRQ$:K\GRQ¶WSHRSOHGRZKDW¶V³JRRG´IRUWKHP"&RPSDULQJWKHSV\FKRORJLHVRIQRQSDUWLFLSDWLRQLQ
leisure, health and exercise behaviors. In E. Jackson (Ed.), Constraints to leisure (pp. 221-232). State College, PA: Venture.
68. Stroebe, W., van Koningsbruggen, G., Papies, E., and Aarts, H. 2013. Why most dieters fail but some succeed: A goal conflict model of eating
behavior. Psychol Rev 120: 110-138.
69. Kubey, R., and Csiksentmihalyi, M. 2002. Television addiction is no mere metaphor. Sci Amer 286: 74-80.
70. Kaplan, S., and Berman, M. 2010. Directed attention as a common resource for executive function and self-regulation. Persp Psychol Sci 5: 43-57.
71. Bargh, J. 1997. Reply to commentaries. In R. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social psychology (pp. 231-246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
72. Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T., and Aarts, H. 2007. Effects of priming and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In J. Bargh (Ed.), Social
psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 51- 132). Philadelphia, PA: Psychological Press.
73. Lerner, J., Small, D., and Loewenstein, G. 2004. Heart strings and purse strings, carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychol Sci
15: 337-341.
74. Antonacci, G. 2014. Dual momentum investing: An innovative strategy of higher returns with lower risk. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
75. Zhong, C., and De Voe, S. 2010. You are how you eat: Fast food and impatience. Psychol Sci 21, 619-622.
76. Thaler, R., and Sunstein, C. 2008. Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
77. Dijksterhuis, A., and Aarts, H. 2010. Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual Rev Psychol 61: 467-490.
78. Schwartz, N. 2010. Feelings-as-information theory. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology
(pp. 289-308). New York, NY: Sage.
79. Evans, S., and Stanovich, K. 2013. Duel-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Persp Psychol Sci 8: 223-241.
80. Sherman, J., Gawronski, B., and Trope, Y. 2014 (Eds.). Dual-process theories of social mind. New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
81. Newell, B., and Shanks, D. 2014. Unconscious influences on decision making: A critical review. Beh Brain Sci 37: 1-61.
82. Aarts, H., and Dijksterhuis, A. 2003. The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 84: 18-28.
83. Wood, W., Tam, L., and Witt, M. 2005. Changing circumstances, disrupting habits. J Pers Soc Psychol 88: 918-933.
84. Libet, B., Gleason, C., Wright, E., and Pearl, D. 1983a. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-
potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106: 623-642.
85. Miller, J., Shepherdson, P., and Treyena, D. 2011. Effects of clock monitoring on electroencephalographic activity: Is unconscious movement
initiation an artifact of the clock? Psychol Sci 22: 103-109.
86. Frieze, M., Hofmann, W., and Wanke, M. 2008. When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in
predicting food choice and consumption behavior. Bri J Soc Psychol 47: 397-419.
87. Libet, B., Wright, E., and Gleason, C. 1983b. Preparation or intention-to-act in relation to pre-event potentials recorded at the vertex. Electr Clin
Neuropsychol 56: 367-372.
88. Fiedler, K., Bluemke, M., and Unkelbach, C. 2009. Exerting control over allegedly automatic associative processes. In J. Forgas, R. Baumeister, &
D. Tice (Eds.),The psychology of self-regulation: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes (pp. 249-269). New York, NY: Psychological
Press.
89. Van Leeuwen, M., van Baaren, R., Martin, D., Dijksterhuis, A., and Bekkering, H. 2009. Executive functioning and imitation: Increasing working
memory load facilitates behavioral imitation. Neuropsychol 47: 3265-3270.
90. Haggard, P. 2008. Human volition: Towards a neuroscience of will. Nature Rev Neurosci 9: 934-946.
91. Yarrow, K., Brown, P., and Krakauer, J. 2009. Inside the brain of an elite athlete: The neural processes that support high achievement in sports.
Nature Rev Neurosci 10: 585-596.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 14 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
92. Bargh, J, and Morsella, E. 2008. The unconscious mind. Pers Psychol Sci 3: 73-79.
93. Wheeler, S., DeMarree, K., and Petty, R. 2014. Understanding prime-to-behavior effects: Insights from the active-self account. In D. Molden (Ed.),
Understanding priming effects in social psychology (pp. 114-128). New York, NY: Guildford Press.
94. Iso-Ahola, S., and Miller, M. 2016. Contextual priming of a complex behavior: Exercise. Psychol Consc: Theory Res Pract 3: 258-269.
95. Locke, E., and Latham, G. 2006. New directions in goal-setting theory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15: 265-268.
96. Schmeichel, B., and Vohs, K. 2009. Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. J Pers Soc Psychol 96: 770-
782.
97. Logel, C., and Cohen, G. 2012. The role of the self in physical health: Testing the effect of a values-affirmation intervention on weight loss. Psychol
Sci 23: 53-55.
98. Hofmann, W., Vohs, K., and Baumeister, R. 2012. What people desire, feel conflicted about, and try to resist in everyday life. Psychol Sci 23: 582-
588.
99. Carraro, N., and Gaudreau, P. 2015. Predicting physical activity outcomes during episodes of academic goal conflict: The differential role of action
planning and coping planning. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 41: 1291-1305.
100. Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., and Teper, R. 2014. Exploring the mechanisms of self-control improvement. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 23: 302-307.
101. Botvinick, M., and Braver, T. 2015. Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Rev Psychol 66: 83-113.
102. Gollwitzer, P. 1999. Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. Amer Psychol 54: 493-503.
103. Anderson, E., Wojcik, J., Winett, R., and Williams, D. 2006. Social-cognitive determinants of physical activity: The influence of social support, self-
efficacy, outcome expectations, and self-regulation among participants in a church-based health promotion study. Health Psychol 25: 510-520.
104. Sheeran, P. 2002. Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology 12: 1-36. Chichester,
UK: Wiley.
105. Sniehotta, F., Scholz, U., and Schwarzer, R. 2005. Bridging the intention-behavior gap: Planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption
and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychol Health, 20: 143-160.
106. Gollwitzer, P., and Sheeran, P. 2006. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and process. Adv Exper Soc
Psychol 38: 69-119.
107. Rebar, A., Elavsky, S., Maher, J., Doerksen, S., and Conroy, D. 2014. Habits predict physical activity on days when intentions are weak. J Sport
Exer Psychol 36: 157-165.
108. De Bruin, M., Sheeran, P., Kok, G., Hiemstra, A., Prins, J., Hospers, H., and van Breukelen, G. 2012. Self-regulatory processes mediate the
intention-behavior relation for adherence and exercise behaviors. Health Psychol 31: 695-703.
109. Botvinick, M., Braver, T., Barch, D., Carter, C., and Cohen, J. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108: 624-652.
110. Hofmann, W., Gschwender, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R., and Schmitt, M. 2008. Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an
individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. J Pers Soc Psychol 95: 962-977.
111. Hofmann, W., Friese, M., and Roefs, A. 2009. Three ways to resist temptation: The independent contributions of executive attention, inhibitory
control and affect regulation to the impulse control of eating behavior. J Exper Soc Psychol 45: 431-435.
112. Vohs, K., Baumeister, R., and Ciarocco, N. 2005.Self-regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory resource depletion impairs impression
management and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory resources. J Pers Soc Psychol 88: 632-657.
113. Myrseth,K., and Fishbach, A. 2009. Self-control, a function of knowing when and how to exercise restraint. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18: 247-252.
114. Shah, J., Friedman, R., and Kruglanski, A. 2002. Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. J Pers Soc Psychol 83:
1261-1280.
115. Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B., and Baddeley, A. 2012. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci 16: 174-180.
116. Daly, M., McMinn, D., and Allan, J. 2013. A biodirectional relationship between physical activity and executive function in older adults. Brit Med J
347: 6750-6762.
117. Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H., Dolan, R., and Frith, C. 2007. How the brain translates money into force: A
neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science 316: 904-906.
118. 0LO\DYVND\D0,Q]OLFKW0+RSH1DQG.RHVWQHU56D\LQJ³QR´WRWHPSWDWLRQWant-to motivation improves self-regulation by
reducing temptation rather than by increasing self-control. J Pers Soc Psychol 109: 677-693.
119. Mann, T., and Ward, A. 2007. Attention, self-control, and health behaviors. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16: 280-283.
120. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Soenens, B., and Lens, W. 2004. How to become a persevering exerciser? Providing a clear, future intrinsic goal in an
autonomy ±supportive way. J Sport Exer Psychol 26: 232-249.
121. Gardner, B., and Lally, P. 2013. Does intrinsic motivation strengthen physical activity habit? Modeling relationships between self-determination,
past behavior, and habit strength. J Beh Med 36: 488-497.
122. Iso-Ahola, S., and St. Claire, B. 2000. Toward a theory of exercise motivation Quest 52: 131-147.
123. Deci, E., Koestner, R., and Ryan, R. 1999. A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.
Psychol Bull 125: 627-668.
124. Bandura, A., and Cervone, D. 1983. Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. J Pers Soc
Psychol 45: 1017-1028.
125. Schunk, D. 1989. Self-efficacy and achievement behaviors. Educ Psychol Rev 1: 173-208.
126. Dishman, R., and Buckworth, J. 1997. Adherence to physical activity. In W. Morgan (Ed.), Physical activity and mental health (pp. 63-80).
Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
127. Bator, R., and Bryan, A. 2007, August. Revised hypocrisy manipulation to induce commitment to exercise. Poster presented at the annual conference
of the American Psychological Association. San Francisco, CA.
128. Royer, H., Stehr, M., and Sydnor, J. 2015. Incentives, commitments and habit formation in exercise: Evidence from a field experiment with workers
at a Fortune-500 company. Amer Econ J: Appl Econ 7: 51-84.
129. Baumeister, R. 2015. Conquer yourself, conquer the world. Sci Amer 312: 61-65.
130. Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. 1998. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol 74: 1252-
1265.
131. Bijleveld, Custers, and Aarts, H. 2012. Human reward pursuit: From rudimentary to higher-level functions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 21: 194-199.
132. Chiou, W., Yang, C., and Wan, C. 2011. Ironic effects of dietary supplementation: Illusory invulnerability created by taking dietary supplements
licenses health-risk behaviors. Psychol Sci 22: 1081-86.
133. Carter, E., Kofler, L., Forster, D., and McCullough, M. 2015. A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to
rely on a limited resource. J Exp Psychol: General 144: 796-815.
134. Kleiber, D., Walker, G., and Mannell, R. 2011. A social psychology of leisure (2nd ed.). State College, PA: Venture.
135. Vara, L., and Epstein, L. 1993. Laboratory assessment of choice between exercise or sedentary behaviors. Res Quart Exer Sport 64: 356-360.
136. Gollwitzer, P., Sheeran, P., Trotschel, R., and Webb, T. 2011. Self-regulation of priming effects on behavior. Psychol Sci 22: 901-907.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 15 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
137. Adriaanse, M., Gollwitzer, P., De Ridder, D., De Witt, J., and Kroese, F. 2011. Breaking habits with implementation intentions: A test of underlying
processes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 37: 502-513.
138. 4XLQQ-3DVFRH$:RRG:DQG1HDO'&DQ¶WFRQWURO\RXUVHOI"0RQLWRUWKRVHEDGKDELWV Pers Soc Psychol Bull 36: 499-511.
139. Sheeran, P., Gollwitzer, P., and Bargh, J. 2013. Nonconscious processes and health. Health Psychol 32: 460-472.
140. Chatzisrantis, N., and Hagger, M. 2007. Mindfulness and intention-behavior relationship within the theory of planned behavior. Pers Soc Psychol
Bull 33: 663-676.
141. Kiviniemi, M., Voss-Humke, A., and Seifert, A. 2007. How do I feel about the behavior? The interplay of affective associations with behaviors and
cognitive beliefs as influences on physical activity behavior. Health Psychol 26: 152-158.
142. Bluemke, M., Brand, R., Schweizer, G., and KaKOHUW'([HUFLVHPLJKWEHJRRGIRUPHEXW,GRQ¶WIHHOJRRGDERXWLW'RDXWRPDWLF
associations predict exercise behavior? J Sport Exer Psychol 32: 137-153.
143. Neal, D., Wood, W., Labrecque, J., and Lally, P. 2012. How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. J Exp Soc
Psychol 48: 492-498.
144. Leadner, N., Shah, J., and Chartrand, T. 2009. Moments of weakness: The implicit context dependencies of temptations. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35:
853-866.
145. Abraham, C., and Sheeran, P. 2004. Deciding to exercise: The role of anticipated regret. Bri J Health Psychol 9: 269-278.
146. Carlsmith, J., and Gross, A. 1969. Some effects of guilt on compliance. J Pers Soc Psychol 11: 232-239.
147. Konecni, V. 1972. Some effect of guilt on compliance: A field replication. J Pers Soc Psychol 23: 30-32.
148. Dismukes, R. 2012. Prospective memory in workplace and everyday situations. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 21: 215-220.
149. Suri, G., Sheppes, G., Leslie, S., and Gross, J. 2014. Stairs or escalator? Using theories of persuasion and motivation to facilitate healthy decision
making. J Exp Psychol: Appl 20: 295-302.
150. Neal, D., Wood, W., Wu, M., and Kurlander, D. 2011. The pull of the past: When do habits persist despite conflict with motives? Pers Soc Psychol
Bull 37: 1428-1437.
151. Smith, K. and Graybiel, A. 2016. Habit formation. Dial Clin Neurosci 18: 33-43.
152. Bargh, J. 2014b. The historical origins of priming as the preparation of behavioral responses: Unconscious carryover and contextual influences of
real-world importance. In D. Molden (Ed.), Understanding priming effects in social psychology (pp. 218-233). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
153. Tate, L., Tsai, P-F, Landes, R., Rettiganti, M., and Lefler, L. 2015. Temporal discounting rates and their relation to exercise behavior in older adults.
Physiol. Beh.152: 295-299.
154. Iso-Ahola, S. 2017. Reproducibility in psychological science: When do psychological phenomena exist? Front. Psychol. 8: 879, 1-16.
Dilemma for Human Mind Journal of Nature and Science (JNSCI), 3(6):e384, 2017
ISSN 2377-2700 | www.jnsci.org/content/384 16 J Nat Sci, Vol.3, No.6, e384, Jun 2017
... [7] It is more a cognitive than physical battle that most people have trouble winning. [8] Ultimately, it is an individual who decides to go for a 30 min. walk, but this decision can easily be "vetoed" nonconsciously [9][10][11] or consciously. ...
... walk, but this decision can easily be "vetoed" nonconsciously [9][10][11] or consciously. [7][8]11] Because every decision to exercise is individually made, it is hardly surprising that media campaigns have been ineffective in promoting physical activity and why interventions at the population level have generally failed, with the number of active participants having remained unchanged over decades. [12] This suggests that interventions would have to be individually tailored for them to be successful. ...
... Although a review of the reported research is beyond the scope of the present paper, it can be found elsewhere. [8] Several points from this research are relevant for understanding why most people choose not to exercise regularly. First, although researchers have recently debated the relative superiority of conscious vs. nonconscious processing in human decision-making in general [14], there is no question about the veracity of empirical evidence for the influence of both. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To explain the global pandemic of physical inactivity. An innate human tendency to choose an alternative that leads to the same goal with the least amount of effort is a major obstacle for sustained physical activity. A path of least resistance for goal achievement is selected because it requires less effort and energy, causes less cognitive strain, and provides faster gratification. Physical activity, in contrast, demands cognitive and physical energy, is cognitively straining, and necessitates delay of gratification. On first glance, then, it is a small wonder that physical inactivity has become a global pandemic. Theory and Evidence: While the human mind, through its conscious and nonconscious processing, inherently works against physical activity, it can be harnessed to make sustained physical activity possible. Conscious processing works against exercise when it makes people think, "should or should I not go for a run/walk"? Nonconscious processing also works against exercise because it, in and of itself, is inclined to select a path of least resistance and immediate gratification (e.g., TV watching). Yet, nonconscious processing can be made to work for physical activity when exercise is continuously repeated as a response to a situational cue (e.g., sneakers placed next to a door) without cognitive deliberations. Constant repeats of the same physical activity strengthen the cue-behavior link and eventually make the behavior nonconsciously driven and automatic. Thus, paradoxically, nonconscious processing seeks to make demanding and effortful activities paths of least resistance through constant repeats of behavior. Conclusion: As exercise is more of a cognitive than physical battle, delegation of the decision to exercise to nonconscious processing increases the likelihood of sustained physical activity. But if the activity is not repeated with regularity, any decision to engage in physical activity has to rely on conscious thoughts, which, at best, can make people only "occasional" exercisers. Practical Implications: Conscious thoughts, however, can be used to serve nonconscious processing when one's environment is rearranged to maximize situational cues for exercise and minimize cues for competing activities. Another important (conscious) strategy is to build an exercise infrastructure via if-then plans of when, where, how, and with whom to exercise. These implementation intentions quickly become nonconsciously operated and automatic, thus enhancing the likelihood of sustained physical activity. In this process, personal physicians can play a major role.
... However, there are logical reasons to expect nonconsciously experienced phenomena to be more replicable because of people's general tendency to delegate conscious thoughts to nonconsciously processed operations (28,37). The more frequently thoughts and behaviors are repeated, the more automatic and nonconscious they become (19,20,21). As nonconscious thoughts are cognitively nondemanding, they are less liable to conscious interference, and thus, other things being equal, more repeatable and replicable. ...
... If we are unable to persuade most people to get vaccinated even when facing serious consequences from not doing it, what hope is there for getting the 78% segment of the population that is sedentary to start exercising regularly? (19,20,21). A lot of original studies and constructive replications remain to be funded and conducted. ...
Article
Full-text available
As there are no universal constants in psychological, medical and economic sciences, only constructive-phenomenon replications are meaningful. Yet, psychologists continue to perform direct replications, as evidenced by recent preregistered multilab attempts at exact replications of the ego depletion effect. Statistics are driving the replication movement into a ditch because of an overemphasis on the determination of statistical magnitude of effects while ignoring commonsense magnitude and other criteria for evaluating phenomena’s validity, reliability, and viability. The nature of the human mind and the variability of psychological phenomena pose difficult challenges for the scientific method and insurmountable obstacles for precise replications in psychological sciences. The situation is no better in medical and economic sciences. The interaction effect of person (genetics) and environment (lifestyle) calls for constructive replications to determine, for example, drugs’ efficacy as a function of group and individual differences. The vaccine-vaccination paradox is an interesting case because psychological and medical sciences meet at this intersection. In all fields, science advances by theory building and model expansion, not by replication tests of statistical hypotheses. Rigorous logical and theoretical analysis always precedes and guides good empirical tests. The nonexistence of an effect is not viable if it can withstand rigorous logical and theoretical analyses. Empirical studies are mainly evaluated for their theoretical relevance and importance, not their success or failure to exactly reproduce the original findings.
... It has been argued that such demanding behaviors as exercise cannot be a choice among other leisure activities (Iso-Ahola, 2013, 2017a. Instead, when freedom of choice is suppressed or eliminated altogether by a long-term decision to undertake this behavior no matter what (e.g., going for a walk/run-rain or shine), nonconscious processing and the attendant automaticity of action is enhanced. ...
... An intriguing aspect of autonomy is that even if daily behaviors are presently viewed as obligatory, they can be turned into intrinsically motivating activities. For example, it is known that regular runners develop personal competence and expertise about their activity (e.g., subscribing to running magazines and buying expensive shoes), which increasingly helps them see running as their freely chosen activity, even if they originally had made a forced choice, or a negative choice, to run every day (Iso-Ahola, 2013, 2017a. A sense of autonomy grows with increased competence (Sheldon et al., 1996). ...
Article
Full-text available
Much of everyday life consists of obligatory long-term behaviors, from work itself to doing dishes. Although some activities (e.g., exercise) are harder than others, even the seemingly easy activities can turn into hard ones when repeated monotonously day after day. It is proposed that this paradox has its roots in two fundamental human tendencies: (a) following the path of least resistance and (b) avoiding monotony, boredom, and even stimulus deprivation. How does the human mind operate in the presence of these tendencies to get everyday tasks and activities completed? The first tendency is manifested in gradual reduction of conscious processing and incremental increase in nonconscious processing. Constant repeats of demanding behaviors reduce cognitive strain and energy consumption, enhancing automaticity through the strengthened cue-behavior link. Automaticity in turn makes task performance easier and more efficient, resulting in a greater likelihood of getting everyday behaviors done. However, the sheer repeating of behavior is not enough; rather, a key is to repeat-with-variety, which posits that conscious interjection of variability into routine patterns facilitates the completion of both demanding and nondemanding behaviors in the long run. It is important, however, that such conscious intervention does not activate a sense of freedom about engagement because if it did, the elevated sense of freedom would undermine attempts to repeat behaviors and complete tasks. Understanding task completion also requires a consideration of the object of consciousness: Being unconscious of the mental processes motivating an action but conscious of the experience of doing the action.
... Unhealthy lifestyle factors account for 53% of all years of life lost prior to the age of 65 years [1]. Effective behavior-altering interventions can reduce these preventable deaths, but most interventions do not reach their potential and produce only limited, short-lived effects, as individuals often struggle to maintain new health behaviors over time [1][2][3]. A widely recognized approach for maintaining healthy behaviors is to establish a habit, which is defined as a frequently repeated behavior occurring as a reflexive response to a contextual cue [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The intensive data typically collected by mobile health (mHealth) apps allows factors associated with persistent use to be investigated, which is an important objective given users’ well-known struggles with sustaining healthy behavior. Objective Data from a commercial meditation app (n=14,879; 899,071 total app uses) were analyzed to assess the validity of commonly given habit formation advice to meditate at the same time every day, preferably in the morning. Methods First, the change in probability of meditating in 4 nonoverlapping time windows (morning, midday, evening, and late night) on a given day over the first 180 days after creating a meditation app account was calculated via generalized additive mixed models. Second, users’ time of day preferences were calculated as the percentage of all meditation sessions that occurred within each of the 4 time windows. Additionally, the temporal consistency of daily meditation behavior was calculated as the entropy of the timing of app usage sessions. Linear regression was used to examine the effect of time of day preference and temporal consistency on two outcomes: (1) short-term engagement, defined as the number of meditation sessions completed within the sixth and seventh month of a user’s account, and (2) long-term use, defined as the days until a user’s last observed meditation session. ResultsLarge reductions in the probability of meditation at any time of day were seen over the first 180 days after creating an account, but this effect was smallest for morning meditation sessions (63.4% reduction vs reductions ranging from 67.8% to 74.5% for other times). A greater proportion of meditation in the morning was also significantly associated with better short-term engagement (regression coefficient B=2.76, P
Article
Full-text available
Humans evolved to become dependent on physical activity for their survival, but they have not evolved to exercise today. Because survival in modern society is heavily reliant on conscious thinking, most people (54%) have evolved away from physical activity and become occasional exercisers. This transition from non-conscious to conscious processing prevents people from capitalizing on evolution’s wisdom for survival and wellbeing as they consciously deliberate on the utility of health practices to achieve certain outcomes (e.g., weight loss). Unlike in early times, people today have a choice of not engaging in physical activity and yet surviving. As a result, they struggle with the question whether the gains from exercising are greater than losses from not doing it, weighing positive gains and losses against negative gains and losses. Such conscious deliberations, however, can easily be overridden by solving cognitive dissonance (e.g., “exercise is good for my health” vs. “I don’t exercise”) through conscious rationalizations and non-conscious dismissal. Today’s exercise problem can only be solved by individually acquiring the mindset of early times of evolution when the initiation of physical activity was largely a matter of non-conscious thoughts and feelings.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined whether patterns of self-organization in physical activity (PA) predicted long-term success in a yearlong PA intervention. Increased moderate to vigorous PA (MVPA) was targeted in insufficiently active adults ( N = 512) via goal setting and financial reinforcement. The degree to which inverse power law distributions, which are reflective of self-organization, summarized (a) daily MVPA and (b) time elapsed between meeting daily goals (goal attainment interresponse times) was calculated. Goal attainment interresponse times were also used to calculate burstiness, the degree to which meeting daily goals clustered in time. Inverse power laws accurately summarized interresponse times, but not daily MVPA. For participants with higher levels of MVPA early in the study, burstiness in reaching goals was associated with long-term resistance to intervention, while stochasticity in meeting goals predicted receptiveness to intervention. These results suggest that burstiness may measure self-organizing resistance to change, while PA stochasticity could be a precondition for behavioral malleability.
Article
This 8‐week longitudinal experience sampling study with N = 346 participants (n = 242 completers) of a high‐intensity interval training program explored how goal focus (i.e., a focus on the process vs. outcome of goal pursuit) is related to procrastination and successful goal pursuit. Specifically, the study investigated the association between goal focus and participants' adherence to their workout plans (i.e., procrastination), their immediate experience during the workout (e.g., pleasantness), and their intentions to work out when they were procrastinating. Moreover, the study examined how process and outcome focus are linked to overall goal achievement, workout satisfaction, and objective fitness gains. Converging with previous research, results suggest that adopting a process focus is adaptive. In addition, the analyses revealed some positive relationships between outcome focus and immediate indicators of successful goal pursuit. However, in contrast to process focus, outcome focus was not related to the overall measures and even seemed maladaptive once people were procrastinating.
Article
Full-text available
Social experience has not figured prominently in the social psychology of leisure in the past, though the qualities of leisure experience itself—feelings of freedom, intrinsic interest, self-determination, and enjoyment—have had a defining and guiding role. The experience design movement has revitalized prospects for leisure service management based to some extent on this earlier work; but both experience design and basic social psychological research into the dynamics and effects of leisure experience might well give more attention to the nature of self in social contexts, the dynamics of social engagement including “shared flow,” and the integrative and disintegrative effects of enjoyable social interaction. Experience design can indeed protect personal autonomy and concern itself with “downstream effects,” but investigators would be wise to consider both personal development and social integration in the process, preferably with participatory action research that monitors social experience.
Article
Full-text available
Scientific evidence has recently been used to assert that certain psychological phenomena do not exist. Such claims, however, cannot be made because (1) scientific method itself is seriously limited (i.e., it can never prove a negative); (2) non-existence of phenomena would require a complete absence of both logical (theoretical) and empirical support; even if empirical support is weak, logical and theoretical support can be strong; (3) statistical data are only one piece of evidence and cannot be used to reduce psychological phenomena to statistical phenomena; and (4) psychological phenomena vary across time, situations and persons. The human mind is unreproducible from one situation to another. Psychological phenomena are not particles that can decisively be tested and discovered. Therefore, a declaration that a phenomenon is not real is not only theoretically and empirically unjustified but runs counter to the propositional and provisional nature of scientific knowledge. There are only “temporary winners” and no “final truths” in scientific knowledge. Psychology is a science of subtleties in human affect, cognition and behavior. Its phenomena fluctuate with conditions and may sometimes be difficult to detect and reproduce empirically. When strictly applied, reproducibility is an overstated and even questionable concept in psychological science. Furthermore, statistical measures (e.g., effect size) are poor indicators of the theoretical importance and relevance of phenomena (cf. “deliberate practice” vs. “talent” in expert performance), not to mention whether phenomena are real or unreal. To better understand psychological phenomena, their theoretical and empirical properties should be examined via multiple parameters and criteria. Ten such parameters are suggested.
Article
Full-text available
To test effects of contextual priming (both conscious and nonconscious) in relation to complex human behavior, the present studies investigated (1) whether nonconscious priming of exercise and antiexercise goals can increase and decrease exercise, respectively; (2) whether conscious priming of exercise goals can increase exercise; and (3) how nonconscious and conscious priming of exercise goals compare in effectiveness to influence exercise. Results showed that nonconscious priming of antiexercise goals decreased exercise, but nonconscious priming of exercise goals did not increase exercise. Conversely, conscious priming of exercise goals increased exercise. Thus, results suggest that exercise can be decreased via nonconscious priming of antiexercise goals but increased by conscious priming of exercise goals. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Habits, both good ones and bad ones, are pervasive in animal behavior. Important frameworks have been developed to understand habits through psychological and neurobiological studies. This work has given us a rich understanding of brain networks that promote habits, and has also helped us to understand what constitutes a habitual behavior as opposed to a behavior that is more flexible and prospective. Mounting evidence from studies using neural recording methods suggests that habit formation is not a simple process. We review this evidence and take the position that habits could be sculpted from multiple dissociable changes in neural activity. These changes occur across multiple brain regions and even within single brain regions. This strategy of classifying components of a habit based on different brain signals provides a potentially useful new way to conceive of disorders that involve overly fixed behaviors as arising from different potential dysfunctions within the brain's habit network.
Article
Full-text available
Do people know when, or whether, they have made a conscious choice? Here, we explore the possibility that choices can seem to occur before they are actually made. In two studies, participants were asked to quickly choose from a set of options before a randomly selected option was made salient. Even when they believed that they had made their decision prior to this event, participants were significantly more likely than chance to report choosing the salient option when this option was made salient soon after the perceived time of choice. Thus, without participants' awareness, a seemingly later event influenced choices that were experienced as occurring at an earlier time. These findings suggest that, like certain low-level perceptual experiences, the experience of choice is susceptible to "postdictive" influence and that people may systematically overestimate the role that consciousness plays in their chosen behavior.
Article
Full-text available
To successfully pursue a goal in the face of temptation, an individual must first identify that she faces a self-control conflict. Only then will the individual exercise self-control to promote goal pursuit over indulging in temptation. We propose a new model that distinguishes between the problems of conflict identification and those of conflict resolution. We then review research on the factors that influence conflict identification and those that determine conflict resolution.
Article
Six studies explore the role of goal shielding in self-regulation:by examining how the activation of focal goals to which the individual is committed inhibits the accessibility, of alternative goals. Consistent evidence was found for such goal shielding, and a number of its moderators were identified: Individuals' level of commitment to the focal goal, their degree of anxiety and depression, their need for cognitive closure, and differences in their goal-related tenacity. Moreover, inhibition of alternative goals was found to be, more pronounced when they serve the same overarching purpose as the focal goal, but lessened when the alternative goals facilitate focal goal attainment. Finally; goal shielding was shown to have beneficial consequences for goal pursuit and attainment.