Content uploaded by Tjang Chandra
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Tjang Chandra on Jun 19, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
Content uploaded by Tjang Chandra
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Tjang Chandra on Jun 19, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
IOSR Journal of Research & Method in Education (IOSR-JRME)
e-ISSN: 2320–7388,p-ISSN: 2320–737X Volume 7, Issue 1 Ver. I (Jan. - Feb. 2017), PP 101-112
www.iosrjournals.org
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 101 | Page
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
Imam Rofiki1, Toto Nusantara2, Subanji2, Tjang Daniel Chandra2
1Doctoral student in Mathematics Education, State University of Malang, Indonesia
2Department of Mathematics, State University of Malang, Indonesia
Abstract: This study explored students' reflective plausible reasoning in solving inequality problem. This
explorative study with the qualitative approach was conducted to seven subjects. Data are derived from the
result of written answer, think aloud, and interview. The data from those subjects were analyzed using a
constant comparative method so that it was obtained the same characteristics of reflective plausible reasoning.
In this article, the authors described two subjects. The results of this study were the characteristics of students'
reflective plausible reasoning shown by these behaviors: (1) students gave the argumentations based on
intrinsic mathematical properties during solving inequality problem, (2) students experienced state of perplexity
in problem solving process, (3) students realized that there was inaccuracy in the problem solving process
which is indicated by feeling suspicious, doubtful, or curious, (4) students conducted inquiry to correct their
error until they found the right solution, and (5) students experienced state of steadiness which is indicated by
feeling sure and satisfied toward the truth of the result.
Keywords: Reflective plausible reasoning, problem solving, inequality, intrinsic mathematical properties
I. Introduction
Reasoning and problem solving are two components which are close interrelated. The researchers and
psychologist have tried to get the students’ reasoning process by analyzing their argumentation during problem
solving. Chi, Bassok, Lewis, Reimann, and Glaser [1] examine the students' argumentation in problem solving
as the way to get deep knowledge that is being the basis of success in problem solving. Chi et al. conclude that
successful problem solver is the one who can make the inference from the given information and give the
explanation about the activity done in problem solving.
Mathematical reasoning is one of a basic mathematics competence that is essential to be trained to the
students. Basic mathematics competence includes problem solving ability, reasoning ability, and conceptual
understanding [2]. Mathematical reasoning is vital to be used in understanding mathematics. By the
mathematical reasoning, mathematics can be understood by student meaningfully [3]. Mathematical reasoning is
very important for mathematics education research. Kamol and Har [4] reveal the importance of knowing the
way of students’ thinking and reasoning to increase the students’ learning achievement in mathematics,
especially the success in mathematical problem solving. Peretz [5] emphasizes that students need to reason and
develop the reasoning on their mind.
Polya [6] divides reasoning into two kinds, namely demonstrative reasoning and plausible reasoning. In
plausible reasoning, the main thing is to differentiate a more reasonable guess from a less reasonable guess,
whereas in demonstrative reasoning the main thing is to differentiate a proof from a guess, that is the
demonstration of a valid proving from the effort of an invalid proving. Furthermore, Polya explains that people
assure their knowledge by demonstrative reasoning, but they support their conjecture by plausible reasoning.
Polya views the inductive reasoning as the certain case of plausible reasoning. The demonstrative reasoning is
also called as strict reasoning [6] or proof reasoning [7].
By referring to the Polya’s idea about plausible reasoning but it is not the definition, Lithner [7]
characterizes the reasoning process of university students in solving mathematical task into two kinds, namely
plausible reasoning (PR), and reasoning based on established experience. Furthermore, the latter term is
abbreviated by EE. PR and EE are the extension of analytical thinking process and pseudo-analytical thinking
process proposed by Vinner [8]. The analytical thinking process happens when a person faces a structure of a
complex problem and his/her scheme does not reach it, so the person will solve the problem into simpler parts
that can be reached out. The difference between analytical thinking process and PR is the degree of certainty in
reasoning. The degree of certainty in PR is higher than analytical thinking process. Meanwhile, the difference
between pseudo-analytical thinking process and EE is on the degree of analyticity. The pseudo-analytical
thinking process is not analytical thinking process, but EE has analytical thinking content, though only a few.
Students who apply pseudo-analytical thinking process can produce a wrong solution or a right solution.
Lithner [7] defines PR in mathematical task solving if the argumentation: (i) is based on mathematical
properties of the component involved in the reasoning, and (ii) is meant to guide toward the truth without
necessarily having to be complete and correct. This component is related to the fact, concept, definition,
operation, principle (axiom, property, theorem, lemma, or corollary), action, process, object, procedure, or
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 102 | Page
heuristic. Lithner explains that plausible reasoning is an extended and a looser version of proof reasoning, but it
is still based on the mathematical property. This mathematical property refers to intrinsic mathematical property.
The intrinsic mathematical property is a property that is relevant to mathematical task solving. It is accepted by
mathematical society as correct. The opponent of the intrinsic mathematical property is surface property. The
surface property has no (or a little) relevance to task solving. Plausible reasoning includes proof as a special case
with the difference that proof requires to a higher degree of certainty in the formal mathematical proof, such as:
complete, correct, and based on deductive logic. Reasoning will be called EE if the argumentation: (i) is based
on the ideas and procedures built on the one's previous experience from the learning environment, and (ii) is
meant to guide toward the truth without necessarily having to be complete and correct [7]. Condition (ii) in the
definition of EE is same as the condition (ii) in the definition of PR because the purpose of reasoning is same.
The fundamental difference between the definition of PR and EE is on the argumentation as described by the
condition (i). In EE, the argumentation is commonly the transfer of property from one situation of familiar task
solving to another situation that has some similarity. Reasoning done by students in EE is often superficial,
without considering intrinsic mathematical property from the component involved in the reasoning. Students use
the procedure of task solving only based on their previous experience without understanding. In this study, PR is
defined as reasoning by giving argumentation based on intrinsic mathematical properties. Whereas EE is
reasoning by giving argumentation based on idea and procedure constructed from the previous experience
without deep understanding.
The studies about PR and EE in solving mathematical task have been examined by some researchers
[7], [9], [10], [11]. Cawley [9] finds that many university students use EE in solving the task of linear equation.
Rofiki et al. [11] find that university student gets the right answer in the problem solving of the quadratic
equation but the university student cannot give argumentation based on intrinsic mathematical properties. The
university student only transfers the old knowledge to solve the problem without deep understanding. Hence, the
university student applies EE. Meanwhile, Lithner shows that many university students do EE and they get
difficulty in doing PR [7], [10].
Students maybe have a difficulty in problem solving so that they do reflective thinking process. Dewey
[12] defines that reflective thinking as active, continue, and careful thinking which supported a
conviction/knowledge and an invention of problem solution. John Dewey is the first expert who introduces the
idea of reflective thinking process in education. Furthermore, Dewey explains that reflective thinking process
moves from a perplexity state (also being called as disequilibrium) as unclear situation, doubtfulness, conflict,
and disorder thinking to a clear situation, coherence, harmony, and steady state (equilibrium). Perplexity
happens when the student faces a problem situation that the complete solution scheme has not been known
clearly. The student's internal experience has not been wholly used maximally. This condition became one cause
of disequilibrium and unsteadiness thinking. This will awake student's intention to balance his/her thinking
process so that it will encourage the student to solve the problem, i.e. to start the inquiry process. Hence, it can
be concluded that reflective thinking process is thought process happened when a student experiences perplexity
and do the inquiry to find the solution of the problem. By referring to the definition of PR and Dewey's
definition of reflective thinking, reflective plausible reasoning in this study is defined as PR followed by
reflective thinking process in problem solving.
One of the ways that can be used to explore students’ plausible reasoning is using problem solving. The
students are asked to solve inequality problem (non-routine task). The inequality problem in this study is the
question of inequality that can be understood by students and it is challenging for them but it cannot be solved
by a routine procedure that known by them. To gain the right procedure, it is needed a deep thinking and
analysis. In other words, students have the aim to solve the problem but the complete solution scheme is not
available immediately on their mind.
Inequality, particularly the solution set of inequality is an essential concept in calculus because the
main discourse of calculus involves function concept and analysis of the function property. The analysis of the
property of particular function needs the solution set of inequality such as in determining the domain of the
irrational function. Moreover, the solution set of inequality is needed to find the monotonicity and concavity of
functions by applying the derivative concept. Consequently, students need to understand the inequality concept
well in order to gain success in learning calculus. Students also need to learn inequality concept to build
comprehension in trigonometry, geometry, discrete mathematics, linear programming, algebra, and real analysis.
The inequality concept becomes an interesting topic to be studied. Because of the importance of
inequality concept in calculus and the other fields of mathematics, this causes increasing studies about
inequality. Yet, some studies show that students get difficulty in solving the inequality problem. Bazzini and
Tsamir [13] find that many students have some problems to solve algebraic inequality. Meanwhile, Fujii [14]
finds that students experience difficulty in finding the solution set of inequality which yield real number set.
Sierpinska [15] finds the candidate of mathematics and statistics university students who does not realize their
error in determining the solution set of absolute value inequalities. Students do not know whether their answer is
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 103 | Page
EE
LPR
GPR
0
1
PR
Figure 1. The position of EE, LPR, GPR, and PR
correct or wrong. Students depend on lecturer's argumentation about the truth of their answer. Students can
solve the problems that have the similar steps with the example from their lecturer but to solve the other
problems that need PR, the students cannot give argumentations based on intrinsic mathematical properties.
The purpose of this study is to explore students’ reflective plausible reasoning in solving inequality
problem. Educators can use the result of this study as consideration for designing the learning strategies to
increase the students’ reflective plausible reasoning in the mathematics classroom. In addition, the result also
gives the contribution to researchers as the theoretical framework or empirical facts about reflective plausible
reasoning and inequality problem.
II. Reasoning Structure And Characterization
Lithner uses the term of reasoning to all kinds of reasoning that related to mathematical task solving
[7], [10]. The mathematical task can be an exercise (routine tasks) and a problem (non-routine tasks).
Furthermore, Lithner defines reasoning as the line of thinking or the way of thinking that is used to produce
statements and reach a conclusion in task solving. Related to reasoning, argumentation and justification solution
are essential to reinforce or refuse a statement. Justification refers to the act of defending or clarifying
statements [16]. While the argumentation is confirmation (verification), part of the reasoning aimed to convince
oneself or others that the performed reasoning is correct [7], [10].
To solve a mathematical task, students can solve a set of subtasks. The way that can be used to describe
the reasoning in solving mathematical task is by structuring student's reasoning through 4 steps, namely 1) A
problematic situation, 2) strategy choice, 3) strategy implementation, and 4) conclusion [7], [10]. This reasoning
structure describes the line of student's reasoning in solving mathematical task starting from face the task to
conclude the obtained result.
Lithner [10] proposed the modification of reasoning characterization by introducing the term of local
plausible reasoning (LPR) and global plausible reasoning (GPR). Reasoning in mathematical task solving is
called LPR if it satisfies at least one of the following two conditions: (i) the strategy choice is based on
identifying similar surface properties in the task and component of situations in the text, but PR is used locally
to determine whether the procedure can be copied to solve the task or not, or (ii) The strategy implementation is
mostly based on copying the solution procedure from the identified situation, but one or a few local steps of this
procedure are modified by construction of PR [10]. While reasoning in mathematical task solving is called GPR
if it satisfies at least one of the following two conditions: (i) the strategy choice is mostly based on analysis and
consideration of intrinsic mathematical properties from the components in the task. The idea is constructed and
supported by PR, or (ii) the strategy implementation is mostly supported by PR [10]. The similarity between
LPR and GPR is in the existence of PR whereas the difference is the range of PR. GPR concerns the whole
solution by the implementation of PR globally while LPR applies PR locally. If a mathematical task is
impossible to be solved by EE or LPR, then GPR or PR needs to be applied.
Based on the explanation above, LPR (GPR) is defined as PR applied locally (globally) in the whole of
problem solving. In LPR, the student gives argumentation only in a few local parts by considering the intrinsic
mathematical properties. While in GPR, argumentation is mainly based on considering the intrinsic
mathematical properties.
The authors further make the position of Lithner’s reasoning characterization based on the lens of the
range of argumentation based on intrinsic mathematical properties (the lens of PR) in the of problem solving.
The reasoning characterization includes EE, LPR, GPR, and PR. The position of the reasoning characterization
is not discrete, but it is continuous. The authors relate this position with fuzzy theory. In the universe of a crisp
set (a classical set), a membership function for a set (also called characteristic function, indicator function, or
discrimination function) is expressed explicitly with 0 (if it is the element of a set) and 1 (if it is not an element
of a set). Whereas fuzzy set allows the membership function to all values in the interval . In other words, a
membership function of a crisp set only has exactly two values (0 and 1) while membership function of the
fuzzy set is a continuous function with range . EE is not PR so the value of EE’s membership function (also
called membership degree) is 0 while the value of PR’s membership function is 1. The membership degree of
LPR and GPR is
and
, respectively. EE (PR) is shown in the leftmost position (the rightmost
position). The membership degree refers to the whole of PR. The membership function moves increasingly from
the leftmost to the rightmost. The position of EE, LPR, GPR, and PR is shown in Figure 1.
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 104 | Page
Students need to do the process of reflective thinking when they find imprecision in the process of
problem solving. The reflective thinking is very crucial for the students because it is a rearrangement of thinking
in order to understand and solve the problem. The role of the reflective thinking is that making students believe
(or do not believe) their solution. If students do the reflective thinking until correcting the mistakes or finding
the solution, then they will feel sure on their solution. On the contrary, students will not believe their solution if
they have done the reflective thinking but they are not able to find the solution. The cause of students' failure in
finding the solution of the problem is not optimally the process of their reflective thinking.
Relating to the reasoning characterization previously, the authors make the reasoning characterization
with the lens of the existence of reflective thinking process. EE (LPR, GPR, or PR) followed by a process of
reflective thinking in problem solving is called by a reflective EE (a reflective LPR, a reflective GPR, or a
reflective PR), whereas EE (LPR, GPR, or PR) that is not followed by a process of reflective thinking in
problem solving is called by a non-reflective EE (a non-reflective LPR, a non-reflective GPR, or a non-
reflective PR). A reflective EE (a reflective LPR, a reflective GPR, a reflective PR, a non-reflective LPR, a non-
reflective GPR, or a non-reflective PR) is abbreviated as RfEE (RfLPR, RfGPR, RfPR, NRfEE, NRfLPR,
NRfGPR, or NRfPR). The position of reasoning characterization (RfEE, RfLPR, RfGPR, RfPR, NRfEE, NRfLPR,
NRfGPR, and NRfPR) is presented in Figure 2.
III. Method
This type of study was exploratory study using the qualitative approach. The number of undergraduate
students included in this study was 41. The students are from a university located in East Java, Indonesia. The
students who become the candidate of research subjects are not randomly selected, but they are chosen by 2
criteria, namely intending to be a subject and getting a recommendation from their lecturer. Furthermore,
students who do RfPR are selected as research subjects while students who do RfLPR, RfGPR, RfPR, NRfEE,
NRfLPR, NRfGPR, or NRfPR are not chosen as research subjects. Subject selection is done continuously until
obtaining a saturation of data. The saturation of data means that the subject to each group has the same pattern.
The data were analyzed with the constant comparative method. The method is called the constant comparative
method because the analysis of the data in this study compares the data with the other data constantly, and then
it compares the category with the other categories regularly [17], [18].
The data collection was carried out by giving a task of inequality problem solving to the subjects. The
problem is to determine the set of all real numbers that satisfies the inequality . The
subjects were asked to express aloud any words what their thinking at first receiving a problem to solving the
problem. The authors recorded the subjects’ utterance and the subjects’ behavior, including the unique things
done by the subjects when solving the problem. This data collection is called think aloud [3], [7], [10] or think
out loud/TOL [19]. The think aloud method can be used to explore the process of students’ cognition/thinking
that can not be observed when students solve a problem [3].
The authors also interviewed the subjects to get information about data of RfPR that has not been revealed in
the written data and the think aloud. In addition, this interview was conducted for confirming the subjects’ work.
In the interview process, the subjects were asked to justify and explain what has been done and give reasons
why they do or answer like that. The authors also recorded the subjects’ conversation and the subject's behavior
during the interview. After collecting the data, the authors transcribed the recording of the think aloud and the
interview. Afterward, the authors analyzed the data from the result of the written answer, the think aloud, and
the interview to get the characteristics of RfPR.
The reflective
thinking process
NRfGPR
NRfPR
NRfLPR
NRfEE
RfGPR
RfPR
RfLPR
RfEE
GPR
PR
LPR
EE
Reasoning
Yes
No
Figure 2. The position of reasoning characterization
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 105 | Page
Figure 3. The S1’s written answer in the first case
IV. Results And Discussion
Of the 41 students in this study, 19 students did EE (15 NRfEE and 4 RfEE), 10 students did LPR (7
NRfLPR and 3 RfLPR), 4 students did GPR (2 NRfPR and 2 RfGPR), and 8 students did PR (1 NRfPR and 7
RfPR). The following Table 1 shows the distribution of students’ reasoning in solving inequality problem.
Table 1. The distribution of students’ reasoning in solving inequality problem
After the authors analyzed data in the RfPR group by a constant comparative method, it is obtained the
result that seven subjects had the same characteristic of RfPR. In this article, the authors described two subjects
that are S1 (a male) and S2 (a female). According to the result of written answer, the think aloud, and interview
transcript, the first activity done by the subjects was reading the problem many times. S1 read twice, whereas S2
read three times. Their reason behind that activity is to more accurate in understanding the information of
problems such as the universe set of real number, inequality objects, and the problem question. A problematic
situation met by the subjects appeared when they thought what should be done to determine the solution set.
They thought hard indicated by silencing for a long time, holding the head, or asking the solution. After thinking
hard, they arranged a strategy. In the strategy choice step, subjects described the problem at 3 cases. S1 and S2
explained as the first case. S1 explained the second and the third case as and
, respectively. Whereas S2 explained the second and the third case as and
, respectively.
In the strategy implementation step, S1 and S2 determined the property of root value as the first case
(the first requirement), namely The subjects gave argumentation that the radicand has to greater
than or equal to zero in order to the result value is still the element of the real number set. If the radicand is less
than zero, then the result value is an imaginary number. The imaginary number is not an element of a real
number set. On the other hand, it is an element of a complex number set. S1 factorized into
11
. S1 showed equivalent of
+11
to
+11>0
or
+11=0
. Further, S1 used theorem in
real number system such as 1) if then or , and 2) if then
or . S1 analyzed all solutions possibilities by applying set and logic concepts to take a decision in
determining the solution set. Moreover, S1 used inequality concept, namely adding/subtracting the same
quantity to both sides of inequality will get the equivalent inequality with the previous inequality. The solution
set obtained by S1 was . The S1’s written answer in the first case is shown in
Figure 3.
While S2 added 1 to both sides so it is obtained . S2 took square root on both sides
of so it is obtained . After getting this result, S2 was silent for a long time while moving the
forefinger. S2 experienced perplexity and asked the truth of the obtained result. S2 said slowly that “is my
answer correct? I think there is a problem in my way.” S2 was doubtful and suspicious with her problem solving
strategy. By this suspicious, S2 did inquiry all solving steps that have to be done. After thinking hard. S2
realized that her answer was wrong. S2 expressed that there were 2 possibilities of x real number that satisfy
, namely . Her reason was the squaring for every real number in or is
Reasoning
EE
LPR
GPR
PR
19
10
4
8
NRfEE
RfEE
NRfLPR
RfLPR
NRfGPR
RfGPR
NRfPR
RfPR
15
4
7
3
2
2
1
7
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 106 | Page
Figure 5. The S2’s written answer in the second case
greater than or equal to 1. S2 tried to use another strategy for convincing the truth of her solution. S1 factorized
so that it is obtained . S2 used the property that if then
or . S2 got 2 possibilities, namely or
. By applying set, logic, and inequality concepts, S2 obtained
as the solution set of . S2 was sure and satisfied with the truth of her answer because she has gotten
the same result by two different strategies. The thought process done by S2 showed that the characteristics of a
reflective thinking process. The S2’s written answer in the first case is shown in Figure 4.
S2 determined as the second case. S2 analyzed 3 possibilities of value in , namely
positive real number, negative real number, or zero. S2 considered that the left side value of
was non-negative real number. Furthermore, S2 got the result of her analysis, namely 1) if the left side is non-
negative and the right side is negative, then it does not satisfy the inequality problem because non-negative is
not less than negative, 2) if the left side is non-negative and the right side is zero, then it does not satisfy the
inequality problem because non-negative is not less than zero, or 3) if the left side is non-negative and the right
side is positive, then it satisfies the inequality problem. S2 gave the reason about the third condition, namely
because the minimum value on the left side of is zero so the right side is greater than zero.
Further, S2 subtracted 2 to both sides of and multiplied
to both sides of the obtained inequality so
that S2 got the solution set of the second case, that is . The S2’s written answer in the second
case is shown in Figure 5.
S2 determined as the third case, whereas S1 determined it as the second case. The
subjects squared both sides of so that it is obtained . They justified
that squaring both sides of the inequality can be done because the value of left side and the right side is non-
negative and positive, respectively. They showed that it is can not be done if one of both sides inequality is
negative. A counterexample given by S1 is but . Whereas S2 gave a counter
Figure 4. The S2’s written answer in the first case
S2 applied the reflective thinking process
The second strategy in the case
The first strategy in the case
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 107 | Page
example that but . The subjects subtracted and added 1 to both sides so
that it is obtained . That result is equivalent to . They factorized
into . They used the property if so or
). They got 2 possibilitiesnamely or
. They also used the concept of inequality, set, and logic to take the decision in determining the solution
set. They obtained the solution set of , namely
. The S2’s and
S1’s written answer in the case are shown in Figure 6 and Figure 7, respectively.
To determine the solution set of inequality problem, S2 intersected the solution set of the first, the
second, and the third case. S2 got . The S2’s written answer in determining the solution set is
shown in Figure 8.
Whereas S1 intersected the solution set of the first and the second case to determine the solution set of
inequality problem. S1 got
. S1 checked the truth of the solution set by substituting
some values of x ( , , and ) to inequality problem S1 explained that and
fulfilled because and was correct statement. Whereas the result of substitution
did not fulfill because is the wrong statement. Therefore, S1 was
suspicious with the truth of
. S1 experienced a complex perplexity. It seemed when S1 was
silent for a long time while holding a head. S1 questioned the truth of
as the solution set of
Figure 8. The S2’s written answer in determining the solution set inequality problem
Figure 7. The S1’s written answer in the second case
Figure 6. The S2’s written answer in the third case
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 108 | Page
The S1’s written answer
The S2’s written answer
Figure 11. The S1’s and S2’s written answer when convincing the validity of the solution set
inequality problem. S1 was doubt and curiosity with his solution. S1 said, “My answer maybe is wrong. How
could it be? How do get the right solution?” Because of those conditions, S1 did inquiry toward his problem
solving previously. After thinking hard, S1 was sure that
did not satisfy the solution set of
inequality problem. His thought process indicated that reflective thinking process occurs. S1 argued that for
showing the statement is wrong it is sufficient to give one counterexample. His counterexample was .
Hence, S1 realized that the obtained solution set was incorrect. S1 rechecked the problem solving in the first and
the second case. S1 found the connection between the first and the second case, namely squaring process can be
done when the left side and right side of the inequality is non-negative and of positive, respectively. His
argumentation was because the minimum value on the left side of the inequality is 0 so has to greater
than 0. Thus, S1 determined as the third case (also called by the second requirement). S1 asserted
that the third case is crucial as the complement of two cases previously. Without involving the third case, the
solution is not complete. By subtracting 2 to both sides of and multiplying
to both sides of the
obtained inequality, S1 found the solution set of the third case, namely . The S1’s written
answer in the third case is shown in Figure 9. Furthermore, S1 intersected the solution set of the first, the
second, and the third case. S1 got . S1 wrote the solution set of inequality problem as shown in
Figure 10.
In the conclusion step, subjects concluded that the solution set of was
.
Subjects justified that the steps used to solve the problem were right because they have applied the
mathematical properties and mathematical concepts. They really believed that the obtained result was correct. In
convincing the result, they gave the general statement. They stated that for every . Their
reason was because for every and the value of is less than for every
. They also gave argumentation that for every because is greater than where is a
positive real number so that is always greater than for every . They justified that
for every real number in because applying the transitive property in
and . It shows that they proved the validity of the result generally by including
algebraic property, transitive property, and order property of real number set. They made the logical inference
based on the transitive property. In proving the result, they could give a logical reason. According to Harel and
Sowder [20], their proof scheme is classified by an analytic proof scheme. Whereas if it is viewed by Balaceff’s
proof taxonomy, their proof is a conceptual proof [21]. Figure 11 below shows their proving to convince the
validity of the solution set.
Figure 9. The S1’s written answer in the third case
Figure 10. The S1’s written answer after applying reflective thinking
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 109 | Page
The subjects explained well the problem solving in each case. They gave argumentation based on
intrinsic mathematical properties such as distributive property, inequality property, inequality concept, factoring
concept, set concept, and logic concept. They experienced the perplexity in problem solving. They felt curiosity
about their process of problem solving. They doubted the truth of their solution. This leads them to inquire the
inaccuracy on their solution. After the main matter was founded, they realized that there was something wrong
with it. Finally, they corrected it. They felt satisfied with the result. This indicates the condition of their steady
thinking. According to Dewey [12], their mental process can be categorized as a reflective thinking process. It
can happen because they are doubt or curiosity of what the problem truly is or how exactly a solution is. They
performed reflective thinking process well because of their tenacity in finding the solution. Moreover, they have
a deep knowledge of the material and a logical thinking ability.
Based on the S1’s and S2’s reasoning process in the discussion above, they experienced RfPR. Their
reasoning was shown by the giving argumentations based on mathematical intrinsic properties but they also
applied reflective thinking process. The structure of their RfPR is presented in Figure 12.
The structure of S1’s RfPR The structure of S2’s RfPR
Figure 12. The structure of S1’s and S2’s RfPR
Based on the analysis of subjects group, there were 5 same characteristics in RfPR. The characteristics
are (1) the existence of giving argumentations based on intrinsic mathematical properties during inequality
problem solving, (2) the existence of a perplexity state in the problem solving process, (3) the existence of
awareness about some inaccuracies in the problem solving process, (4) the existence of an inquiry to correct the
error until finding the solution set of inequality problem, and (5) the existence of steady thinking followed by
feeling sure and satisfied toward the truth of obtained result. In general, the process of RfPR can be illustrated in
Figure 13.
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 110 | Page
Students who did RfPR could give logical reasons why the rules/procedures work or can be applied.
Moreover, they also gave the counterexample if the statement did not work. Students could apply a variety of
concepts and properties related to the problem solving. The concepts of inequality, factoring, set and logic were
used to determine the solution set of inequality problem. Students constructed knowledge by connecting
between what is being faced with the existing knowledge. Students did not memorize the concepts, rules,
Figure 13. The process of reflective plausible reasoning (RfPR)
Note:
AIMp : Argumentation based on
intrinsic mathematical properties
RTP : Reflective thinking process
Problem structure
Reasoning structure
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 111 | Page
procedures, or properties but they understood it well by relating to their knowledge previously. The learning
process students’ RfPR is consistent with the meaningful learning theory [22]. According to the terminology of
Hiebert and Lefevre [23], students’ knowledge is categorized by conceptual knowledge. Meanwhile, if it is
viewed by the terminology of understanding, the students have a relational understanding [24] or conceptual
understanding [25].
V. Conclusion
In this study students performed plausible reasoning well in the problem solving. Students also could
overcome the difficulty during the problem solving because of applying reflective thinking process maximally.
Therefore, in the learning process the educators should provide greater opportunities for students to take
reflection process so that they can find the solution of the problem and perform reflective plausible reasoning
optimally. Another result of this study is a few students performed plausible reasoning during the inequality
problem solving. Most students used the learning experience previously without deep understanding. In other
words, many students performed EE. This can also be found in previous studies (e.g., [7], [9], [10]), which
reveal that EE is more dominant than PR. Moreover, many students applied superficial reasoning. Therefore, it
is very essential for an educator to familiarize students to use plausible reasoning by explaining the process of
solving the problem, justifying the problem solving the steps, and convincing the truth of the result. Further
research is required to examine the students’ failure in plausible reflective reasoning. In addition, there is still an
open study to investigate the trigger of students doing EE.
References
[1] M. Chi, M. Bassok, M. Lewis, P. Reimann, and R. Glaser, Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to
solve problems, Cognitive Science, 13, 1989, 145-182.
[2] B. Jonsson, M. Norqvist, Y. Liljekvist, and J. Lithner, Learning mathematics through algorithmic and creative reasoning, Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 36, 2014, 20–32.
[3] I. Rofiki, Subanji, T. Nusantara, and T. D. Chandra, Student’s creative reasoning in solving pattern generalization problem: A case
study, in R. Ekawati, S. Fiangga, L. Hakim, R. Ismawati, I. F. Kurniawan, D. Maulana, A. Mustofa, A. Nugroho, D. Nurhadi, A.
Ridwan, & A. Wardhono (Eds.), Proceeding of International Conference on Educational Research and Development (ICERD) 2015
(Surabaya: The State University of Surabaya, 2015), 358-365.
[4] N. Kamol and Y. B. Har, Upper primary school students’ algebraic thinking, in L. Sparrow, B. Kissane, & C. Hurst (Eds.), Shaping
the future of mathematics education: Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia, Fremantle: MERGA, 2010, 289-296.
[5] D. Peretz, Enhancing reasoning attitudes of prospective elementary school mathematics teachers, Journal of Mathematics Teacher
Education, 9, 2006, 381–400.
[6] G. Polya, Mathematics and plausible reasoning, Volume I: Induction and analogy in mathematics, (Princeton NJ: Princeton
University Press, 1954).
[7] J. Lithner, Mathematical reasoning in school tasks, Educational Studies in Mathematics, 41(2), 2000, 165–190.
[8] S. Vinner, The pseudo-conceptual and the pseudo-analytical thought processes in mathematics learning, Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 34, 1997, 97–129.
[9] A. Cawley, Mathematical perceptions and problem solving of first year developmental mathematics students in a four-year
institution, 17th annual Conference on Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education (RUME), Denver, Colorado, 2014.
[10] J. Lithner, Students’ mathematical reasoning in university textbook exercises, Educational Studies in Mathematics, 52(1), 2003, 29–
55.
[11] I. Rofiki, T. Nusantara, Subanji, and T. D. Chandra, Penalaran plausible versus penalaran berdasarkan established experience, in G.
Muhsetyo, E. Hidayanto, & R. Rahardi (Eds.), Proceeding of National Conference on Mathematics Education 2016 (Malang: State
University of Malang, 2016), 1012–1021.
[12] J. Dewey, How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process (Boston: Heath, 1933).
[13] L. Bazzini and P. Tsamir, Research based instruction: Widening students’ understanding when dealing with inequalities,
Proceedings of the 12th ICMI Study Conference, Melbourne, Australia: University of Melbourne, Vol. 1, 2001, 61-68.
[14] T. Fujii, Probing students’ understanding of variables through cognitive conflict: Is the concept of a variable so difficult for students
to understand?, in N. A. Pateman, B. J. Dougherty & J. Zilliox (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2003 joint meeting of the PME, 1, 2003,
49–65.
[15] A. Sierpienska, I need teacher to tell me if I am right or wrong, in J. H. Woo, H. C. Lew, K. S. Park, & D. Y. Seo (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 31st Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol. 1, (Seoul: PME,
2007), 45-64.
[16] P. Tsamir, D. Tirosh, and E. Levenson, Exploring the relationship between justification and monitoring among kindergarten
children, in V. Durand-Guerrier, S. Soury-Lavergne, & F. Arzarello (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixth Congress of the European
Society for Research in Mathematics Education (Lyon: Institut National de Recherche Pédagogique (INRP), 2009), 2597-2606.
[17] J. W. Creswell, Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (Boston: Pearson
Education, Inc, 2012).
[18] M. S. Kolb, Grounded theory and the constant comparative method: Valid research strategies for educators, Journal of Emerging
Trends in Educational Research and Policy Studies (JETERAPS), 3(1), 2012, 83-86.
[19] R. Subanji and A. M. Supratman, The pseudo-covariational reasoning thought processes in constructing graph function of reversible
event dynamics based on assimilation and accommodation frameworks, Journal of the Korean Society of Mathematical Education,
Series D, Research in Mathematical Education, Vol. 19, No. 1 (Issue 61), 2015, 61-79.
[20] G. Harel and L. Sowder, Students proof schemes: Results from exploratory studies, in A. Schoenfeld, J. Kaput, & E. Dubinsky
(Eds.), Research in collegiate mathematics education III (American Mathematical Society: Washington, D.C, 1998), 234–282.
[21] N. Balacheff, Aspects of proof in pupils’ practice of school mathematics, in D. Pimm, (Ed.), Mathematics, teachers and children
(London, U.K.: Hodder & Stoughton, 1988), 216-230.
Reflective Plausible Reasoning in Solving Inequality Problem
DOI: 10.9790/7388-070101101112 www.iosrjournals.org 112 | Page
[22] D. P. Ausubel, The acquisition and retention of knowledge: A cognitive view (Dordrecht: Springer-Science+Business Media, B.V.,
2000).
[23] J. Hiebert and P. Lefevre, Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis, in J. Hiebert (ed.),
Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1986),
1–27.
[24] R. R. Skemp, Relational understanding and instrumental understanding, Mathematics Teaching, 77, 1976, 20–26.
[25] J. Hiebert and T. P. Carpenter, Learning and teaching with understanding, in D. A. Grouws (ed.), Handbook of Research on
Mathematics Teaching and Learning (New York: Macmillan Publishing Company, 1992), 65–97.