Access to this full-text is provided by PLOS.
Content available from PLOS One
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
resembles post-aggression consolation in
chimpanzees
Marie Rosenkrantz Lindegaard
1,2☯
*, Lasse Suonpera
¨Liebst
2☯
, Wim Bernasco
1,3☯
, Marie
Bruvik Heinskou
4
, Richard Philpot
5
, Mark Levine
5
, Peter Verbeek
6
1Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement (NSCR), Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
2Department of Sociology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark, 3Department of Spatial
Economics, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 4Department of Sociology and
Social Work, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark, 5Department of Psychology, University of Exeter,
Exeter, United Kingdom, 6Department of Anthropology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham,
Alabama, United States of America
☯These authors contributed equally to this work.
*Mlindegaard@nscr.nl
Abstract
Post-aggression consolation is assumed to occur in humans as well as in chimpanzees.
While consolation following peer aggression has been observed in children, systematic evi-
dence of consolation in human adults is rare. We used surveillance camera footage of the
immediate aftermath of nonfatal robberies to observe the behaviors and characteristics of
victims and bystanders. Consistent with empathy explanations, we found that consolation
was linked to social closeness rather than physical closeness. While females were more
likely to console than males, males and females were equally likely to be consoled. Further-
more, we show that high levels of threat during the robbery increased the likelihood of
receiving consolation afterwards. These patterns resemble post-aggression consolation in
chimpanzees and suggest that emotions of empathic concern are involved in consolation
across humans and chimpanzees.
Introduction
Within social philosophy, consolation is seen as a fundamental human achievement [1]. Con-
solation, in this view, soothes anxiety and our existential vulnerability, and draws on a shared
sense of humanity, empathy, trust and perspective-taking. Scholarship on consolation dates
back to the Greco-Roman world, and unlike mere empathic perspective-taking that does not
necessitate action, consolation requires action in response to the recognition of the plight of
the other [2].
While the extensive philosophical literature on consolation is extensive, few observational
or ethological studies are available on human consolation in naturalistic settings. This paucity
likely reflects the difficulty of carrying out unobtrusive and systematic observation of human
behavior in natural conflict [3]. Here we introduce surveillance camera footage as a new
method to systematically measure post-aggression consolation in adult humans. Specifically,
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 1 / 16
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Lindegaard MR, Liebst LS, Bernasco W,
Heinskou MB, Philpot R, Levine M, et al. (2017)
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
resembles post-aggression consolation in
chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 12(5): e0177725. https://
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725
Editor: Allan Siegel, University of Medicine &
Dentistry of NJ—New Jersey Medical School,
UNITED STATES
Received: December 2, 2016
Accepted: May 2, 2017
Published: May 31, 2017
Copyright: ©2017 Lindegaard et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: We fully respect and
endorse the philosophy and derived data and
computer code availability policy of PLOS, and
would in fact welcome and encourage replications
and extensions of our work. Access to the raw
CCTV footage data will be granted by signing an
agreement stating that the applicant (1) will use the
data only for scientific purposes, (2) will not make
the data accessible to third parties, and (3) will not
publish results that will disclose the identity of the
subjects in the data. To request access to the raw
we analyzed the immediate aftermath of nonfatal robberies to look for adult human consola-
tion. Surveillance camera footage offers a unique and ethically sound way of studying adult
behavior in high-danger contexts [4].
In nonhuman primates, consolation was first observed in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
Consolation was termed as such because it visibly reduced distress in victims of aggression [5,
6] and subsequent research has confirmed this function [6,7]. Behaviorally, consolation in
nonhuman primates is commonly defined as interactions in which an uninvolved bystander
initiates friendly contact with a recent recipient of aggression [8]. Consolation so defined is a
highly replicable finding in chimpanzee populations [7,9,10], and has since been further doc-
umented in bonobos (Pan paniscus) [11,12], mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) [13],
Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) [14], and Tonkean macaques (Macaca ton-
keana) [15]. In monkeys, bystanders have also been observed to offer friendly contact when
solicited to do so by victims (e.g., Cebus apella) [16,17]. As later research showed, the differen-
tiation between (a) consolation initiated by the bystander (“consolation proper”), and (b)
friendly bystander contact solicited by recipients of aggression, is important to understand the
functional significance as well as the cognitive and emotional implications of the behavior
[18]. Indicative of convergent evolution, consolation, as defined for nonhuman primates, has
now also been observed in non-primate species such as canids, corvids, rodents and elephants
[19].
Research on post-aggression consolation in humans has mainly focused on children from
various cultures [20–22]. For example, Japanese children as young as 4 years of age offered
consolation to victims of peer aggression. The frequency of this consolation increased rapidly
between 4 and 5 years of age, most likely a reflection of developing socio-cognitive skills such
as the ability to attribute mental states to others (“Theory of Mind”), and to understand that
another’s point of view about an issue may contrast with one’s own [23,24]. In this context,
consolation in human adults can be seen as an empathy-related behavior that involves sharing
another’s distress via emotional contagion, combined with sympathizing and perspective-tak-
ing [25–27].
Almost three decades ago, in 1989, de Waal called for more naturalistic studies on human
adults so as to overcome the view that the human-nonhuman primate comparisons simply
show that “other primates are mentally like human children” [28]. Here we compare post-
aggression consolation in human adults with post-aggression consolation that prior research
has identified in adult chimpanzees. Through this comparison with our evolutionary closest
kin, we shed light on the mental processes involved in post-aggression consolation across
human and non-human primates [29]. Such comparison is in agreement with the principle of
evolutionary parsimony advocated by de Waal, suggesting that if closely related species behave
similarly, then the underlying mental processes are presumably similar too [29].
A growing body of research across chimpanzee populations in varied social conditions
shows how bystander affiliation may serve vital peacemaking and peacekeeping functions. For
example, chimpanzee bystander affiliation can appease aggressors—especially if the bystander
has a close relationship with the aggressor [8]—which can protect bystanders against redi-
rected aggression [30]. Chimpanzees also offer consolation to victims of aggression, often by
hugging and kissing the victim. It can be assumed that these different types of bystander affilia-
tion and the functions that they serve rely on different social and individual motivations [31].
Post-aggression consolation in chimpanzees is promoted by social closeness [9,32,33].
Although several earlier studies did not find evidence associating age and sex with the likeli-
hood of providing consolation [6,34,35], a recent study that draws on data from an exception-
ally large sample of spontaneous consolation in chimpanzees, offers compelling evidence that
female individuals are more likely to provide post-aggression consolation than males [9].
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 2 / 16
footage files or inquire about the conditions, please
contact Ho-Young Wisselink, datamanager at the
Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and
Law Enforcement (NSCR), at email address
nscr@nscr.nl. The analyzed data file with coded
observations of the video footage and the code
descriptions are added as supplementary files (S1
File and S2 File) to the manuscript submission.
This allows readers to replicate the analytical parts
of our research.
Funding: Data collection was supported by the
Police and Science grant program of the
Netherlands National Police (grant no. PW/OC/
2014/07). The funders had no role in study design,
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
Given that social closeness is known to facilitate empathy in both humans and other animals
and that females are generally more empathic than males, these findings are consistent with an
empathy explanation [36].
We analyzed the video-recorded behavior of 249 individuals who were present in the after-
math of 22 commercial robberies, immediately after the offenders had left the scene. We inves-
tigated whether and how providing and receiving consolation were affected by social closeness
and gender. Specifically, as documented in chimpanzees and predicted by the empathy expla-
nation, we hypothesized that consolation is promoted by social closeness and that females are
more likely than males to provide consolation.
According to theory, and supported by empirical findings in ethology and in the social sci-
ences, we hypothesized that consolation might be further facilitated by additional factors.
First, based on the homophily principle that similarity breeds connection [37], we conjectured
that consolation is more likely between individuals that are similar in terms of ethnicity and
age; two of the factors that create the strongest divides in our personal environments [37]. This
hypothesis is supported by findings demonstrating that empathic response is amplified by sim-
ilarity and that individuals are more likely to help distressed others that are similar to them-
selves in terms of age and ethnicity [38–40].
Second, a comprehensive literature in social psychology has studied the conditions under
which human bystanders respond to the needs of victims in emergency situations. A recurring
finding is the ’bystander effect’, the phenomenon that individual bystanders are less likely to
intervene with increasing numbers of other bystanders present [41,42]. The bystander effect is
typically ascribed to a psychological process by which the presence of other bystanders diffuses
the responsibility for helping. To test for the bystander effect, we measured the total numbers
of individuals present in the aftermath of the robbery.
Because individuals who have better opportunities to respond to distressed victims are report-
edly more likely to respond [42], and because physical proximity between individuals has been
demonstrated to facilitate consolation among human children [23], we measured the physical dis-
tance between all pairs of individuals at the start of the robbery aftermath. We hypothesized that
consolation is more likely to occur when individuals are in closer proximity to each other.
A third theoretical framework from which we draw is the aversive-arousal reduction
hypothesis [43]. It proposes that the empathic arousal evoked by witnessing the distress of
another person is an unpleasant feeling, which can be alleviated by victim helping. As human
bystander research suggests that people are more likely to help distressed victims in high-dan-
ger compared to low-danger situations [42], we also measured the degree of threat that poten-
tial recipients of consolation had been exposed to during the robbery. We hypothesized that a
higher level of threat exposure would make an individual more likely to receive consolation
from others. In addition, but more tentatively, we hypothesized that arousal itself, independent
of its origin, is reduced by providing consolation. To examine this possibility, we also included
the degree of threat that potential providers of consolation had been exposed to during the
robbery. This allowed us to test whether the individuals most severely affected by threat during
the robbery would be more likely to console others than individuals less affected.
Finally, sociological theory and cross-cultural empirical studies on gender suggest that
social norms depict females to be more vulnerable, and hence more eligible for helping behav-
ior than males [44]—particularly in dangerous situations [45]. It has also been demonstrated
that it is more socially acceptable to touch women than men [46]. To investigate the validity of
these findings–which point towards females as more likely recipients of consolation than
males—we included the gender of potential consolation recipients as a variable of interest.
Based on the premise that bystanders are in a state of lower emotional involvement than
victims, all prior empirical studies on post-aggression consolation have assumed that victims
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 3 / 16
and bystanders are discrete categories. This implies that only bystanders and not victims of vio-
lence can provide consolation, and that only victims and not bystanders can receive it. However,
distinctions between victims and bystanders in robberies can be fluid, and emotional involve-
ment need not necessarily differentiate strongly between those who have been exposed to
aggression firsthand (victims) and those who have been exposed by proxy (bystanders). For this
reason, we did not label the individuals as either victims or bystanders, but instead measured
for each individual the extent to which they had been subjected to threat during the preceding
robbery. The observed level of threat directed towards the individual was thus included as a var-
iable. To reiterate, this was not used to create a dichotomy between victims and bystanders.
In addition, we did not make any assumptions on who could console whom. We considered
all dyads in both directions as potential consolation opportunities. This approach made it pos-
sible to analyze multiple instances of consolation between multiple individuals involved in the
same incident. Simultaneously, this allowed the observation of unexpected behavioral patterns.
For example, it allowed us to include in the analysis potential cases in which an individual who
experienced a brutal attack could console an individual who only witnessed the event. Further
details of the analytical approach are provided in the ‘Materials and methods’ section.
Consolation was defined as affiliative physical contact, including not only ‘hugging’, but
also gentle touching of the target’s head, upper body, arms or hands (see S1–S3 Tables for the
codesheets that were used).
Materials and methods
CCTV footage files
The Police departments of Amsterdam and Rotterdam (the Netherlands) gave the first and
third author access to unprocessed CCTV footage of incidents that the police had classified
as a robbery or as an attempted robbery. A robbery was defined as an event where goods or
money are taken by force or by threat of force. Access was provided under the condition that
data would be securely stored, not be publicly shared, and that the identity of the individuals
visible in the footage would be protected. The research was authorized by the Dutch Ministry
of Justice and approved by the Ethics Committee for Legal and Criminological Research
(CERCO) of the Faculty of Law of Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.
In all cases, the footage had been recorded by CCTV systems installed by the proprietors of
the retail businesses in which the robberies took place. The materials had been stored on police
computers for investigative purposes. Footage was not removed when the suspects were prose-
cuted or convicted. The police department of Rotterdam provided access to all robbery footage
collected between June 2014 and August 2015. The police department of Amsterdam provided
all footage that had been used in producing reality TV broadcastings aimed at eliciting public
information. The material obtained for the research was all original, unedited material.
The video files were stored in many different formats; often these were proprietary formats
that could only be watched with a designated video player program. Therefore, before coding
commenced, the program CamStudio was used to convert all video files to AVI format. Occa-
sionally, Windows Movie Maker was used to amalgamate footage from multiple cameras on a
single site together into one file. Cases where images were blurred or where coverage of the
interactions among individuals involved was incomplete were excluded from analysis. Of the
58 remaining cases, 32 did not include footage of the aftermath of the robbery and were thus
excluded. Four of the remaining 26 cases contained less than 2 seconds of aftermath footage.
These four cases were also not used in the analysis, as we judged this time interval too short for
anyone to be able to provide consolation. The final sample for analysis comprised of 22 cases,
the minimum footage duration was 14 seconds.
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 4 / 16
Coding scheme and observer instructions
Using the Observer XT software [47], all footage—both the robbery and its aftermath—was
observed and independently coded by two observers. Prior to coding, these two observers
undertook two days of Observer XT training; with particular emphasis given to the tailored
coding scheme that included measures of setting, individual and dyad characteristics.
As summarized in S1 Table in supplementary materials, for each setting analyzed we report
(1) the number of individuals present in the aftermath, (2) the function of the location, e.g.,
supermarket or hotel lobby, (3) the size of the setting, coded as ‘large’ if it would take an aver-
age person more than five seconds to walk from one side to the other, or ‘small’ otherwise.
Robbery victimization was defined as the psychological or physical suffering induced by
the presence or activities of offenders, and its extent was assumed to vary between individuals.
The first recorded individual characteristic of interest was (1) the level of victimization the
individual had suffered during the preceding robbery. This was either (1a) stood close to the
offender, (1b) received a threat only, (1c) suffered physical force, (1d), suffered both threat
and physical force, or (1e) none of these (for details see S2 Table in supplementary materials).
Other observed individual characteristics included (2) whether the individual was an employee
or a customer (based on clothing, the type of activity the person displayed, or on position in
the room, e.g., behind a desk), (3) whether the individual was male or female (based on body
form, hair, clothing and other body wear), (4) the estimated age of the individual, (5) the eth-
nic origin of the individual based on hair and skin color.
As summarized in S3 Table in the supplementary materials, observed dyad characteristics
included (1) a light form of consolation from provider towards recipient, whereby the provider
touched a hand or lower or upper arm of the recipient, (2) a strong form of consolation from
provider towards recipient, whereby the provider initiated and completed upper body contact
including hugging and embracing of the recipient. The distinction between ‘light’ and ‘strong’
consolation was not based on prior literature, but on initial qualitative exploration of the mate-
rial. As it has been demonstrated that the perceived intimacy/intrusiveness of being physically
touched by others depends to a large extent on which area of the body is being touched [46], it
seemed useful to make the distinction between light and strong consolation dependent on the
area of the body that was touched. The duration of consolation was not taken into account. As
the footage did not have recorded sound, words of consolation and other verbal communica-
tion between the subjects could not be recorded. We also measured (3) the physical distance
between two individuals at the start of the robbery aftermath, coded as ‘large’ if they were
located more than 2 meters apart, and ‘small’ otherwise.
The two observers–who worked in isolation—coded the characteristics and behavior of
every individual present in the footage of each of the robberies. Consolation behavior was
coded directionally, identifying both the provider and the recipient of consolation.
Assessment of coding reliability
To analyze the amount of correspondence between the measures for both observers, each indi-
vidual visible in the aftermath of a robbery was assigned a unique reference number. To that
aim, and before observations and coding commenced, for each robbery a numbered list was
created describing each individual by gender, hair style and hair color, size, clothing and posi-
tion at the beginning of the robbery aftermath. Without such a detailed list, coders can easily
mix up individuals [48].
Four measures were central to the analyses. To inspect levels of agreement on these four
coded measures, we calculated the Krippendorff’s alpha (Kα) coefficient of interrater reliabi-
lity between coders [49]. This is a generic measure of the level of correspondence between
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 5 / 16
observers, whereby values at or below 0 indicate no or negative correspondence, and values
near the maximum of 1 imply perfect agreement. The results were (1) Kα= .81 for the level of
victimization during preceding robbery, (2) Kα= .99 for whether an individual was an
employee or a customer, (3) Kα= .79 for physical distance, and Kα= .78 for (light or strong)
consolation. As it is customary to consider measures reliable when α.80 [49], these values
are acceptable. With all four measures included in the same multivariate regression analysis, it
is particularly reassuring that not a single measure is far below .80.
As discussed above, the distinction between light consolation (hand and upper arm contact)
and strong consolation (upper body contact) was not unambiguous. The Krippendorff αwas
only .54 and .67 for light and strong consolation, respectively. Follwowing recommendations
in the methodological literature on interrater reliability [49] we combined the light and strong
consolation measures into a single consolation measure. Intercoder reliability on this com-
bined measure was acceptable (Kα= .78).
Given that the observers did not perfectly agree on all measures (as Kα<1) we had to
decide on which codes to use in the analyses. This was done with a simple randomization pro-
cedure: We selected all observations of one of the two coders by throwing a dice.
Descriptive statistics and data file setup
The coded materials can be explored at three separate levels of analysis, namely (1) incident
level (Ni = 22), in which in each robbery is a single observation, (2) individual level (N
s
= 249),
where the individual subjects visible in the post-robbery footage are the units of analysis, and
(3) dyad level (N
d
= 3680), where all pairs of individuals present in the aftermath of the same
robbery are considered. Each dyad represents a potential directional consolation relationship,
and thus the dyads A!B and B!A are different; the first one indicating individual A provid-
ing consolation to B, and the second one indicating individual B providing consolation to A.
This implies that bystanders can console victims as well as other bystanders, and that the same
holds for victims.
S1 File is the single source data file with directed dyads as the unit of analysis. S2 File is the
Stata script that can be used to replicate all descriptive statistics reported below as well as the
model estimates described in the main text and in the supplementary information. The vari-
able ROBB_ID identifies unique incidents. Within each incident, variables PROV_ID and
RECI_ID identify unique individuals, such that PROV_ID identifies a potential provider of
consolation and RECI_ID identifies a potential receiver of consolation. As self-consolation (in
the form of self-touching) is a related phenomenon but distinct from consolation [50], this
behavior was excluded from the analyses, so that PROV_ID 6¼ RECI_ID for all records. Other
variables included are N_PEOPLE to indicate the number of people (victims and bystanders)
present, PROV_FEM and RECI_FEM to denote the gender of potential consolation providers
and receivers, PROV_ROLE and RECI_ROLE to denote their role (employee or customer),
and PROV_AGE and RECI_AGE and PROV_ETNIC and RECI_ETNIC to code their esti-
mated ages and ethnicities. PROV_VICTIM indicates whether a potential provider of consola-
tion was subjected to weapon threat or physical force in the robbery incident. RECI_VICTIM
indicates whether a potential recipient of consolation was subjected to weapon threat or physi-
cal force in the robbery incident.
DISTANCE indicates the physical distance between the potential provider and the potential
recipient of consolation. The binary response variable CONSOLATION indicates whether or
not the potential provider (PROV_ID) consoled the potential recipient (RECI_ID).
At the incident level, there are 22 robbery aftermath recordings. The aftermath starts the
moment the last offender leaves the scene and terminates when the recording (as obtained
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 6 / 16
from the police files) is cut off or completed. The mean duration of the 22 recordings is 94 sec-
onds (range is 14–324 seconds). Nine recordings are less than a minute, seven last longer than
two minutes. Robberies took place in various settings, the most common type (n = 8) being a
supermarket. The mean number of individuals present in the aftermath of the robberies is 11;
the median is 10 (range is 3–27). In most cases, the individuals present included males and
females. There was only one all-male case and only one all-female case. As the events are
commercial robberies, the individuals present in the cases are typically either employees
(including proprietors) or customers. The mean number of customers was 7.5 and the mean
number of employees was 3.6. If we call ‘victims’ those who had experienced a threat with a
weapon or who had been subjected to physical force during the preceding robbery, and ‘by-
tanders’ those who had not, the number of victims ranges between 1 and 4 (mean is 2), the
number of bystanders ranges between 0 and 26 (mean is 9).
At the individual level, the footage included 249 individuals, 132 males and 117 females, 166
customers, 79 employees and 4 individuals that could not be identified as either employees or
customers. Threat level during the preceding robbery was categorized as “no threat” (190 indi-
viduals), “stand near offender” (15), “threat with weapon” (23), “physical force” (5), “weapon
threat and physical force” (14) and “unknown” (2). Thus, if a victim is defined as an individual
who has received a threat and/or has been subjected to physical force during the robbery, and
if a bystander is defined as an individual who was present but not a victim, the 249 individuals
comprise of 42 victims, 205 bystanders and 2 individuals whom could not be assigned to either
category. Ethnicity was categorized as White (162 individuals), Moroccan or Turkish (41),
Black (26), Hindustan (5), Asian (2), other ethnicity (7) and unknown (6 individuals). Age was
estimated in years, and observers were allowed to code “Unknown”. Aggregated to 10-year cat-
egories, the distribution was 11–20 (49 individuals), 21–30 (104), 31–40 (24), 51–60 (16), 61+
(7) and unknown (5 individuals).
At the dyadic level there were 3710 pairwise combinations. The gender distribution was
1188 (32.0%) male-male, 784 (21.1%) male-female and 1738 (46.8%) male-female. Of the 3658
dyads where the roles of both individuals were known, 232 (6.3%) were employee-employee.
Only 96 (2.6%) of the dyads were located within 2 meters from each other at the start of the
aftermath of the robbery. Victim-victim dyads (58) accounted for 1.6% of the dyads, victim-
bystander dyads (796) accounted for 21.5%, and the majority (2856, 77.0%) were bystander-
bystander dyads. Cases containing variables with unknown values were excluded from the
analyses on a case-by-case basis. For example, individuals with unknown ethnicity were only
excluded if ethnicity was part of the analysis.
Statistical model
The standard methodology used in previous research on consolation is the PC-MC method [5,
6]. This method compares observed post-conflict (PC) interactions between victims and
bystanders with matched controls (MC), i.e., observed interactions between the same individu-
als in situations not preceded by conflict. Our material did not allow this method, as we have
no access to observations of interactions between the individuals prior to their interaction in
the post-robbery CCTV footage. Instead, we adopted a regression approach that models the
likelihood of post-robbery consolation as a function of specific characteristics of the dyads
(e.g., whether the potential provider is female, and the distance between them).
A strategy that maximally uses the available data is to define the individuals present in the
aftermath of a robbery incident as a social network. This allows examination of every possible
relation between two individuals in a network as a potential opportunity for consolation.
Whether or not consolation is provided is likely to depend on the characteristics of the
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 7 / 16
potential provider, on the characteristics of the potential receiver, on the characteristics of the
setting, and on the social and physical distance between potential provider and receiver.
Gravity models [51,52] are well equipped to handle this type of data. They have been
applied predominantly in economic geography for modeling flows of people, goods and infor-
mation between spatial units of analysis, but have also been used in other disciplines, such as
anthropology [53], to model the spatial organization of human activity. Moreover, gravity
models are equally applicable to the analysis of dyadic relations in non-spatial network data
[54]. In the application of a gravity model to consolation, we consider all possible N ×(N-1)
pairs of individuals present in the aftermath of a robbery, where N is the number of individu-
als. For example, in a robbery aftermath with three persons A, B and C, there are 6 consolation
possibilities, namely A!B, B!A, A!C, C!A, B!C and C!B. Gravity models predict
which factors determine whether an individual X provides consolation to individual Y, condi-
tional on the characteristics of X, the characteristics of Y, the characteristics of the setting, and
the social distance between X and Y.
Note that in the gravity model the observations are potential consolation relations. Only a
small minority of the potential is realized and leads to consolation. We use the terms ‘provider’
and ‘recipient’ throughout, to prevent the cumbersome notation of ‘potential consolation pro-
vider’ and ‘potential consolation recipient’. As consolation is either provided or not, we used a
logit regression equation:
log pij
1pij
!¼b1X1ij þb2X2ij þ. . . þbkXkij
Where p
ij
is the probability that individual iprovides consolation to individual j, where X
kij
are the characteristics of the robbery event, individual i, individual j, or their relation to each
other, and β
k
are regression weights. The logit command of software package Stata/MP version
11.2 was used to estimate the models.
The structure of the dyadic data prohibits a straightforward maximum likelihood estima-
tion of the logit model. The maximum likelihood logit model assumes independent observa-
tions, but dyads challenge this assumption for two reasons. First, dyads are nested in robberies,
and may thus share unobserved characteristics with other dyads in the same robbery. Second,
within a given robbery incident the dyads together represent a network structure. As a result,
dyads involving the same individual as a provider of consolation or as a receiver of consola-
tion, share unobserved characteristics of that individual. Both types of dependencies generate
downwardly biased standard errors of maximum likelihood estimates. To account for these
dependencies, we used the multiple regression quadratic assignment procedure (MRQAP) to
estimate the logit models. The MRQAP is a statistical strategy to test regression coefficient in
situations where independence assumptions are violated, and is typically used to correct for
interdependencies in dyadic network data [54,55]. We used the Stata module ‘qap’ to imple-
ment the quadratic assignment procedure.
Diagnostics
We checked for linear relations amongst the explanatory variables for all models by calculating
variance inflation factors of the explanatory variables and the condition numbers of the matri-
ces of independent variables [56]. The results are satisfactory and are not indicative of degrad-
ing collinearity issues. All variance inflation factors in all estimated models were below 1.25,
where values above 10 are generally seen as potentially problematic. The condition numbers of
all models were below 5, whereas values above 30 indicate potentially serious collinearity [56].
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 8 / 16
Results
The binary response variable in our analysis is whether consolation is provided by one individ-
ual to another. This variable is observed for every pairwise combination of individuals in the
aftermath of the same robbery. This setup allows for the observation of a variety of patterns,
including a single individual providing consolation to multiple others, and a single individual
being consoled by multiple others. Consolation was not observed in the aftermath of 4 (18.2%)
of the 22 robberies. In the remaining 18 cases, at least one person consoled at least one other
person. Of the 249 individuals that were observed in the aftermath of the 22 robberies, 38
(15.3%) provided consolation to others, and 24 (9.6%) received consolation from others.
As our interest in consolation behavior focused on who is providing consolation to whom,
we analyzed the data at the dyadic level. For each robbery iwe considered all possible N
i
× (N
i
-
1) (directed) pairs of individuals present in its aftermath, where N
i
is the number of individuals
present in the aftermath of robbery i. The 249 individuals in 22 robberies included 3710 dyads.
Using a logit model, we assessed which factors made consolation less or more likely to take
place between the two members of a pair. Table 1 displays the roles of the four most significant
factors.
Social closeness predicted consolation. When both potential provider and potential recipi-
ent were employees of the same business (and thus presumably familiar having shared com-
mon experiences), the odds of consolation were 7.4 times greater than when either one or both
of the dyad, were customers. Females were more likely to provide consolation than males. The
odds of consolation were 2.7 times greater when the potential provider was female. Although
some males provided consolation, females were more likely to offer it. Consolation was more
likely to be provided to individuals exposed to high levels of threat (i.e., exposure to physical
force or weapon threat). Victimization of a prospective recipient during the preceding robbery
was the most important factor predicting consolation. For victims of either force or threat with
a weapon, the odds of receiving consolation increased by a factor of 36.3.
To account for an unresponsive bystander effect due to a diffusion of responsibility [41],
the model included the total number of individuals present in the robbery aftermath. Contrary
to the hypothesized bystander effect, the odds of being consoled increased by two percent for
every additional individual present in the aftermath of the robbery. To assess whether consola-
tion is facilitated by opportunity, a model was estimated that included the physical distance
between dyad members, measured by whether the pair were more than 2 meters apart at the
start of the robbery aftermath. The results, reported in S4 Table, demonstrate that physical dis-
tance has no significant effect on the likelihood of providing consolation.
Although it has been argued that bystanders tend to help others in order to reduce their
own stress [57], the results presented in S5 Table demonstrate no significant relationship
Table 1. Logit analysis of consolation among 3680 dyads
4
. Quadratic assignment procedure.
Explanatory variable b
1
or
2
p
3
Socially close 1.999 7.38 .000
Potential provider female .978 2.66 .002
Potential recipient is victim 3.593 36.34 .000
Number of subjects in aftermath .019 1.02 .000
1
estimate (average of 1000 iterations)
2
odds ratio = exp(b)
3
p-value (based on 1000 iterations)
4
Four individuals’ roles (employee or customer) were unknown, affecting 30 dyads in total.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725.t001
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 9 / 16
between exposure to weapon threat or physical force and the likeliness of providing consola-
tion. High levels of threat entitled individuals to receive consolation but did not make them
more likely to provide it. Moreover, we did not find a relationship between receiving consola-
tion and gender (see S6 Table). Exposure to weapon threat or physical force, not gender, deter-
mined whether individuals were eligible to receive consolation. Male victims were as likely as
female victims to be touched by female consolers.
Based on the homophily principle, we tested whether consolation was more likely between
individuals similar in terms of age and ethnicity. Individuals were coded as similar in terms of
age if their estimated ages differed by less than 10 years (dyads with missing age estimates were
removed from the analysis). Individuals were coded as ethnically similar if they were estimated
to belong to the same ethnic group (dyads with missing ethnicity estimates were removed
from the analysis and individuals in the ‘other’ category were never coded as ethnically similar
to anyone). The outcomes of these models are displayed in S7 Table, and show that similarity
based on age or ethnicity was unrelated to the probability of consolation.
Finally, all potentially relevant factors measured were included simultaneously in a single
model. The results are displayed in Table 2, and again confirm that consolation is significantly
more likely to be provided amongst affiliated individuals (i.e., employees), to be given by
females, to be received by victims of threat and force (rather than received by bystanders) and
to be provided when more individuals are present. All other potential factors were non-
significant.
Discussion
Contemporary views in primatology consider empathic perspective-taking: putting oneself
emotionally and cognitively into the other’s shoes, to be an integral component of human and
nonhuman primate consolation [58]. According to this view, a chimpanzee bystander who
offers consolation most likely acts on empathic perspective-taking, while a monkey bystander
that offers comfort after being solicited to do so by a victim does not.
In the present study we showed that among human adults, as has been found in studies
with chimpanzees, social closeness increased the likelihood of post-aggression consolation.
Our study thus provides additional evidence that pro-social behavior is more likely among
people who are familiar [27]. It might be the case that it is easier for familiars to interpret each
Table 2. Logit analysis of consolation among 3364 dyads
4
. Quadratic assignment procedure.
Explanatory variable b
1
or
2
p
3
Socially close 2.040 7.69 .000
Potential provider female 1.002 2.72 .000
Potential recipient is victim 3.500 33.12 .000
Number of subjects in aftermath .037 1.04 .000
Physically close (<2m) .991 2.69 n.s.
Same age group .176 1.19 n.s.
Same ethnic group .032 1.03 n.s.
Potential provider is victim -.884 0.41 n.s.
Potential recipient is female .439 1.55 n.s.
1
estimate (average of 1000 iterations)
2
odds ratio = exp(b)
3
p-value (based on 1000 iterations), n.s. = p .05
4
Missing values on 346 dyads due to unknown values on the explanatory variables
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725.t002
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 10 / 16
other’s signals of distress [59]. Consolation may also be a way of establishing relationships or
strengthening existing ones [60]. Note that unlike measures of social closeness in other studies,
which are usually based on observed behavior that could indicate close affiliation (e.g., groom-
ing, food-sharing), our measure of social closeness was based on the assumption that employ-
ees know each other and share similar experiences. This assumption does not typically hold
for customers, nor for employees and customers.
In line with recent evidence on consolation in chimpanzees [9], we also found that females
were more likely to console than males. This supports the notion that females are more prone
to empathy-driven pro-social behavior than males [61–64]. One explanation for this finding is
that females may be more sensitive to the emotional signals of others [65] due to maternal func-
tions, social upbringing [66,67] and cultural expectations [68]. Across cultures, it is also more
acceptable for females than males to communicate through touching [46]. Taken together, the
above results are consistent with an empathy-based explanation of differences in consolation,
and suggest homologous mental processes of empathy in humans and chimpanzees.
Interestingly, females were not more likely to receive consolation than males. This chal-
lenges previous findings suggesting differential gender roles, particularly in dangerous situa-
tions [44,45]. It also challenges studies showing that it is more socially acceptable to touch
women than men [46]. Furthermore, our findings showed that for a given pair of individuals,
the likelihood of consolation increased with the number of other individuals present. This con-
tradicts the hypothesized bystander effect and confirms recent evidence that violence may
diminish the bystander effect [42] or even reverse it [4]. Unlike prior findings on consolation
in bonobos [12], our findings demonstrate that the physical distance between two individuals
does not affect the probability of consolation. This suggests that providing consolation
depends more on motivation than on opportunity.
Subjects who had been confronted with physical force or threats with weapons were much
more likely to receive consolation than subjects who had experienced lower levels of threat.
This shows the importance of considering threat levels while studying consolation, potentially
also in studies of non-human apes.
The finding that individuals who had been subjected to physical violence or threat of vio-
lence during the robbery were not more likely to provide consolation to others, suggests that
consolation was not primarily provided as a means of reducing one’s own stress; as the aver-
sive-arousal reduction hypothesis would predict. Rather consolation was provided as a means
of reducing the stress of others, thus supporting the empathy hypothesis. Future studies need
to investigate whether consolation in adults also exists in low intensity conflict, and whether it
follows the same patterns found here in high intensity conflicts.
Among humans, a sense of shared humanity is believed to mediate consolation [2]. Does a
shared sense of ‘chimpness’ mediate consolation in adult chimpanzees? At the perceptual level,
it appears that chimpanzees share such a common chimpness. Chimpanzees demonstrate self-
recognition by passing the mirror self-recognition test [69] and exhibit self-awareness by distin-
guishing in real time an object that is under their control from one that is not [70]. Chimpan-
zees discriminate chimpanzee faces from the faces of other primate species, recognize kin
relations by looking at conspecific faces, and tell apart and categorize expressions that are
modelled by the faces of other chimpanzees [71]. Fights arouse special attention in chimpan-
zees. Controlled video tests of emotional scenes show that chimpanzees pay more attention to
conspecific aggressive scenes than to other social situations, including play and general excite-
ment [72].
Comparative research across species can offer clues about how species-typical social experi-
ences and perception may be a factor in consolation. Delineating the roots and components of
consolation through comparative research reminds us that the differences in the mental lives
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 11 / 16
of animals are one of degree and not of kind. This ought to have implications for the practices
of behavioral sciences and for the way we attend to animals other than humans.
Supporting information
S1 File. ConsolationRobbery.csv (datafile).
(CSV)
S2 File. ConsolationRobberyStataCode.do (Stata script analysis).
(DO)
S1 Table. Codesheet for static variables during aftermath of robbery.
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Codesheet of dynamic variables during robbery. Partial ethogram of offender
behavior.
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Codesheet for dynamic variables during aftermath of robbery (ethogram).
(DOCX)
S4 Table. Logit analysis of consolation among 3680 dyads. Quadratic assignment procedure.
(DOCX)
S5 Table. Logit analysis of consolation among 3650 dyads. Quadratic assignment procedure.
(DOCX)
S6 Table. Logit analysis of consolation among 3680 dyads. Quadratic assignment procedure.
(DOCX)
S7 Table. Logit analysis of consolation among 3390 dyads. Quadratic assignment procedure.
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
CCTV footage was made accessible by the police forces of Amsterdam and Rotterdam, The
Netherlands. The video materials were coded by Tom de Vries and Floris Mosselman. The
Research and Documentation Centre (WODC) of the Netherlands Ministry of Security and
Justice supported the use of Observer XT software.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: MRL LL WB.
Data curation: WB.
Formal analysis: LL WB.
Funding acquisition: MRL WB.
Investigation: MRL WB.
Methodology: MRL LL WB.
Project administration: MRL.
Software: WB.
Supervision: MRL.
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 12 / 16
Validation: WB.
Writing – original draft: MRL LL WB.
Writing – review & editing: MRL LL WB PV MH RP ML.
References
1. Pavesich V. Hans Blumenberg: Philosophical Anthropology and the Ethics of Consolation. In: Honen-
berger P, editor. Naturalism and Philosophical Anthropology: Nature, Life, and the Human between
Transcendental and Empirical Perspectives. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK; 2015. p. 66–93.
2. Reich WT. From ancient consolation and negative care to modern empathy and the neurosciences.
Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics. 2012; 33(1):25–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11017-012-9212-6
PMID: 22278435
3. Collins R. Violence: A Micro-Sociological Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2008.
4. Levine M, Taylor PJ, Best R. Third Parties, Violence, and Conflict Resolution: The Role of Group Size
and Collective Action in the Microregulation of Violence. Psychological Science. 2011; 22(3):406–12.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611398495 PMID: 21303991
5. de Waal FBM, van Roosmalen A. Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behav Ecol
Sociobiol. 1979; 5(1):55–66.
6. Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F. Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences. 2008; 105(25):8557–62.
7. Palagi E, Cordoni G, Tarli SB. Possible Roles of Consolation in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglo-
dytes). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2006; 129(1):105–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.
20242 PMID: 16229027
8. Romero T, Castellanos MA, de Waal FBM. Post-Conflict Affiliation by Chimpanzees with Aggressors:
Other-Oriented versus Selfish Political Strategy. PLoS ONE. 2011; 6(7):e22173. https://doi.org/10.
1371/journal.pone.0022173 PMID: 21799788
9. Romero T, Castellanos MA, de Waal FBM. Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern
among chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010; 107(27):12110–5.
10. Wittig RM, Boesch C. Receiving Post-Conflict Affiliation from the Enemy’s Friend Reconciles Former
Opponents. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5(11):e13995. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013995 PMID:
21085592
11. Palagi E, Norscia I. Bonobos Protect and Console Friends and Kin. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8(11):e79290.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079290 PMID: 24223924
12. Clay Z, de Waal FBM. Bonobos Respond to Distress in Others: Consolation across the Age Spectrum.
PLoS ONE. 2013; 8(1):e55206. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055206 PMID: 23383110
13. Watts DP. Post-conflict Social Events in Wild Mountain Gorillas. II. Redirection, Side Direction, and
Consolation. Ethology. 1995; 100(2):158–74.
14. Cordoni G, Palagi E, Tarli SB. Reconciliation and consolation in captive western gorillas. International
Journal of Primatology. 2006; 27(5):1365–82.
15. Palagi E, Dall’Olio S, Demuru E, Stanyon R. Exploring the evolutionary foundations of empathy: conso-
lation in monkeys. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2014; 35(4):341–9.
16. Verbeek P, de Waal FBM. Postconflict Behavior of Captive Brown Capuchins in the Presence and
Absence of Attractive Food. International Journal of Primatology. 1997; 18(5):703–25.
17. Puga-Gonzalez I, Butovskaya M, Thierry B, Hemelrijk CK. Empathy versus Parsimony in Understand-
ing Post-Conflict Affiliation in Monkeys: Model and Empirical Data. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9(3):e91262.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091262 PMID: 24637720
18. Fraser ON, Koski SE, Wittig RM, Aureli F. Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression?
Communicative & Integrative Biology. 2009; 2(3):285–91.
19. Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FBM, Young LJ. Oxytocin-dependent consolation
behavior in rodents. Science. 2016; 351(6271):375–8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4785 PMID:
26798013
20. Verbeek P. Peace ethology. Behaviour. 2008; 145(11):1497–524.
21. Butovskaya M, Verbeek P, Ljungberg T, Lunardini A. A multicultural view of peacemaking among young
children. In: Aureli F, de Waal FBM, editors. Natural conflict resolution. Berkeley, CA: University of Cal-
ifornia Press; 2000. p. 243–58.
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 13 / 16
22. Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M, Wagner E, Chapman M. Development of concernfor others. Devel-
opmental Psychology. 1992; 28(1):126–36.
23. Fujisawa KK, Kutsukake N, Hasegawa T. Peacemaking and consolation in Japanese preschoolers wit-
nessing peer aggression. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2006; 120(1):48–57. https://doi.org/10.
1037/0735-7036.120.1.48 PMID: 16551164
24. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Spinrad TL. Prosocial development. In: Eisenberg N, editor. Handbook of
Child Psychology, Volume 3: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. 3. 6th ed. New Yor,
NY: Wiley; 2006. p. 646–718.
25. De Waal FBM. Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy. Annual Review of
Psychology. 2008; 59:279–300. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 PMID:
17550343
26. Eisenberg N, Shea CL, Carlo G, Knight GP. Empathy-Related Responding and Cognition: A “Chicken
and the Egg” Dilemma. In: Kurtines WM W.M., Gewirtz JL, editors. Handbook of Moral Behavior and
Development Volume 2: Research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2014. p. 63–88.
27. Preston SD, de Waal FB. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
2002; 25(1):1–20. PMID: 12625087
28. de Waal FBM. Peacekeeping among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1989.
29. de Waal FBM. Anthropomorphism and anthropodenial: consistency in our thinking about humans and
other animals. Philosophical Topics. 1999; 27(1):255–80.
30. Koski SE, Sterck EHM. Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: what’s in it for the third party?
American Journal of Primatology. 2009; 71(5):409–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20668 PMID:
19206165
31. Romero T. The peacemaking and peacekeeping role of chimpanzee bystanders. In: Verbeek P, Peters
BA, editors. Peace ethology: Behavioral processes and systems of peace. Chichester, UK: Wiley;
forthcoming. p. forthcoming.
32. Fraser ON, Aureli F. Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees.
American Journal of Primatology. 2008; 70(12):1114–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20608 PMID:
18767113
33. Romero T, de Waal FBM. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) consolation: Third-party identity as a window
on possible function. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2010; 124(3):278–86. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0019144 PMID: 20695659
34. Koski SE, Sterck EHM. Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation con-
sole? Animal Behaviour. 2007; 73(1):133–42.
35. de Waal FBM, Aureli F. Consolation, reconcilation, and a possible cognitive difference between
macaques and chimpanzees. 1996. In: Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes [Internet].
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; [80–110].
36. De Waal FBM. Prosocial Primates. In The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior (pp. 61–85). Oxford
University Press, USA. In: Schroeder DA, Graziano WG, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial
Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2015. p. 61–85.
37. McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks. Annual
Review of Sociology. 2001; 27:415–44.
38. Levine M, Cassidy C, Brazier G, Reicher S. Self-Categorization and Bystander Non-intervention: Two
Experimental Studies1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2002; 32(7):1452–63.
39. Saucier DA, Miller CT, Doucet N. Differences in Helping Whites and Blacks: A Meta-Analysis. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Review. 2005; 9(1):2–16. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0901_1
PMID: 15745861
40. Shah KSV, Shah ASV, Bhopal R. Systematic review and meta-analysis of out-of-hospital cardiac arrest
and race or ethnicity: black US populations fare worse. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology.
2014; 21(5):619–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/2047487312451815 PMID: 22692471
41. Darley JM, Latane B. Bystander Intervention in Emergencies: Diffusion of Responsibility. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology. 1968; 8(4, Pt.1):377–83. PMID: 5645600
42. Fischer P, Krueger JI, Greitemeyer T, Vogrincic C, Kastenmu¨ller A, Frey D, et al. The bystander-effect:
a meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psy-
chological Bulletin. 2011; 137(4):517. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023304 PMID: 21534650
43. Dovidio JF, Piliavin JA, Gaertner SL, Schroeder DA, Clark RD III. The arousal: cost-reward model and
the process of intervention: A review of the evidence. In: Clark MS, editor. Prosocial Behavior. Review
of personality and social psychology, Vol. 12. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1991. p. 86–118.
44. Udry JR. Biological Limits of Gender Construction. American Sociological Review. 2000; 65(3):443–57.
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 14 / 16
45. Stanko EA. Everyday violence: How women and men experience sexual and physical danger: Harper-
Collins; 1990.
46. Suvilehto JT, Glerean E, Dunbar RIM, Hari R, Nummenmaa L. Topography of social touching depends
on emotional bonds between humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015; 112
(45):13811–6.
47. Zimmerman PH, Bolhuis JE, Willemsen A, Meyer ES, Noldus LPJJ. The Observer XT: A tool for the
integration and synchronization of multimodal signals. Behavior Research Methods. 2009; 41(3):731–5.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.731 PMID: 19587185
48. Caro TM, Roper R, Young M, Dank GR. Inter-Observer Reliability. Behaviour. 1979; 69(3):303–15.
49. Krippendorff K. Reliability in Content Analysis. Human Communication Research. 2004; 30(3):411–33.
50. Troisi A. Displacement Activities as a Behavioral Measure of Stress in Nonhuman Primates and Human
Subjects. Stress. 2002; 5(1):47–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/102538902900012378 PMID: 12171766
51. Wilson AG, Bennett RJ. Mathematical Models in Human Geography. New York: John Wiley & Sons;
1985.
52. Haynes KA, Fotheringham AS. Gravity and Spatial Interaction Models. Thrall GI, editor. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage; 1984.
53. Kasakoff AB, Adams JW. Spatial Location and Social Organisation: An Analysis of Tikopian Patterns.
Man. 1977; 12(1):48–64.
54. Krackhardt D. Predicting with networks: Nonparametric multiple regression analysis of dyadic data.
Social Networks. 1988; 10(4):359–81.
55. Dekker D, Krackhardt D, Snijders TAB. Sensitivity of MRQAP Tests to Collinearity and Autocorrelation
Conditions. Psychometrika. 2007; 72(4):563–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-007-9016-1 PMID:
20084106
56. Belsley DA. A Guide to using the collinearity diagnostics. Computational Economics. 1991; 4(1):33–50.
57. Fischer P, Greitemeyer T, Pollozek F, Frey D. The unresponsive bystander: Are bystanders more
responsive in dangerous emergencies? European journal of social psychology. 2006; 36(2):267–78.
58. Plotnik JM, de Waal FBM. Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) reassure others in distress. PeerJ.
2014; 2:e278. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.278 PMID: 24688856
59. Sternglanz RW, DePaulo BM. Reading nonverbal cues to emotions: The advantages and liabilities of
relationship closeness. J NonV Beh. 2004; 28(4):245–66.
60. Aureli F, Cords M, Van Schaik CP. Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a pre-
dictive framework. Animal Behaviour. 2002; 64(3):325–43.
61. Eisenberg N, Lennon R. Sex Differences in Empathy and Related Capacities. Psychological Bulletin.
1983; 94(1):100–31.
62. Hertenstein MJ, Keltner D. Gender and the communication of emotion via touch. Sex roles. 2011; 64(1–
2):70–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9842-y PMID: 21297854
63. Taylor SE. Tend and Befriend: Biobehavioral Bases of Affiliation Under Stress. Current Directions in
Psychological Science. 2006; 15(6):273–7.
64. Norscia I, Demuru E, Palagi E. She more than he: gender bias supports the empathic nature of yawn
contagion in Homo sapiens. Royal Society Open Science. 2016; 3(2).
65. Han S, Fan Y, Mao L. Gender difference in empathy for pain: an electrophysiological investigation.
Brain research. 2008; 1196:85–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.062 PMID: 18221733
66. Chaplin TM, Cole PM, Zahn-Waxler C. Parental socialization of emotion expression: gender differences
and relations to child adjustment. Emotion. 2005; 5(1):80. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80
PMID: 15755221
67. Preston SD. The Origins of Altruism in Offspring Care. Psychological Bulletin. 2013; 139(6):1305–41.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031755 PMID: 23458432
68. Fischer AH, Rodriguez Mosquera PM, Van Vianen AEM, Manstead ASR. Gender and Culture Differ-
ences in Emotion. Emotion. 2004; 4(1):87–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.87 PMID:
15053728
69. Suddendorf T, Butler DL. The nature of visual self-recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013; 17
(3):121–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.004 PMID: 23410584
70. Kaneko T, Tomonaga M. The perception of self-agency in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proceedings
of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2011; 278:3694–702.
71. Parr LA, Micheletta J, Waller BM. Nonverbal Communication in Primates: Observational and Experi-
mental Approaches. In: Matsumoto D, Hwang HC, Frank MG, editors. APA Handbook of Nonverbal
Communication. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2015.
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 15 / 16
72. Kano F, Tomonaga M. Attention to Emotional Scenes Including Whole-Body Expressions in Chimpan-
zees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2010; 124(3):287–94. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0019146 PMID: 20695660
Consolation in the aftermath of robberies
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177725 May 31, 2017 16 / 16
Content uploaded by Marie Bruvik Heinskou
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marie Bruvik Heinskou on Jun 02, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.