Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
www.nature.com/scientificreports
The calming eect of a new
wearable device during the
anticipation of public speech
Ruben T. Azevedo1, Nell Bennett2, Andreas Bilicki2, Jack Hooper2, Fotini Markopoulou2 &
Manos Tsakiris1
We assessed the calming eect of doppel, a wearable device that delivers heartbeat-like tactile
stimulation on the wrist. We tested whether the use of doppel would have a calming eect on
physiological arousal and subjective reports of state anxiety during the anticipation of public speech, a
validated experimental task that is known to induce anxiety. Two groups of participants were tested in a
single-blind design. Both groups wore the device on their wrist during the anticipation of public speech,
and were given the cover story that the device was measuring blood pressure. For only one group, the
device was turned on and delivered a slow heartbeat-like vibration. Participants in the doppel active
condition displayed lower increases in skin conductance responses relative to baseline and reported
lower anxiety levels compared to the control group. Therefore, the presence, as opposed to its absence,
of a slow rhythm, which in the present study was instantiated as an auxiliary slow heartbeat delivered
through doppel, had a signicant calming eect on physiological arousal and subjective experience
during a socially stressful situation. This nding is discussed in relation to past research on responses
and entrainment to rhythms, and their eects on arousal and mood.
Wearable devices are becoming ubiquitous in everyday life, serving a range of functions, from measuring phys-
ical activity and a range of physiological variables to providing feedback on emotional states. Across most such
instantiations of wearable technologies, the overarching aim seems to be the quantication of the self1. However,
as recent studies suggest, the eects of such tracking devices are debatable2, seem to be short-lived, and con-
sumers oen stop using them aer a short period of time3. As a result, questions about what would be a more
intuitive wearable device that can assist people in their everyday life have motivated the development and design
of a new wearable device by Team Turquoise Ltd (http://www.doppel.london/). e doppel device, rather than
measuring physiological variables, aims at providing a sensory experience to help people manage the pressures of
time and stress in their daily lives by modulating the users’ physiological and potentially emotional and cognitive
states during a wide range of everyday life tasks. e doppel delivers an on-demand, discrete, user-controlled,
heartbeat-like vibration applied through a wristband. In the final product, the wristband is connected via
Bluetooth to a smart phone app where the user measures their resting heart rate and chooses their preferred
speed and intensity of tactile stimulation in relation to their own heartrate.
e design of doppel was inspired by several research strands that show how humans respond to and possibly
entrain to dierent rhythms. Entrainment is a broadly used term that, in the context of human behavior and phys-
iology, reects the voluntary or involuntary synchronization of our brains and bodies to the environment4. For
example, the tempo of a song can naturally alter our breathing rate and heart rate5. As a real-life example, choir
singers not only harmonise their voices, they can also synchronise their heartbeats6. Vickho and colleagues6
monitored the heart rates of singers and found that as the members sang in unison, their pulses began to speed
up and slow down at the same rate. Beyond music, several studies highlight entrainment eects in responses to
biological rhythms, and the heartbeat is perhaps the most ubiquitous biological rhythm in nature. e mother’s
own heartbeat is a particular salient multisensory signal that accompanies the development of the foetus and, not
surprisingly, has been shown to have benecial eects for the infant’s development. For example, the perception
of the maternal heartbeat in the womb provides the foetus with an important rhythmic experience7 that is thought
to facilitate the formation of the neural basis for entrainment and synchrony skills necessary for a wide range of
1Lab of Action & Body, Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham, TW20 0EX, UK.
2Team Turquoise Ltd, 1 Bermondsey Square, London, SE1 3UN, UK. Correspondence and requests for materials
should be addressed to M.T. (email: manos.tsakiris@rhul.ac.uk)
Received: 15 December 2016
Accepted: 18 April 2017
Published: xx xx xxxx
OPEN
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
cognitive processes that underpin vocal, gestural, and gaze communication during mother–infant interactions8.
Such entrainment eects to physiological rhythms are evidenced in a wide range of contexts in later life. For
example, the heartbeats of a mother and baby will synchronise with one another when they interact closely9, and
similar eects have been observed in couples10.
Beyond specic entrainment eects, more general, our brains and bodies also respond naturally to rhythm.
In music, tempo has been consistently shown to play an important role in mood induction and physiological
changes11–14. For example, listening to slower tempo results in lower arousal and subjective states of positive or
calm emotional states12, 15–17, see ref. 18 for review. Conversely, fast rhythms are associated with high arousal
and positive and/or arousing emotional states such as joy, excitement, surprise, fear or anger12, 18. Beyond the
perception of tempo in music, the physiological importance of faster and slower heart rate as well as of high and
low heart rate variability for physical and mental health is well-documented, as well as their role for experienced
arousal and cognitive processing in general19. Not only high arousal is physiologically correlated with increased
heart rate, and calmness with lower heart rate, as we also intuitively associate higher and lower heart rate with
anxiety or high arousal and calmness, respectively. Based on the aforementioned lines of research about the
importance of heartbeats as pervasive physiological signals involved in emotional regulation and social interac-
tions, and the eects of slow rhythms on mood, doppel was designed to embed on the users’ body, the ubiquitous
biological rhythm of a beating heart as a means of using the felt faster or slower rhythm to modulate levels of
arousal and calmness. To that end, the present study was designed to test whether the use of doppel, as opposed
to its absence, could have a calming eect on physiological arousal and subjective reports of experienced anxiety
during a stressful situation.
Stressful situations are typically associated with increases in physiological arousal and negative affect.
However, stressful contexts dier from one another in character. For example, a mental arithmetic challenge dif-
fers from a respiratory challenge, which both dier from a social stress challenge. Socially stressful situations may
involve the threat of negative social evaluation (e.g., public speaking challenge) or may be accompanied by nega-
tive social evaluation resulting in social rejection (e.g., social exclusion manipulation). Given the signicance of
interoception in cognitive-aective processing20–22, potential changes in interoceptive signals (i.e. signals originat-
ing from visceral organs such as the heart) in stressful, negative and aective situations may inuence emotional
experience, emotion regulation and decision making in these contexts. In such situations, the ability to regulate
one’s physiological arousal and emotions may be important for lowering anxiety and for successful performance.
A classic task that has been used in controlled psychological experiments to elicit an ecologically valid and
socially stressful situation, is the anticipation of public speech task. Across a large number of studies, it has been
shown that anticipation of public speaking is particularly eective at inducing social anxiety with concomitant
increases in physiological arousal23–26. erefore, the current study utilized the speech anticipation manipulation
to examine how the use of doppel could enable participants to cope better with their anxiety while they were
preparing to deliver a speech to a group of people. Moreover, given the intended use of the product, we were
particularly interested in testing the ecacy of its use against a condition where participants were not using it.
Importantly, for the purposes of this study, participants were unaware of the true function of doppel as we wanted
to minimize expectations and potential confounds. To that end, participants were told that doppel was a device
designed to measure their blood pressure. Two groups of participants were tested. Both groups wore the device
on their wrist, but only for one group doppel was turned on and vibrated during the anticipation period. Doppel
was set to vibrate with a frequency ~20% slower than the participant’s heartbeats, as measured at rest, because
previous research has shown that slower rhythms tend to be associated with low arousal and subjective states of
calmness/serenity, sadness or boredom18. Before instructions were given and throughout the anticipatory period,
levels of physiological arousal were measured through continuous recordings of skin conductance and heart
rate. In addition, we collected psychometric data to quantify state anxiety levels before and aer the anticipation
of public speech task. It was hypothesized, that the doppel device active group would display lower increases in
physiological arousal and report lower levels of state anxiety. It should be noted that the method adopted here
with doppel diers from biofeedback or false feedback techniques27–29 in which exteroceptive stimulation reects
ongoing cardiac activity and participants are encouraged to pay attention and/or regulate it. Instead, we tested
whether periodic vibrotactile stimulation unrelated to participant’s ongoing heartbeats, as opposed to the absence
of such stimulation, could have a positive calming eect.
Methods
Participants. A total of 52 (20 Male; mean age = 26.4, s.d. = 5.7) volunteers took part in the study. Twenty
five participants (9 Male; mean age = 25.9, s.d. = 5.2) were assigned to the experimental group (i.e. doppel
active condition) and 27 (11 Male; mean age = 26.8, s.d. = 6.1) to the control group (i.e. doppel turned o con-
dition). Participants were randomly assigned to each group prior to study start. All participants provided writ-
ten informed consent to take part in this study, which was approved by the College Ethics Committee of Royal
Holloway University of London research ethics committee, and all methods were performed in accordance with
the relevant guidelines and regulations of the College Ethics Committee.
Procedure. A graphical illustration of the study’s procedure and timeline can be seen in Fig.1. Participants
were comfortably sat in an armchair, given written information about the study and asked to sign a consent form.
Skin conductance Ag/AgCl electrodes (MLT117F) with 0.5%-NaCl electrode gel were attached to the phalanges
of the middle and ring ngers of the participant’s non-dominant hand. e signal was recorded using a Powerlab
8/35 (https://www.adinstruments.com/), a GSR Amp unit (22 mV constant voltage at 75HZ) and LabChart (v 8.1)
soware with a recording range of 40 µS and a sampling rate of 1 kHz. A pulse transducer (TN1012/ST; https://
www.adinstruments.com/) was also attached to the thumb of non-dominant hand to monitor cardiac activity.
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
3
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
e doppel device (see Fig.1A) was tted to the participant’s non-dominant wrist, in a rm but not tight posi-
tion, and remained o until the period preceding task instructions. e device sits on the inside of the wrist where
users expect to feel their pulse. e unit is 38 mm in diameter & 9 mm thick, held in place with a silicon strap.
e device provides a double heartbeat-like rhythm tactile sensation. e main casing has a steep metal base and
a plastic cover. e casing houses the electronic components: a coin vibration motor that delivers the stimulus, a
low energy Bluetooth chip that communicates with an app or computer to upload new rhythms to the device, and
a lithium ion battery that powers the device.
For the purposes of this study, participants were told that it was a new generation device to monitor blood
pressure, and none of the participants expressed any suspicion about the purpose of the device at any point
throughout the entire duration of the study. A single-blind protocol was adopted as necessary to provide the
experimenter with appropriate control over doppel (i.e. switch o to those assigned to the control group) and to
prevent participants from using cognitive strategies during the task related to the study’s goals.
At the beginning of the experiment, participants were instructed to rest for 5 minutes while sitting still, with
the purpose of measuring baseline levels of physiological activity. Average heart rate and skin conductance levels
during this period were taken as indices of pre-task physiological activity. Aer this rest period, the experimenter
told participants “I am going to change the settings of the device you have on the wrist to start measuring blood
pressure. You might feel some vibration. If you do, don’t worry it’s normal”. e device was then turned on in
all cases but switched o aer 10 seconds if the participant had been assigned to the control group. e device’s
vibration frequency was individually tailored to a frequency ~20% slower (mean = 58.2, s.d. = 6.3) than the par-
ticipant’s baseline HR (mean = 75.8, s.d. = 12.3), as measured during the rest period. However, upper (65 bpm)
and lower (40 bpm) frequency limits were imposed to prevent vibrations to be perceived as fast or unnaturally
slow, respectively. In the experimental group, the lower limit was applied to one participant (rest HR = 44.7) and
the upper limit to 9 participants (mean rest HR = 88.1, s.d. = 5.5). e average absolute adjustment to lower and
upper limits was of 4.4 bpm (s.d. = 3.9). Next, task instructions were provided verbally by the experimenter: “You
should now prepare a speech about the use of animals for research. You may present both the pros and cons of
using animals in research or give your personal point of view on this issue. You can use this pen and paper to take
notes if you wish to. You have 5 minutes to prepare it. Aer that we will move to the room next door where you
should give the speech to 3 or 4 colleagues of mine. e speech should last for 5 minutes”.
At the end of the study, to assess the success of our manipulation, participants were invited, in a
semi-structured way, to share their subjective experiences during task. First they were asked “how were you feel-
ing about giving a speech to unfamiliar people”? en the experimenter directly enquired if participants believed
they would actually give the speech. All participants, but one, were convinced they would give the speech.
Subjective Measures. Participants were asked to complete the STAI-Y-130 twice to measure self-reported
anxiety levels before the delivery of task instructions and aer the task completion. e STAI-Y-1 questionnaire
is composed of 20 items (e.g. “I am tense”; “I feel secure”) assessing state anxiety. Each item is rated on a 4-point
scale ranging from “not at all” to “very much so”. High scores reect high anxiety levels. To control for possible
dierences between groups in trait apprehension about other’s evaluations, participants were asked to complete
the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation questionnaire (bFNE)31. e bFNE is composed of 12 items (e.g. “I am
afraid others will not approve of me”) scored on a 5-point scale ranging from “not at all characteristic of me” to
“extremely characteristic of me”. High scores reect high concern about others’ evaluation. To control for pos-
sible between-group dierences in participants’ general disposition towards public speaking and self-reported
trait social anxiety, participants were asked to state their agreement, on 8-point likert scales, on the following
sentences: “I like to speak in public” and “I can easily become anxious in social situations” (1- strongly disa-
gree; 8 – strongly agree). ese questions and the bFNE were only asked aer task completion to avoid inducing
task anticipation anxiety or evaluation concerns during baseline physiological recordings. Finally, they rated on
7-point likert scales, their experience during the task according to the dimensions: from relaxing (1) to stressful
(7); and from unpleasant (1) to pleasant (7). e questionnaire assessing self-reported social anxiety, attitudes
towards public speaking and subjective ratings of participants’ experience during the task was not administered
to the rst 6 participants (Experimental group: n = 2; Control group: n = 4) due to an experimental error.
Figure 1. e doppel device (A) and the timeline of the experimental procedure (B).
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
4
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
Analyses of psychophysiological data. Non-specic-SCR or spontaneous uctuations in skin conduct-
ance (NS-SCRs) analyses were carried out using the SCRalyze b2.18 soware (http://pspm.sourceforge.net/ 32)
according to a convolution model to estimate the area and the curve (AUC), or skin-conductance level-corrected
time integral. e AUC is closely related to the conventional methods of estimation of the number and amplitude
of NS-SCRs (for further details see ref. 32). is method has been shown to be a better predictor of autonomic
arousal than conventional analysis, and has the advantages of not requiring subjective evaluations as well as being
computationally inexpensive32. HR was estimated with the HRV function implemented in Labchart (v 8.1). Visual
inspection and manual correction was carried out to correct unidentied or misidentied peaks. Data from three
participants (control group) were discarded from HR analyses due to technical failure during pulse recording.
e entire ve-minute periods during rest and speech preparation were analysed to estimate baseline and task
NS-SCRs and HR levels, respectively. Skin conductance levels and HR were entered into separate repeated meas-
ures ANOVAs with Time (Baseline; Task) and Group (Experimental; Control) as within- and between-subjects
factors, respectively.
Results
Psychophysiological Data. e analysis of NS-SCRs levels revealed a signicant main eect of Time (F(1,
50) = 36.69, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.42) conrming, as expected, that speech anticipation resulted in an overall increase
in physiological arousal (see Fig.2A), providing a proof that the task was successful in eliciting arousal. e
main eect of Group was not signicant (F(1, 50) = 3.21, p = 0.079, ƞ2 = 0.060). Crucially, the critical Time x
Group device (F(1, 50) = 5.23, p = 0.027, ƞ2 = 0.095) interaction was signicant. Follow-up independent t-tests
conrmed lower skin conductance levels in the Experimental group than in Control group during the task (t(1,
50) = 2.24, p = 0.029, Hedges’ g = 0.54) but no dierence at baseline (t(1, 50) = 0.271, p = 0.79, Hedges’ g = 0.074).
We then investigated changes in average Heart Rate. While the main eect of Time (F(1, 47) = 30.02, p < 0.001,
ƞ2 = 0.39) on HR was signicant because HR increased during anticipation relative to baseline, the Time x Group
interaction (F(1, 47) = 0.47, p = 0.50, ƞ2 = 0.010) was not signicantly dierent. Furthermore, the main eect of
Group was not signicant (F(1, 47) = 0.67, p = 0.42, ƞ2 = 0.014).
Subjective measures. To assess dierences in self-reported anxiety at baseline and aer speech preparation,
pre and post STAI scores (see Table1) were entered into a repeated measures ANOVA with Time (baseline and
post-task) and Group (Experimental; Control) as within- and between-subjects factors, respectively. Results con-
rmed that speech preparation successfully induced anxiety in the participants, as revealed by the main eect of
Time (F(1, 50) = 53.91, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.52). e interaction Time x Group (F(1, 50) = 4.14, p = 0.047, ƞ2 = 0.077,
see Fig.2B) and the main eect of Group (F(1, 50) = 6.75, p = 0.012, ƞ2 = 0.12) were also signicant. Independent
t-tests revealed no group dierences in anxiety at baseline (t(1, 50) = 1.34, p = 0.19, Hedges’ g = 0.36) but lower
levels of anxiety in the Experimental group aer speech preparation (t(1, 50) = 2.79, p = 0.007, Hedges’ g = 0.77).
We also carried out independent t-tests on the participants’ ratings on the task experience as relaxing/stressful
and unpleasant/pleasant. Results showed that participants in the Experimental group rated the experience as less
stressful than those in the control group (t(1, 44) = 2.35, p = 0.023, Hedges’ g = 0.68). Ratings on the pleasant-
ness question did not reach statistical signicance (t(1, 44) = 1.94, p = 0.059, Hedges’ g = 0.56). Control analy-
ses revealed no dierences between groups on the obtained trait measures: (i) fear of negative evaluation (t(1,
Figure 2. Average Skin Conductance levels across conditions and groups (A), and average state anxiety scores
(B). Error bars indicate S.E.M.
STAI pre STAI Post *bFNE Social
anxiety Public
speaking Pleasantness Stressfulness*
Experimental Group 30.2 (8.0) 38.1 (12.6) 36.1 (9.8) 5.00 (2.2) 3.90 (2.2) 5.36 (1.2) 2.96 (1.5)
Control Group 33.1 (7.5) 47.0 (10.3) 37.0 (10.2) 5.09 (1.8) 3.65 (1.9) 4.39 (1.8) 4.09 (1.7)
Table 1. Average (s.d.) scores on each questionnaire in the Experimental and Control groups. *p < 0.05.
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
5
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
50) = 0.30, p = 0.76, Hedges’ g = 0.082); (ii) social anxiety (t(1, 44) = 0.15, p = 0.88, Hedges’ g = 0.044); (iii) and
attitude towards public speaking (t(1, 44) = 0.43, p = 0.67, Hedges’ g = 0.12).
Discussion
We tested the ecacy of a new wearable device on calmness using a task that typically induces high anxiety,
namely, the preparation and anticipation of giving a short public speech to an unfamiliar audience. doppel gives an
on-demand, discrete, user-controlled, heartbeat-like vibration applied through a wristband and it aims at mod-
ulating the alertness or calmness of the user by adjusting the frequency of the “tactile heartbeat”. In the present
experiment, we hypothesized that a slow heart-rate, as opposed to the absence of any vibro-tactile stimulation,
would enable participants to remain calmer during the anticipation of public speech. To that end, we told partici-
pants that doppel was a blood pressure monitoring device, and only for one of the two groups we turned on doppel
to deliver its tactile vibrations during the anticipation of public speech. Additionally, subsequent oral debrieng
conrmed that participants believed that they would actually give the speech and thought that, indeed, doppel
monitored blood pressure.
Even though, at the beginning of the experiment, the two groups displayed comparable levels of arousal (as
measured by Skin Conductance) and of state anxiety, participants in the doppel active condition showed a signif-
icantly smaller increase in arousal compared to the control group. Similarly, at the end of their speech prepara-
tion, and before they were debriefed, participants in the doppel active condition reported a signicantly smaller
increase in their state anxiety compared to the control group. No signicant dierences in average heart rate were
found as a result of doppel use. Finally, participants in the doppel active condition found the task signicantly less
stressful than participants in the control condition. Taken together, the results highlight that the use of doppel had
a clear and signicant calming eect in both physiological measures of arousal and subjective reports of anxiety
during a task that is eective in inducing social stress, suggesting that doppel enabled participants to stay calmer
and less anxious, as compared with the condition where the device was worn but was not performing its intended
function.
Previous studies have used pharmacological means to study anxiolytic-like eects during anticipation of
public speech or during simulated public speaking33. For example, de Oliveira and colleagues33 used intranasal
administration of oxytocin, a neuropeptide that is known to be involved in anxiety, as well as cardiovascular and
hormonal regulation, and showed that the oxytocin, but not the placebo, group had lower skin-conductance levels
during anticipation and actual speech. Biofeedback techniques have also been used to down-regulate anxiety and
physiological reactions during emotional situations27, 28. In particular, autonomic regulation training through bio-
feedback has been shown to reduce subjective and physiological arousal during public speaking27. Here, we add
to this literature by showing that the use of heartbeat-like vibrotactile stimulation, as opposed to its absence, can
also have an anxiolytic eect. ese two techniques (i.e. biofeedback and doppel use), however, dier considerably
in terms of the expectations and attention given to the exteroceptive stimuli. Unlike in biofeedback or false feed-
back approaches, in our active doppel condition, participants were not instructed to regulate their heart rate as a
function of these vibrations. Rather than feedback, doppel stimulation is thought to be related to physiological
entrainment mechanisms that are not dependent on direct attention or appraisals of interoceptive-exteroceptive
matching.
Numerous past studies have also shown how our bodies, including physiological cycles such as the respiratory
and cardiac cycles, entrain to dierent rhythms and how such responses may inuence mood11–14, 34. Reviewing a
large body of relevant research, Phillips-Silver and colleagues35 show that the ability to perceive and synchronize
to a beat is possible across dierent sensory modalities. e authors suggest a general model of entrainment that
builds upon pre-existing adaptations that allow us to perceive and produce rhythmic stimuli, as well as inte-
grate their production and perception with sensory feedback. Extending the eects of rhythms on mood, beyond
entrainment per se, Tajadura-Jiménez, Väljamäe and Väställ36 reported that heartbeat sounds and their rhythm
signicantly aected participants’ physiological reactions as well as the emotional judgments of pictures, and
their recall. In that study, listening to heartbeat sounds resulted in a small but signicant increase in participants’
heart rate with respect to the silence condition, and the sounds signicantly inuenced emotional responses to
aective visual stimuli. In particular, a fast-rate heartbeat (e.g. 110 bpm) sound resulted in higher arousal ratings
and enhanced pictures’ memorability during a free-recall task. It is noteworthy that in the present study we did
not nd any evidence of signicant modulations of heart rate, other than the overall increase of heart rate during
the anticipation period. is is not surprising given that typically entrainment eects on heart rate are relatively
small11, 12, 36, and in the present study we employed a task (i.e., anticipation of public speaking) that is meant to
increase heart rate. Entrainment eects on physiological activity have been mostly studied in response to musical
stimuli11–14. However, tempo-related entrainment eects on heart rate are less consistent than in other physiolog-
ical indices, such as skin conductance and respiration frequency, especially when entraining to slow tempo stim-
uli11, 12, 37, 38. It is possible that this might be related to the non-linear dynamics of cardiac responses to stressors or
emotional stimuli. e continuous uctuations of heart rate due to excitatory and inhibitory regulation processes
may explain why average heart rate, like the one reported here, is a less sensitive index to entrainment eects.
Unlike past studies that often use auditory feedback of heartbeats29, 36, 39, 40, instead of sounds, we used
heartbeat-like tactile stimulation to examine the eects of a calming “heartbeat” rate on anxiety. While no signif-
icant dierences were observed in participants’ heartrate, the consistent patterns of changes in skin conductance
levels and subjective anxiety reports support the hypothesis that the presence of a slow rhythm, which in the
present study was instantiated as an auxiliary slow heartbeat delivered through doppel, can inuence physiolog-
ical arousal and mood. We used slower frequencies from the participants’ own heart rate because, as previous
research has suggested, slower rhythms tend to be associated with low arousal and subjective states of positive
or calm emotional states12, 18. However, it remains to be tested whether faster or equivalent frequencies to the
participant’s heart rate can have similar eects. It is possible that anxiolytic eects can be observed to a range of
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
6
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
heart-like vibrotactile stimulation frequencies and even that the “ideal” frequency varies from person to person.
Importantly, while we here show the calming eects for slower frequencies, doppel users can select the vibrating
frequency that corresponds better with their state and possible the one that seems to be more ecient for their
own needs and lifestyles.
While the precise underlying neurophysiological mechanism by which doppel elicits such eects was not
directly investigated in the present study, we suggest that the calming eect may be mediated by neural responses
to the rhythmic tactile input that the device delivers, especially neural responses in areas that are involved in
somatosensation and interoception such as the somatosensory cortex and the insula41,42 and in emotion and
rhythm processing such as the basal ganglia43. Future studies must specically investigate such neural responses
in parallel with the attenuated changes in skin conductance to complement the signicant and converging phys-
iological and subjective measures of reduced arousal and anxiety obtained through the use of doppel, as reported
here.
References
1. Lupton D, e Quantied Self, Polity Press (2016).
2. Jaicic, J. M. et al. Eect of Wearable Technology Combined With a Lifestyle Intervention on Long-term Weight Loss. JAMA 316,
1161–1171, doi:10.1001/jama.2016.12858 (2016).
3. Ledger, D. & McCarey, D. How the Science of Human Behavior Change Oers the Secret to Long-Term Engagement. etrieved 4
March 2017 from https://endeavourpartners.net/assets/Endeavour-Partners-We arables-White-Paper-20141.pdf (2014).
4. oss, J. M. & Balasubramaniam, . Physical and neural entrainment to rhythm: human sensorimotor coordination across tass and
eector systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 576, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00576 (2014).
5. Larsen, P. D. & Galletly, D. C. e sound of silence is music to the heart. Heart 92, 433–4, doi:10.1136/hrt.2005.071902 (2006).
6. Vicho, B. et al. Music structure determines heart rate variability of singers. Front. Psychol. 4, 334, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00334
(2013).
7. Webb, A. ., Heller, H. T., Benson, C. B. & Lahav, A. Mother’s voice and heartbeat sounds elicit auditory plasticity in the human
brain before full gestation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 3152–7, doi:10.1073/pnas.1414924112 (2015).
8. Feldman, . Infant Biological Foundations Synchrony and Developmental Outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 340–345,
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x (2007).
9. Feldman, ., Magori-Cohen, ., Galili, G., Singer, M. & Louzoun, Y. Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes
of interaction synchrony. I nfant B ehav. Dev. 34, 569–577, doi:10.1016/j.ineh.2011.06.008 (2011).
10. Ferrer, E. & Helm, J. L. Dynamical systems modeling of physiological coregulation in dyadic interactions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88,
296–308, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.10.013 (2013).
11. Etzel, J. A., Johnsen, E. L., Dicerson, J., Tranel, D. & Adolphs, . Cardiovascular and respiratory responses during musical mood
induction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 61, 57–69, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.025 (2006).
12. Gomez, P. & Danuser, B. elationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion 7,
377–387, doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377 (2007).
13. Trost, W., Labbé, C. & Grandjean, D. hythmic entrainment as musical emotion induction mechanism. Neuropsychologia 96,
96–110, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.004 (2017).
14. rabs, . U., En, ., Teich, N. & oelsch, S. Autonomic eects of music in health and Crohn’s disease: e impact of isochronicity,
emotional valence, and tempo. PLoS One 10, 1–30, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126224 (2015).
15. Ziegler, G., Dahne, ., Yeragani, V. . & Bär, .-J. e relation of ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity and heart rate uctuations
at rest. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 2205–2210, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07008.x (2009).
16. Van Der Zwaag, M. D., Westerin, J. H. D. M. & Van Den Broe, E. L. Emotional and psychophysiological responses to tempo, mode,
and percussiveness. Music. Sci. 15, 250–269 (2011).
17. Bresin, . & Friberg, A. Emotion rendering in music: ange and characteristic values of seven musical variables. Cortex 47,
1068–1081, doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.009 (2011).
18. Gabrielson, A. In Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (ed. Cross, I., Hallam, S., aut, M.) 141–150 (Oxford University Press,
2009).
19. reibig, S. D. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 84, 394–421, doi:10.1016/j.
biopsycho.2010.03.010 (2010).
20. Dunn, B. D. et al. Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision maing. Psychol. Sci.
21, 1835–1844, doi:10.1177/0956797610389191 (2010).
21. Garnel, S. N., Seth, A. ., Barrett, A. B., Suzui, . & Critchley, H. D. nowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive
accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74, doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004 (2015).
22. Herbert, B. M. & Pollatos, O. e Body in the Mind: On the elationship Between Interoception and Embodiment. Top. Cogn. Sci.
4, 692–704, doi:10.1111/tops.2012.4.issue-4 (2012).
23. Durli, C., Brown, G. & Tsairis, M. Enhanced interoceptive awareness during anticipation of public speaing is associated with fear
of negative evaluation. Cogn. Emot. 28, 530–540, doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.832654 (2014).
24. Eglo, B., Schmule, S. C., Burns, L. . & Schwerdtfeger, A. Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaing tass:
Associations with negative aect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion 6, 356–366, doi:10.1037/1528-
3542.6.3.356 (2006).
25. Moscovitch, D. A., Suva, M. . & Hofmann, S. G. Emotional response patterns during social threat in individuals with generalized
social anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls. J. Anxiety Disord. 24, 785–791, doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.05.013 (2010).
26. Stevens, S. et al. Heartbeat perception in social anxiety before and during speech anticipation. Behav. Res. er. 49, 138–143,
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.009 (2011).
27. Mcinney, M. E. & Gatchel, . J. e comparative eectiveness of heart rate biofeedbac, speech sills training, and a combination
of both in treating public-speaing anxiety. Biofeedback Self. Regul. 7, 71–87, doi:10.1007/BF00999056 (1982).
28. Peira, N., Fredrison, M. & Pourtois, G. Controlling the emotional heart: Heart rate biofeedbac improves cardiac control during
emotional reactions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 225–231, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.12.008 (2014).
29. Gray, M. A., Harrison, N. A., Wiens, S. & Critchley, H. D. Modulation of Emotional Appraisal by False Physiological Feedbac
during fMI. PLoS One 2, e546, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000546 (2007).
30. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, . L., Lushene, ., Vagg, P. ., & Jacobs, G. A. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Title.
(Consulting Psychologists Press) (1983).
31. Leary, M. . A Brief Version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 9, 371–375,
doi:10.1177/0146167283093007 (1983).
32. Bach, D. ., Friston, . J. & Dolan, . J. Analytic measures for quantication of arousal from spontaneous sin conductance
uctuations. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 76, 52–5, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.011 (2010).
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
7
Scientific RepoRts | 7: 2285 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-02274-2
33. de Oliveira, D. C., Zuardi, A. W., Grae, F. G., Queiroz, . H. & Crippa, J. A. Anxiolytic-lie eect of oxytocin in the simulated public
speaing test. J. Psychopharmacol. 26, 497–504, doi:10.1177/0269881111400642 (2012).
34. Anishcheno, V. S., Balanov, A. G., Janson, N. B., Igosheva, N. B. & Bordyugov, G. V. Entrainment Between Heart ate and Wea
Noninvasive Forcing. Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos 10, 2339–2348, doi:10.1142/S0218127400001468 (2000).
35. Phillips-Silver, J., Atipis, C. A. & Bryant, G. A. e ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music
Percept. 28, 3–14, doi:10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.3 (2010).
36. Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A. & Väställ, D. Self-epresentation in Mediated Environments: e Experience of Emotions
Modulated by Auditory-Vibrotactile Heartbeat. CyberPsychology Behav. 11, 33–38, doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.0002 (2008).
37. halfa, S., oy, M., ainville, P., Dalla Bella, S. & Peretz, I. ole of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological dierentiation of
happy and sad music? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 68, 17–26, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001 (2008).
38. Bernardi, L., Porta, C. & Sleight, P. Cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and respiratory changes induced by dierent types of music in
musicians and non-musicians: the importance of silence. Heart 92, 445–52, doi:10.1136/hrt.2005.064600 (2006).
39. Lenggenhager, B., Azevedo, . T., Mancini, A. & Aglioti, S. M. Listening to your heart and feeling yourself: eects of exposure to
interoceptive signals during the ultimatum game. Exp. Brain Res. 230, 233–241, doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3647-5 (2013).
40. Azevedo, . T. et al. Participants’ above-chance recognition of own-heart sound combined with poor metacognitive awareness
suggests implicit nowledge of own heart cardiodynamics. Sci. Rep. 6, 26545, doi:10.1038/srep26545 (2016).
41. Couto, B. et al. e man who feels two hearts: the dierent pathways of interoception. Soc. Cogn. Aect. Neurosci. 9, 1253–60,
doi:10.1093/scan/nst108 (2014).
42. halsa, S. S., udrauf, D., Feinstein, J. S. & Tranel, D. e pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1494–1496,
doi:10.1038/nn.2411 (2009).
43. Trost, W. et al. Getting the beat: Entrainment of brain activity by musical rhythm and pleasantness. Neuroimage 103, 55–64,
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.009 (2014).
Acknowledgements
The study was commissioned and funded by Team Turquoise Ltd. Team Turquoise Ltd provided the direct
research expenses related to the reimbursement of the participating volunteers and the salary costs for RA.
Author Contributions
M.T. and R.A. designed the study. R.A. collected the data, M.T. and R.A. analyzed the data and co-authored the
manuscript. F.T., A.B., N.B. & J.H. contributed to the authorship of the manuscript.
Additional Information
Competing Interests: N.B., A.B., J.H. and F.T. co-own Team Turquoise Ltd. M.T. is an independent consultant
for Team Turquoise Ltd.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2017