ArticlePublisher preview available

Developmental Continuity and Change in Physical, Verbal, and Relational Aggression and Peer Victimization From Childhood to Adolescence

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

To investigate the developmental course of aggression and peer victimization in childhood and adolescence, distinct subgroups of children were identified based on similarities and differences in their physical, verbal and relational aggression, and victimization. Developmental continuity and change were assessed by examining transitions within and between subgroups from Grades 1 to 11. This longitudinal study consisted of 482 children (50% females) and was based on peer report data on multiple forms of aggression and peer victimization. Using person-centered methods including latent profile and latent transition analyses, most of the identified subgroups were distinguishable by their frequencies (i.e., levels) of aggression and victimization, rather than forms (physical, verbal, and relational), with the exception of 1 group that appeared to be more form-specific. Across subgroups, multiple developmental patterns emerged characterized as early and late-onset, social interactional continuity, desistance, and heterotypic pathways. Collectively, these pathways support the perspective that the development of aggression and peer victimization in childhood and adolescence is characterized by heterogeneity. (PsycINFO Database Record
Developmental Continuity and Change in Physical, Verbal, and Relational
Aggression and Peer Victimization From Childhood to Adolescence
Idean Ettekal
University at Buffalo, State University of New York
Gary W. Ladd
Arizona State University
To investigate the developmental course of aggression and peer victimization in childhood and adoles-
cence, distinct subgroups of children were identified based on similarities and differences in their
physical, verbal and relational aggression, and victimization. Developmental continuity and change were
assessed by examining transitions within and between subgroups from Grades 1 to 11. This longitudinal
study consisted of 482 children (50% females) and was based on peer report data on multiple forms of
aggression and peer victimization. Using person-centered methods including latent profile and latent
transition analyses, most of the identified subgroups were distinguishable by their frequencies (i.e.,
levels) of aggression and victimization, rather than forms (physical, verbal, and relational), with the
exception of 1 group that appeared to be more form-specific. Across subgroups, multiple developmental
patterns emerged characterized as early and late-onset, social interactional continuity, desistance, and
heterotypic pathways. Collectively, these pathways support the perspective that the development of
aggression and peer victimization in childhood and adolescence is characterized by heterogeneity.
Keywords: aggression, relational aggression, peer victimization, bullying, latent transition analysis
It is widely recognized that peer victimization and aggression
are pervasive social problems in childhood and adolescence. In-
deed, one inference that can be made from a growing body of
research is that the perpetration of peer directed aggression and
experiences of peer victimization impact children and adolescents
of different ages, genders, social classes, ethnic groups, and na-
tionalities (Juvonen & Graham, 2001;Lansford et al., 2012). When
examined conjointly, some children are both perpetrators and
victims of peer-directed aggression—aggressive-victims—and ap-
pear to have individual characteristics and social experiences that
are unique from those of children who are primarily aggressors or
victims (Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2003;Pellegrini, Bartini, & Brooks,
1999;Schwartz, 2000;Schwartz, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997).
Although distinguishing aggressive-victims from aggressors and
(nonaggressive) victims appears to be an important conceptual
distinction with significant implications for children’s adjustment
more broadly, most of the investigations on the development of
peer aggression and victimization have focused on these factors
independently of one another. Thus, additional longitudinal re-
search is warranted to provide further insights into the distinct
developmental course of aggressive-victims from childhood to
adolescence. Accordingly, this study had two specific aims: (a) to
identify the nature (frequencies and forms) of aggressive-victims,
aggressors, and victims in childhood and adolescence, and (b) to
investigate the continuity and changes between and within these
distinct subgroups during this developmental period.
Theoretical Perspectives on the Development of
Childhood Aggression
Early and Late-Onset
Several developmental theories have been proposed to explicate
the development of antisocial behaviors, and aggression in partic-
ular, during childhood and adolescence. Moffitt’s (1993) develop-
mental taxonomy theorized two primary types of antisocial chil-
dren. Those with life-course persistent antisocial behavior are
characterized by childhood onset aggression that remains stable
throughout adolescence. In contrast, those with adolescence-
limited antisocial behavior are characterized by adolescent-onset
antisocial behavior which is more transient in nature such that it
occurs primarily during adolescence. Alternatively, Patterson and
colleagues (1989) proposed an early and late-starter model of
antisocial behavior which differentiated antisocial children based
on their age of onset. These two perspectives were similar in that
This article was published Online First May 22, 2017.
Idean Ettekal, Research Institute on Addictions, University at Buffalo,
State University of New York; Gary W. Ladd, Sanford School and De-
partment of Psychology, Arizona State University.
This investigation was conducted as part of the Pathways Project, a
larger longitudinal investigation of children’s social/psychological/scho-
lastic adjustment in school contexts that is supported by the National
Institutes of Health (1 R01MH-49223, 2-R01MH-49223, R01HD-045906
to GWL). Portions of this article are based on a dissertation submitted by
Idean Ettekal to Arizona State University while under the advisement of
Gary Ladd. The authors thank committee members Larry Dumka, Justin
Jager, Karen Kochel, and Becky Kochenderfer-Ladd for their valuable
feedback. Special appreciation is expressed to all the children, teachers,
and parents who made this study possible, and to members of the Pathways
Project for assistance with data collection.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Idean
Ettekal, Research Institute on Addictions, University at Buffalo, State
University of New York, 1021 Main Street, Buffalo, NY 14203. E-mail:
iettekal@ria.buffalo.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Developmental Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 53, No. 9, 1709–1721 0012-1649/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000357
1709
... Peer victimization includes overt victimization and relational victimization (Crick, 1996). Studies have shown that while overt peer victimization tends to decrease from childhood through adolescence (Casper & Card, 2017), relational peer victimization tends to increase over this period (Casper & Card, 2017;Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). This shift might occur because adolescents have developed the social cognitive skills necessary to engage in relational aggression and recognize that relational aggression can be as destructive as overt aggression but with fewer risks of retaliation from peers or punishment by adults (Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). ...
... Studies have shown that while overt peer victimization tends to decrease from childhood through adolescence (Casper & Card, 2017), relational peer victimization tends to increase over this period (Casper & Card, 2017;Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). This shift might occur because adolescents have developed the social cognitive skills necessary to engage in relational aggression and recognize that relational aggression can be as destructive as overt aggression but with fewer risks of retaliation from peers or punishment by adults (Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). Experiencing relational victimization also appears to have a more detrimental impact on adolescents' mental health than overt victimization. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although evidence from previous studies suggests that adolescents with negative coping styles who experienced victimization are more likely to exhibit depressive symptoms, these associations have not yet been disentangled to separate between-person differences from within-person effects. To investigate the within-person bidirectional relationships among relational victimization, coping styles and depressive symptoms, this study conducted a four-wave random intercept cross-lagged panel model analysis. The final sample consisted of 1506 adolescents, 72.6% of whom were male, with a mean age of 10.90 years (SD = 1.12) at the first time point. The findings revealed a prospective within-person association between relational victimization and depressive symptoms, and a reciprocal within-person relationship between depressive symptoms and avoidance coping. The approach coping style negatively predicted depressive symptoms over time, whereas depressive symptoms did not affect the approach coping style. These findings show how depressive symptoms, relational victimization, and coping styles are related at the individual level, extending previous research through the demonstration of stable within-person associations over time.
... Understanding the developmental trajectories of peer aggression and victimization during early adolescence is crucial, as it provides insights into how aggression and victimization evolve and helps identify critical periods for intervention. Although past studies have examined heterogeneous developmental trajectories of peer aggression and victimization in adolescents [5], these studies have examined their developmental trajectories at a holistic level (e.g., mean scores for physical and relational aggression and victimization were used). To date, few studies have explored the co-developmental trajectories of these different types of peer aggression and victimization. ...
... The stabilizing aggression-victimization group identi ed in our study aligns with the bully-victim group observed in previous research [9,13]. Adolescents in this group may exhibit inherent aggressive tendencies [5,10], using aggression to assert control or protect themselves from perceived threats. This behavior can provoke negative reactions from peers, leading to cycles of victimization [38]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This study examined heterogeneous co-developmental trajectories of physical and relational aggression and victimization during early adolescence, as well as the relations between these identifiably distinct trajectories and adolescent internalizing problems and suicidal ideation. Using an accelerated longitudinal design, 4731 Chinese early adolescents (44.9% girls; Mage = 10.91 years, SD = 0.72) in two cohorts (grades 4 and 5) were included, and four measures were completed over a 2-year period. Parallel process latent category growth models revealed four distinct groups: low aggression-high victimization (9.6%); High drop relational aggression and victimization but recovering physical aggression (5.0%); low aggression-low victimization (79.9%); stabilizing aggression-victimization (5.5%); all of which were associated with indicators of children's internalization problems and suicidal ideation. Adolescents with low aggression-high victimization and stabilizing aggression-victimization were more likely to experience more internalizing problems and suicidal ideation. Understanding these dynamics can aid in designing targeted interventions to address both relational and physical aggression and victimization, thereby promoting healthier social interactions and mental well-being among adolescents.
... Overt victimization, however, could not be described by constant or year-to-year change using LCS modeling, indicating stability. This finding is not necessarily surprising considering that compared to childhood, adolescents are less likely to use this form of PV, compared to other forms (e.g., relational victimization; Casper & Card, 2017;Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). Yet, a recent study assessing change in PV across adolescence demonstrated an increasing trend in overt victimization from Age 12.5 to 14, where it peaked and then began decreasing through Age 16 (Cho et al., 2022). ...
... Coupling effects provide information about the temporal sequencing between processes and do not necessarily reflect causal effects; determining causal effects would require additional steps to rule out possible confounders and previous differences (Cáncer et al., 2021). Third, given the well-established link between aggression and PV, whereby many victims are also aggressors (Ettekal & Ladd, 2017), in addition to conceptualizations of aggression that posit roots in emotion dysregulation (Baroncelli & Ciucci, 2014), future research is needed to disentangle whether and how aggression may be dynamically involved in associations between PV and emotion dysregulation across adolescence. Finally, we note that when the effects of sex were accounted for, the observed paths from emotion dysregulation to relational victimization became nonsignificant. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion dysregulation emerges from an interaction between individual factors and environmental factors. Changes in biological, cognitive, and social systems that characterize adolescence create a complex array of environmental factors contributing to emotion dysregulation during this developmental period. In particular, peer victimization (PV) has long-term consequences for emotion dysregulation. Yet, previous research has also indicated that emotion dysregulation can be both an antecedent to and outcome of PV. The present study evaluated reciprocal associations between longitudinal changes within repeated measures of PV and emotion dysregulation across adolescence and into young adulthood. The sample included 167 adolescents (53% male, Mage = 14.07 years at Time 1) who participated in a longitudinal study across five time points, with approximately 1 year between each assessment. Latent change score modeling was used to examine reciprocal associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Results emphasize bidirectional associations between PV and emotion dysregulation. Consistent with social information processing theory, greater emotion dysregulation predicted greater relational and overt victimization over time. Moreover, higher overt victimization predicted increases in emotion dysregulation. Our results offer insights toward developmentally informed longitudinal, transactional models linking negative social environments, and emotion dysregulation development across adolescence and into young adulthood.
... De aDolesCentes haCia Pares Gran parte de la investigación acerca del acoso se centra en factores de riesgo (Ettekal & Ladd, 2017), mientras que se limita la investigación acerca de aspectos positivos que favorecen la convivencia escolar, misma que implica relaciones respetuosas, pacíficas e inclusivas entre los actores del proceso educativo (Fierro-Evans y Carbajal-Padilla, 2019). Durante la adolescencia, se presentan desarrollos cognitivos, emocionales y sociales que favorecen la posibilidad de implicarse en conductas prosociales, en comparación con sus posibilidades durante la niñez (Hardy et al., 2014;Malti et al., 2021). ...
... A major limitation was missing data, with high proportions of missingness for some variables, combined with a relatively small sample size, which is a common challenge in longitudinal studies (Shin et al., 2017). Given that traditional deletion methods are considered problematic (Enders, 2010) and the valuable insight provided by longitudinal data to developmental processes, interpreting findings when missing data is present is a common option in developmental research (e.g., Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). Enders (2022) reported simulations suggesting that maximum likelihood estimation produces unbiased estimates even with small samples and large amounts of missing data. ...
Article
Full-text available
Links between reflective functioning (RF; the ability to conceive of mental states and to interpret human behavior accordingly) and concurrent attachment security have been found in both childhood and adulthood. However, the respective contributions of early and concurrent attachment security in adult RF remain unknown. This study examines the contributions of attachment security to the mother in early childhood and of concurrent attachment security to each parent in young adults’ RF. Eighty-one low-risk participants (49 girls and 32 boys) from average income families took part in this longitudinal study. Attachment security was assessed at 4 years of age with a composite measure of mother-reported Attachment Q-Sort and observer ratings of the quality of mother–child interactions. At age 23, the Attachment Multiple Model Interview was administered to assess participants’ attachment security to each parent. RF was coded from the participants’ attachment narratives using the Reflective Functioning Scale. Attachment security to the mother at age 4 was found to be associated with RF in adulthood. Also, an interactive effect between attachment to the mother and attachment to the father in adulthood was related to RF, suggesting that attachment with one parent moderates the effect of attachment with the other parent on RF. These findings emphasize the importance of both early and concurrent attachment security in the capacity to understand mental states in adulthood.
... They also have fewer friends (Eslea et al., 2004), more academic difficulties (Graham et al., 2006) and internalizing problems (Haynie et al., 2001;Özdemir & Stattin, 2011), lower self-esteem (O'Moore & Kirkham, 2001;Pollastri et al., 2010), and more externalizing problems and suicidal ideation (Kelly et al., 2015) than either pure bullies or victims. Even bully-victims, however, seem to comprise a diverse array of individuals, in particular during adolescence (Ettekal & Ladd, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
We identified different types of adolescent bullying perpetrators and nonbullies based on peer-reported bullying, victimization, and peer status (popularity, likeability, and rejection) and examined differences between bully subtypes in typical forms of bullying perpetrated. Moreover, we studied how bully subtypes differed from nonbullies with varying levels of victimization and peer status in academic and psychosocial adjustment. The study utilizes data from 10,689 adolescents (48.3% boys, mean age 14.7 years). Latent profile analysis identified three distinct subgroups of bullies: popular-liked bullies (13.5%), popular-rejected bully-victims (5.8%), and bully-victims (6.9%), and four groups on nonbullies. High-status bullies (popular-liked and popular-rejected) resembled nonbullies in many ways and had even lower social anxiety, whereas bully-victims were the most maladjusted group. Overall, popularity seems to protect adolescents from social anxiety, and victimization is related to internalizing problems. Results suggest that bullying, victimization, and peer status can be used to identify distinct subtypes of bullies.
Article
Bullying victimization is a significant public health concern and has been associated with various mental health problems. This study aims to investigate the heterogeneity and transition patterns of bullying victimization among Chinese adolescents, and the influence of interpersonal relationships on the dynamic process of victimization over a 12‐month period. The valid sample consists of 1246 adolescents (51.8% girls) in seventh–eighth grade, aged 11–15 years ( M age = 13.58, SD = 0.75) at the first measurement. Latent profile analysis and latent transition analysis were utilized to identify latent profiles and transition patterns of victimization among adolescents. The results revealed three subgroups based on the forms and severity of victimization: the high victimized group, the moderately victimized group, and the low victimized group. The low victimized group is the most stable. Individuals who initially belong to low victimized group rarely transition into moderately or high victimized groups over time. The moderately and high victimized groups changed greatly, with both showing a trend of decreasing levels of victimization. Peer relationships played an important role in preventing victimization, but it was not found that teacher–student relationships had a significant predictive effect.
Article
Full-text available
The goals of the present study were to investigate links between changes in peer victimization from elementary to high school and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 1), whether heightened autonomic nervous system (ANS) reactivity to social and nonsocial stress increases risk for adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 2), and whether increased ANS reactivity strengthens the association between changes in victimization and adolescent reactive aggression (Goal 3). Participants included 145 adolescents (Mage = 16; 54% female; 76% European American, 13% African American, 11% Latino American, 7% Asian American, 5% of mixed race or ethnicity; 60% with family incomes of $100,000 or greater). We collected self-report data in elementary (Time 1 [T1]); (Time 2 [T2]); middle (Time 3 [T3]); and high school (Time 4 [T4]) to assess victimization. At T4, we measured self-reported reactive and proactive aggression, and ANS reactivity (preejection period [PEP], respiratory sinus arrhythmia [RSA]) to peer rejection and nonsocial frustration. More positive victimization slope over time (meaning both less decreasing slopes and increasing slopes) predicted greater adolescent reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 1). Greater RSA augmentation to peer rejection and more PEP reactivity to nonsocial frustration predicted more reactive, but not proactive aggression (Goal 2). The link between victimization slope and reactive aggression emerged only for adolescents exhibiting RSA augmentation to peer rejection (marginal; Goal 3).
Article
Full-text available
A developmental model of antisocial behavior is outlined. Recent findings are reviewed that concern the etiology and course of antisocial behavior from early childhood through adolescence. Evidence is presented in support of the hypothesis that the route to chronic delinquency is marked by a reliable developmental sequence of experiences. As a first step, ineffective parenting practices are viewed as determinants for childhood conduct disorders. The general model also takes into account the contextual variables that influence the family interaction process. As a second step, the conduct-disordered behaviors lead to academic failure and peer rejection. These dual failures lead, in turn, to increased risk for depressed mood and involvement in a deviant peer group. This third step usually occurs during later childhood and early adolescence. It is assumed that children following this developmental sequence are at high risk for engaging in chronic delinquent behavior. Finally, implications for prevention and intervention are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This investigation’s aims were to map prevalence, normative trends, and patterns of continuity or change in school-based peer victimization throughout formal schooling (i.e., Grades K–12), and determine whether specific victimization patterns (i.e., differential trajectories) were associated with children’s academic performance. A sample of 383 children (193 girls) was followed from kindergarten (Mage = 5.50) through Grade 12 (Mage = 17.89), and measures of peer victimization, school engagement, academic self-perceptions, and achievement were repeatedly administered across this epoch. Although it was the norm for victimization prevalence and frequency to decline across formal schooling, 5 trajectory subtypes were identified, capturing differences in victimization frequency and continuity (i.e., high-chronic, moderate-emerging, early victims, low victims, and nonvictims). Consistent with a chronic stress hypothesis, high-chronic victimization consistently was related to lower—and often prolonged—disparities in school engagement, academic self-perceptions, and academic achievement. For other victimization subtypes, movement into victimization (i.e., moderate-emerging) was associated with lower or declining scores on academic indicators, and movement out of victimization (i.e., early victims) with higher or increasing scores on these indicators (i.e., “recovery”). Findings provide a more complete account of the overall prevalence, stability, and developmental course of school-based peer victimization than has been reported to date.
Article
Full-text available
We evaluated the extent to which aggressive victims show unique developmental pathways that are different from those of passive victims, bullies, and uninvolved children. A total of 1,722 children were followed from 4th grade to 6th grade, and the prevalence and stability of each group were assessed. Aggressive victims became less prevalent and passive victims and bullies became more prevalent with age. Although it was common for aggressive victims and bullies to move from one group to the other across time, there was little overlap with the passive victim group. Stability estimates were higher for the bully and aggressive victim groups than for the passive victim group, and patterns of stability were influenced by peer rejection and exposure to violence.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the awareness that victims of peer aggression are a behaviorally heterogeneous group of children, current classification procedures limit the number of variables that may be used to identify victim subtypes. Findings from this study, which followed 379 racially diverse children (50% female) from kindergarten to 3rd grade, demonstrated the utility of cluster analysis to classify children using several criteria: teacher-rated aggressiveness and asocial tendencies and self-reported peer victimization. Specifically, four subtypes of victims were identified: nonaggressive nonasocial; aggressive; asocial; and both aggressive and asocial. Group differences in the stability of victimization are discussed. Moreover, early levels of aggression predicted increases in victimization and chronicity; the role of asocial behavior was less clear. While more aggressive victims were rejected by their peers than nonaggressive nonasocial victims, victims who were rejected, regardless of behavioral tendencies, were more likely to remain victimized than their better accepted counterparts. Developmental trends are discussed.
Article
Distinguishing between relational and physical aggression has become a key feature of many developmental studies in North America and Western Europe, but very little information is available on relational aggression in more diverse cultural contexts. This study examined the factor structure of, gender differences in, and associations between relational and physical aggression in China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States. Children ages 7 to 10 years (N = 1410) reported on their relationally and physically aggressive behavior. Relational and physical aggression shared a common factor structure across countries. Unsurprisingly, boys reported being more physically aggressive than girls across all nine countries; surprisingly, there were no significant gender differences in relational aggression. In all nine countries, relational and physical aggression were significantly correlated (average r = .49). The countries differed significantly in the mean levels of both relational and physical aggression that children reported using and with respect to whether children reported using more physical than relational aggression or more relational than physical aggression. Despite mean level differences in relational and physical aggression across countries, the findings provided support for cross-country similarities in associations between relational and physical aggression, as well as links between gender and aggression.
Article
Gender differences in regard to aggressive behaviour were investigated in a series of studies of schoolchildren of different age cohorts: 8-year-olds (N = 85), 11-year-olds (N = 167), and 15-year-olds (N = 127). Different types of aggressive behaviour were measured with peer nomination techniques, supported by self-ratings. The social structure of the peer groups wene also studied. The results of the 11-year-old cohort were previously persented by Lagerspetz et al. [1988; Aggressive Behavior l4:403-414], but they are compared here with the other age groups. The principal finding was that girls of the two older cohorts overall make greater use of indirect means of aggression, whereas boys tend to employ direct means. Previously, the main dilference between the genders has been thought to be that boys use physical aggressive strategies, while girls prefer verbd ones. Our studies suggest that the differentiation between direct and indirect strategies of aggression presents a more exact picture. Indlrect aggressive shategies were not yet fully developed among the 8-year-old girls, but they were already prominent among the 11-year-old girls. Aggressive behaviour was assessed overall by the children themselves to be the highest in ttris age gmup.
Article
This study identified classes of adolescents who differed in their patterns of reported aggression and victimization, examined the stability of these patterns, and explored factors associated with changes in patterns across time. Participants were 477 students from an urban and an adjoining county school system. The overall sample was 48% male and had an average age of 11.3 years. The urban sample was predominantly African American (80%); the county sample was primarily Caucasian (40%) and African American (38%). Self-report aggression and victimization measures completed at the beginning of sixth grade and the end of seventh grade were analyzed using latent class analyses and latent transition analyses. Support was found for four classes: nonvictimized aggressors, aggressive-victims, predominantly victimized, and well-adjusted youth. Emotion dysregulation, anxiety, and site were associated with membership in the aggressive-victim class in the expected direction, providing support for the validity of the classes. The well-adjusted class was the most stable in class membership over time; the predominantly victimized class was the least stable. In addition, nonvictimized aggressors and predominantly victimized youth were more likely than those in the well-adjusted class to transition into the aggressive-victim class. These findings suggest notable stability in aggressor/victim classes over time and emphasize the importance of developing prevention programs that target the unique needs of distinct aggressor/victim classes in adolescence.