ArticlePDF Available

Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic Children and Developmental Indexes

Authors:
  • Instutute of Ortophonology
  • Istituto di Ortofonologia

Abstract and Figures

Objective: In developmental age the drawing process represents one of the best tools to assess the level of maturity reached by the child in his various areas of functioning. This study considers a sample of 84 children aged between 2.5 and 15 years, all diagnosed with autism, evaluated by an expert team of professionals, in order to highlight the drawing stage reached and the correlation with the severity of autistic symptoms assessed by the ADOS and cognitively evaluated with the Leiter-R nonverbal scale. Methods: The drawing level was observed in spontaneous production by establishing a scale ranging from the presence of the disordered scribbling to the presence of the body schema, taking into account the stereotypes that can lead to some echo-graphic representations that are similar to the echo-lalic productions of the verbal language. Results: The results showed a significant correlation between the drawing production and the Social-Affection component of the ADOS, so demonstrating that the delay in the drawing is heavily influenced by an emotional as well as intellectual inhibition. Conclusion: Through some examples of drawings it is also emphasized the relationship between drawing and chronological age and the difference that emerges between drawings made in the evaluation context and those that come to life during the therapeutic process.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
Research Article Open Access
Di Renzo et al., J Psychol Psychother 2017, 7:2
DOI: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Research Article OMICS International
Journal of Psychology & Psychotherapy
J
o
u
r
n
a
l
o
f
P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
y
&
P
s
y
c
h
o
t
h
e
r
a
p
y
ISSN: 2161-0487
Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic Children and
Developmental Indexes
Magda Di Renzo*, Chiara Marini, Federico Bianchi di Castelbianco, Lidia Racinaro and Monica Rea
Institute of Ortofonologia (IdO), Via Tagliamento 25, 00198 - Rome, Italy
Abstract
Objective: In developmental age the drawing process represents one of the best tools to assess the level of
maturity reached by the child in his various areas of functioning. This study considers a sample of 84 children aged
between 2.5 and 15 years, all diagnosed with autism, evaluated by an expert team of professionals, in order to
highlight the drawing stage reached and the correlation with the severity of autistic symptoms assessed by the ADOS
and cognitively evaluated with the Leiter-R nonverbal scale.
Methods: The drawing level was observed in spontaneous production by establishing a scale ranging from the
presence of the disordered scribbling to the presence of the body schema, taking into account the stereotypes that
can lead to some echo-graphic representations that are similar to the echo-lalic productions of the verbal language.
Results: The results showed a signicant correlation between the drawing production and the Social-Affection
component of the ADOS, so demonstrating that the delay in the drawing is heavily inuenced by an emotional as
well as intellectual inhibition.
Conclusion: Through some examples of drawings it is also emphasized the relationship between drawing and
chronological age and the difference that emerges between drawings made in the evaluation context and those that
come to life during the therapeutic process.
Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder; Drawing; Graphic activitie;
Emotional components; Developmental prole; ADOS 2; Leiter-R
Introduction
In developmental age, drawing is one of the best tools to objectify
the level of maturity reached by the child in psychomotor, cognitive
and emotional areas and, at the same time, it represents a valuable
communication instrument in both educational and therapeutic elds.
In fact, of any graphic image or sign we can analyze many elements
that lead to subjectivity and then to the inner world of the subject who
realized it, as can be seen also from the drawing tests elaborated in the
psychodiagnostic context. Drawing is, in fact, for the child, a unique
experience of representation of the self and the world, so constituting
one of the main channels of expression of his mental and physical
condition. rough the use of his gestures the child impresses on the
sheet a trace that gradually becomes the tool to explore the environment
and to leave a shareable mark of himself. According to most of the
authors, the maturation of drawing expressiveness is proportional to
the maturation of cognitive and emotional functions [1-4].
Research shows that children with autism spectrum disorder have
a lower somatosensory perception than normotypical age-matched
children [5-8]. Other studies also emphasized that the hand function is
less developed compared with children of typical development [9]. Huri
stressed that the preference of autistic children for hard objects respect
to so ones allows considering a minor tactile awareness compared
to normotypical children [5]. is view is in line with what has been
shown by Frances Tustin [10] as a result of a separation that did not
occur from the care-giver. In addition, the diculty of initiating and
planning activity is oen impaired in children with autism, and it is
related to the presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors [11] and
this could inhibit the motor planning necessary for the realization of
the drawing.
In an interesting research where autistic children with and without
an upper extremity trauma history were compared there was evidence
that children with the presence of motors trauma had lower scores at the
*Corresponding author: Magda Di Renzo, Institute of Ortofonologia (IdO), Via
Salaria 30, 00198, Rome, Italy, Tel: 0039-068542038; Fax: 0039-068413258;
E-mail: m.direnzo@ortofonologia.it
Received November 29, 2016; Accepted March 02, 2017; Published March 09, 2017
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017)
Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic Children and Developmental
Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Copyright: © 2017 Di Renzo M, et al. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original author and source are credited.
Sensory Integration and Praxis Test and in somatosensory perception
[5]. is study demonstrates the relationship between somatosensory
perception and hand function in autistic children with upper extremity
trauma. Although the presence of unusual sensory behaviors is still not
part of the diagnostic criteria, the DSM 5 emphasizes their importance
in the structuring of autism. e somatosensory perception plays a key
role in the early years of life. In autistic children a lower somatosensory
perception is associated with decits in communication, in motor skills
and social abilities [8].
e drawing activity, object of analysis in this research, is a key
element of both the evaluation phase and of the therapeutic work
that children with autism spectrum disorder receive at the Institute of
Ortofonologia, in Rome. e therapeutic project in which the child,
his family and the school are involved provides outpatient activities
(for a total of 10 h per week), home-care intervention (4 h per week),
informative sessions and individual or group counseling for parents,
in addition to educational support at school as well as medical,
neuropsychological, physiatrist, psychological and speech therapist
evaluations. e purpose of this treatment model is to emphasize
the emotional and relational dimension to expand the repertoire
of communicative behavior, thus allowing the expression of latent
intellectual and social abilities.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 2 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
A study conducted on a sample of autistic children included in the
therapeutic project, well highlighted that the drawing developmental
stages of these children are slowed if compared with those of typically
developed children of the same age but however could express the
characteristics of personality of each one [12]. Correlating drawing
development to the level of autistic symptomatology of each child we
found that the decrease of the ADOS score corresponded to an increase
in drawing that in its manifestation took a greater communicative
value, so becoming a valuable tool for assessing the relational openness.
In fact, when a child begins to use his own drawings to
spontaneously represent objects of the surrounding world, this opens
an important channel of communication which allows him to express
needs and desires. Unlike words, which evoke in the child invisible
sound tracks, drawings leave tangible traces that allow the child a more
concrete comparison with the world. Considering the diculties that
the child with autism spectrum disorder encounters in integrating the
various senses and in conceptualizing them, it is better understood as
how important is draw, if intended as act of knowledge.
e diculties in sensory integration are responsible for most of
the problems of autistic children. Ayres stressed the importance of an
adequate sensory organization in order to use the objects and interact
with the world [13]. In a psychodynamic framework, the lack of sensory
integration is a result of the defensive mechanism of dismantling which
protects the child from a relationship with the objects that is perceived
as too threatening. e trace that the child produces can be a rst step
towards the integration process because it is acted by the child himself
who can go up to where his potentialities allow.
A not appropriate drawing of the children is not the consequence
of an altered aesthetic or physical development (such as visual or ne
motor problems), but of a strong inhibition of the socio-relational
components. is observation is supported by the fact that also those
children with autism spectrum disorder with have an appropriate
intellectual level show delays in drawings [14]. Children with autistic
disorder showed, in fact, many diculties to express and give shape
to their emotional state because of an aective attunement decit that
interfered on both verbal and non-verbal behaviors such as drawing
[15,16]. e drawing productions of these children were aected by the
marked decrease in social integration and communication. As already
pointed out, in fact, the drawing development, both in production
and in spontaneous copying of graphic stimuli, resulted correlated
with the severity of autistic symptoms and delayed in comparison to
chronological age.
According to Machover [17] we know that the gure drawn
represents the author himself and provides information about the
perception that he has of his body and desires. On the basis of the
presence of certain details and the manner in which the various parts of
the body are elaborated or omitted it is possible to infer the emotional
condition of the subject, that otherwise would not be communicable in
a symbolic way.
e representation of the body is always, in fact, in the early stages
of development, a transposition of the knowledge achieved and at the
same time a stimulus for new bodily sensations. So that the concept
of image of the human gure and the feelings with it associated could
become meaningful for the child there must be body awareness and self-
perception. is perception typically starts around the second half of
the rst year of life when the child begins to separate the outside world
from the inside, to make himself aware of each part of his body, of his
position in the environment, of his ability to control movements and to
respond to sensations coming from the outside world [18,19]. Because
of the diculty that the autistic child has to attune with the outside
world, all these experiences are greatly compromised and require a
careful observation by therapists to promote those stimulations which
are necessary to better attune.
In the therapeutic project of the Institute of Ortofonologia, which
provides constant monitoring through clinical evaluations, it was
noted that the rst signicant drawings oen come to life within the
therapeutic setting, there where the communication with the other
acquires its importance. In fact, the child, in the therapeutic context,
within a signicant relationship, becomes able to lower his defense
mechanisms and to express, so more, his emotional experiences
through nonverbal instruments, unlike what happens in assessment
context. Only within a meaningful context the drawing could become,
for the autistic child, a means to tell to another his own needs, desires
and emotions even if in the absence of verbal language.
Objectives of the research here presented are: to evaluate the
drawing development of children with autism spectrum disorder, both
in relation to the severity of the symptoms and of the cognitive level; to
investigate the dierences in drawings in connection with the age and
the time spent in the therapy path.
Method
Participants
e research sample consisted of 84 children with a diagnosis of
autism spectrum disorders (ASD), who had an age comprised between
the 2.58 and 15.00 years (M=7.63 years; SD=3.2) at the time of the
assessment. Of these, 15 were female and 69 were male (Table 1). Most
children were Italian (93%); there also were an African minority (2%),
Asian (2%) and Eastern Europe (3%).
Procedures
Children who participated in this study came in consultation at the
Institute of Ortofonologia in Rome (IdO), between 2006 and 2015, with
a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders and a request for treatment.
e diagnosis of these children was conrmed according to the
diagnostic criteria of DSM-IV and DSM-5 and to the score obtained
at the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, First and Second
Edition (ADOS-2) [20,21] that indicates their severity. e assessments
Mean SD Minimum Maximum
Chronological Age, years 7.63 3.4 2.58 15.00
ADOS 16.08 6.2 1 26
SA 11.79 4.8 1 20
RRB 4.18 2.2 0 8
IQ 68.77 21.4 36 103
Drawing Level 2.94 1.6 0 5
Legend: SA: Social Affect ADOS-subscale; RRB: R Restricted and Repetitive Behavior; IQ: Intelligence Quotient Score
Table 1: Description of the sample of 84 children with ASD (mean, ds, max-min range): age, ADOS-2 scores, IQ scores, drawing Level.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 3 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
considered in this study refer to the period between 2014 and 2015, so
include both children newly arrived at the Institute both children who
were already taken into care. Aer four years of therapy, some of these
children were not more in the ADOS autism spectrum category.
e diagnosis of children involved in autism research was prepared
by a group of experts with at least 10 years of experience (psychologists/
psychotherapists, child neuropsychiatrists, neurologists and other
specialized gures). e sample of this research did not include
children with certied neurological damage, sensory disabilities
or under 2 years old. is study was carried out in accordance with
the recommendations of the national guidelines and the APA ethic.
All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they
participated in the study. e study was conducted in accordance with
the Declaration of Helsinki.
Measures
Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition: e
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition [20,21]
is a semi-structured standardized assessment concerning the areas
of communication, social interaction, play, and repetitive and
restricted behaviors. It is considered a gold standard so being the most
commonly standardized measure used in research protocols. It has
strong psychometric properties, including good reliability and validity.
e ADOS-2 includes ve modules, but during the assessment process,
just one module is administered and it is selected on the basis of the
expressive language level and chronological age of the child:
Toddler Module: For children between 12 and 30 months of age
who do not constantly use sentences in their speech. is module
allows you to precisely identify children at risk of autism spectrum
disorders (ASD);
Module 1: For children aged from 31 months who do not
continuously use sentences in their speech;
Module 2: For children of all ages who use sentences in the speech,
without being verbally uent;
Module 3: For verbally uent children and adolescents;
Module 4: For verbally uent older adolescents and adults.
In Modules 1 to 4, the algorithm scores are compared to get one
of three classications: Autism, Autism Spectrum, and Not Spectrum.
e cut-o for the ADOS-2 classications ADOS-2 varies according
to the module used and to the child’s language level. e ADOS-2,
in addition to providing an overall score, also measures the domain
of Social Aection (SA) and the domain of Repetitive and Restricted
Behaviors (RRB). e SA includes the evaluation of aspects related
to communication and reciprocal social interaction. e RRB
includes unusual sensory interests, mannerisms, repetitive interests
and behaviors and stereotyped-idiosyncratic use of words and/or
vocalizations.
Leiter international performance scale-revised: e Leiter
International Performance Scale-Revised [22] is designed for the
evaluation of intellectual functions of children and adolescents aged
between 2 and 20 years. e Leiter-R is formulated to meet the clinical
need of evaluating the non-verbal intelligence through a comprehensive
analysis of the strengths and weaknesses of the child, with a view to a
diagnosis that includes both neuropsychological and cognitive aspects.
Non-verbal cognitive abilities do not require the ability to perceive,
manipulate and reason with words and numbers, so that the scale
can be administered completely without the use of verbal language,
including instructions, because it does not require verbal responses
from the subject. e validity coecients of the IQ score was calculated
for each age group (alpha 0.92 to 0.93). e Leiter-R has good evidence
of its validity, with data from a wide analysis from the criterion related
studies, the accuracy of classication of intellectual disability and
various studies related to the construct [22]. e IQ score has a mean
of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. e intellectual disability is
indicated by a composite score that is two standard deviations or more
below the average, so that a score of 70 constitutes the borderline value.
e drawing evaluation: e drawing process was evaluated in
semi-structured observation in which an examiner unknown to the
child asked him to spontaneously draw, providing markers and A4
sheet of paper. e examiner was not the therapist who has followed the
child in the course of therapy and this in order to make the evaluation
as objective as possible, so avoiding excessive distortions dictated by
familiarity built within the therapeutic relationship.
e drawings obtained were evaluated by referring both to the
process of drawing (through semi-structured observations) both to
the nal product, considering the close relationship of space and time
implied by the drawings of that child at that time of his evolution.
Specically, with regard to the process of drawing, the following
parameters were observed:
- e attitude of the child during the administration and the activity.
It was observed how that activity was accepted (if the child
starts drawing waiting for the administration, or anticipating
it; if the child initially refuses the activity going away and/or if
he interrupts the activity several times).
- Eye contact during the action (if present, discontinuous or
absent).
- e use of color (if the child tends to use only one or several).
- e prehension of the instrument (functional, high into a st or
between the ngers).
- e presence of interaction with the examiner during the activity,
by observing if the drawing is used in a relational manner or
not.
- e duration of the drawing and the attentive level.
- e use of the space in the sheet.
- e form and the order of the elements that constitute the
drawing.
- e line, shape, proportion and erasures (if the child asks for
other sheets or gets them individually).
- e child’s posture while drawing.
Considering the above mentioned parameters detected during the
activity, the drawing was then placed in a specic phase of the drawing
development, following the stadial theories [19]. ese theories
provide a key to understanding the dierent developmental steps of
drawings, distinguishable according to age, always keeping in mind the
individuality of the child and the context. In this way the drawings of
the sample were evaluated, codied and placed within the following
5-point scale [14,19].
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 4 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
Level 0. Refusal to draw or absence of productions.
Level 1. Disordered, random scribbling (age range 2 to 3.6 years):
uncontrolled graphic marks, in which all the surface of the sheet is
used, with little or no visual control over motor activity.
Level 2. Controlled scribbling (age range 2 to 3.6 years): Graphic
marks are smaller and controlled and there is a growing ability to
visually control the motor activity.
Level 3. Named scribbling, with meaning attribution (age range 2
to 3.6 years old): the child connects his graphic marks to objects of the
outside world so originating the representative world.
Level 4. Preschematic (Age range 3.7-5, 6 years): deliberate attempts
to represent reality even if the dierent elements are not well organized
on the sheet.
Level 5. Schematic (age range 7-8 years): the drawing has
appropriate form to represent an object of reality; the space-time
parameters are achieved and the drawing is well recognizable.
Following the evaluation of the drawing level, the child was asked
to draw a human gure giving access to a pencil and a sheet, in order
to verify the presence of the body image in children with ASD. Given
the variety and severity of autistic symptoms of the sample, it was
impossible to use the Human Figure Test through the typical modes of
the standardized Machover test [8], because it was not possible to use
the same administrations to all subjects (because of the verbal language
decits and the discontinuous attentive diculty), so preventing the
reliable and valid use of the relative scale.
Data analysis: We used the Statistical Package for Social Sciences
(SPSS) version 21 for data analysis. Signicance level was set at alpha
0.05 (two-tailed). Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to evaluate
dierences between groups. Eect sizes were reported as partial eta
squared (η2
p), A η2
p of 0.02 was considered a small eect size, 0.13 a
medium eect size and 0.23 a large eect size. Correlation analysis was
performed to analyze the relationship between dependent variables.
Chi-squared analyses were conducted to examine group dierences in
demographic variables between the categorical variables. Finally, the
linear regression analysis was conducted to identify predictors of the
level of drawing.
Results
e 84 children in the sample were divided according to the ADOS-
2 scores in: children with severe autism (AUT; 78.6%), children with
autism spectrum disorder (SpD; 8.3%); children out of the cut-o for
the spectrum (No-spectrum; 13.1%). e children were in therapy, on
average, from 3 years and 3 months (SD=2.4).
e drawing development in children with ASD
As shown in Table 2, in the AUT Group about the 6% of children
refuses to draw or is not able to, about the 44% of children were in the
scribbling phase, about the 47% of the children achieved preschematic
and schematic drawings.
In addition, all the AUT children with preschematic and schematic
drawings also reached the ability to produce the body schema.
In the SpD group, about the 14% of children refuses to draw or is
not able to, approximately the 70% of children were in the scribbling
phase, about the 14% of children produced preschematic drawings; no
child realized schematic drawings. Only 1 child was able to draw the
body schema.
In the No-spectrum group, nobody refuses to draw or is not able to
and nobody realizes disordered scribbling, approximately the 45.5% of
children produced controlled scribbling or with meaning attribution,
about the 55% realized preschematic and schematic drawings. More
than the 50% of children was able to draw the body schema.
Investigate chronological age dierences and therapy
duration
Dividing the children into age groups, that is: 2-5 years (N=32), 6-9
years (N=31) and 10-15 years (N=21) a signicant dierence emerged
(F2,83=18.47; p<0.001; η2
q=0.33), so that the drawing level in ASD
children increased with the age, and the body schema started to appear
in about the half of them between 6 and 9 years of age, while it is almost
always present aer 10 years of age.
e children, also, were divided according to the time already
spent in therapy at the time of the drawing assessment. Children who
had about 3-4 years of therapy behind them showed a signicantly
higher level of drawing (F3,83=5.80, P<0.001; η2
q=0.17) than those who
attended therapy from 1-2 years. ese dierences are accentuated
with the increasing of the children’s chronological age (Table 3).
As for the representation of the body schema, there was a signicant
dierence compared to the chronological age of children: of the 38
children with the body schema, 3 were preschoolers, 17 were 6-9 years
old and 18 were 10-15 years old (Chi square=31.6, p<0.001).
Correlation between drawing level, cognitive skills, autistic
symptoms
e analysis of the correlations showed that the drawing level
improves with the increase of the age of children and the improve
of the ADOS scores, and in particular with the improve of the Social
Aections (SA). However there is no correlation between drawing and
restricted, repetitive behaviors (RRB).
Finally, the drawing scores have a higher correlation with IQ scores
than with ADOS scores (Table 4).
To show that ADOS scores relate directly to drawing level, a
regression model was conduct, with drawing level as the dependent
variable and IQ entered as the rst predictor and ADOS scores as
Drawing level AUT (N=66) % SpD (N=7) % No-spectrum (N=11) %
Refuses or is not able to 4 6.1 0 0.0 0 0.0
Disordered, Random Scribbling 14 21.2 1 14,3 0 0.0
Controlled Scribbling 15 22.7 2 28.6 2 18.2
Named Scribbling, with meaning attribution 2 3.0 3 42.9 3 27.3
Preschematic 19 28.8 0 0.0 1 9.1
Schematic 12 18.2 1 14.3 5 45.5
Body Schema 31 47.0 1 14.3 6 54.5
Table 2: Division according to the drawing level of AUT (N=66), SpD (N=11) and No-spectrum (N=7) children.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 5 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
the second (Figure 1). When ADOS scores was adding as a predictor,
results in a signicant change in the variance explained by the model,
so we can assume that ADOS scores inuence drawing over and above
IQ (Table 5).
By way of explanation in Figure 2 we can examine a spontaneous
drawing that is in delay if compared to typical development: the child
is 8 years old and has an autistic disorder with severe symptoms
(ADOS=19) and an IQ that cannot be assessed, neither with the Leiter-R
scale, because of the his relational closure and his labile attentive times.
His spontaneous production can be placed in the disordered scribbling
stage that normally is acquired at about two years of age.
In Figure 3, there is an example of a stereotypical image: here we
nd small squares, colored with a precise succession of colors (red,
green and yellow) which cover all the space of the sheet. In this drawing,
no living objects appear and the movement is absent. e author is a 10
years old child who has an autistic disorder assessed using the ADOS
with a score of 19 and an intellectual disability assessed through the
Leiter-R with an IQ score of 50.
During the execution of the drawing, which took place in an
observational context, the child showed a strong relational closure
also through an echolalic verbal language, a rigid body posture and a
discontinue eye contact that made it impossible any intervention to
change the interaction and its realization, since stereotypies prevent
the exibility necessary to modify the process in reference to external
stimuli.
Observing and evaluating the drawings of autistic children we
notice that many of them do not use the drawing in a communicative
and relational way, but in a stereotyped way to evade the context. In
fact, as well as many children use bizarre verbal language so oen
repeating words, sounds or phrases they heard say (echo-lalia), in the
same way they produce stereotyped drawings, copying or repeating
forms many times (echo-graphic).
From 2 to 5 years From 6 to 9 years From 10 to 15 years
Drawing Score 1.9 (0.2) 3.2 (0.2) 4.1 (0.3)
Refuses or is not able to, % 12,5 19,4
Disordered Scribbling, % 25,0 19,4 4,8
Controlled Scribbling, % 34,4 6,5 9,5
Named Scribbling, with meaning attribution, % 18,8 29,0
Preschematic, % 6,3 25,8 42,9
Schematic, % 3,1 19,4 42,9
Body Schema, % 9.4 54.8 85.7
Legend: SA: Social Affect ADOS-subscale; RRB: R Restricted and Repetitive Behavior; IQ: Intelligence Quotient Score
Table 3: Drawing development in children divided by age.
SA RRB IQ Drawing level
ADOS2 0.95** 0.74** -0.52** -0.46**
SA 0.55** -0.48** -0.51**
RRB -0.46** -0.15
IQ 0.39**
Legend: SA: Social Affect ADOS-subscale; RRB: R Restricted and Repetitive Behavior; IQ: Intelligence Quotient Score
Table 4: Correlation between drawing scores, ADOS-2 scores and IQ scores.
Figure 1: Drawing trend in ASD children according to chronological age and the therapy duration.
Predictors R R squared B SE P 90%–95% CI Exp(B) Collinearity statistics
Lower / Upper Tolerance VIF
IQ scores .397 .147 .029 .007 .001 .014 / .044 1.00 1.00
ADOS scores .464 .215 -.117 -.464 .001 -.166 / -.068 1.00 1.00
Table 5: Regression Model.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 6 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
Stereotyped drawings are distinguished from those spontaneous for
their static character, rigid and close to the surrounding environment.
e drawing, in these cases, does not lead the child to an adaptation
nor to an interaction neither to the nal “product” nor to the drawing
process. In stereotyped drawings, in fact, the dialogical component fails
and this means of communication is used as a tool of estrangement
from the surrounding environment.
In the Figures 4-6 it can be seen as the rst attempts to represent
the human gure are connected to the emotional-relational state rather
than to the chronological age and cognitive level. However, it is again
emphasized that there are completely dierent ways of development
and that it cannot assess the aective and cognitive level of a child on
the basis of a single drawing.
In the Figure 4, without taking account of the orientation in the
vertical direction, it is represented a circle that would be the head with
all the elements of the face, eyes, nose, mouth, ears and hair. e author
is a child of six years and four months with autistic symptomatology in
the absence of intellectual disability, which obtained an ADOS score of
13 and an IQ score of 71 in the Leiter-R, which is a borderline value.
In the Figure 5, a nine years old child, who has severe autistic
symptoms as measured by the ADOS with a score of 14 and a cognitive
development that cannot be assessed through the Leiter-R, produces
only some elements of the face without the contour of the head and the
body. It stands out in particular the choice of excluding the contour of
the face, since it is unusual, and of including, instead, the pupils.
Both designs are incomplete and strongly inadequate if compared
Figure 2: 6 years old girl; ADOS score: 21 (severe symptoms); IQ: 52 (moderate retardation).
Figure 3: 10 years old boy; ADOS score: 19 (severe symptoms); IQ: 50 (moderate retardation).
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 7 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
Observing the Figures 4 and 6 we can see that these two children,
albeit presenting the same IQ, show a dierent capacity of representing
the body schema and the gure 6, which belongs to a younger child,
is better than the other. Considering many variables, this dierence
seems attributable to the lower level of autistic symptoms and not to
the age.
If we consider two drawings of the same child we can do further
reections. In the Figures 7 and 8, in fact, we observe the body schema
produced in two dierent contexts in a time frame of about a month. It
is a 7 years old child with an ADOS score of 12 and an IQ score of 116.
e Figure 7 was carried out in an assessment context while the Figure
8 was made in a therapeutic setting. ey both are not complete of all
the elements of the body schema and appear immature and altered with
respect to the age and especially if compared to the child’s IQ but both
express the emotional state of the child although if in dierent ways for
the quality and quantity. In particular in the Figure 7 emerges a strong
inhibition, an emotional block related to the performance requested
by the operator. is interpretation is conrmed by the observation of
the child’s behavior during the execution of the drawing because even
through his body posture he shows a strong inhibition and high anxiety
levels. Instead the second drawing (Figure 8) reveals a greater peace in
the drawing process by which the child is able to express with a soer
style his self-perception.
In comparing drawings of the same child in dierent contexts,
we found that they oen dier signicantly. In particular we found
that in the therapeutic setting: there are fewer occasions of refusal in
performing drawings; the emotional connotation of the graphic image
Figure 4: 6 years old boy; ADOS score: 13 (severe symptoms); IQ: 71.
Figure 5: 9 years old boy; ADOS score: 14 (severe symptoms); IQ: not evaluable.
to the age criterion, but at the same time they both express, in their
diversity, the specicity of each individual author. e rst drawing
of the human gure, even if in late, also indicates the way in which
the child perceives and shows the beginning of a more structured
intellectual process.
In the Figure 6, however, we can examine the drawing of the so
called “big head”: the autistic child who drew it at the age of 4 years and
11 months by submitting had an autistic disorder with mild symptoms
(ADOS score of 8) and no intellectual disability (IQ score of 71). His
drawing production was impaired because these kinds of representations
usually appear around the age of three. It should be noted that this drawing
was realized as a result of a treatment path, thanks to which the changed
his ADOS diagnosis. At his rst ADOS administration, in fact, the child
obtained a score of 20 which placed him in a severe symptoms category
(it was at the intake that is two years before this drawing). It appears
that the child, thanks to the good course of treatment and then to the
reduction of symptoms, could acquire a greater perception of the self and
the surrounding world that allowed him also to draw the human gure.
is drawing production can be read as a positive indicator, predictive of
his emotional state. As evidence of this, the same child in later years of
his course of treatment achieved further signicant improvements in all
the developmental areas.
Figure 6: Four 11 years old boy; ADOS score: 8 (mild symptoms); IQ: 71.
Figure 7: Body schema made by a 7 years old boy; ADOS score: 12
(sever symptoms); IQ: 116.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 8 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
is stronger than that produced in observational settings; the child
uses this production to express his emotional experiences in order to
communicate them to the therapist (who can then reuse drawings to
work on the main elements emerged).
Conclusion
e results demonstrate the close relationship of drawing with the
level of autistic symptomatology. All children who are part of an autism
spectrum disorder have a signicant delay in the drawing process, and
this delay is not related to the IQ, but rather to the Social Aection
area investigated by the ADOS-2. Within the Social Aect area may be
included all the tactile and proprioceptive feelings that allow the child
a relational opening. A signicant fact that emerges from the research
is that the drawing development improves with age and duration of
therapy. e increased presence of the body schema in children aged
from 6 to 9 years is in fact due to therapy duration of at least 2 or 3
years.
Children who at the time of the assessment obtained a score of No-
Spectrum (determined by four years of prior therapy) show graphic
capabilities far superior to that of other groups, thanks to a greater
social openness that allows the child to use drawings communicatively.
e fact that children come out of the ADOS diagnoses of autism have
signicantly improved their autistic production shows how the initial
diculties were pervasive and not specic. In children from 10 to 15
years old, instead, the appearance of the humane gure is linked not
only to a greater maturity, but also the various stimuli received in
family, educational and therapeutic contexts. It is important to stress
that when there is the representation of the human gure, this appears
immature and impaired with respect to age. However, we could also
show that the presence of the human gure constitutes a sign of a
positive development of autistic symptoms. In fact, the appearance of
the body schema is an important relational opening index for an autistic
child, since it demonstrates his initial ability to recognize himself with
his own body and then to dierentiate himself from the other.
e highlighted data allow us to state that none of the children
included in the Turtle Project has a special ability to draw. is
is in contrast to a part of the scientic literature that emphasized
extraordinary drawings abilities of a specic sample with autism
spectrum disorder in absence of intellectual disability [23,24]. In
particular, it was found that the characteristic of the artistic work of
these individuals was their visual realism, specically the linearity,
which captures the contours, and the prospect of an object or scene.
However, some other studies [25,26] already refuted the notion of a
connection between drawings skills and autism, in particular showing
that autistic children do not get better performance than those of
children with typical development with corresponding mental age, and
that visual realism is not a feature of all autistic children’s drawings.
ese studies also found that the artistically gied autistic individuals
may have much in common with not autistic people artistically gied
[23].
In this research we observed that children of our sample have a
diculty in their drawing ability that increases with the severity of
symptoms and, in addition, that none of them possesses unique skills.
In conclusion we can say that the delay in drawing development
supports the concept of a pervasive disorder noting that the lack of
communication aects not only the verbal level, but all the expressive
channels and imaginative processes, as it is clear also from the poorness
or lack of symbolic play.
It seems important to emphasize the fact that a therapeutic
approach, which could properly take account of the emotional
component of development, promotes the natural unfolding of some
of the child’s abilities including those inherent the drawing process.
e spontaneous appearance of the drawing, in our experience, it is
always a result of a greater relational opening of the child and then an
expansion of the cognitive strategies necessary to adapt to the outside
world. Recent researches [27-29] documented how generally the
cognitive level and also intellectual inhibition are closely related to the
ADOS score and if this decreases, thanks to the course of treatment,
as a result the intellectual inhibition will reduce and the IQ score of
the subject will increase. Similarly the autistic symptoms may block the
drawing development, in the sense that serious symptoms correspond
to a low IQ score and to a drawing level which strongly deviates from
the norm.
In patients belonging to the Turtle Project we found that some
children, at a distance of approximately one year from the rst
observation, signicantly improved their drawings and this nding also
was conrmed at the ADOS retest and at the Leiter-R with a positive
change of diagnosis and with an IQ improvement.
Many of the children, in the course of treatment, signicantly
changed the type of response given to the various tests and in some
cases, following the ADOS score, went from an autistic disorder to
an autism spectrum or to an absence of the disorder, accompanying
these results also to a positive intellectual change. In particular, for a
subsample of 78 children in the Turtle Project who were administered
the ADOS procedure in dierent time periods, there was a signicant
improvement: 31 children (40%) achieved a less severe ADOS
diagnosis, among which the 24% got an ADOS score lower than 7, so
placing out of the autistic spectrum.
Finally, in this paper we also highlighted the usefulness and desirability
of using the drawing tool both in an observational context and in therapy,
especially considering the lack of attention until now attributed to this
aspect. Children’s scribbling and drawings, even those of autistic children,
are valuable materials for reading the path of growth, through the evolution
of signs, forms and composition: real and proper elements of a grammar
and syntax of the grapho-pictorial language.
Figure 8: Body schema made by the same child of the Figure 7, in a
therapeutic setting.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic
Children and Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-0487.1000291
Page 9 of 9
Volume 7 • Issue 2 • 1000291
J Psychol Psychother, an open access journal
ISSN: 2161-0487
Acknowledgement
This article is based on the activities of the Institute of Ortofonologia (IdO) of
Rome. We are grateful to the psychologist, child psychiatrist, speech therapist,
parents and children whose participation made this work possible.
References
1. Olivero Ferraris A (2012) Il signicato del disegno infantile. Bollati Boringheri,
Torino.
2. Pizzo Russo L (2015) Il disegno infantile. Storia, teoria, pratiche. Aesthetica,
Palermo.
3. Cannoni E (2005) Lo sviluppo del disegno [The drawing development]. Carocci,
Roma.
4. Crocetti G (2008) I disegni dei bambini. Metafore e simboli del benessere
bambino [The drawings of children. Metaphors and symbols of child welfare].
Armando Editore, Roma.
5. Huri M, Mehr BK, altuntaş O, Kayiha H (2014) Yaygın gelişimsel bozukluğu
olan ve normal gelişim gösteren çocukların taktil tercihlerinin karşılaştırılması.
Ergoterapi ve Rehabilitasyon Dergisi 2: 21-28.
6. Huri M, Şahin S, Kayıhan H (2016) Investigation of hand function among
children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder with upper extremity trauma
history. Ulus Travma Acil Cerrahi Derg 22: 559-565.
7. Roley SS, Mailloux Z, Parham LD, Schaaf RC, Lane CJ, et al. (2015) Sensory
integration and praxis patterns in children with autism. Am J Occup Ther 69: 1-8.
8. Azouz HG, Khalil M, El Ghani HMA, Hamed HM (2014) Somatosensory evoked
potentials in children with autism. Alexandria Journal of Medicine 50: 99-105.
9. Provost B, Heimerl S, Lopez BR (2008) Levels of gross and ne motor
development in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Physical and
occupational Therapy In Pediatrics 27: 21-36.
10. Tustin F (2013) Autistic states in children. Routledge, London, UK.
11. Di Renzo M, di Castelbianco FB, Vanadia E, Petrillo M, Racinaro L, et al. (2016)
Assessment of executive functions in preschool-aged children with autism
spectrum disorders: Usefulness and limitation of BRIEF-P in clinical practice. J
Child Adolesc Behav, pp: 1-7.
12. Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F (2013) Il processo graco
del bambino autistico [The drawing process of the autistic child]; Magi, Roma.
13. Ayres AJ (2004) Sensory integration and praxis tests manual: Updated edition.
Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, CA.
14. Luquet G (2001) Children’s drawings. Free Association Books, London.
15. Grandin T (2001) Autism and visual thought. Erikson, Trento.
16. Jolley RP, O’Kelly R, Barlow CM, Jarrold C (2013) Expressive drawing ability in
children with autism. Br J Dev Psychol 31: 143-149.
17. Machover K (1949) Personality projection: In the drawing of a human gure.
Charles Thomas Publisher, Springeld.
18. Longobardi C, Pasta T, Quaglia R (2012) Manuale del disegno infantile.
Vecchie e nuove prospettive in ambito educativo e psicologico [The manual of
the infantile drawing. Old and new perspectives in education and psychology];
UTET University, Verona.
19. Pizzo Russo L (2015) Il disegno infantile. Storia, teoria, pratiche [The infantile
drawing. History, theory, practices]; esthetica, Palermo.
20. Lord C, Rutter M, Di Lavore PC, Risi S (2005) Autism diagnostic observation
schedule. Organizzazioni Speciali, Florence, Italy.
21. Lord C, Rutter M, Di Lavore PC, Risi S, Luyster RJ, et al. (2013) Autism
diagnostic observation schedule. Hogrefe, Florence, Italy.
22. Roid GH, Miller LJ (2002) Leiter–R: Leiter International Performance Scale–
Revised. Organizzazioni Speciali, Florence, Italy.
23. Simmons DR, Robertson AE, McKay LS, Toal E, McAleer P, et al. (2009) Vision
in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res 49: 2705-2739.
24. O’Connor N, Hermelin B (1990) The recognition failure and graphic success of
idiot-savant artists. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 31: 203-215.
25. Smith Ad, Kenny L, Rudnicka A, Briscoe J, Pellicano E (2016) Drawing rmer
conclusions: Autistic children show no evidence of a local processing bias in a
controlled copying task. J Autism Dev Disord 46: 3481-3492.
26. Allen ML, Craig E (2016) Brief report: Imaginative drawing in children with
autism spectrum disorder and learning disabilities. J Autism Dev Disord 46:
704-712.
27. Di Renzo M, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Petrillo M, Racinaro L, Rea M (2015)
Assessment of a long-term developmental relationship-based approach in
children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Rep 117: 26-49.
28. Di Renzo M, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Vanadia E, Petrillo M, Racinaro L, et
al. (2016) From the emotional integration to the cognitive construction: The
developmental approach of turtle project in children with autism spectrum
disorder. Autism Open Access 6: 1-9.
29. Di Renzo M, Petrillo M, Bianchi di Castelbianco F (2011) Le potenzialità
intellettive nel bambino autistico. Nuove prospettive attraverso l’interpretazione
del Test Leiter-R. [The intellectual potential of the autistic child: new perspectives
through the interpretation of the Leiter-R scale]; Magi, Rome.
Citation: Di Renzo M, Marini C, Bianchi di Castelbianco F, Racinaro L, Rea
M (2017) Correlations between the Drawing Process in Autistic Children and
Developmental Indexes. J Psychol Psychother 7: 291. doi: 10.4172/2161-
0487.1000291
OMICS International: Publication Benefits & Features
Unique features:
Increased global visibility of articles through worldwide distribution and indexing
Showcasing recent research output in a timely and updated manner
Special issues on the current trends of scientic research
Special features:
700+ Open Access Journals
50,000+ editorial team
Rapid review process
Quality and quick editorial, review and publication processing
Indexing at major indexing services
Sharing Option: Social Networking Enabled
Authors, Reviewers and Editors rewarded with online Scientic Credits
Better discount for your subsequent articles
Submit your manuscript at: http://www.omicsonline.org/submission/
... Magda Di Renzo examines the correlation between drawing ability and the level of development in autistic children [32]. Drawing level was observed in spontaneous production by creating a scale ranging from the presence of disordered scribbling to the presence of body schema. ...
... In observing and evaluating the drawings of autistic children, Di Renzo M [32] notes that many of them do not use drawing in a communicative and relational way but in a stereotypical way to escape context. When comparing drawings of the same child in different contexts, they found that children often differed significantly. ...
... The results show the close relationship of drawing with the level of autistic symptomatology. All children who are part of ASD have a significant delay in the drawing process, and this delay is not related to the IQ but rather to social affection [32]. In our research, we found that there is no strong correlation between IQ level and the ability of drawing. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The peculiarities of sensory perception and perception of one's own body in children with autism are the basis for understanding their cognitive and social development difficulties. Objective: The study aims to structure different categories of drawings of children with autism and compare them with the severity of autism measured by CARS2. Methods: 120 children aged 3 to 9 years were studied (X= 6.26, SD = 3.16). Drawings of autistic children. The children have a white sheet, pencils, a children's drawing table, and the experimenter asks them to draw a person. The children were studied with CARS. 2. Clinical method: includes observation, direct work with the child on each of the topics of the methodology used, interview with parents, diagnostic discussion with the clinical team. Psychodiagnostic method: includes an examination of children with mental developmental stairs, assessment of cognitive, communication, socioemotional and sensorimotor functions. CARS 2 Childhood Autism Rating Scale | Second Edition Statistical. Method: includes data processing using the SPSS programme. Descriptive statistics, correlation analysis, a frequency distribution of data are used for data analysis. Results: The results show eight main categories of drawings in autistic children: 1. circles, water; 2. patches of colour covering the human figure or representing a human figure without an external boundary; 3. figures and letters; 4. human figures fenced as a bubble, a human figure composed of parts of objects (the elements are not connected); 5. objects with geometric shapes (buildings, roads with markings, apartment blocks, strange shells; 6. road signs, logos. Conclusions: There is no statistical dependence between the severity of autistic symptomatology and the types of drawings. We can draw some conclusions about how a child with autism perceives his own body from the presented results. Because we see that in mild and moderate degrees of autism CARS 2, the whole variety of drawing categories was evaluated, while in severe and very severe degrees of expression of the disorder circles, colourful spots with vague boundaries predominate. Children with autism often identify with non-living objects, street signs, eccentric houses and towers.
... Moreover, drawing promotes relaxation and can serve as a calming activity during therapy sessions, fostering a positive therapeutic environment. By harnessing the power of drawing, therapists can enhance communication, promote emotional regulation, and support the overall development of children with ASD (Di Renzo et al., 2017). The findings of this study imply an intriguing potential for drawing interventions. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The relationship between symbolic thinking and language abilities is a topic of intense debate. We have recently discovered three distinct phenotypes of language comprehension, which we defined as command, modifier and syntactic phenotypes (Vyshedskiy et al., 2024). Individuals in the command phenotype were limited to comprehension of simple commands, while those in the modifier phenotype showed additional comprehension of color, size, and number modifiers. Finally, individuals in the most-advanced syntactic phenotype added comprehension of spatial prepositions, verb tenses, flexible syntax, possessive pronouns, complex explanations, and fairytales. In this report we analyzed how these three language phenotypes differed in their symbolic thinking as exhibited through their drawing abilities. In a cohort of 39,654 autistic individuals 4- to 21-years-of-age, parents reported that 'drawing, coloring and art' was manifested by 36.0% of participants. Among these individuals, representational drawing was manifested by 54.1% of individuals with syntactic-, 27.7% of those with modifier-, and 10.1% of those with command-phenotype (all pairwise differences between the phenotypes were statistically significant, p < 0.0001). The ability to draw a novel image per parent's description (e.g. a three-headed horse) was reported by 34.6% of individuals with syntactic-, 7.9% of those with modifier-, and 1.9% of individuals with command-phenotype (all pairwise differences between the phenotypes were statistically significant, p < 0.0001). These results show strong association between the representational drawing ability and the syntactic-language-comprehension-phenotype, suggesting a potential benefit of drawing interventions in language therapy.
... On the other hand, students with autism often benefit from a form of visual thinking, which, as described by Grandin (1995), is the ability to think and reason through images and visual systems. This type of thinking may often manifest through the use of drawings, which serves as a crucial mode of expression, especially given challenges in communication inherent to the disorder (Di Renzo et al., 2017). In the domain of problem-solving, this has been manifested through the frequent use of drawing-based strategies in students with autism when solving algebraic tasks and word problems (Goñi-Cervera et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the multiplicative problem-solving strategies used by a 14-year-old student with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disabilities during an instructional process based on the Conceptual Model-based Problem Solving (COMPS) approach. The instruction aimed to enhance conceptual comprehension of problem-solving by the use of model diagrams representing mathematical relations involved in word problems. These diagrams aid in selecting the appropriate operation for each type of multiplicative problem, including equal-groups, multiplicative comparison, and combination problems. We further discuss how the instructional process promoted conceptual understanding for the three problem types, highlighting the development of strategies (modeling, counting, and operations) and the pivotal role of the COMPS methodology components in this progression. The results indicate that the student adeptly adopted the COMPS approach, facilitating the transition from informal modeling to written operations, with his choice of strategies varying depending on the type of problem. While there was a higher utilization of modeling strategies in equal-groups and combination problems, modeling was not as frequently employed in comparison problems. We speculate how this differentiated strategy preference could be associated with certain characteristics of the disorder.
... Di Renzo et al. (2017) tvrdia, že u dieťaťa s PAS, ktoré má potvrdenú priemernú inteligenciu, možno zaznamenať výrazné oneskorenie v kresbe. ...
Article
The paper describes the theoretical-research background oriented to the drawing of a child with autism spectrum disorder with a comparison of characteristics in the drawing of a child with intact development. Through analyses of drawings of a child with Asperger's syndrome, the authors present unique characteristics of drawing expression. They analyze 13 of the authors' drawings of a boy with Asperger's syndrome that were collected over a two-year period. Specifically, this was the developmental period of the boy with Asperger's syndrome when he began to be drawing active (4 years and 6 months) and at a marked progression in drawing representation (6 years and 7 months). The authors consider the qualitative assessment of drawings as a possible evaluative support for further research on the appreciation of drawings of children with autism spectrum disorder.
... Possiamo ritrovare quest'espansione dell'esperienza nell'intreccio tra produzione grafica e sfera affettiva, studiata da Di Renzo e colleghi (2017). Parallelamente al passaggio progressivo da scarabocchi stereotipati, indicatori di un'evasione dalla relazione (Di Renzo et al., 2017), a forme concentriche che, necessitando di un maggiore controllo per la loro realizzazione, potremmo considerare precursori di forme pre-schematiche di rappresentazione, abbiamo osservato un approfondimento della relazione di Andrea con il mondo di cui è parte. Ipotizziamo quindi che l'evoluzione riscontrabile nell'attraversamento delle tre cornici riportate, sia stata espressa anche attraverso l'organizzazione degli elementi grafici sullo spazio-foglio. ...
Article
Full-text available
In questo lavoro abbiamo inteso coniugare l’approccio psicodinamico con le recenti acquisizioni neuro-scientifiche sullo sviluppo dell’intersoggettività, con attenzione particolare all’area comunicativo-linguistica. Il materiale empirico, raccolto attraverso un’osservazione longitudinale semi-strutturata di un bambino diagnosticato nello spettro autistico, mostra l’intreccio tra i processi intersoggettivi e lo sviluppo della dimensione simbolica nelle diverse modalità espressive in cui si esplicita, rappresentate nel nostro caso dal linguaggio e dal disegno. L’interconnessione dei tre registri - fonetico, linguistico e grafico - ha permesso di identificare l’avvento del linguaggio come produzione intenzionale che segnala la configurazione della soggettività nel campo relazionale.
Preprint
Full-text available
The relationship between symbolic thinking and language abilities is a topic of intense debate. We have recently identified three distinct language comprehension phenotypes: command, modifier and syntactic (Vyshedskiy et al., 2024). Individuals in the command phenotype were limited to comprehension of simple commands, while those in the modifier phenotype showed additional comprehension of color, size, and number modifiers. Finally, individuals in the most-advanced syntactic phenotype added comprehension of spatial prepositions, verb tenses, flexible syntax, possessive pronouns, complex explanations, and fairytales. In this report we analyzed how these three language phenotypes differed in their symbolic thinking as evidenced by their drawing abilities. In a cohort of 39,654 autistic individuals 4- to 21-years-of-age, parents reported that ‘drawing, coloring and art’ was manifested by 36.0% of participants. Among these individuals, representational drawing was manifested by 54.1% of individuals with syntactic-phenotype, 27.7% of those with modifier-phenotype, and 10.1% of those with command-phenotype (all pairwise differences between the phenotypes were statistically significant, p < 0.0001). The ability to draw a novel image per parent’s description (e.g. a three-headed horse) was reported by 34.6% of individuals with syntactic-phenotype, 7.9% of those with modifier- phenotype, and 1.9% of individuals with command-phenotype (all pairwise differences between the phenotypes were statistically significant, p < 0.0001). These results demonstrate strong association between representational drawing ability and the syntactic-language-comprehension-phenotype, suggesting a potential benefit of drawing interventions in language therapy.
Conference Paper
In the fast-moving world of today, the recent statistics of population patron, 1 in 63 children are affected with Autism. Up-to-date no well-defined treatment for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) has been invented yet. In this study, an easy, cost-effective, time saving and attractive software package is introduced. The main objective of developing such a package is to differentiate neurotypical and ASD children's behavior patterns. This package was designed by considering three main impaired areas of ASD which are Eye Contact, Maturity Level, and Intelligence Level. Therefore, the developed software package is comprised of an Eye Movement Tracking tool, Maturity Level Analyzer, and Intelligence Level Measuring tool. In the Eye Movement Tracking tool, a common sample video can be shown to the participants (Neurotypical Children and ASD Children) and their eye movements shall be recorded. Then the recorded data will be processed using symmetric mass center algorithm and displayed the results in three different graphs. Maturity Level Analyzer provides a drawing canvas where participants shall be allowed to draw a given shape. Then, the analysis of the drawing shall be done by comparing the standard deviation (SD) of participants' data. Intelligence level Measuring Tool compromised with color and number-based activities where participants' responses will be taken to decide the child's condition. Testing and evaluation of the system were done with two different types of participants as three (3) ASD patients and ten (10) neurotypical persons from the age groups of 3–5 years. To evaluate the accuracy and performance of the tools, two tests as ‘pre-test’ and ‘post-test’ were done with the participants. The tested results have shown a significant difference between ASD patients and neurotypical children's behavior. Thus, this set of tools is more effective for taking decisions in ASD prediction and measuring the improvements of ASD children with treatments.
Chapter
Full-text available
Developmental approaches characterized interventions based on the typical sequences of the child's development, paying particular attention to the linguistic, motor and socio-emotional aspects, within a relational perspective. The purpose of this paper was to identify developmental interventions for children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) aged between 0 and 12 years old. A literature review was carried out searching on MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and PsycARTICLES and manually on key journals and reference lists of key articles [...] This review highlights the need for improved research on intervention programs and the important service needs for families and children with Autism Spectrum Disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The present study was designed to compare hand function in autistic children with history of upper extremity trauma with that of autistic children those who do not have history of trauma. Methods: The study group included total of 65 children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and was divided into 2 groups: children with trauma history (Group I) and control group (Group II) (Group I: n=28; Group II: n=37). Hand function was evaluated with 9-Hole Peg Test and Jebsen Hand Function Test. Somatosensory function was evaluated using somatosensory subtests of Sensory Integration and Praxis Test. Results were analyzed with Student's t-test and Mann-Whitney U test using SPSS version 20 software. Results: Hand function and somatosensory perception test scores were statistically significantly better in children without upper extremity trauma history (p<0.05). When association between hand function tests and upper extremity somatosensory perception tests was taken into account, statistically significant correlations were found between all parameters of hand function tests and Manual Form Perception and Localization of Tactile Stimuli Test results (p<0.05). Conclusion: Autistic children with upper extremity trauma history had poor somatosensory perception and hand function. It is important to raise awareness among emergency service staff and inform them about strong relationship between somatosensory perception, hand function, and upper extremity trauma in children with ASD in order to develop appropriate rehabilitation process and prevent further trauma.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the context of executive functioning in preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASDs) and verifies the degree of agreement between the compilation of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) by parents and therapists. To this end, parents and professionals of 46 ASD children aged between 24 and 71 months completed the BRIEF-P in order to highlight the level of agreement between these two different evaluators and then investigate the correlations between executive functions impairment, the degree of autistic symptomatology, the level of cognitive development, the chronological age and the therapy duration. The results showed that parents tend to underestimate the impairment of executive functions in ASD children compared to what estimated by specialized operators. A low level of agreement was found between the two evaluators. Moreover, both for parents and professionals, the EF impairment was not related to the severity of autistic symptoms, assessed with the ADOS-2, while it was correlated to cognitive development in professional evaluations, and to chronological age and therapy duration in parental evaluations. These results indicate that, even if among the pathogenic assumptions about ASDs there has been included also that of the EF impairment, this, if present, does not correlate with the severity of autistic symptoms. With regard to cognitive development, the greater correlation between cognitive and executive impairment seems to be rather obvious. The use of the BRIEF-P completed by parents allows them a greater awareness but does not add clinical data to the diagnosis.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing tasks are frequently used to test competing theories of visuospatial skills in autism. Yet, methodological differences between studies have led to inconsistent findings. To distinguish between accounts based on local bias or global deficit, we present a simple task that has previously revealed dissociable local/global impairments in neuropsychological patients. Autistic and typical children copied corner elements, arranged in a square configuration. Grouping cues were manipulated to test whether global properties affected the accuracy of reproduction. All children were similarly affected by these manipulations. There was no group difference in the reproduction of local elements, although global accuracy was negatively related to better local processing for autistic children. These data speak against influential theories of visuospatial differences in autism.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Children with autism spectrum disorder show a deficit in neurobiological processes. This deficit hinders the development of intentional behavior and appropriate problem-solving, leading the child to implement repetitive and stereotyped behaviors and to have difficulties in reciprocal interactions, empathy and in the development of a theory of mind. The objective of this research is to verify the effectiveness of a relationship-based approach on the positive evolution of autistic symptoms. Method: A sample of 80 children with autism spectrum disorder was monitored during the first four years of therapy, through a clinical diagnostic assessment at the time of intake and then in two follow-up. Results: The results showed that through the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule it is possible to assess the socio-relational key elements on which the therapy is based. There was evidence, in fact, of significant improvements after two and four years of therapy, both for children with severe autistic symptoms and for those in autistic spectrum. Conclusions: Socio-relational aspects represent the primary element on which work in therapy with autistic children and can be considered as indicators of a positive evolution and prognosis that will produce improvements even in the cognitive area.
Article
Full-text available
Here we examine imaginative drawing abilities in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and learning disabilities (LD) under several conditions: spontaneous production, with use of a template, and combining two real entities to form an 'unreal' entity. Sixteen children in each group, matched on mental and chronological age, were asked to draw a number of 'impossible' pictures of humans and dogs. Children with ASD were impaired in spontaneous drawings and included fewer impossible features than children with LD, but there was no difference when a template was provided. An autism-specific deficit was revealed in the task involving combining entities. Results suggest that children with ASD do not have a general imaginative deficit; impairment is instead related to planning demands.
Book
Full-text available
La rappresentazione grafica costituisce uno strumento per oggettivare il livello di maturazione raggiunto dal bambino in ambito psicomotorio, cognitivo ed emotivo e, nello stesso tempo, si pone come strumento prezioso di espressione sia in ambito educativo che terapeutico. Il comprendere e il saper valutare la produzione grafica di un bambino autistico è un indispensabile supporto per capire i diversi aspetti della sua evoluzione e la sua personalità. La valutazione grafica in produzione spontanea di un campione di 100 bambini autistici ha permesso di delineare come il disegno libero e il disegno della figura umana mostrino la specificità di un linguaggio diverso da quello verbale che consente manifestazioni peculiari sia sul versante cognitivo – esplorato anche attraverso le prove di Santucci – sia su quello affettivo, permettendo una descrizione del bambino con autismo. La correlazione con altri strumenti diagnostici (come con l'ADOS-G e la Leiter-R) ha consentito un arricchimento della valutazione e della prognosi e una migliore progettualità terapeutica. Nel volume vengono riportati gli esiti del test grafico-proiettivo – reattivo di Wartegg –, somministrato a 40 bambini, che ha consentito di mettere in luce aspetti della loro personalità. La possibilità di somministrare il test di Wartegg si è resa possibile solo dopo un percorso terapeutico.
Article
Full-text available
-90 Italian children (72 boys, 18 girls) with a diagnosis of infantile autism (age range = 2.5-16.5 yr.) were assessed with a non-verbal intelligence test (Leiter-R). The test was repeated 3 times in four years. The measures used were IQ and Fluid Reasoning (FR), as evaluation of inductive and deductive reasoning, and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), indicative of the severity of autistic symptoms at the beginning and at the end of therapy. The increase in the average IQ and FR scores at retests demonstrates the effectiveness of a treatment that emphasizes the centrality of the relationship-based approach. Moreover, the FR score at intake was predictive of a significant decrease of ADOS scores after four years of treatment, and of the increase in IQ observed in later evaluations. The data support the hypothesis that a relationship-based intervention allows cognitive improvement regardless of the autism severity expressed in the ADOS score.
Article
Full-text available
OBJECTIVE. We sought to characterize sensory integration (SI) and praxis patterns of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and discern whether these patterns relate to social participation. METHOD. We extracted Sensory Integration and Praxis Tests (SIPT) and Sensory Processing Measure (SPM) scores from clinical records of children with ASD ages 4-11 yr (N = 89) and used SIPT and SPM standard scores to describe SI and praxis patterns. Correlation coefficients were generated to discern relationships among SI and praxis scores and these scores' associations with SPM Social Participation scores. RESULTS. Children with ASD showed relative strengths in visual praxis. Marked difficulties were evident in imitation praxis, vestibular bilateral integration, somatosensory perception, and sensory reactivity. SPM Social Participation scores were inversely associated with areas of deficit on SIPT measures. CONCLUSION. Children with ASD characteristically display strengths in visuopraxis and difficulties with somatopraxis and vestibular functions, which appear to greatly affect participation. Copyright © 2015 by the American Occupational Therapy Association, Inc.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder in the category of pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), which is characterized by widespread abnormalities of social interactions, communication, and severely restricted interests and highly repetitive behavior. Children with autism show sensory and perceptual abnormalities. They have either hyposensitivity or hypersensitivity to sensory, auditory, and visual stimuli. Objectives: The aim of this work was to study somatosensory evoked potential (SSEPs) changes among children with autism, and their relation to somatosensory manifestations and severity of autism. Subjects: Thirty children with autism aged 2–12 years were included in the study, all of them fulfilling criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV–TR). Methods: All cases were subjected to thorough history taking including autistic symptoms and sensory abnormalities, comprehensive medical examination, psychiatric assessment according to DSM–IV–TR criteria for diagnosing autism, assessment of severity of autism using Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) and measurement of somatosensory evoked potentials elicited by median nerve stimulation at wrist. Results: The majority of the cases were males (86.7%), according to CARS 53.3% were classified as mild to moderate autism, while 46.7% were severe. Sensory abnormalities were present in 56.7% of cases. Somatosensory abnormalities were present in 36.76% of the cases. There was a statistically significant relationship between sensory symptoms with SSEP abnormalities (P = 0.040). The presence of abnormal SSEPs was not statistically associated with higher score in CARS. Conclusions: Children with autism have abnormal SSEP changes and were significantly related to the presence of sensory abnormalities, indicating central cortical dysfunction of somatosensory area. On the other hand, these abnormal SSEP changes were not related to the severity of autism.
Article
The autistic impairments in emotional and social competence, imagination and generating ideas predict qualitative differences in expressive drawings by children with autism beyond that accounted by any general learning difficulties. In a sample of 60 5-19-year-olds, happy and sad drawings were requested from 15 participants with non-savant autism and compared with those drawn by three control groups matched on either degree of learning difficulty (MLD), mental age (MA) or chronological age (CA). All drawings were rated by two artists on a 7-point quality of expression scale. Contrary to our predictions, the drawings from the autistic group were rated similar to those of the MA and MLD groups. Analysis of the people and social content of the drawings revealed that although children with autism did not draw fewer people, they did draw more immature forms than mental age controls. Furthermore, there was tentative evidence that fewer social scenes were produced by the autism sample. We conclude that the overall merit of expressive drawing in autism is commensurate with their general learning difficulties, but the social/emotional impairment in autism affects their drawings of people and social scenes.