ArticlePDF Available

Mechanisms of Response Prevention and the Use of Exposure as Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

Authors:

Abstract

The combination of exposure therapy and response prevention (ERP) is the most widely used and to date the most effective treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder. We review the two main theoretical mechanisms proposed to account for the effectiveness of ERP: (1) habituation of anxiety due to exposure and (2) extinction due to withholding reinforcement of behaviors undertaken to decrease anxiety. Both of these mechanisms have their origin in behavior theory based upon classical and instrumental conditioning, and relate to the view that OCD is an anxiety-related or anxiety- based disorder. DSM- 5, however, no longer lists OCD as an anxiety disorder, instead positing an obsessive - compulsive disorder spectrum (OCDS), and emphasizing the diversity of OCD symptoms. More recent cognitive and neuroscience approaches have also stressed mechanisms involved in the control of emotional and behavioral responses. In this paper we review habituation and extinction accounts and attempt to integrate the newer neuroscience perspectives, moving toward a more complete framework for understanding OCD treatment.
Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 1 of 8
Int J Psychiatry, 2017
Mechanisms of Response Prevention and the Use of Exposure as erapy for
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Review Article
1Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
2Dept of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene.
Abstract
The combination of exposure therapy and response prevention (ERP) is the most widely used and to date the most
effective treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder. We review the two main theoretical mechanisms proposed to
account for the effectiveness of ERP: (1) habituation of anxiety due to exposure and (2) extinction due to withholding
reinforcement of behaviors undertaken to decrease anxiety. Both of these mechanisms have their origin in behavior
theory based upon classical and instrumental conditioning, and relate to the view that OCD is an anxiety-related or
anxiety- based disorder. DSM- 5, however, no longer lists OCD as an anxiety disorder, instead positing an obsessive
- compulsive disorder spectrum (OCDS), and emphasizing the diversity of OCD symptoms. More recent cognitive
and neuroscience approaches have also stressed mechanisms involved in the control of emotional and behavioral
responses. In this paper we review habituation and extinction accounts and attempt to integrate the newer neuroscience
perspectives, moving toward a more complete framework for understanding OCD treatment.
International Journal of Psychiatry
Habibollah Ghassemzadeh1, Mary K. Rothbart2 and Michael I.Posner2*
*Corresponding author
Michael I. Posner, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR USA, E-Mail:
mposner@uoregon.edu.
Submitted: 14 Nov 2016; Accepted: 26 Dec 2017; Published: 30 Apr 2017
Keywords: ERP, Habituation, Extinction, OCDS, Neuroscience
perspectives
Introduction
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) is a common, chronic
disorder in which a person has uncontrollable, reoccurring
thoughts or images (obsessions) and/or behaviors (compulsions)
that he or she feels the urge to repeat over and over– mostly in
a ritualistic manner. OCD has many subtypes or clusters of
symptoms. Rasmussen and Eisen studied 560 OCD patients in
the USA meeting DSM-III or DSM-III-R criteria [1]. The most
prevalent obsession theme in that study was contamination (50%),
and the least prevalent was sexual impulse (24%). The other themes
included pathologic doubt, somatic concerns, need for symmetry,
and aggressive impulses. The most prevalent compulsion was
checking (61%) and the least prevalent was hoarding (18%).
Other compulsive behavior included cleaning/washing, counting,
seeking exactness and expressing a need to confess.
Obsessive- compulsive disorder has been indicated as the fourth
most common psychiatric disorder and the tenth leading cause
of disability in the world (World Health Organization [2]. The
lifetime prevalence of OCD worldwide is approximately 2-3 %
and the prevalence in ve US communities ranged from 1.9 to
3.3 % [3, 4]. According to the National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) more than 2.2 million Americans suffer from obsessive-
compulsive disorder. It strikes men and women in roughly equal
numbers and usually appears in childhood, adolescence, or early
adulthood. One-third of adults with OCD developed symptoms
as children, and research indicates that OCD may run in families.
Although the prevalence of various symptoms tends to vary
widely across different cultures and studies, the overall pattern of
symptoms is more or less similar [5, 6].
In this paper we examine exposure therapy combined with response
prevention (ERP), one of the most widely applied psychological
treatments in anxiety-related disorders in general and obsessive-
compulsive disorder (OCD) in particular.It is important to note,
however, that in the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of the American Psychiatric Association the disorder
was removed from the set of anxiety disorders and given its own
spectrum [7]. Because of the new diagnostic category and recent
imaging ndings we focus on relating issues of exposure to those of
response prevention to develop a common framework for therapy.
In a recent meta-analysis, different pharmacological and
psychotherapeutic interventions for OCD in adults were
systematically reviewed and analyzed [8, 9]. The three main
categories of psychotherapeutic interventions were examined:
behavioral therapy (BT), cognitive therapy (CT), cognitive
behavioral therapy (CBT) and CBT with ERP. The pharmacological
agents included clomipramine, uvoxamine, and SSRIs. The
investigators also considered the combinations of psychological
and pharmacological treatments. The criterion for efcacy was
based on Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale and the waiting
list was taken as a control group in most of these studies.
Results showed that all three psychotherapeutic interventions were
more effective than drug placebo. Comparing these 3 interventions,
cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) was less effective than BT
Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 2 of 8Int J Psychiatry 2017
and CT and was not different from the psychological placebo
(general stress management). But excluding waiting list controls
led to a larger effect of CBT. The general conclusion was that
all 3 psychotherapies “were more likely to lead to a larger effect
than were medications” (p.7). The combined treatment of both
medication and psychological treatment was more effective than
the latter alone. This nding differs from the Foa et al. study,
which showed CBT combined with pharmacological treatment
(clomipramine) was not more effective than CBT alone [10].
Although some practitioners using cognitive models of OCD
have found cognitive methods to be the best choice for reducing
obsessions and compulsions, cognitive therapy and cognitive
behavior therapy in this review “are no more effective than
ERP”(p.145) [11]. Also as Abramowitz has indicated, “the
prognosis for individuals for OCD has changed from poor to very
good as a result of the development of ERP” (p.407) [12]. We now
consider some of the most popular theoretical models of ERP,
and results of brain imaging studies suggesting alternative views
of the effectiveness of ERP and the possibility of new treatment
approaches.
Exposure and exposure therapy
Exposure therapy and response prevention (ERP) involves a set
of psychological treatment approaches and/or techniques for
improving anxiety-related disorders, including OCD. The common
core of these approaches and techniques is asking patients to
confront their anxiety provoking situations or fearful thoughts
while controlling their usual response to the situation.
Two main theoretical mechanisms have been proposed to
account for the effectiveness of ERP: habituation due to response
exposure, and extinction due to withholding reinforcement of
the behaviors undertaken to decrease anxiety [13]. Both of these
mechanisms have their origin in behavior theory based on classical
and instrumental conditioning, and are for the most part based on
the view that OCD is an anxiety disorder. There are problems,
however, regarding the theoretical basis of this set of treatments
and the mechanism of their efcacy. As argued by Abramowitz, it
is crucial to have an up to date theoretical framework in order to
design and execute effective treatment [13].
In this paper we review the historical basis for dening the role of
habituation and extinction in ERP. In addition, we consider new
studies using imaging to identify mechanisms of control of responses
in OCD. In particular, brain systems of executive attention have
been found to exercise control over both emotional and behavioral
responses [14]. We hope this review might aid clinicians to clarify
the theory underlying existing therapies and aid in the development
of possible new therapies that could improve treatment of OCD.
Historically the application of exposure/response prevention goes
back to Meyer’s innovative work on the treatment of two patients:
one with compulsive washing and another with sexual obsessions
[15]. The main rationale for this approach was that if OCD patients
are asked and persuaded in a fear situation to withhold carrying
out the compulsive rituals, they will eventually realize that (1)
the feared consequences of not performing the ritual do not occur
and (2) their expectations of “disastrous consequences” are not
fullled [15]. Meyer reported some success in his rst study and
in a later study, used the same technique with 15 OCD patients.
In the second study ten patients were either “much improved” or
totally improved [16]. Meyer’s main conceptualization was that “a
completely successful modication of expectations would lead to
a complete elimination of ritualistic behavior” [15].
Although Meyer’s treatment was based on modication of
expectations, a likely cognitive interpretation, many researchers
and clinicians shifted their attention from a cognitive strategy
to a behavioral mechanism, attempting to explain the possible
treatment effects of ERP within the framework of conditioning
terminology [16].
At this time behavior was associated with physical reexes, for
example, Isaac Marks published an article entitled “the current
status of behavioral psychotherapy: theory and practice”,
suggesting exposure as a common principle of most behavioral
approaches to the treatment of phobic disorders and compulsive
rituals [17]. He refers to Wolpe’s desensitization in fantasy as
an early form of exposure treatment (imaginal exposure) [18].
He also explains exposure in vivo, which gives quicker results
by bringing the patient into contact with the anxiety-provoking
situation without relaxation [19].
Emotional processing theory
Probably the most inuential recent theoretical approach to OCD
has been emotional processing theory. Lang’s bio- informational
theory described anxiety as an emotional memory stored within
a semantic brain network [20, 21]. The emotional memories
were hypothesized to contain three categories of information,
(1) information about the stimulus or situation that evokes
the emotional memory structure; (2) information regarding an
individual’s responses (physiological, motor and cognitive);
and (3) elaboration that denes the meaning of the stimulus and
response. Emotions are dened as stored action dispositions,
released when a fear structure is activated. Any response pattern
depends on arousal level, valence of the stimuli, and degree of
control.
Foa extended the ideas of Lang and Rachman and proposed a
processing theory, hypothesizing that anxiety occurs as the result
of a pathological “fear structure” held in memory [20-23]. A fear
structure is a propositional network of information related to a
program to escape or avoid danger.
From this perspective, in any systematic exposure therapy session,
three important events occur. The rst is activation of the fear
structure in memory. The second is introduction of corrective
information through repeated and prolonged exposure to the feared
stimulus, leading to a modication of the fear structure, allowing
habituation within the session. The third involves changes in
the meaning of the activated fear structure. This change occurs
between sessions and is more reliably associated with long-term
therapeutic change.
Foa and McNally further revised the emotional processing
approach in a more cognitive direction by suggesting that any
successful exposure therapy goes beyond the mere modication
of existing fear structures [23]. New structures are created, which
override the previous associations.
In a more recent revision, Foa, Ruppert & Cahill proposed
that symptom reduction is though modication of erroneous
associations, not through habituation per se [24]. Accordingly we
next examine habituation and extinction as the mechanisms used
Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 3 of 8Int J Psychiatry 2017
to account for effectiveness of therapy.
Habituation
Habituation may be dened as the waning of a response to a
stimulus that occurs when the stimulus is repeatedly presented
[25]. In contrast to extinction, which is explained as associative
learning, habituation is seen as non-associative learning.
Habituation as used in stress neurobiology is conceptualized as
reduction in physiological responses to a repeated stressor in
comparison with initial response to the stressor [26]. In exposure
therapy, habituation refers to reductions in fear over time as a
person encounter fear-inducing stimuli [27, 28]. Habituation is
often measured through physiological variables such as heart rate
and skin conductance or through self-report measures such as the
Subjective Unit of Distress (SUDS) [29].
Thompson indicated that the notion of habituation is as old as
humankind but that experimental studies about habituation began
at the end of nineteenth century and early twentieth century [30].
After reviewing the basic properties of habituation as described
in classic works, Thompson refers to Thompson and Spencer’s
review in which nine basic parameters or common characteristics
of habituation were identied [30, 31]. These parameters are mostly
related to short-term or within - session habituation, emphasizing
the importance of repetition, spontaneous recovery, frequency of
stimulation, and generalization. Rankin et al. reviewed and revised
some of the nine parameters of Thompson and Spencer and added
an item that is mostly related to long-term habituation [31, 32].
They proposed that some stimulus repetition protocols might result
in response decrement lasting hours, days or weeks, suggesting
between session effects that are discussed in the next section.
The problem of within-and between session habituation
One of the main problems in the formulation and application
of emotional processing based exposure therapy is the relation
between what has been called within-session habituation to
between-session habituation. Foa’s initial position was that “The
activation of affect, its reduction during exposure sessions, and its
decrease across sessions, appear positively related to treatment
outcome, denoting evocation and modication of fear memories
during therapy” [24]. In most studies, however, a direct relationship
between within-session habituation and symptom reduction has
not been found [29].
Foa’s group discussed this issue in their 2006 update [23]. They
conclude that “within-session habituation is not a reliable indicator
of emotional processing” and suggest that “ some information may
take time to be processed, such that disconrming information
that had been presented during exposure is not fully incorporated
until some time after the exposure exercise (i.e., between sessions)
rather than within the sessions”(p.9).
Foa and McLean further suggest that factors such as distraction
and cognitive avoidance may interfere with full incorporation of
new information in the structure of memory so that the true change
in the structure of memory occurs after the exposure session [33].
Therefore, they propose a “full engagement with an exposure
exercise” (without any distraction or cognitive avoidance) to reach
a lasting outcome.
Reviewing the research on initial fear activation, within-session
habituation, and between-session habituation, Craske et al.
conclude that there is not any established relation between these
indices and therapeutic outcome [34]. Their suggestion is that we
need to move away from immediate fear reduction toward longer
term fear tolerance as a primary goal of exposure therapy. As an
explanation of exposure therapy they emphasize the inhibitory
learning central to extinction as an alternative account of what
happens in therapy. We now consider the concept of extinction.
Extinction
Extinction refers to the gradual weakening of an instrumental
response that results in the behavior decreasing or disappearing.
Extinction can occur if the trained behavior is no longer reinforced
or if the type of reinforcement used is no longer rewarding.
As has been shown in extensive empirical work, extinction may
be characterized as a form of inhibitory learning rather than an
erasure of acquired fear [35]. In other words, it is not simply an
unlearning or forgetting but rather a new process that changes the
relation between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned
stimulus. The amygdala has been suggested as the main area that
controls such a process [36]. Another area active in extinction
learning is the medial prefrontal cortex including the anterior
cingulate cortex (ACC), thought to regulate the function of the
amygdala [36]. The idea that exposure therapy is an automatic,
low-level process, has been challenged and it is believed today that
exposure therapy is based on extinction and involves many high-
level cognitive elements [37].
In studies of mice, the ability to extinguish fear by extinction has
been improved by a reinstatement procedure [38]. Reinstatement
involves repeating a fear-inducing stimulus. If after such a
reinstatement one introduces a drug that blocks norepinephrine
(e.g. propranolol) or carries out extinction trials within a short
period of time after reinstatement of the fear, the effectiveness in
reducing fear is increased. A recent mouse study has found that
stimulation of a circuit from the amygdala to the striatum either
optogenetically or by inducing a reward may improve extinction
of fear by reducing the tendency for it to spontaneously recover
[39].
These ndings in mice t with the importance of the production of
anxiety as a predictor of the effectiveness of exposure therapy in
patients with OCD [33]. However, subsequent studies of patients
with anxiety have shown that extinction may not always occur
and we do not know if the reduction of fear by itself will result
in improvement of the OCD symptoms [40]. However, the use
of reinstatement or simultaneous stimulation of reward pathways
may result in improvement of the existing exposure techniques as
a treatment for OCD.
Neuroscience approaches to OCD
In the new classication of psychiatric disorders (DSM-5), OCD
has been integrated within an obsessive-compulsive disorders
spectrum (OCDS). Although some psychologists have criticized
this approach, it has led investigators to pay more attention to
the different kinds of OCD that may involve different biological
mechanisms, including those involved in response prevention [41,
42]. Thus using the term OCD spectrum may be helpful both in
research and clinical practice. Imaging studies have led to a better
understanding of the regulatory mechanisms by which responses
are prevented and we discuss these mechanisms in relation to
different OCD symptoms.
OCDSs in this new system have been characterized by three
features:(1) compulsivity, which includes body dysmorphic
disorder (BDD), anorexia nervosa (AN), hypochondrias is, and
depersonalization disorder; (2) impulsivity, including sexual
compulsions, self-injuring behavior, trichotillomania (obsessive
hair pulling), kleptomania, compulsive buying, and pathological
gambling (PG); and (3) OCDSs with signicant neurological
symptoms, including Sydenham’s chorea, Tourette’s syndrome
(TS), and autism [43].
Graybiel and Rauch in search of a neurobiological basis for OCD
have indicated some key features of OCD which makes their
approach very similar to the concept of OCDSs [44]. They have
mentioned ve features summarized as follows:
OCD patients are usually aware that their compulsions and
obsessions are senseless, but they cannot control them despite
effort;
The symptoms usually are not bizarre;
There is a considerable degree of consistency in the themes
of OCD across cultures, with some degree of heterogeneity in
specic symptoms;
Some patients suffer mainly obsessions or compulsions
and others both. In some cases the disorder shows itself as
cognitive- affective and in others as executive- behavioral.
The two concepts may in fact be related as executive attention
serves as a control over affect [14, 45].
The obsessions as thoughts, images and urges and the
compulsions, including washing , cleaning, checking, and
doing things right may continue for hours and the only way to
stop them is to get enough assurance from others.
These features of OCD suggest that there are neural circuits that
trigger repetitive and resistant behaviors and thoughts, and that
most often the patient is aware of the existence of these intrusive
events [44]. It is important to note that exposure therapy is not
applicable for most of the conditions classied under the title of
OCDSs. Abramowitz and Jacoby believe this is because “exposure
is derived from a specic psychological mechanism involving
excessive fear that is maintained by avoidance and ritualistic
behavior [41]. This pattern is present in OCD and body dysmorphic
disorder, but not in hair pulling, compulsive skin picking, or
hoarding” (p.282).
Abramowitz and Jacoby discuss the use and misuse of exposure
therapy in OCD and related disorders [41]. The aim of exposure
is to facilitate extinction-related reduction in the conditioned
anxiety/fear response associated with the feared stimulus. If this is
the case, a broadened view of the disorder may help in developing
additional treatment approaches to control obsession, impulsivity
and compulsivity that are likely to share a common neurobiological
basis [44]. Even in these cases, however, preventing response in
the presence of relevant stimuli may be important.
Self-Regulation and OCD
One of the main problems in OCD seems to be related to an
inhibitory mechanism reected in the difculty patients have in
stopping the behavior or thoughts. Attention helps the individual
to select stimuli that are most relevant and disregard or ignore
irrelevant informational sources [46]. In set shifting and exibility
in problem solving, OCD patients show lower performance than
controls [47-49]. This decit is supported by a meta-analysis of
110 previous studies of OCD patients showing a broad impairment
in executive functions [50]. The brain system most likely central
to these decits is the executive attention network that includes the
anterior cingulate (ACC) and underlying striatum [14].
Compulsivity in OCD has also been associated with addiction.
In a review article, Figee et al. demonstrated that compulsivity
is not only a central feature of OCD but it is also a key element
in addiction [49]. The term addiction in this context includes
behavioral addiction along with non-drug - related disorders that
have compulsivity as their common feature, such as pathological
gambling, and compulsive eating or buying.“Receptor-binding
studies indicate hyperactivity of the striatal dopaminergic system
in OCD, with decreased striatal availability of dopamine D1
receptors and D2-like receptors in [OCD] patients versus controls,
which is also found in individuals with substance-use disorders
and in some studies with obese patients”[49]. Compulsivity in
addictions and OCD may both be related to negative reinforcement.
Negative reinforcement in this case may involve avoidance or
relief of many kinds of distress based on abnormalities in brain
reward and anxiety circuits. The main brain areas responsible
for this include bed nucleus stria terminals, amygdala, habenula
and medial prefrontal cortex. Moreover, compulsivity in OCD
and addictions entails cognitive and behavioral inexibility,
which may be rooted in a shared impairment of ACC and ventro
medial prefrontal top-down regulation, along with serotonergic
defects and excessive dopamine and glutamate signaling. Finally,
habitual responding regardless of its consequences is an aspect of
compulsivity that may be related to imbalances between ventral
and dorsal frontostriatal activity [49]. As in addictions the OCD
patient appears forced to carry out particular behaviors even when
resistance is desired. By examining recent efforts to understand the
neurobiology of addictive disorders we may gain some perspective
on the diagnosis and treatment of OCD.
One view of addictions is to regard them as involving a decit in
self- regulation [51]. The ability to voluntarily regulate behavior
including both emotion and memory retrieval has been thought
to involve the executive attention network, including the anterior
cingulate cortex [14, 51]. There is an anatomical distinction
between more dorsal cingulate areas involved in cognitive control
and more ventral areas related to emotion regulation [45, 51]. Brain
systems of executive attention have been found to exercise control
over both emotional and behavioral responses [14]. Abnormalities
of these areas are clearly involved in OCD as recognized in meta-
analyses of grey and white matter [52, 53].
It is known that craving for drugs often involves the limbic circuit,
including the anterior cingulate and ventral striate cortex [54].
One recent study recruited smokers and nonsmokers who were
interested in reducing stress. No mention was made of a desire
to quit or reduce smoking. Smoking status was one of many
variables assessed after recruitment. The study found that tobacco
consumption was reduced by 60% in those smokers assigned to
two weeks of meditation training, even among those not seeking
to reduce smoking [55]. There was no change in smoking among
those given relaxation training (the control).The mechanism for this
Int J Psychiatry 2017 Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 4 of 8
reduced smoking appears to involve less craving through a change
in ventral ACC activity and connectivity to the striatum. Before
training, smokers showed reduced activity in the ACC compared to
non-smokers. After training, ACC activity and connectivity to the
striatum was increased for those smokers assigned to meditation,
but not for smokers in the control condition. The smokers’ desire
to quit smoking was unrelated to the reduction found in smoking
behavior.
A meta-analysis of activation studies of OCD patients using
functional imaging concludes that there is a consensus that
a dysregulation of the frontostriatal circuit is involved in the
psychopathology of OCD [8. 9]. A meta-analysis of resting state
MRI data conrmed that compared to other anxiety disorders there
was a unique abnormality in frontal-striatal pathways in OCD [56].
As with addictions, OCD has been reported to show abnormalities
in functional connectivity between midline frontal areas,
including connections between the ACC and striatum. However,
unlike tobacco and some other addictions [57]. the abnormality
sometimes appears as hyper- rather than hypo-connectivity
[58]. This dramatic difference in connectivity and the absence
of anxiety in early stages of addiction suggests that OCD might
not be an addictive disorder despite the possibility of considering
compulsive thoughts as being addictions.
The Tian et al. study showed that functional connectivity, that is the
correlation among selected brain areas, is increased in patients with
OCD compared to controls [58]. This nding suggests stronger
connectivity between the ACC and striatum in OCD. However a
meta-analysis of studies using Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI)
to study abnormalities in this circuitry showed hypoconnectivity
related to the dorsal more cognitive areas of the cingulate and
hyperconnectivity to the more ventral emotional control areas
[59]. Moreover altered white matter abnormalities in both hypo
and hyper connectivity occurred more widely than just in the ACC
striatal connections [59].
A later DTI study of 231 OCD patients compared to controls
showed reduced Fractional Anisotropy (FA) thus indicating
weaker connectivity of the ACC [60]. This nding was extended
in a family based approach which had similar ndings for un
medicated OCD patients and their siblings compared to normal
controls [61]. Another DTI study in children [62]. found reduced
fractional anisotropy (FA), suggesting reduced connectivity in
some of the same regions in which the Tian et al. study found
stronger functional connectivity [58]. Overall, the ndings indicate
evidence for abnormal white matter in the cingulate to striate
connections, but studies differ on whether the connections are
stronger or weaker. It is certainly possible that some connections
are stronger in OCD than controls while in others they are weaker.
We discuss this possibility below.
While there remains uncertainty about the direction of the
connectivity abnormalities in OCD, the studies are consistent
with abnormal connectivity between the anterior cingulate and
striatum in OCD patients. Recent meta-analysis studies of OCD
patients have veried abnormalities in white and gray matter in the
dorsal and ventral ACC [52, 53]. These ndings are supported by
another meta-analysis showing that problems in ACC connectivity
with the striatum are central to the symptoms found in OCD [63].
The importance of the striatum and its connections has also been
supported by the success of some forms of deep brain stimulation
in treatments, which have generally targeted areas of the ventral
striatum in efforts to relieve the symptoms of OCD [64].
In monkeys the ACC involves multiple connections to other brain
areas including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex which has been
involved in the inhibition of motor and posterior areas in the go-
no-go task [65].
A recent paper has examined the response of OCD patients to
making an error. The study found that while patients showed a
normal response to error, they were less likely than controls to slow
their responses on the next trial. Thus in this study OCD patients
seem to have impaired ability to control response output [66]. One
paper has specied a specic cognitive role for the ACC in the
etiology of OCD [63]. According to this view dysfunctioning of
the ACC generates an inappropriate signal related to the expected
value of stimuli, which favors the compulsion in comparison with
long term goals. Over-valuation of stimuli related to the compulsion
leads to repetitive behaviors by giving priority to stimuli related to
the compulsion. This idea is compatible with an important role for
the ACC in OCD and provides a possible explanation of that role.
Direction of connectivity
Another approach to ACC function is based on the idea that some
pathways to and from the ACC are abnormal in connectivity.
For example, pathways carrying information about compulsive
events from the striatum (e.g. itching or dirty hands) could be
hyper connected, while others such as used for slowing responses
following an error could be hypoconnectivity (these pathways
could be involved in controlling motor response of scratching
or hand washing). We speculate based on the symptoms of OCD
that medial frontal pathways involved in self-regulation may be
hypoconnected, while those involved in frontal striatal loops are
hyperconnected.
Even though more research is clearly needed regarding the
direction of connectivity abnormalities in OCD found in various
pathways, recent neurobiological studies might point the way
toward methods of improving control of compulsive symptoms.
Oligodendrocytes are involved in increasing connectivity though
increased myelination. Thus activating oligodendrocytes as has
been done with motor learning in mice could perhaps be used
to increase connectivity and thus provide improved response
prevention in OCD treatment [67]. Recent studies in mice and
humans have shown improved connectivity following various
forms of learning, providing some support for this possibility [68].
Other glial cells including the astrocytes have been implicated in
remodeling connections through reduction of myelin [69]. Thus
biological ndings support the possibility of adjustments to both
increase underconnectivity and reduce overconnectivity.
Since medial frontal pathways involved in self-regulation may be
hypoconnected, while those involved in frontal striatal loops are
hyperconnected, it is possible that methods can be constructed
to improve both of these tendencies A preliminary report with
OCD patients suggests that transcortical magnetic stimulation
with a coil designed for deep stimulation and repeated 20 times
per second induced EEG changes in the anterior cingulate and
increased responses to error in the Stoop task [70]. If methods
Int J Psychiatry, 2017 Volume 2 | Issue 1| 5 of 8
could be found to carryout remodeling of myelin connections
in humans, they might prove useful in the treatment of OCD by
strengthening the mechanisms of self- regulation while reducing
repetitive activation. Approaching OCD through changes in
connectivity as suggested in this section, would not necessarily
be incompatible with the view that the disorder involves errors in
ACC computation of expected value [63].
Since the ACC to hippocampus pathway has been identied as
a possible route for control of retrieval from memory, increased
activity and connectivity of the ACC could result in improved
ability to control obsessive thoughts as well as behaviors [71]. One
caution about this approach, however, is that although theoretical
models and empirical studies suggest that OCD is caused by
functional and structural abnormalities in orbitofrontal and ACC
to striatal circuits, there is also evidence that other brain areas may
be involved [59,72].
Future directions for understanding the theoretical basis of
psychological treatment of OCD
As stated earlier, exposure therapy and response prevention
(ERP) involves a set of psychological treatment approaches and/
or techniques for improving anxiety-related disorders, including
OCD. The common core of these approaches and techniques is
asking patients to confront their obsessive or fearful thoughts while
controlling their response to the situation. Exposure therapy alone
or in combination with response prevention has been identied as
the rst and most effective psychotherapy for some OCD patients
and there is no doubt that Foa and her group has had enormous
effect on the evaluation, treatment and investigation of OCD in the
past 30 years [73]. The main problem with this set of approaches
and techniques is its mechanisms of action on the one hand and
the theory underlying this treatment approach on the other [13,74,
75]. By incorporating the neurobiological approaches this paper
has attempted to both specify mechanisms and move toward an
improved theory of treatment.
In all explanations the most characteristic feature of habituation
has been identied as repetition of stimulation. But in exposure
therapy in general and exposure therapy applied in OCD in
particular, something new happens that goes beyond mere
repetition or adaptation. It is the patient’s deliberate and systematic
confrontation with a supposedly threatening situation. That is, the
patient is actively involved in the process of exposure therapy.
First, the person wishes to be relieved of OCD symptoms and
seeks help. Second, the patient trusts the therapist and generates
positive expectations about the effects of therapy. Third, in CBT
approaches, the patient is presented with a rationale for the ERP
treatment and given assurances that the therapy will work.
These cognitive features and their emotional consequences have
in part been addressed by an improved understanding of the
mechanisms of attentional control of emotions, thoughts and
behavior. Improved self-regulation may thus be a key to response
prevention during exposure as well as between therapeutic
sessions. The use of meditation training has proven effective in the
control of some addictions and may be one of several mechanisms
to foster improved response prevention.
We are still in need of a fully satisfactory theoretical and explanatory
basis of exposure therapy. Although the new classication
of an OCD spectrum may cause some practical problems for
clinicians, it can also foster research on neurobiological and
cognitive commonalities of different impulsive and compulsive
disorders, providing us with new ways of looking at the nature
and psychopathology of OCD. In this review we have found clear
evidence of abnormal pathways involved in the disorder. These
include the ACC and its connections to the striatum. Moreover,
at least some results allow speculation concerning methods of
improving self-regulation by altering connectivity. This work
may help clinicians nd new insights about treatment. Most
importantly, moving in this direction of connecting cognition and
neuroscience with exposure therapy may throw light on the whole
area of psychopathology and phenomenology of disorders that
involve difculties in self-regulation [76].
This work was begun by HG and evolved into the current paper
while he served as a visitor at the University of Oregon Institute
for Cognitive and Decision Sciences. We express our thanks to
Prof. Dennis Galvan, Vice Provost for International affairs, and
to Peter DeFazio, and Ron Wyden for their help in arranging this
visit.
References
1. Rasmussen SA, Eisen JL (1992) The epidemiology and clinical
features of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics
of North America 15: 743-758.
2. World Health Organization (1996) The global burden of
disease. Geneva, WHO.
3. Weissman MM, Bland RC, Canino GJ, Greenwald S, Hwu HG,
et al. (1994) The cross national epidemiology of obsessive-
compulsive disorder. The cross National Collaborative Group.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 55: 5-10.
4. Karno M, Golding JM, Sorenson SB, Burnam A (1988). The
epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in ve US
communities. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45: 1094-1099.
5. Rasmussen SA, Eisen JL (1989) Clinical features and phenomenology of
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Annals 19: 67-73.
6. Rasmussen SA, Eisen JL (1998) The epidemiology and
clinical features of Obsessive-compulsive disorder. 15:
743-758. In MA Jenike, L Baer, and WE Minicheillo, (eds),
Obsessive-compulsive disorders- Practical management. St.
Louis, Mosby. 12-43.
7. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5 ed., Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
8. Skapinakis P, Caldwell D, Hollingworth W, Bryden P, Fineberg
N, et al. (2016) Pharmacological and interventions for
management of obsessive-compulsive disorders: A systematic
review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry15: 1-10.
9. Skapinakis P, Caldwell D, Hollingworth W, Bryden P,
Fineberg N, et al. (2016) A systematic review of the clinical
effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of pharmacological and
psychological interventions for the management of obsessive-
compulsive disorder in children/adolescents and adults.
Health Techno l Assess 20: 1-392.
10. Foa EB, Liebowitz MR, Kozak MJ, Davies S, Campeas R,
et al. (2005) Treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder
by exposure and ritual prevention, clomipramine, and their
combination: A randomized placebo controlled trial. American
Journal of Psychiatry 162: 151-161.
11. Abramowitz JS, Taylor S, McKay D (2005). Potentials and
limitations of cognitive treatment for obsessive-compulsive
Int J Psychiatry, 2017 Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 6 of 8
disorder. Cognitive Behavior Therapy 34: 140-147.
12. Abramowitz JS (2006) The psychological treatment of
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Can J Psychiatry 51: 407-416.
13. Abramowitz JS (2013) The practice of exposure therapy:
relevance of cognitive- behavioral theory and extinction
theory. Behavior Therapy 44: 548-558.
14. Petersen SE, Posner MI (2012). The Attention system of the
human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience
35: 73-89.
15. Meyer V (1966) Modication of expectations in cases with
obsessional rituals. Behavior Research and Therapy 4: 273-80.
16. Meyer V, Levy R, Schnurer A (1974) A behavioral treatment
of obsessive-compulsive disorders. In H. R. Beech (ed.),
Obsessional states. London: Methuen.
17. Marks IM (1976) The current status of behavioral
psychotherapy: Theory and practice. The American Journal of
Psychiatry 133: 253-261.
18. Wolpe J (1973) The practice of behavior therapy. New York:
Pergamon Press.
19. Rachman S, Hodgson R, Marks IM (1971) The treatment of
chronic obsessive compulsive neurosis. Behavior Research
and Therapy 9: 237-247.
20. Lang PJ (1977) Imagery in therapy: An information processing
analysis of fear. Behavior Therapy 8: 862-886.
21. Lang PJ (1985) The cognitive psychophysiology of emotion: Fear
and anxiety. In A H Turner, and J Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the
anxiety disorders. Hillsdale, NJ. : Lawrence Erlbaum 131-170.
22. Rachman S (1980) Emotional processing. Behavior Research
and Therapy 18: 51-60.
23. Foa EB, McNally RJ (1996) Mechanisms of change in
exposure therapy. In R.M. Rapee(Ed.), Current controversies
in the anxiety disorders. New York: Guilford 329-343.
24. Foa EB, Huppert JD, Cahill SP (2006) Emotional processing
theory-An update. In B.O. Rothbaum (Ed), Pathological
anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and treatment. New
York: Guilford Press 3-24.
25. Watts FN (1979) Habituation model of systematic
desensitization. P. Psychologica Bulletin 86: 627-637.
26. Grissom N, Bhatnagar S (2009) Habituation to repeated stress:
get used to it. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem 92: 215-224.
27. Foa EB, Kozak MJ (1986) Emotional processing of fear:
Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin
99: 20-35.
28. Marks IM, Dar E (2000) Fear reduction by psychotherapies-
Recent ndings and future directions. The British Journal of
Psychiatry 176: 507-511.
29. Baker A, Mystkowski J, Culver N, Yi R, Mortazavi A, et al.
(2010) Does habituation matter? Emotional processing theory
and exposure therapy for acrophobia. Behavior Research and
Therapy 48: 1139-1143.
30. Thompson RF (2009) Habituation: a history. Neurobiol.
Learn. Mem 92: 127-134.
31. Thompson RF, Spencer WA (1966) Habituation: a model
phenomenon for study of neuronal substrat of behavior.
Psychol Rev 73: 16-43.
32. Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF,
et al. (2009) Habituation revisited: an updated and revised
description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation.
Neurobiology. Learn. Mem 92: 135-138.
33. Foa EB, McLean CP (2016) The efcacy of exposure therapy for
anxiety- related disorders and its underlying mechanisms: The case of
OCD and PTSD. Ann Rev of Clinical Psychology 12: 1-28.
34. Craske OG, KIrcanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J,
Chowdhury N, et al. (2008) Optimizing inhibitory learning during
exposure therapy. Behavior Research and Therapy 46: 5-27.
35. Myers KM, Davis M (2007) Mechanisms of fear extinction.
Mol. Psychiatry 12: 120-150.
36. Phelps EA, Mauricio R. Delgado MR, Nearing KI, Ledoux JE,
(2004) Extinction Learning in Humans: Role of the Amygdala
and vm PFC. Neuron 43: 897-906.
37. Hoffman SG (2008) Cognitive processes during fear
acquisition and extinction in animals and humans-Implications
for exposure therapy of anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology
Review 28: 199-210.
38. Schiller D, Kanen JW, LeDoux JE, Monls MH, Phelps EA
(2013) Extinction during reconsolidation of threat memory
diminishes prefrontal cortex involvement Proceedings of the
US National Academy of Science 110: 20040-20045.
39. Correia SS, McGrath AG, Lee A, Graybiel AM, Gossens KA
(2016) Amygdala- ventral striatum circuit activation decreases
long-term fear. E Life 5: 12669 .
40. Klkucken T, Kruse O, Schweckendiek J, Kuepper Y, Mueller
EM et al. (2016) No evidence for blocking the return of fear
by disuprting reconsiolidation prior to extinction learning.
Cortex 79: 112-122.
41. Abramowitz JS, Jacoby RJ (2014) The use and misuse of
exposure therapy for obsessive-compulsive and related
disorders. Current Psychiatry Review 10: 277-283.
42. Storch EA, Abramowitz JS, Goodman WK (2008) Where
does obsessive-compulsive disorder belong in DSM-V?
Depression and Anxiety 4: 336-347.
43. Hollander E, Evers M (2004) Review of obsessive-compulsive
spectrum disorders: What do we know ? Where are we go?
Clinical Neuropsychiatry 1: 32-51.
44. Graybiel AM, Rauch SL (2000) Toward a neurobiology review
of obsessive- compulsive disorder. Neuron 28: 343-347.
45. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional
inuences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 4: 215-222.
46. Muller J, Roberts J (2005) Memory and attention in obsessive–
compulsive disorder: A review. Anxiety Disorders 19: 1-28.
47. Evans D, Lewis MD, Iobst E (2004) The Role of the orbito-
frontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like
behaviors and obsessive- compulsive disorder. Brain and
Cognition 55: 220-234.
48. Ghassemzadeh H, Mojtabai R, Karamghadiri N, Noroozian
M, Shari V, et al. (2012) Neuropsychological and
neurological decits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: The
role of comorbid depression,International Journal of Clinical
Medicine 3: 200-210.
49. Figee M, Pattij T, Willuhn I, Luigjes J, van den Brink W, et al.
(2016) Compulsivity in obsessive- compulsive disorder and
addictions. European Neuro psycho pharmacology 26: 856-868.
50. Snyder H, Kaiser R, Warren S. Heller W. (2015) Obsessive-
Compulsive Disorder Is Associated With Broad Impairments in
Executive Function: A Meta-Analysis. Clinical Psychological
Science. Clinical Psychological Science; 3: 301–330.
51. Tang YY, Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Volkkow ND (2015)
Circuitry of self control and its role in reducing addiction.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19: 439-445.
52. Eng GK, Sim K, Chen A (2015) Meta-analytic investigations
of structural grey matter, executive domain-related functional
Volume 2 | Issue 1 | 7 of 8Int J Psychiatry, 2017
activations, and white matter diffusivity in obsessive
compulsive disorder: An integrative review. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews 52: 233-257.
53. Magioncalda P, Martino M, Ely BA, Inglese M, Stern ER
(2016) Micro structural white-matter abnormalities and
their relationship with cognitive dysfunction in obsessive-
compulsive disorder. Brain and Behavior 6: e00442.
54. Jasinska AJ, Stein EA, Kaiser J, Naumer MJ, Yalachkov Y
(2014) Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues
in addiction: a survey of human neuroimaging studies.
NeurosciBiobehavior 38: 1-16.
55. Tang YY, Tang R, Posner MI (2013) Brief meditation training
induces smoking reduction. Proceedings of the US National
Academy 110: 13971-13975.
56. Petereson A, Thome J, Frewen P, Lanius RA (2014) Resting-
state neuroimaging studies: a new way of identifying
differences and similarities among the anxiety disorders.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 59: 294-300.
57. Hong LE, Gu H, YangY, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, et al. (2009)
Association of nicotine addiction and nicotine’s actions
with separate cingulate cortex functional circuits. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 66: 431- 441.
58. Tian L, Meng C, Jiang Y, Tang Q, Wang S, et al. (2016)
Abnormal functional connectivity of brain network hubs
associated with symptom severity in treatment-naive patients
with obsessive–compulsive disorder: A resting-state functional
MRI study. Progress in Neuro- Psychopharmacology &
Biological Psychiatry 66: 104-111.
59. Piras F, Piras F, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G (2013) Brain
circuitries of obsessive compulsive disorder: A systematic
review and meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37: 2856-2877.
60. Radua J, Grau M, van den Heuvel OA, Thiebuatmde Schotten
M, Stein DJ, et al. (2014) Multimodal voxel-based meta-analysis
of white matter abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 1547-1557.
61. Fan S, van den Heuvel OA, Cath DC, van deer Werf Y, de Wit
S, et al. (2015) Mild white matter changes in un-medicated
obsessive-compulsive disorder patients and their unaffected
siblings. Human Fronties 9: 495.
62. Rosso IM, Olson EA, Britton JC, Stewart SE, Papadimitriou
G, et al. (2014) Brain white matter integrity and association
with age at onset in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders 4: 1-10.
63. McGovern RAk, Sheth SA (2016) Role of the dorsal anterior
cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder: converging
evidence from cognitive neuroscience and psychiatric
neurosurgery Journal of Neurosurgery, Ahead of Print : 1-16.
64. Alonso P, Cuadras D, Gabriëls L, Denys D, Goodman W, et
al (2015) Deep Brain Stimulation for Obsessive- Compulsive
Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Treatment Outcome and
Predictors of response 10: e0133591.
65. Medalla M, Barbas H (2010) Anterior cingulate synapses in
prefrontal areas 10 and 46suggest differential inuence in
cognitive control. Journal of Neuroscience 30: 16068-16081.
66. Agram Y, Greenberg JL, Isom M, Falkenstein MJ, Jenike E,
et al. (2014) Aberrant error processing in relation to symptom
severity in obsessive – compulsive disorder: A multimodal
neuroimaging study. 17: 141-151.
67. McKenzie IA, Ohayon D, Li H, De Faria, JP Emery B, et al.
(2014) Motor skill learning requires active central myelination.
Science 346: 318-322.
68. Wang S, Young KM (2014) White matter plasticity in
adulthood. Neuroscience 276: 148-160.
69. Fields RD (2016) Oligodendrocyte and myelin plasticity in
information processing and learning. Paper presented at the
Virtual Neuroscience Conference on White Matter.
70. Harrison P (2016) High-frequency, deep TMS offers novel
approach to OCD Medscape.
71. Anderson MC, Bunce JG, Barbas H (2016) Prefrontal–
hippocampal pathways underlying inhibitory control
overmemory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 134:
145-161
72. Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian
BJ, et al. (2008) Integrating evidence from neuro imaging and
neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder:
The orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience and
Bio behavioral Reviews 32: 525-549.
73. Kozak MJ, Coles ME (2005) Treatment for OCD: Unleashing
the power of exposure. In JS. Abramowitz and AC.Houts,
Concepts and controversies in obsessive-compulsive
disorder.293-304.
74. McNally RJ (2007) Mechanisms of exposure therapy: How
neuroscience can improve psychological treatments for
anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review 27: 750-759.
75. MacLeod C, Mathews A (2012) Cognitive bias modication
approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 8: 189-
217.
76. Wolpe J (1973) The practice of behavior therapy. New York:
Pergamon Press.
Copyright: ©2017 Michael I.Posner, et al. is is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License,
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Volume 2 | Issue1 | 8 of 8
Int J Psychiatry, 2017
... OCD is associated with a decrease in the quality of life in social relations, family, occupation, and health (Stein et al., 2019). The effects of pharmacotherapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) in the treatment of OCD have been shown in different studies (Jónsson et al., 2015;Olatunji et al., 2013;Rosa-Alcázar et al., 2008;Şafak et al., 2014;Türkçapar & Şafak, 2012) Exposure and response prevention (ERP) and cognitive restructuring (CR), which are included in CBT, are most effective treatment methods for OCD (Farmer & Chapman, 2016;Ghassemzadeh et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive-behavioral group therapy (CBGT) effectively treats obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Therefore, we aimed to evaluate the variables that predict the success of CBGT in OCD treatment. The variables can predict treatment success, obtained by a demographic data form, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV for Axis-I Disorders, Beck Anxiety Inventory, Thought Action Fusion Scale, General Assessment of Functioning Scale, and White Bear Suppression Inventory. Data of 56 participants who attended fourteen sessions of CBGT were evaluated. Having reactive type obsessions, high initial functionality, low thought action fusion (TAF), and high suppression levels predict the success of CBGT with medium to large effect sizes (p < .001, p < .001, p = .037, p = .024). According to results, obsession type, initial functionality, TAF, and suppression can be worthy of personalizing treatment before CBGT. Specific interventions can be developed for these variables.
... In this paper, we attempt to tie the brain networks revealed by imaging studies to underlying cells and their connections found in various animal models. We have recently reviewed evidence for the role of attention in obsessive compulsive disorder [6]. Repetitive concentration of attention upon a potential failure appears a key feature of this disorder. ...
Article
Full-text available
The attention networks of the human brain have been under intensive study for more than twenty years and deficits of attention accompany many neurological and psychiatric conditions. There is more dispute about the centrality of attention deficits to these conditions. It appears to be time to study whether reducing deficits of attention alleviate the neurological or psychiatric disorder as a whole. In this paper we review human and animal research indicating the possibility of improving the function of brain networks underlying attention and their potential clinical role. © 2019, Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine Inc. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Advances in the study of brain networks can be applied to our understanding of anxiety disorders (eg, generalized anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and posttraumatic stress disorders) to enable us to create targeted treatments. These disorders have in common an inability to control thoughts, emotions, and behaviors related to a perceived threat. Here we review animal and human imaging studies that have revealed separate brain networks related to various negative emotions. Research has supported the idea that brain networks of attention serve to control emotion networks as well as the thoughts and behaviors related to them. We discuss how attention networks can modulate both positive and negative affect. Disorders arise from both abnormal activation of negative affect and a lack of attentional control. Training attention has been one way to foster improved attentional control. We review attention training studies as well as efforts to generally improve attention networks through stimulation in self-regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Several interventions are available for management of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults, but few studies have compared their relative efficacy in a single analysis. We aimed to simultaneously compare all available treatments using both direct and indirect data. Methods: In this systematic review and network meta-analysis, we searched the two controlled trials registers maintained by the Cochrane Collaboration Common Mental Disorders group for trials published up to Feb 16, 2016. We selected randomised controlled trials in which an active psychotherapeutic or pharmacological intervention had been used in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. We allowed all comorbidities except for schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. We excluded studies that focused exclusively on treatment-resistant patient populations defined within the same study. We extracted data from published reports. The primary outcome was symptom severity as measured by the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. We report mean differences with 95% credible intervals compared with placebo. This study is registered with PROSPERO, number CRD42012002441. Findings: We identified 1480 articles in our search and included 53 articles (54 trials; 6652 participants) in the network meta-analysis. Behavioural therapy (mean difference -14·48 [95% credible interval -18·61 to -10·23]; 11 trials and 287 patients), cognitive therapy (-13·36 [-18·40 to -8·21]; six trials and 172 patients), behavioural therapy and clomipramine (-12·97 [-19·18 to -6·74]; one trial and 31 patients), cognitive behavioural therapy and fluvoxamine (-7·50 [-13·89 to -1·17]; one trial and six patients), cognitive behavioural therapy (-5·37 [-9·10 to -1·63]; nine trials and 231 patients), clomipramine (-4·72 [-6·85 to -2·60]; 13 trials and 831 patients), and all SSRIs (class effect -3·49 [95% credible interval -5·12 to -1·81]; 37 trials and 3158 patients) had greater effects than did drug placebo. Clomipramine was not better than were SSRIs (-1·23 [-3·41 to 0·94]). Psychotherapeutic interventions had a greater effect than did medications, but a serious limitation was that most psychotherapeutic trials included patients who were taking stable doses of antidepressants (12 [80%] of the 15 psychotherapy trials explicitly allowed antidepressants). Interpretation: A range of interventions is effective in the management of obsessive-compulsive disorder, but considerable uncertainty and limitations exist regarding their relative efficacy. Taking all the evidence into account, the combination of psychotherapeutic and psychopharmacological interventions is likely to be more effective than are psychotherapeutic interventions alone, at least in severe obsessive-compulsive disorder. Funding: National Institute for Health Research.
Article
Full-text available
Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) is a relatively common and disabling condition. Objectives To determine the clinical effectiveness, acceptability and cost-effectiveness of pharmacological and psychological interventions for the treatment of OCD in children, adolescents and adults. Data sources We searched the Cochrane Collaboration Depression, Anxiety and Neurosis Trials Registers, which includes trials from routine searches of all the major databases. Searches were conducted from inception to 31 December 2014. Review methods We undertook a systematic review and network meta-analysis (NMA) of the clinical effectiveness and acceptability of available treatments. Outcomes for effectiveness included mean differences in the total scores of the Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale or its children’s version and total dropouts for acceptability. For the cost-effectiveness analysis, we developed a probabilistic model informed by the results of the NMA. All analyses were performed using OpenBUGS version 3.2.3 (members of OpenBUGS Project Management Group; see www.openbugs.net ). Results We included 86 randomised controlled trials (RCTs) in our systematic review. In the NMA we included 71 RCTs (54 in adults and 17 in children and adolescents) for effectiveness and 71 for acceptability (53 in adults and 18 in children and adolescents), comprising 7643 and 7942 randomised patients available for analysis, respectively. In general, the studies were of medium quality. The results of the NMA showed that in adults all selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and clomipramine had greater effects than drug placebo. There were no differences between SSRIs, and a trend for clomipramine to be more effective did not reach statistical significance. All active psychological therapies had greater effects than drug placebo. Behavioural therapy (BT) and cognitive therapy (CT) had greater effects than psychological placebo, but cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) did not. BT and CT, but not CBT, had greater effects than medications, but there are considerable uncertainty and methodological limitations that should be taken into account. In children and adolescents, CBT and BT had greater effects than drug placebo, but differences compared with psychological placebo did not reach statistical significance. SSRIs as a class showed a trend for superiority over drug placebo, but the difference did not reach statistical significance. However, the superiority of some individual drugs (fluoxetine, sertraline) was marginally statistically significant. Regarding acceptability, all interventions except clomipramine had good tolerability. In adults, CT and BT had the highest probability of being most cost-effective at conventional National Institute for Health and Care Excellence thresholds. In children and adolescents, CBT or CBT combined with a SSRI were more likely to be cost-effective. The results are uncertain and sensitive to assumptions about treatment effect and the exclusion of trials at high risk of bias. Limitations The majority of psychological trials included patients who were taking medications. There were few studies in children and adolescents. Conclusions In adults, psychological interventions, clomipramine, SSRIs or combinations of these are all effective, whereas in children and adolescents, psychological interventions, either as monotherapy or combined with specific SSRIs, were more likely to be effective. Future RCTs should improve their design, in particular for psychotherapy or combined interventions. Study registration The study is registered as PROSPERO CRD42012002441. Funding details The National Institute for Health Research Health Technology Assessment programme.
Article
Full-text available
OBJECTIVE Advances in understanding the neurobiological basis of psychiatric disorders will improve the ability to refine neuromodulatory procedures for treatment-refractory patients. One of the core dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a deficit in cognitive control, especially involving the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). The authors' aim was to derive a neurobiological understanding of the successful treatment of refractory OCD with psychiatric neurosurgical procedures targeting the dACC. METHODS First, the authors systematically conducted a review of the literature on the role of the dACC in OCD by using the search terms “obsessive compulsive disorder” and “anterior cingulate.” The neuroscience literature on cognitive control mechanisms in the dACC was then combined with the literature on psychiatric neurosurgical procedures targeting the dACC for the treatment of refractory OCD. RESULTS The authors reviewed 89 studies covering topics that included structural and functional neuroimaging and electrophysiology. The majority of resting-state functional neuroimaging studies demonstrated dACC hyperactivity in patients with OCD relative to that in controls, while task-based studies were more variable. Electrophysiological studies showed altered dACC-related biomarkers of cognitive control, such as error-related negativity in OCD patients. These studies were combined with the cognitive control neurophysiology literature, including the recently elaborated expected value of control theory of dACC function. The authors suggest that a central feature of OCD pathophysiology involves the generation of mis-specified cognitive control signals by the dACC, and they elaborate on this theory and provide suggestions for further study. CONCLUSIONS Although abnormalities in brain structure and function in OCD are distributed across a wide network, the dACC plays a central role. The authors propose a theory of cognitive control dysfunction in OCD that attempts to explain the therapeutic efficacy of dACC neuromodulation. This theoretical framework should help to guide further research into targeted treatments of OCD and other disorders of cognitive control.
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies have detected subtle microstructural abnormalities of white matter (WM) in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). However, findings have been inconsistent, and it is unclear whether WM abnormalities are related to cognitive processes. The aim of this study was to explore the relationship of WM alterations with cognitive variables in OCD in order to investigate the structural correlates of behaviorally relevant features of the disorder. Methods We compared DTI-derived fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), and radial diffusivity (RD) measures between OCD patients (n=16) and healthy controls (n=18) using a whole-brain tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) approach. We also explored the correlations of WM alterations with clinical and cognitive variables. ResultsPatients with OCD demonstrated increases in MD in the bilateral posterior corona radiata; left anterior corona radiata; bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus; genu, body, and splenium of the corpus callosum; and left posterior limb of the internal capsule. An increase in RD values was also found in some of the same tracts (right posterior corona radiata, right superior longitudinal fasciculus, left anterior corona radiata, and corpus callosum). Furthermore, increased MD value in the internal capsule was correlated with the percentage of errors made during a target detection task, which was greater in the OCD group overall. Conclusions These findings indicate that OCD patients show greater diffusivity in several white-matter regions. The correlation between cognitive performance and diffusivity in the internal capsule suggests that microstructural WM alternations may have functional consequences for the disorder.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a common neuropsychiatric disorder with moderate genetic influences and white matter abnormalities in frontal-striatal and limbic regions. Inconsistencies in reported white matter results from diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies can be explained, at least partly, by medication use and between-group differences in disease profile and stage. We used a family design aiming to establish whether white matter abnormalities, if present in un-medicated OCD patients, also exist in their unaffected siblings. Method: Forty-four OCD patients, un-medicated for at least the past 4 weeks, 15 of their unaffected siblings, and 37 healthy controls (HC) underwent DTI using a 3-Tesla MRI-scanner. Data analysis was done using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS). Fractional anisotropy (FA), axial diffusivity (AD), radial diffusivity (RD), and mean diffusivity (MD) values were compared within seven skeletonised regions of interest (ROIs), i.e., corpus callosum, bilateral cingulum bundle, bilateral inferior longitudinal fasciculus/frontal-occipital fasciculus (ILF/FOF) and bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF). Results: Un-medicated OCD patients, compared with HC, had significantly lower FA in the left cingulum bundle. FA was trend-significantly lower in all other ROIs, except for the corpus callosum. Significant three-group differences in FA (and in RD at trend-significant level) were observed in the left cingulum bundle, with the unaffected siblings representing an intermediate group between OCD patients and HC. Conclusions: OCD patients showed lower FA in the left cingulum bundle, partly driven by trend-significantly higher values in RD. Since the unaffected siblings were found to be an intermediate group between OCD patients and HC, this white matter alteration may be considered an endophenotype for OCD.
Article
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is a part of the brain's limbic system. Classically, this region has been related to affect, on the basis of lesion studies in humans and in animals. In the late 1980s, neuroimaging research indicated that ACC was active in many studies of cognition. The findings from EEG studies of a focal area of negativity in scalp electrodes following an error response led to the idea that ACC might be the brain's error detection and correction device. In this article, these various findings are reviewed in relation to the idea that ACC is a part of a circuit involved in a form of attention that serves to regulate both cognitive and emotional processing. Neuroimaging studies showing that separate areas of ACC are involved in cognition and emotion are discussed and related to results showing that the error negativity is influenced by affect and motivation. In addition, the development of the emotional and cognitive roles of ACC are discussed, and how the success of this regulation in controlling responses might be correlated with cingulate size. Finally, some theories are considered about how the different subdivisions of ACC might interact with other cortical structures as a part of the circuits involved in the regulation of mental and emotional activity.
Article
Compulsive behaviors are driven by repetitive urges and typically involve the experience of limited voluntary control over these urges, a diminished ability to delay or inhibit these behaviors, and a tendency to perform repetitive acts in a habitual or stereotyped manner. Compulsivity is not only a central characteristic of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) but is also crucial to addiction. Based on this analogy, OCD has been proposed to be part of the concept of behavioral addiction along with other non-drug-related disorders that share compulsivity, such as pathological gambling, skin-picking, trichotillomania and compulsive eating. In this review, we investigate the neurobiological overlap between compulsivity in substance-use disorders, OCD and behavioral addictions as a validation for the construct of compulsivity that could be adopted in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). The reviewed data suggest that compulsivity in OCD and addictions is related to impaired reward and punishment processing with attenuated dopamine release in the ventral striatum, negative reinforcement in limbic systems, cognitive and behavioral inflexibility with diminished serotonergic prefrontal control, and habitual responding with imbalances between ventral and dorsal frontostriatal recruitment. Frontostriatal abnormalities of compulsivity are promising targets for neuromodulation and other interventions for OCD and addictions. We conclude that compulsivity encompasses many of the RDoC constructs in a trans-diagnostic fashion with a common brain circuit dysfunction that can help identifying appropriate prevention and treatment targets.
Article
Objective: Abnormal brain networks have been observed in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). However, detailed network hub and connectivity changes remained unclear in treatment-naive patients with OCD. Here, we sought to determine whether patients show hub-related connectivity changes in their whole-brain functional networks. Methods: We used resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data and voxel-based graph-theoretic analysis to investigate functional connectivity strength and hubs of whole-brain networks in 29 treatment-naive patients with OCD and 29 age- and gender-matched healthy controls. Correlation analysis was applied for potential associations with OCD symptom severity. Results: OCD selectively targeted brain regions of higher functional connectivity strength than the average including brain network hubs, mainly distributed in the cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) circuits and additionally parietal, occipital, temporal and cerebellar regions. Moreover, affected functional connectivity strength in the cerebellum, the medial orbitofrontal cortex and superior occipital cortex was significantly associated with global OCD symptom severity. Conclusion: Our results provide the evidence about OCD-related brain network hub changes, not only in the CSTC circuits but more distributed in whole brain networks. Data suggest that whole brain network hub analysis is useful for understanding the pathophysiology of OCD.
Article
The psychological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is one of the great success stories within the field of mental health. Within the span of about 20 years, the prognosis for individuals with OCD changed from poor to very good as a result of the development of cognitive-behavioral techniques. This chapter describes the procedures of exposure, response prevention, and cognitive therapy; their hypothesized mechanisms of action; and the results from treatment outcome research attesting to their efficacy and effectiveness. The chapter also addresses the efficacy of combining exposure therapy techniques with medications in the treatment of OCD.