ArticlePDF Available

Driver's vehicle horn use and its effects on other drivers and pedestrians: A case study in South Korea

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In previous studies, questionnaire surveys were used to investigate the current circumstances of vehicle horn use and its effects on drivers and pedestrians in Japan. Several cities in other countries face more serious noise problems related to the use of car and motorbike horns. In urban areas of South Korea, frequent vehicle horn use on roads was found through onsite inspections. Measurements of noise at crossroads with heavy traffic revealed frequent horn use with high sound pressure level. In such areas, it is necessary to clarify the current circumstances of vehicle horn use, including its effects. Therefore, a survey on such use, similar to the previous work, was carried out. The survey included questions on the latest or last-remembered case of horn use in various situations in which the respondent was a driver or pedestrian. It was found that many pedestrians had experiences of being honked at by a single honk, two short honks and a long honk. Such honking mostly aroused negative psychological reactions such as feeling startled, a sensation of noisiness and feeling irritated. There were no significant relationships between questionnaire items regarding the driver's own horn use, suggesting there is no particular manner of such use. Relationships between driver awareness of their own horn use and its mode suggested that drivers who did not usually honk had feelings of reluctance to use the horn, and that they briefly honked out of necessity.
Content may be subject to copyright.
J-STAGE Advance Publication date: 13 April, 2017
Paper No.16-00433
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Vol.4, No.2, 2017Bulletin of the JSME
Mechanical Engineering Journal
Driver’s vehicle horn use and its effects on other drivers and
pedestrians: A case study in South Korea
Masayuki TAKADA*, Satoshi SUZUKI**, Ki-Hong KIM***, Jong-Hyun SHIN****
and Shin-ichiro IWAMIYA*
* Faculty of Design, Kyushu University
4-9-1 Shiobaru, Minami-ku, Fukuoka 815-8540, Japan
E-mail: takada@design.kyushu-u.ac.jp
** Graduate School of Design, Kyushu University
4-9-1 Shiobaru, Minami-ku, Fukuoka 815-8540, Japan
*** Faculty of Media and Information Resources, Surugadai University
698 Azu, Hanno, Saitama 357-0046, Japan
**** Soundscape co., Ltd.
2B2, 71-184, Sanggye-Dong, Nowon-Ku, Seoul, South Korea 139200
Abstract
In previous studies, questionnaire surveys were used to investigate the current circumstances of vehicle horn
use and its effects on drivers and pedestrians in Japan. Several cities in other countries face more serious noise
problems related to the use of car and motorbike horns. In urban areas of South Korea, frequent vehicle horn
use on roads was found through onsite inspections. Measurements of noise at crossroads with heavy traffic
revealed frequent horn use with high sound pressure level. In such areas, it is necessary to clarify the current
circumstances of vehicle horn use, including its effects. Therefore, a survey on such use, similar to the previous
work, was carried out. The survey included questions on the latest or last-remembered case of horn use in
various situations in which the respondent was a driver or pedestrian. It was found that many pedestrians had
experiences of being honked at by a single honk, two short honks and a long honk. Such honking mostly
aroused negative psychological reactions such as feeling startled, a sensation of noisiness and feeling irritated.
There were no significant relationships between questionnaire items regarding the driver’s own horn use,
suggesting there is no particular manner of such use. Relationships between driver awareness of their own horn
use and its mode suggested that drivers who did not usually honk had feelings of reluctance to use the horn,
and that they briefly honked out of necessity.
Key words : Vehicle horn, Intent, Timing pattern, Driver, Pedestrian, Driver awareness, Psychological reaction
1. Introduction
The horn was originally installed in vehicles for warning others of danger and avoiding dangerous situations during
driving. In Japan, drivers use their vehicle horn as a signal in various situations such as when passing each other,
although the Japanese Road Traffic Law states that the horn should be used only in an emergency or in dangerous
locations (National Police Agency of Japan, 2015). The actual horn use may create a noise issue in neighborhoods.
Furthermore, drivers often honk at pedestrians and cyclists who are unaware of the driver’s car approaching from
behind. The A-weighted sound pressure level of a vehicle horn as measured in front of the vehicle is very high (93112
dB) (Ministry of Land, Infrastructure, Transport and Tourism, 2003), and potential adverse effects of horn use on
people outside the vehicle, such as pedestrians and cyclists, has therefore been a concern. To investigate the issue of
vehicle horn use described above, questionnaire surveys were used in previous studies in Japan (Takada et al., 2013,
2014). The questionnaire results revealed that vehicle horns were frequently used by drivers to communicate with other
drivers (e.g., for expressing gratitude or gaining another’s attention). The previous surveys also reported negative
1
Received: 15 July 2016; Revised: 21 March 2017; Accepted: 9 April 2017
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
psychological impacts of vehicle horn use, not only on drivers but also pedestrians.
Situations in several cities outside Japan are more serious. Many face noise problems related to the use of car and
motorbike horns (Onuu, 2000; Ali and Tamura, 2002; Phan et al., 2010; Roy et al., 2012). In considering
countermeasures for the adverse effects of horn use in such areas, it is necessary to investigate the current state of such
use. It is assumed that a questionnaire survey similar to that used in earlier studies in Japan (Takada et al., 2013, 2014)
would be able to clarify that state in areas facing the noise problem.
For noise problems of vehicle horn use, design of the car horn sound is an effective countermeasure against adverse
effects. In other studies that have proposed methods to synthesize car horn sounds for recognition as genuine, quality
assessments were made for existing horn sounds, and acoustic properties that are important to listener recognition of
car horn sound were investigated (Lemaitre et al., 2007, 2009). The design of a dual-shell car horn with good sound
quality in the car compartment was also developed based on the sound quality assessment and physical optimization
using numerical methods (Kang et al., 2015). These research works mainly addressed the sound quality of vehicle
horns, and did not address their effects on people outside the vehicle. For hearing horn sounds both inside and outside
the vehicle, our previous psychoacoustic studies investigated effects of the acoustic properties of horn sound on
auditory impressions and interpretations of the reasons for horn use by drivers (Takada et al., 2010; Takayama et al.,
2012). These were done to address how negative reactions to horns can be reduced, and revealed that negative
impressions of vehicle horns could be ameliorated by modifying the acoustic properties of horn sound, such as their
temporal and spectral features.
The international regulation standard regarding audible warning devices of motor vehicles, which links with similar
standards and laws in various countries including Japan, states that the A-weighted sound pressure level of a horn
should be not less than 93 dB and not more than 112 dB, measured at a distance of 7 m in front of a vehicle (UN
Vehicle Regulations, 1972, 1984). In view of recent reductions of road traffic noise in various countries, a proposal for
reduction of the lower limit of the A-weighted sound pressure level (from 93 to 87 dB) in the current standard was
submitted and agreed upon in the Working Party on Noise of the World Forum for Harmonization of Vehicle
Regulations (Economic and Social Council of United Nations, 2014). In the near future, this proposal will be reflected
by similar standards concerning horn sound in various countries. However, the noise problem of horn use may still
exist, because the higher limit of A-weighted sound pressure level (112 dB) remains the same.
Any change to the sound of vehicle horns may not completely solve the noise problem related to their use as long
as the current horn system, in which sounds with high sound pressure level are emitted outside of vehicles, continues to
exist in society. To fundamentally solve the problem of vehicle horn use, it may be necessary to alter human traffic
behavior, e.g., unnecessary horn use. In traffic psychological research, horn use has been regarded as a behavior
reflecting driver aggression (Doob and Gross, 1968). In the present study, therefore, we also considered driver horn use
as a type of traffic behavior, and investigated traffic psychological factors affecting that use.
The purpose of the present research was to find various countermeasures to reduce the adverse impacts of vehicle
horn use on humans or environment in areas facing noise problems caused by that use. To achieve our objective,
relationships between the manner of driver horn use and various factors such as traffic conditions and driver intent, and
effects of horn use on listeners’ psychological reactions should be clarified. We therefore carried out questionnaire
surveys to investigate the above-mentioned relationships concerning vehicle horn use in urban areas of South Korea,
where frequent horn use on roads was found through onsite inspection. First, in a preliminary survey, road traffic noise
was measured at crossroads with heavy traffic to grasp the current state of horn use. Noise measurements indicated
frequent use at these sites. We thus used the questionnaire survey on vehicle horn use similar to that previously used, to
which questions from the perspective of traffic psychology were added.
2. Noise measurements
2.1 Procedure
As a preliminary survey to grasp the current state of vehicle horn use, noise measurements were conducted at
crossroads with heavy traffic in Seoul, South Korea. For reasons such as safety, convenience of noise measurement and
police permission, two crossroads near subway stations (Gangnam-gu office station and Seonjeongneung station at
Gangnam-gu, Seoul), where roads with three or four lanes in each direction cross, were chosen for noise measurement.
Measurements were made over 2.5 days in November 2013 at the crossroad near Gangnam-gu office station, and 5
2
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
days in October 2014 at the crossroad near Seonjeongneung station. Noise level was continuously measured for 11
hours (8:0019:00), although measurement on a weekend in November 2013 was done only for 6 hours (13:0019:00
on Sunday) because of the adverse weather. Data of noise level and road traffic noise including horn sounds were
recorded using a sound-level meter (RION NL52). A microphone was located 1.5 m above the ground at the road
edge. Times of vehicle horn use were also recorded during the noise measurement.
2.2 Results
Frequency and noise level values of horn sounds were obtained using data management software (RION
AS-60RT), in which noise levels are indicated by comparing with recorded horn sounds correspondent in time.
Figure 1 shows the frequency of vehicle horn use in each time interval on each day. Median frequencies on each
day aside from one weekday were between 49 and 93. The frequency on one weekday (Tuesday) in November 2013
was higher than those on the other days. There were on average more than 100 horn uses per hour on that day (the
median frequency was 131). In South Korea, drivers are permitted to turn right at a crossroad even if the traffic light
controlling traffic flow straight ahead is red. There were thus frequent cases in which a driver wanting to turn right at
the crossroad had to wait behind another driver waiting for the light to turn green, so the former honked to request the
latter to get out of the way. Such traffic regulation appears to be one reason that horns were frequently used at the
surveyed crossroads. The noise level of horn sounds in each time interval on each day varied. Most mean noise levels
measured at the sites were around 71 to 75 dB, although there were some levels > 75 dB, and values were widely
distributed according to distances between vehicles and the sound-level meter.
Korean standards concerning vehicle horn sound (Ministry of Environment, 2015) are approximately equivalent to
the international standards mentioned above (UN Vehicle Regulations, 1972, 1984). The A-weighted sound pressure
level of horn sound thus reaches ~100 dB in proximity to the vehicle. In urban areas, therefore, people outside the
vehicle such as pedestrians and cyclists may be exposed to frequent horn sounds with high sound pressure level. In fact,
the present measurements indicate that people near crossroads were often exposed to horn sounds with noise levels >
70 dB. The previous psychoacoustic study investigating the effects of horn sounds on auditory impressions in both the
driving and pedestrian conditions suggested its negative effects, particularly on pedestrians (Takada et al., 2010).
Fig. 1 Frequency of vehicle horn use in each time interval on each day. Measurements were conducted at two crossroads in
(a) November 2013 and (b) October 2014, respectively.
3. Questionnaire survey on vehicle horn use
3.1 Outline of survey
It should be difficult to perform on-the-spot investigations of drivers’ or pedestrians’ reactions because horn use
does not steadily occur, and it is impossible to forecast the occurrence of horn use. Therefore, to clarify the detailed
state of vehicle horn use including effects of that use on people, the questionnaire survey on horn use was executed.
A questionnaire form similar to that used in our previous studies (Takada et al., 2013, 2014) was used. The main
questions related to the use of vehicle horns. Respondents were asked about the latest or last-remembered case of (I)
their own horn use and (II) their experiences of being honked at by another driver in various situations when driving.
Time interval
8:00-9:00
9:00-10:00
10:00-11:00
11:00-12:00
12:00-13:00
13:00-14:00
14:00-15:00
15:00-16:00
16:00-17:00
17:00-18:00
18:00-19:00
Frequency of vehicle horn use
[horn/hour]
40
80
120
160
200 weekend (Sun)
weekday (Mon)
weekday (Tue)
(a) Measurement dates:
November 2013
Time interval
8:00-9:00
9:00-10:00
10:00-11:00
11:00-12:00
12:00-13:00
13:00-14:00
14:00-15:00
15:00-16:00
16:00-17:00
17:00-18:00
18:00-19:00
Frequency of vehicle horn use
[horn/hour]
40
80
120
160
200 weekend (Sat)
weekend (Sun)
weekday (Mon)
weekday (Tue)
weekday (Wed)
(b) Measurement dates:
October 2014
3
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
They were also asked about (III) their experiences of being honked at by a driver while a pedestrian. With regard to (I)
and (II), the questionnaire sought details such as time, place, traffic volume, intent of horn use, and timing pattern of
honking. Moreover, respondents were asked about the target of their horn use and their psychological reactions when
honked at by another driver. With regard to (III), respondents were asked about the driver’s reason for using the horn,
timing pattern of honking, and their psychological reaction to it. The main parts of the questions were represented in
Appendix.
The questionnaire also included questions similar to those in a form proposed by the Acoustical Society of Japan
for a social survey of noise problems (Namba et al., 2010), such as a query concerning sounds that the respondent hears
in daily life at home and their responses (e.g., whether they found a sound to be annoying), and their personal attributes
(e.g., age, gender and occupation). Regarding sounds heard at home, the questionnaire included questions on the nature
of the annoyance felt by respondents in response to hearing a vehicle horn, the time when they heard the horn, and their
coping behaviors. For almost all questions, respondents selected one among multiple choices (see Appendix).
Vehicle horn use appears to be affected by various factors such as traffic conditions, the object, and driver
intention. Our previous survey in Japan revealed relationships between circumstances surrounding drivers who used
their horn and the type of honking (Takada et al., 2013, 2014). These factors are considered to be extrinsic factors of
driver horn use. The intrinsic factors of drivers, such as attitude, personal norms (i.e., social motive or another person’s
appreciation) and moral obligation (i.e., ethical viewpoints), also may affect a driver’s horn use. According to a traffic
behavior model (Fujii, 2002) (see Fig. 2), factors including personal attitude (defined as preference), personal norms,
perceived behavioral control (i.e., expectation concerning the ease of initiating certain traffic behavior), and moral
obligation affect behavioral intention, which then evokes certain traffic behaviors. A similar model may apply to driver
horn use as a traffic behavior. Therefore, we used five questions to evaluate respondent awareness regarding four types
of psychological factors in a lower layer of the traffic behavior model, and its effects on driver horn use (i.e., Q5-1 to
Q5-5 in Appendix):
Q5-1: Is vehicle horn use good behavior? (question concerning respondent attitude toward horn use)
Q5-2: Can a vehicle horn sound become annoying for people outside the vehicle, such as pedestrians and cyclists?
(question concerning respondents norm consciousness of horn use).
Q5-3: Can vehicle horn use toward other drivers cause offense to them? (another question concerning respondents
norm consciousness of horn use).
Q5-4: Do you (respondents) usually honk your horn? (question concerning ease or difficulty of horn use, i.e.,
perceived behavioral control).
Q5-5: Should traffic regulations, no matter how minor, be obeyed? (question concerning respondents ethical
viewpoint, i.e., moral obligation).
Fig. 2 Traffic behavior model (drawn from original figure of Fujii (2002)). The model represents that factors including
personal attitude (defined as preference), personal norms, perceived behavioral control (i.e., expectation concerning
the ease of initiating certain traffic behavior), and moral obligation affect behavioral intention, which then evokes
certain traffic behaviors.
For each question, respondents answered regarding their awareness using five Likert-type scale categories (1 to 5), in
which both extremes for each question corresponded to “strongly agree” and “strongly disagree”.
The questionnaire survey was done in two cities of South Korea, Seoul and Busan, which are the capital and
4
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
second-largest city of the country, respectively. In the surveys of both cities, questionnaire forms were distributed to
participants by cooperators in the present study. All participants provided informed consent and were not given
financial incentives to participate. They were asked to fill out the questionnaire at home. Our cooperators received the
filled-in questionnaire forms from participants after about 25 weeks. We obtained questionnaire answers from 123
participants living in Seoul and its suburbs, and 57 participants in Busan. After eliminating blank forms submitted by
two participants in Busan, responses of 178 participants were analyzed. All responses were examined together with the
following procedures, because no significant difference in results of a driver’s own horn use between the two cities was
found through a chi-square test (χ2 = 31.1, d.f.=37, not significant, indicated by n.s.).
One hundred and two males and 76 females (57.3% and 42.7% of the total 178 respondents, respectively) in their
teens through seventies participated (teens, 1.1%; twenties, 47.8%; thirties, 19.1%; forties, 20.2%; fifties, 9.6%; sixties,
1.1%; seventies and over, 1.1%). Respondents mainly comprised salaried workers and students (self-employed person,
6.7%; salaried worker, 36.5%; independent professional, 3.4%; full-time housewife, 5.6%; part-time worker, 0.0%;
student, 41.6%; person without a regular occupation, 1.1%; other, 4.5%; no answer, 0.6%).
3.2 Results
3.2.1 Simply totaled results of vehicle horn use
Table 1 presents simply totaled results concerning vehicle horn use. There were 98 drivers among the respondents.
Of these drivers, 89 recalled using their horn while driving (Group I, as defined in Sect. 3.1). Additionally, 85 had
experiences of being honked at by another driver (Group II). There were 91 respondents who recalled being honked at
while walking (Group III).
Regarding the reason for horn use (intent of use), more than half the 89 drivers in Group I honked to gain another’s
attention or to alert another to danger while driving (for Group I, to gain another’s attention: 22.5%, to inform of
danger: 46.1%). Moreover, many drivers experienced being honked at for similar purposes (for Group II, to gain
another’s attention: 30.6%, to inform of danger: 28.2%).
There were a few drivers who had used their horn to express gratitude and had experiences of being honked at for a
similar purpose (4.5% of Group I and 3.5% of Group II). Although many cases in which drivers honked their horn to
express gratitude were reported in Japan (Takada et al., 2013, 2014), the present results suggest that such horn use is
uncommon among drivers in the surveyed areas. There were more than a few cases in which drivers used their vehicle
horn to vent anger and in which drivers were honked at by another driver for a similar reason (to vent anger:11.2% of
Group I and 14.1% of Group II).
Many pedestrians had experienced being honked at by a driver to alert them to danger, to gain their attention, or to
vent anger (for Group III, to inform of danger: 49.5%, to gain another’s attention: 17.6%, and to vent anger: 16.5%).
Concerning the types of honking, a single honk or two short honks (onomatopoeically represented by “puQ” and
“puQ puQ,” respectively) were more frequently used than long honks (onomatopoeic representations such as “puuu”
and “puuu puuu”). Specific results were for a single honk and two short honks: 52.8% and 23.6% of Group I, 29.4%
and 37.6% of Group II, 29.7% and 38.5% of Group III; a long honk and two long honks: 18.0% and 1.1% of Group I,
22.4% and 2.4% of Group II, 23.1% and 1.1% of Group III. The survey reveals many cases in which pedestrians were
honked at with long honks by drivers and that the types of honking for drivers and pedestrians in the surveyed areas
were similar to those in Japan (Takada et al., 2013, 2014).
These horn uses were in various places from narrow lanes to main roads with various traffic conditions, mainly
during daytime and evening.
Regarding the psychological reaction when honked at by a driver, there were more than a few drivers and
pedestrians who had no particular feeling (no particular feeling: 16.5% of Group II, and 14.3% of Group III). However,
pedestrians more frequently had negative psychological reactions, e.g., they considered the horn to be startling, noisy,
or irritating more often than drivers (the sum of the three items “startled,” “sensation of noisiness,” and “irritated” were
48.2% of Group II and 75.8% of Group III), as shown in Table 1.
For the questions regarding sound sources heard at home, 98 respondents recalled hearing horns there. Eighteen
respondents declared that they had been annoyed by horn sounds (18.4% of respondents who recalled hearing horns at
home). Thirteen of the 18 respondents who had been annoyed by horns were disturbed from their sleep.
5
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Table 1 Rates of responses (%) for questionnaire items in each respondent group (Group I: drivers who used their horn
while driving; Group II: drivers who had experiences of being honked at by another driver; Group III: pedestrians
who had experiences of being honked at while walking; N: number of respondents, : no question).
Questionnaire items
Rate of respondents (%)
Group II
(N=85)
Group III
(N=91)
Time
Early morning
14.1
Daytime
57.6
Evening
23.5
Nighttime
4.7
Middle of the night
0.0
No answer
0.0
Place
Narrow lane (neighborhood road)
12.9
One-lane road (
without separation between opposing traffic
)
11.8
Two-lane road (one lane in each direction)
38.8
Main road (
two lanes or more in each direction, or a national road
)
27.1
Expressway
3.5
Other
3.5
No answer
2.4
Target of horn
use
Car
Motorcycle
Cyclist
Pedestrian
Other
No answer
Traffic volume
Respondent’s and another driver’s car only
28.2
A few cars in front and behind
28.2
Many cars
24.7
Traffic congestion
12.9
Other
2.4
No answer
3.5
Intent of horn
use
To express gratitude
3.5
1.1
To gain another’s attention
30.6
17.6
To inform of danger
28.2
49.5
To vent anger
14.1
16.5
Incomprehensible
15.3
11.0
Other reasons
5.9
4.4
No answer
2.4
0.0
Timing pattern
of honking
Single honk
29.4
29.7
Two short honks
37.6
38.5
Three or more short honks
4.7
6.6
Long honk
22.4
23.1
Two long honks
2.4
1.1
Three or more long honks
3.5
0.0
Other
0.0
1.1
No answer
0.0
0.0
Psychological
reaction when
honked at
No particular feeling
16.5
14.3
Startled
23.5
37.4
Sensation of noisiness
15.3
18.7
Irritated
9.4
19.8
Feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s way
34.1
8.8
Comfortable
0.0
0.0
Other
1.2
1.1
No answer
0.0
0.0
6
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
3.2.2 Simply totaled results of driver awareness about vehicle horn use
Table 2 shows simply totaled results of driver awareness about vehicle horn use from 98 drivers. According to
results of respondent attitudes toward horn use (i.e., Q5-1), there were more drivers with positive attitudes than those
with negative ones (“agree”, 37.8%; “disagree”, 29.6%). Comparing results for the question regarding respondent norm
consciousness of horn use toward people other than drivers (Q5-2) with similar results for drivers (Q5-3), it was found
that there were more drivers who were conscious of other drivers’ annoyance than those who were conscious of people
other than drivers, such as pedestrians and cyclists (“strongly agree” and “agree” of Q5-2, 47.0%; similar responses to
Q5-3, 72.4%). With respect to the ease or difficulty of vehicle horn use for drivers (Q5-4), many drivers responded that
they did not often use their horn (“disagree” and “strongly disagree”, 84.7%). This result suggests that respondents
were reluctant to use their horns. Concerning respondent’s ethical viewpoint (Q 5-5), the majority of drivers
participating in the survey believed that traffic regulations should be obeyed (“strongly agreeand “agree”, 87.8% ).
Table 2 Rates of responses (%) on driver awareness of vehicle horn use. Q5-1 is the question concerning respondent attitude
toward horn use (“Is vehicle horn use good behavior?”). Q5-2 is the question concerning respondent’s norm con-
sciousness of horn use (“Can a vehicle horn sound become annoying for people outside the vehicle, such as pedes-
trians and cyclists?”). Q5-3 is another question concerning respondent’s norm consciousness of horn use (“Can ve-
hicle horn use toward other drivers cause offense to them?”). Q5-4 is the question concerning ease or difficulty of
horn use, i.e., perceived behavioral control (“Do you usually honk your horn?”). Q5-5 is the question concerning
respondent’s ethical viewpoint, i.e., moral obligation (“Should traffic regulations, no matter how minor, be
obeyed?”).
Strongly agree
Agree
Neither agree
nor disagree
Disagree
Strongly
disagree
No answer
Q5-1
0.0
37.8
26.5
29.6
0.0
6.1
Q5-2
4.1
42.9
13.3
34.7
0.0
5.1
Q5-3
2.0
70.4
9.2
13.3
0.0
5.1
Q5-4
1.0
4.1
5.1
56.1
28.6
5.1
Q5-5
23.5
64.3
4.1
3.1
0.0
5.1
4. Analysis
4.1 Manner of driver horn use
To address relationships between the manner of driver horn use and various factors, such as traffic conditions and
driver intent, questionnaire results of Group I were cross-tabulated between questionnaire items concerning vehicle
horn use. Statistical testing using Cramer’s V (Cramer, 1966), a statistic measuring the strength of association between
nominal variables, was conducted to examine the significant relationships. If V is close to zero, the association between
two nominal variables is weak. V close to 1 indicates a strong association between the two variables. As a result, we
found no significant relationships between questionnaire items related to driver horn use at a 0.05 significance level.
Accordingly, the statistical testing implies no particular manner of honking (e.g., short honks to express gratitude or
gain another’s attention). However, the cross-tabulation should describe the current state of driver horn use. Therefore,
results of that tabulation are discussed in the following.
Figure 3 illustrates relationships between the location and timing pattern of horn use, Fig. 4 between the target and
timing pattern of horn use, Fig. 5 between the driver’s intent and the timing pattern of horn use, and Fig. 6 between the
target and driver’s intent of horn use.
According to Fig. 3, a single honk was often used in various places such as narrow lanes, two-lane roads, and main
roads. However, although two short honks and a long honk were more frequently used on wide thoroughfares such as
main roads than on narrower roads, such cases were less frequent than those with a single honk. Figure 5 shows that a
single honk or two short honks were frequently used to alert another to danger or gain another’s attention. In Japan,
such horn use was also used to express gratitude (Takada et al., 2013, 2014). Driver horn use for a similar reason was
rarely found in the present survey. Horn use to express gratitude may not be widely practiced among drivers in the
survey areas. A long honk was sometimes used not only to alert another to danger but to vent anger.
According to both Figs. 4 and 6, drivers stated that they frequently honked at another driver using short honks (a
7
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
single honk or two short honks) or a long honk for alerting them to danger and gaining their attention, or to vent anger.
Furthermore, drivers occasionally honked at pedestrians using short honks to alert them to danger, although they
mainly directed their horn at other cars.
Fig. 3 Relationship between place and timing pattern of
horn use (V = 0.152, not significant). Each bar rep-
resents the number of respondents for each type of
horn honking used in each place.
Fig. 4 Relationship between the target and timing pattern of
horn use (V = 0.220, not significant). Each bar rep-
resents the number of respondents for each type of
horn honking toward each target.
Fig. 5 Relationship between the driver intent and timing
pattern of horn use (V = 0.245, not significant). Each
bar represents the number of respondents for each
type of horn honking with each driver’s intent.
Fig. 6 Relationship between target and driver intent of horn
use (V = 0.295, not significant). Each bar represents
the number of respondents for each driver’s intent of
honking toward each target.
4.2 Effects of vehicle horn use on the driver and pedestrian psychological reactions
The type of honking and another driver’s intent of horn use are assumed to arouse various psychological reactions
in people hearing a horn (i.e., other drivers and pedestrians). Because pedestrians are generally exposed to vehicle horn
sounds at high sound pressure levels, they may have negative psychological reactions. Therefore, to investigate the
effects of horn use on driver and pedestrian psychological reactions, relationships between questionnaire items were
analyzed by cross-tabulation between variables. Examples of such relationships were between another driver’s intent of
horn use and driver or pedestrian psychological reaction, and between honking type and those reactions. In the same
manner as for the cross-tabulated results of driver horn use mentioned above, statistical testing using Cramer’s V was
performed to find the significant effects of horn use.
4.2.1 Driver psychological reactions to another driver’s vehicle horn use
As a result of cross-tabulation of driver psychological reactions, no significant relationships were found at a 0.05
significance level. However, the cross-tabulation should show the effects of another driver’s horn use on driver
psychological reactions.
Figure 7 shows the relationship between another driver’s intent of horn use, as deemed by the respondent (i.e.,
driver), and the respondent psychological reaction when honked at. Drivers were mainly honked at by another driver to
gain their attention or alert them to danger. Such horn use aroused various psychological reactions such as no particular
0
10
20
30
single honk two short
honks three or
more short
honks
long honk two long
honks three or
more long
honks
other
Number of respondents
Timing pattern
narrow lane
one-lane road
two-lane road
main road
expressway
other
0
10
20
30
40
single honk two short
honks three or
more short
honks
long honk two long
honks three or
more long
honks
other
Number of respondents
Timing pattern
car
motorcycle
cyclist
pedestrian
other
0
10
20
30
single honk two short
honks three or
more short
honks
long honk two long
honks three or
more long
honks
other
Number of respondents
Timing pattern
to express gratitude
to gain another's attention
to inform of danger
to vent anger
other
0
10
20
30
40
to express
gratitude to gain another's
attention to inform of
danger to vent anger other
Number of respondents
Intent of horn use
car
motorcycle
cyclist
pedestrian
other
8
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
feeling, feeling startled, or feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s way. The absence of significant statistical results
(according to Cramer’s V) suggested no particular tendency (Fig. 7).
Figure 8 depicts the relationship between the timing pattern of honking and respondent psychological reaction
when honked at, which was again not statistically significant. Short honks such as a single honk followed by two short
honks aroused driver psychological reactions such as no particular feeling or feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s
way, as well as negative psychological reactions (feeling startled, a sensation of noisiness and feeling irritated). Similar
reactions were induced by a long honk.
The results of driver psychological reactions and of driver horn use presented in the previous section suggest that
there is no definite manner of driver horn use, so the vehicle horn is not used for communication between drivers. In
Japan, drivers frequently use their horn in this way, e.g., when a driver briefly honks after being given the right of way
by another driver on a narrow road (Takada et al., 2013, 2014). Such horn use was rarely found in the present survey.
Fig. 7 Relationship between another driver’s intent of horn
use, as deemed by drivers, and their psychological
reactions when honked at (V = 0.271, not signifi-
cant). Each bar represents the number of respond-
ents for each driver’s psychological reaction in-
duced by another driver’s horn use with each intent.
Fig. 8 Relationship between timing pattern of honking and
driver psychological reactions when honked at (V =
0.257, not significant). Each bar represents the
number of respondents for each driver’s psycholog-
ical reaction induced by each type of horn honking.
4.2.2 Pedestrian psychological reactions to vehicle horn use
Regarding pedestrian psychological reactions, we found significant (p < 0.05) effects of driver horn use directed at
pedestrians.
Figure 9 shows the significant relationship between driver intent of horn use, as regarded by the respondent (i.e.,
pedestrian), and the respondent psychological reaction when honked at (V = 0.363, p < 0.05). Although horn use to alert
pedestrians to danger induced various psychological reactions, they prominently reported that such horn use startled
them. Driver horn use to vent anger mostly incited negative psychological reactions such as a feeling that the horn was
startling, noisy, or irritating.
Figure 10 indicates the significant relationship between the timing pattern of honking and respondent (i.e.,
pedestrian) psychological reaction when honked at (V = 0.301, p < 0.05). Short honks such as a single honk and two
short honks aroused various psychological reactions in pedestrians. According to both Figs. 9 and 10, although there
were some cases in which short honks used to gain a pedestrian’s attention or alert them to danger induced no particular
feeling, there were many cases in which pedestrians were startled by short honks intended to alert them to danger. A
long honk provoked negative psychological reactions in pedestrians, for any purpose of horn use.
The finding that pedestrians were negatively affected by horn use was similar to that for Japan (Takada et al., 2013,
2014). According to a comparison of the present results of pedestrian and driver psychological reactions to vehicle horn
use (see Figs. 710) and the simply totaled results (Table 1), horn use aroused negative psychological reactions much
more in pedestrians than in drivers. A likely reason is the difference in sound level of a horn inside and outside a
vehicle. Pedestrians are generally exposed to a horn with high sound pressure level that is approximately 40 dB higher
than the sound heard by a driver inside a vehicle (Takada et al., 2010). Drivers should be aware that a vehicle horn
directed at pedestrians will generate adverse psychological reactions.
0
10
20
30
no
particular
feeling
startled sensation
of noisiness irritated feeling
sorry for
blocking
comfortable other
Number of respondents
Driver’s psychological reaction when honked at
to express gratitude
to gain another's attention
to inform of danger
to vent anger
incompre- hensible
other
0
10
20
30
no
particular
feeling
startled sensation
of noisiness irritated feeling
sorry for
blocking
comfortable other
Number of respondents
Driver’s psychological reaction when honked at
single honk
two short honks
three or more short honks
long honk
two long honks
three or more long honks
other
9
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Fig. 9 Relationship between driver intent of horn use, as
deemed by pedestrians, and their psychological re-
actions when honked at (V = 0.363, p < 0.05). Each
bar represents the number of respondents for each
pedestrian’s psychological reaction induced by
driver’s horn use with each intent.
Fig. 10 Relationship between timing pattern of honking
and pedestrian psychological reactions when
honked at (V = 0.301, p < 0.05). Each bar repre-
sents the number of respondents for each pedestri-
an’s psychological reaction induced by each type
of horn honking.
4.3 Psychological factors affecting driver horn use
To address the intrinsic factors of drivers such as attitude, personal norms (i.e., social motive or another person’s
appreciation) and moral obligations (i.e., ethical viewpoints) affecting his or her own horn use, relationships between
questionnaire items were analyzed by cross-tabulation between variables. An example of such relationships was
between respondent awareness about driver horn use and the manner of its use (questionnaire results of Group I), such
as the intent and type of honking. Statistical testing using Cramer’s V was carried out to find the significant effects of
the intrinsic factors of drivers on their horn use.
As a result of cross-tabulation, we found a significant relationship between driver awareness concerning ease or
difficulty of vehicle horn use (Q5-4) and the intent of that use (V = 0.309, p < 0.05, Fig. 11). There were many cases in
which drivers who did not usually use their horn honked to alert another to danger or gain their attention. The
relationship between driver awareness regarding ease or difficulty of horn use (Q5-4) and the timing pattern of honking
was also statistically significant (V = 0.447, p < 0.05, Fig. 12). Most drivers who did not usually use their horn mainly
used short honks, such as a single and two short honks or sometimes a long honk. Conversely, drivers who usually used
their horn seemed to do long honks, such as a long honk and three or more long honks, although a small number of
such cases were found.
Fig. 11 Relationship between driver awareness concerning
ease or difficulty of vehicle horn use (Q5-4) and
their intent of use (V = 0.309, p < 0.05). Each bar
represents the number of honking with each intent
of respondents who had awareness concerning ease
or difficulty of vehicle horn use.
Fig. 12 Relationship between driver awareness concerning
ease or difficulty of vehicle horn use (Q5-4) and
timing pattern of honking (V = 0.447, p < 0.05).
Each bar represents the number of each honking
type used by respondents who had awareness con-
cerning ease or difficulty of vehicle horn use.
0
10
20
30
no
particular
feeling
startled sensation
of noisiness irritated feeling
sorry for
blocking
comfortable other
Number of respondents
Pedestrian’s psychological reaction when honked at
to express gratitude
to gain another's attention
to inform of danger
to vent anger
incompre- hensible
other
0
10
20
30
no
particular
feeling
startled sensation
of noisiness irritated feeling
sorry for
blocking
comfortable other
Number of respondents
Pedestrian’s psychological reaction when honked at
single honk
two short honks
three or more short honks
long honk
two long honks
three or more long honks
other
0
10
20
30
to express
gratitude to gain another's
attention to inform of
danger to vent anger other
Number of respondents
Intent of horn use
strongly agree
agree
neither agree nor disagree
disagree
strongly disagree
0
10
20
30
a single
honk two short
honks three or
more short
honks
a long honk two long
honks three or
more long
honks
other
Number of respondents
Timing pattern
strongly agree
agree
neither agree nor disagree
disagree
strongly disagree
10
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Q5-4 was used to investigate the intrinsic factor of respondents regarding perceived behavioral control (i.e.,
expectation concerning the ease of initiating certain traffic behavior) in their own horn use. In the relevant traffic
behavior model (Fig. 2), therefore, the perceived behavioral control of the driver’s intrinsic factors was interpreted as
significantly affecting their horn use as traffic behavior. Respondents who denied frequent horn use (i.e., those who
answered “disagree” and “strongly disagree” to Q5-4) had difficulties using their horn. These results suggest that
drivers who did not usually honk were reluctant to use their horn, and that they briefly honked to alert another to danger
or gain their attention out of necessity. It may therefore be interpreted that the feeling of reluctance restrains their
liberal horn use.
5. Discussion
Korean road traffic act prohibits drivers from generating any noise that damages other persons such as repeated or
continuous horns without justifiable grounds (National Police Agency of South Korea, 2015). The points of this law are
similar to those of Japanese road traffic law. Although the majority of drivers briefly honked to alert another to danger
or gain their attention out of necessity according to this law, they occasionally honked, not only at drivers but also
pedestrians. The present results concerning psychological reactions to vehicle horn use revealed that horn use aroused
negative psychological reactions much more in pedestrians than in drivers. Psychoacoustic studies concerning effects
of the acoustic properties of horn sound on auditory impressions, in which all of the participants were Japanese, also
reported similar results (Takada et al., 2010; Takayama et al., 2012). Moreover, results of these psychoacoustic studies
revealed that auditory impressions (i.e., pleasantness and annoyance) became worse as the number of honking
increased, and even a single honk aroused negative impressions in the pedestrian condition. Given the high sound level
of vehicle horns, it is natural that pedestrians have negative perceptions of vehicle horns. Although a proposal for
reduction of the lower limit of the noise level in the current international standard was submitted (Economic and Social
Council of United Nations, 2014), it may be necessary to conduct further investigation of the volume of vehicle horns
(particularly the higher limit of the noise level). It should at least be emphasized that drivers should not honk at
pedestrians and cyclists.
Measurements of road traffic noise including vehicle horn sounds at crossroads were limited in area. However, in
the survey areas, we found frequent horn use and many cases of such use caused by the traffic regulation allowing
drivers to turn right at a crossroads even if the traffic light controlling traffic flow straight ahead is red. Questionnaire
results also indicated that driver horn use and experiences of being honked at to gain another’s attention or alerting to
danger were frequent (Table 1). Considering these results, an amendment of related traffic regulations, so that drivers
are prohibited not only to go straight but also to turn right at a crossroad if the traffic light controlling traffic flow
straight ahead is red, may reduce driver horn use in such situations.
In the traffic psychological research on aggressive driving, driver horn use was regarded as a behavior reflecting
aggression (Shinar, 1998). Further, this research addressed effects of various factors such as time pressure or congestion
(e.g., driving in weekday work-hour traffic or on weekends), duration of traffic signal green phase, road type, and
personal attributes on driver horn reaction time when waiting behind a car that fails to start after the traffic signal
change to green. According to this research, factors affecting driver aggression may be related to the manner of driver
vehicle horn use. Consequently, in the present study, relationships between that manner (timing pattern of horn honking
in Group I) and situations of use such as time and place, which approximately correspond to factors of “time pressure
or congestion” and “road type” in the traffic psychological research (Shinar, 1998), were not statistically significant
(time vs. timing pattern: V=0.198, n.s, figure not presented; place vs. timing pattern: V=0.152, n.s., Fig. 3). Road type
should affect whether drivers use their horn, but should not be related to the manner of horn use.
In questions concerning the manner and driver awareness of horn use, drivers might answer politely. There were
actually a few respondents who answered their negative attitudes towards horn use and driving (see Table 2). Such
respondents’ attitudes might give biased results in Figs. 11 and 12. However, we found that the perceived behavioral
control of the driver’s intrinsic factors was significantly correlated with the manner of their horn use, such as the intent
of that use and the timing pattern of honking. The significant relationships suggest that a driver’s feeling of reluctance
to use the horn restrains this use for purposes other than original intentions, such as warning others of danger or
avoiding such situations. Accordingly, as a countermeasure to reduce driver horn use, arousal of the driver’s feeling of
reluctance in horn use seems to be effective. Consequently, it is necessary to make drivers understand the adverse
11
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
impacts of the horn sound on individuals honked at (especially pedestrians), and to encourage drivers to keep their horn
use to a minimum. According to the simply totaled results, around 20% of respondents who recalled hearing vehicle
horns at home felt annoyed by them. Not a few residents or workers in neighborhoods with crossroads may be
continually annoyed by frequent horn use. An approach to control frequent driver horn use may improve the noise
problem related to horn sounds, in which people other than drivers are annoyed.
Some cross-cultural studies on driving behavior have indicated that whether vehicle horn use reflecting driver
aggression was observed was dependent on country or region (Lajunen et al., 2004; Ӧzkan et al., 2006). Extended
studies using people with various attributes and residence areas may reveal whether other intrinsic factors of drivers,
such as norm consciousness and moral obligation, affect their own horn use. Furthermore, it is possible that other
extrinsic factors of drivers such as traffic regulation of horn use and social customs in traffic influence that use.
6. Conclusions
Noise measurements at crossroads in Seoul, South Korea, revealed frequent horn use, although there were
differences in frequency depending on the day of measurement. Mean noise levels of vehicle horn sounds measured at
the sites were around 7175 dB.
The questionnaire survey revealed that drivers frequently honked at another driver with short honks or a long honk,
not only to alert them to danger and gain their attention but also to vent anger. Few drivers used their horn to express
gratitude, although such horn use was widespread among drivers in Japan (Takada et al., 2013, 2014). Furthermore,
another driver’s horn use to gain attention and to alert people to danger aroused various psychological reactions, such
as feeling startled or feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s way; however, some did not have any particular
feelings. There were no significant relationships between questionnaire items concerning driver use of the horn,
suggesting that there may be no definite manner of that use in urban areas of South Korea. The results suggest that the
vehicle horn is not used in communicating with other drivers. Moreover, significant negative effects of driver horn use
on pedestrians were also found. Drivers must understand the adverse impacts of the horn sound, especially on the
pedestrians honked at.
The present questionnaire form also included questions on respondent awareness of their own horn use. Correlation
results between driver awareness of their horn use and its manner suggested that drivers who did not usually honk were
reluctant in that use, and that they briefly honked to alert another to danger or gain their attention out of necessity.
The present study partially revealed the current state of vehicle horn use in the surveyed areas. However, regarding
the intrinsic factors of respondents, only the effect of the perceived behavioral control factor (i.e., expectation
concerning the ease of initiating certain traffic behavior) on driver horn use was found. Questionnaire surveys for
people with various attributes and areas of residence may reveal general tendencies of driver horn use. Furthermore, as
stated in prior traffic psychological research on driving behavior (Lajunen et al., 2004; Ӧzkan et al., 2006), if there are
regional differences in horn use, global surveys are needed.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all participants in the present survey. They are also grateful to Dr. Jong-Dae Choi, Ms. Lee-Jin
Jo, and Dr. Jung-Howan Hong for their help with the survey. This study was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant
Numbers JP25560005, JP17H00812.
References
Ali, S. A. and Tamura, A., Road traffic noise mitigation strategies in Greater Cairo, Egypt, Applied Acoustics, Vol.63
(2002), pp.12571265.
Cramer, H., Mathematical Methods of Statistics, Asian text ed. (1966), Princeton University Press.
Doob, A. N. and Gross, A. E., Status of frustrator as an inhibitor of horn-honking responses. Journal of Social
Psychology, Vol.76 (1968), pp.213218.
Economic and Social Council of United Nations, Proposal for amendments to Regulation No. 28 (Audible warning
devices) (2014), ECE/TRANS/WP.29/GRB/2014/4.
Fujii, S., Social psychological approach of travel behavior. In: Modeling travel behavior (2002), Kitamura, R. and
Morikawa, T. eds., pp.3552, Gihodo shuppan (in Japanese).
12
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Kang, H. S., Shin, T., Lee, S. K. and Park, D. C., Design optimization of a dual-shell car horn for improved sound
quality based on numerical and experimental methods, Applied Acoustics, Vol.90 (2015), pp.160170.
Lajunen, T., Parker, D. and Summala, H., The manchester driver behaviour questionnaire: a cross-cultural study,
Accident Analysis and Prevention., Vol.36 (2004), pp.231238.
Lemaitre, G., Susini, P., Winsberg, S., McAdams, S. and Letinturier, B., The sound quality of car horns: A
psychoacoustical study of timbre, Acta Acustica united with Acustica, Vol.93 (2007), pp.457468.
Lemaitre, G., Susini, P., Winsberg, S., McAdams, S. and Letinturier, B., The sound quality of car horns: Designing new
representative sounds, Acta Acustica united with Acustica, Vol.95 (2009), pp.356372.
Ministry of Environment, Noise and vibration control act (2015), Chap. 4, Arts. 30 and 35, Table 13 (entitled Vehicle
noise acceptance criteria) annexed to Arts. 30 and 35 (in Korean).
Ministry of Land, Infrastructure, Transport and Tourism, Safety standard for road transportation vehicles (2003), Chap.
2, Art. 43 (in Japanese).
National Police Agency of Japan, Road traffic law (2015), Chap. 3, Sec. 10, Art. 54 (in Japanese).
National Police Agency of South Korea, Road traffic act (2015), Chap. 4, Art. 49 (in Korean and English).
Namba, S., Kuwano, S., Kaku, J., Kuno, K., Sasaki, M., Tachibana, H., Tamura, A., Mishina, Y., Yano, T. and Yamada,
I., Proposal of fundamental items for social survey on noise problems, Acoustical Science and Technology, Vol.31
(2010), pp.124128.
Onuu, M. U., Road traffic noise in Nigeria: Measurements, analysis and evaluation of nuisance, Journal of Sound and
Vibration, Vol.233 (2000), pp.391405.
Ӧzkan, T., Lajunen, T., Chliaoutakis, J. E., Parker, D. and Summala, H., Cross-cultural differences in driving
behaviours: A comparison of six countries, Transportation Research Part F, Vol.9 (2006), pp.227242.
Phan, H. Y. T., Yano, T., Sato, T. and Nishimura, T., Characteristics of road traffic noise in Hanoi and Ho Chi Minh
City, Vietnam, Applied Acoustics, Vol.71 (2010), pp.479485.
Roy,T. K., Mukhopadhyay, A. R., Ghosh, S. K. and Majumder, G., Influence of honking on the noisier situation (L10),
Noise Control Engineering Journal, Vol.60 (2012), pp.132136.
Shinar, D., Aggressive driving: the contribution of the drivers and the situation, Transportation Research Part F, Vol.1
(1998), pp.137160.
Takada, M., Takeno, A. and Iwamiya, S., Effects of vehicle horn acoustic properties on auditory impressions and the
interpretation of reasons for horn use by other drivers, Noise Control Engineering Journal, Vol.58 (2010),
pp.259272.
Takada, M., Fukuda, Y. and Iwamiya, S., Questionnaire survey on vehicle horn use and its effects on drivers and
pedestrians, Acoustical Science and Technology, Vol.34 (2013), pp.187196.
Takada, M., Murasato, H. and Iwamiya, S., Comparisons of vehicle horn use in urban areas of Japan, Acoustical
Science and Technology, Vol.35 (2014), pp.309317.
Takayama, T., Takada, M. and Iwamiya, S., Effects of timing patterns on auditory impressions and the interpretation of
reasons for horn use by other drivers, Proc. of the 2012 spring meeting of ASJ (2012), 2-8-13 (in Japanese).
UN Vehicle Regulations, Regulation No. 28 - Audible warning devices (1972).
UN Vehicle Regulations, Regulation No. 28 - Audible warning devices Amendment 1 (1984).
Appendix
PQ. We would like to ask you about yourself. Please circle the appropriate choice in the following items.
Gender
[ female male ]
Age
[ teens twenties thirties forties fifties sixties seventies and over ]
Occupation
[ self-employed person salaried worker independent professional full-time housewife part-time worker
student person without a regular occupation other ( ) ]
Q0. Do you have a driving license? If your answer is yes”, please circle the appropriate choice in the
following questions.
Frequency of driving
[ everyday a few times a week once a week a few times a month a few times a year ]
13
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
[ ] years
What type of car do you drive?
[ light vehicle compact car medium-size vehicle large-size vehicle truck other ( ) ]
Q1. This question is for drivers only. Do you have any experience of honking at? If your answer is “yes”,
please circle the appropriate choice in the following details about the latest or last-remembered case.
Time
[ early morning daytime evening nighttime middle of the night ]
Place
[ narrow lane one-lane road two-lane road main road expressway other ( ) ]
Target of horn use
[ car motorcycle
c
yclist pedestrian other ( ) ]
Traffic volume
[ respondent’s and another driver’s car only a few cars in front and behind
many cars
t
raffic congestion other ( ) ]
Intent of horn use
[ to express gratitude to gain another’s attention to inform of danger to vent anger other ( ) ]
Type of honking
[ single honk two short honks three or more short honks long honk two long honks
three or more long honks other ( ) ]
Q2. This question is for drivers only. Do you have any experience of being honked at by another driver while
driving? If your answer is “yes”, please circle the appropriate choice in the following details about the
latest or last-remembered case.
Time
[ early morning daytime evening nighttime middle of the night ]
Place
[ narrow lane one-lane road two-lane road main road expressway other ( ) ]
Traffic volume
[ respondent’s and another driver’s car only a few cars in front and behind
many cars
t
raffic congestion other ( ) ]
Intent of horn use
[ to express gratitude to gain another’s attention to inform of danger to vent anger
incomprehensible other ( ) ]
Type of honking
[ single honk two short honks three or more short honks long honk two long honks
three or more long honks other ( ) ]
How did you feel when honked at?
[ no particular feeling startled sensation of noisiness irritated
feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s way comfortable other ( ) ]
Q3. Do you have any experience of being honked at by a driver while walking? If your answer is “yes”, please
circle the appropriate choice in the following details about the latest or last-remembered case.
Type of honking
[ single honk two short honks three or more short honks long honk two long honks
three or more long honks other ( ) ]
Drivers intent of horn use
[ to express gratitude to gain another’s attention to inform of danger to vent anger
incomprehensible other ( ) ]
How did you feel when honked at?
[ no particular feeling startled sensation of noisiness irritated
feeling sorry for blocking another driver’s way comfortable other ( ) ]
Q4. How do you feel about the volume and sound quality of vehicle horns? Please circle the appropriate
choice in each item.
Volume
[ too loud fairly loud moderate fairly low too low ]
14
The length of time you have driven
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
Sound quality
[ good unpleasant insufficient neither good nor bad indifferent other ( ) ]
Q5. This question is for drivers only. Please circle the appropriate number on the five Likert-type scale
categories in the following questions.
Note: 1 = Strongly agree”, 2 = “Agree”,
3 = “Neither agree nor disagree”,
4 = “Disagree”, 5 = Strongly disagree
1
2
3
4
5
Q5-1. Is vehicle horn use good behavior?
Q5-2. Can a vehicle horn sound become
annoying for people outside the vehicle,
such as pedestrians and cyclists?
Q5-3. Can vehicle horn use toward other
drivers cause offense to them?
Q5-4. Do you usually honk your horn?
Q5-5. Should traffic regulations, no matter
how minor, be obeyed?
GQ1. Are the following sounds audible in your house? Are you annoyed by the sounds? Please mark in the
appropriate column.
No
Sound source
inaudible
audible
not annoying
annoying
1
road traffic noise
2
car horns
3
sirens of ambulance or patrol car
4
noise from motorcycle gang
5
traffic signals
6
train noise
7
Shinkansen (bullet) train noise
8
aircraft noise
9
noise from factories
10
construction noise
11
sounds from school or kindergarten
12
announcements of street vendors using a loudspeaker
13
sounds from a car collecting garbage
14
sounds from shops
15
voices of children / cries of babies
16
idling sounds of cars
17
sounds from air conditioners
18
banging of doors or windows
19
sounds of musical instruments
20
sounds from televisions or stereo sets
21
sounds from the floor above
22
cries of pets
23
sounds of a festival and fireworks
24
sounds of insects / twittering of birds / murmuring of a brook
25
sounds from special events
26
other ( )
15
2
© 2017 The Japan Society of Mechanical Engineers
Takada, Suzuki, Kim, Shin and Iwamiya, Mechanical Engineering Journal, Vol.4, No.2 (2017)
[DOI: 10.1299/mej.16-00433]
GQ2. In which way are you annoyed by car horns in GQ1? Please circle the appropriate choices. You can
mark as many as you like.
( ) The sound is a little annoying, but not so serious.
( ) I cannot relax because of the sound.
( ) The sound is irritating.
( ) The sound makes me unhealthy.
( ) The sound disturbs listening to telephone, television, or radio.
( ) The sound disturbs working or reading.
( ) The sound disturbs conversation.
( ) The sound disturbs sleep.
( ) other ( )
GQ3. At what time of day are you annoyed by car horns? Please circle the appropriate choice.
[ early morning daytime evening nighttime middle of the night whole day not fixed ]
16
... From these studies, driver horn use can be considered one of the traffic behaviors and is influenced by the intrinsic (psychological) factors of the driver. A study conducted in South Korea investigated the relationship between driver awareness of their horn use and the manner of its use [38]. It was suggested that drivers who do not normally use horns are reluctant to use them, and that they sound a short horn when necessary to warn other drivers of danger or for the purpose of alerting them. ...
Article
Full-text available
Car horns were originally installed in vehicles for safety. However, many urban areas in several countries face noise problems related to the use of car and motorbike horns. To propose measures to suppress the use of horns, relationships between horn use and factors including driver awareness and behavior, traffic environment, and the transportation system should be investigated. The present study therefore conducted surveys to grasp the current circumstances of horn use and traffic at urban intersections in Taiwan. The relationship between horn use and the traffic volume of standard-sized vehicles was found. According to an analysis of horn use during traffic signal cycles, in many cases, horns were honked after entering intersections to turn left. In particular, horns were honked when the driver waited more than 4 s for the car in front to start moving after the green light allowing left turns was turned on. An analysis of noise levels at intersections showed that the maximum noise level value (LAmax) could be reduced if vehicle horns were not used. Multiple regression analysis also indicated that LAmax values increased with the frequency of horn use. The equivalent continuous A-weighted sound pressure level (LAeq,10min) did not change with driver horn use, and increased with the traffic volume of motorcycles.
... suggest a positive clue on hydraulic horn uses[23]. A contrast picture found from our study regarding horn use, which was related to drivers' expression of gratitude[24].Among the motor vehicle drivers in Dhaka city study revealed that 48.5% respondent's age were below[30]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to identify the crucial determinants provoking the hydraulic honking behaviors among motor vehicle drivers. It was a cross-sectional study with 206 drivers randomly selected from Dhaka city. Data were collected using a semi-structured pre-tested questionnaire. Miserably, the study revealed that is almost half (44.7%) of the respondents were identified as hydraulic horn users. Most of them were used in overcrowding (23%) and overtaking (18%) situations. The study analyzed the significant predictors associated with the behavior of hydraulic horn use. Youngers (AOR= 5.81, p < 0.01); illiterate (AOR= 10.6, p<0.01) drivers used hydraulic horn in the motor vehicles.
... communication purposes with others drivers, which suggest a positive clue on hydraulic horn uses[23]. A contrast picture found from our study regarding horn use, which was related to drivers' expression of gratitude[24].Among the motor vehicle drivers in Dhaka city study revealed that 48.5% respondent's age were below[30]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to identify the crucial determinants provoking the hydraulic honking behaviors among motor vehicle drivers. It was a cross-sectional study with 206 drivers randomly selected from Dhaka city. Data were collected using a semi-structured pre-tested questionnaire. Miserably, the study revealed that is almost half (44.7%) of the respondents were identified as hydraulic horn users. Most of them were used in overcrowding (23%) and overtaking (18%) situations. The study analyzed the significant predictors associated with the behavior of hydraulic horn use. Youngers (AOR= 5.81, p < 0.01); illiterate (AOR= 10.6, p<0.01) drivers used hydraulic horn in the motor vehicles. Keywords: Hydraulic horn; Transport vehicles; Driver’s behavior
... Honking can have several effects on pedestrians and other drivers. Takada et al. (2017) indicated that honking mostly aroused negative psychological reactions such as feeling startled, a sensation of noisiness, and feeling irritated. The frequency of honking is increased as the traffic volume increases during peak hours. ...
Article
Full-text available
In Nepal, much emphasis has been given to the pollution of air, water, and solid waste both in terms of research and policymaking. Noise is an ignored pollutant, yet a growing public health concern in Kathmandu. This study is aimed at making an assessment of noise pollution in Kathmandu and study the effectiveness of the enforcement of noise standards and No Horn Regulation to control noise pollution. The study was conducted in 23 locations of four different zones (high traffic, low traffic, commercial, and residential) where 12 sites were considered for evaluating the effectiveness of No Horn Regulations. Noise level was studied at five different times of the day using a sound level meter to compute maximum, minimum, equivalent, and average noise level. This study concludes that (i) there is noise pollution problem in Kathmandu, (ii) No Horn Regulation is in place but it has not been strictly followed by the drivers and riders and monitored adequately by concerned authorities, and (iii) the regulation has been effective in reducing the noise level significantly. The average noise level of Kathmandu was recorded as 66.8 dB(A) with the highest noise level in High traffic zones, followed by commercial, low traffic, and residential zones. In 65.2% of the sampled sites, the noise level was beyond the permissible limit of WHO and National Sound Quality Standard of Nepal, 2012. Of the total honking events, 48.1% were against the provision of the No Horn Regulation. After the enforcement of the regulation, noise level was reduced significantly by 2.1 dB(A) in high traffic, low traffic, and residential zones. A comprehensive noise strategy with more emphasis on compliance monitoring and regular noise assessment should be formulated to address environmental noise as a major public health issue.
... These results suggest that driver horn use, which is regarded as a traffic behavior, is affected by intrinsic (psychological) factors. A questionnaire survey conducted in South Korea (4) reported a relationship between drivers' psychological factors and the manner of horn use. Results revealed that drivers who did not usually use their horn were reluctant to do so, and that they generally briefly honked to alert another driver to danger or to gain their attention out of necessity. ...
Conference Paper
In urban areas in many countries, noise pollution is generated by the frequent use of car and motorcycle horns. The final goal of the present study is to clarify the causes of horn use and to propose a transportation system to suppress horn use. To this end, we have investigated relationships between horn use and factors such as driver awareness, behavior, and vehicular environment. Our previous study investigated causal relationships between horn use and the vehicular environment in Taiwan, and found that horn use was more frequent when gridlock occurred at intersections. The present study analyzed details of gridlock situations at three intersections in Taipei during which vehicle horns were used. We found many cases where honking began after vehicles entered the intersection to turn left during the green-light phase. In particular, drivers often used their horns when they had to wait for more than 4 seconds for the vehicles in front of them to start moving after a green light allowed left turns. Logistic regression analysis revealed that the probability of a driver using a vehicle horn was significantly affected by the duration between the signal turning green and the vehicle in front of them starting to move.
... Data of vehicle horn use were analysed in periods (10 minutes in each hourly time zone) during which the traffic volume was counted. According to previous studies 6,7 , timing patterns of horn use can be categorized as a single honk, two short honks, three or more short honks, a long honk, two long honks, three or more long honks, and other honking. Figure 3 illustrates the temporal variation of the total traffic volume at each intersection. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Car horns were originally installed in vehicles for safety. However, many urban areas in several countries face serious noise problems related to the use of car and motorbike horns. a) The ultimate goals of the present study are to clarify the causes of driver horn use and to propose a transportation system that suppresses horn use. To this end, relationships between horn use and factors including driver awareness and behaviour, the vehicular environment and the transportation system should be investigated. The present study therefore conducted surveys to grasp the current circumstances of horn use and traffic at three intersections in an urban area of Taiwan. It was found that the frequency of honking increased with the traffic volume during hours other than weekday rush hours. It is supposed that high traffic volumes during the morning and evening rush hours on weekdays were due to the presence of many motorcycles. In terms of vehicle type, the relationship between driver horn use and the traffic volume of standard-sized vehicles was found. Moreover, the analysis of horn use during traffic signal cycles at each intersection revealed that the frequency of horn use increased when left-turning vehicles obstructed traffic in other lanes.
Article
Full-text available
A study of noise due to honking and its impact on L 10 has been carried out at Kolkata metropolis. The relation between L 10, honking, traffic and the percentage of heavy vehicle has been determined. L 10, the noise level that exceeds 10% of the monitoring time, is a measure of the higher noise level. In this paper, regression equation between L 10, honking, traffic and the percentage of heavy vehicle has been developed for the prediction of L 10. The same equation can be used for reducing L 10 by exercising appropriate control over the suitable predictor( s). The article also delineates as to what kind of control is called for reducing L 10.
Article
Full-text available
In Japan, drivers frequently use their vehicle horn as a signal in various situations such as when passing each other. Furthermore, pedestrians are honked at by drivers. Such horn use may create noise problem for people nearby. Therefore, a questionnaire survey was carried out to ascertain the current circumstance of vehicle horn use in Kanto and Kansai of Japan, while a similar survey was previously carried out in Fukuoka. Respondents were asked about the latest or last-remembered case of horn use in various situations of being drivers and pedestrians. With regard to an experience of being honked at by another driver, the questionnaire included questions relating to the aim of horn use, the timing pattern of the horn, and the respondent's psychological reaction. The results revealed that drivers' brief and frequent vehicle horn use to express gratitude or gain another's attention in various places from narrow lanes to main roads was similar among the three surveyed areas including Fukuoka. Long honks and horn use to gain another's attention or to alert another to danger remarkably aroused negative psychological reactions in pedestrians. No significant difference in the manner of the driver's horn use was found among the three surveyed areas.
Article
Full-text available
The first aim of the present study was to investigate the applicability of the three-factor structure (aggressive violations, ordinary violations, and errors) of the Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (DBQ) and then to compare these driving behaviours across the six countries (Finland, Great Britain, Greece, Iran, The Netherlands, and Turkey). The third aim of the present study was to evaluate the role of driving styles in the relationship between traffic cultures (countries) and the number of traffic accidents utilizing a mediational framework. The fourth aim of this paper was to investigate the relationship between the three factors of DBQ and the number of traffic accidents in each country. Two hundred and forty-two drivers were chosen from each of the six countries, matched for age and sex. The results of confirmatory factor analyses showed that the fit of the three-factor model of DBQ was partially satisfactory in each country. Exploratory factor analyses together with target (Procrustes) rotation and factorial agreement indexes showed that the “ordinary violations” factor was fully congruent and the “errors” factor was fairly congruent across countries. Reliabilities of the scales were at the same level as in the original British data. ANOVA results revealed differences between drivers from “safe” Western/Northern European and Southern European/Middle Eastern countries on DBQ items and scales. Results demonstrated that driving style mediates the relationship between traffic culture (i.e. country) and the number of accidents. Poisson and negative binomial regression analyses also showed that the importance of driver characteristics and behaviours in predicting the number of traffic accidents varies from country to country.
Article
Full-text available
The investigation comprised field measurements of road traffic noise at over 60 sites in 8 cities in South-Eastern Nigeria and psychological survey, by questionnaires, of respondents living and working close to the sites. Instantaneous and 24 h noise measurements were made at the noisiest points, near the facades, of the houses of more than 150 respondents in the neighbourhood of each site. Several noise descriptors were either measured or calculated. Measured values ofLmax were as high as 105 dB (A) while residents were exposed to instantaneous levels of road traffic noise as high as 110 dB (A). Values of Leqand Ldnwere 84·6 and 68·0 dB (A), respectively, and sometimes higher in some of the sites. The calculated L10correlated positively with field data with a correlation coefficient of 0·8551. It was found that the measuredL10 was always higher than the calculated L10by about 4·5–8·8 dB (A) probably because of the reckless use of horn by motorists and reflection from the hills and trees. The type of house lived in, the disturbance of various activities by road traffic noise and neighbourhood noises which were most annoying to residents, were found to strongly affect the percentage of responses obtained with respective correlation coefficients of 0·9925, 0·9714 and 0·7237. The usual poor correlation, ranging from 0·3 to 0·4, between dissatisfaction response and noise exposure were obtained in this investigation. There appeared to be an income bias with respect to community response to road traffic noise, with low-income neighbourhoods reporting less annoyance and disruption of various activities by road traffic noise, and some evidence of adaptation to road traffic noise by residents of busy cities in South-Eastern Nigeria.
Article
Full-text available
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
The objective of this article is to describe the design method of a dual-shell car horn (DSCH) with good interior sound quality based on the boundary element method and transfer path analysis (TPA). A prediction model of the perceived sound quality was developed to determine the DSCH's two fundamental frequencies. A boundary element model for the design of a new DSCH was also explored, and the results were applied to a drawing for the implementation of a prototype car horn. To evaluate the interior sound quality during operation of the prototype, an index for estimating the sound quality level is required. The index was developed using the slope of the spectral decay with respect to the frequency in the spectral analysis of interior sound measured inside a car. The effect of the car body on the interior sound quality was also investigated using TPA. The mounting location of the car horn on the body was changed, and the mounting bracket was modified to improve the interior sound quality. The proposed method was successfully applied to design a new DSCH with superior sound quality.
Article
The Japanese Road Traffic Law states that a vehicle horn should be used only in an emergency or in dangerous locations where a horn sign has been installed. However, drivers frequently use their vehicle horn as a signal when passing each other. Furthermore, pedestrians and cyclists are also honked at by drivers. There may be a negative effect of vehicle horn use on people outside the vehicle. Therefore, a questionnaire survey was carried out to ascertain the current circumstances of vehicle horn use. Respondents were asked about the latest or last-remembered instance of horn use in various situations, both when they were driving and when they were pedestrians. With regard to experiences of being honked at by another driver, the questionnaire included questions concerning place, traffic volume, the aim of horn use, the timing pattern of horn use, and the respondent's psychological reaction when honked at by another driver. It was found that drivers briefly honked their horn to express gratitude, or to gain another's attention, in various places. Long honks such as one long honk or two long honks mostly evoked negative psychological reactions in drivers, such as a feeling that the sound of the horn was startling, noisy, or irritating. Furthermore, pedestrians frequently had negative psychological reactions to horns used to gain their attention or to alert them to danger. More than 40% of nondrivers were displeased with the volume and sound quality of horns.
Article
Social survey is a useful tool to examine the effect of noise on people in daily life situations. Social survey is an essential approach to estimate the permissible level of noise in living environment. However, it needs much effort and cost to conduct a social survey. Moreover, it is not easy to retest the survey as often done in laboratory experiments. If social surveys conducted in various places can be compared with each other, it gives useful information. It is also necessary to compare the results with those of other surveys in order to examine the validity of the survey. For this purpose it is required that common items are included in the social surveys to be compared. The committee of the social survey on noise problems was organized by the Acoustical Society of Japan (ASJ) in order to examine the comparability of social surveys. The Committee proposed fundamental items for a questionnaire that can be used commonly in social surveys in Japanese in 1992 [1] after preliminary surveys conducted in seven research organizations which the committee members belonged to. The English version of the questionnaire was published in 1996 [2].
Article
Noise pollution due to road traffic is a major global concern because of its negative impact on the quality of life in communities everywhere. In Vietnam, traffic noise has become an increasingly noticeable and serious problem in large cities like Hanoi and Ho Chi Minh City. To gain more insight into the characteristics of this noise, intensive noise measurements were conducted in Hanoi and Ho Chi Minh City in September 2005 and September 2007, respectively. A comprehensive dataset of noise was obtained that included 24-h noise measurements as well as short-term noise recordings. The volume of traffic was also quantified by reproducing video camera recordings. Noise datasets from both cities were then compared with a dataset of Japanese traffic noise obtained in Kumamoto. The results showed that the traffic noise in Hanoi and Ho Chi Minh City was characterized by relatively high noise exposure levels due to the large number of motorbikes and frequent horn sounds. The sound of horns contributed a definite impact of 0–4dB on noise exposure in Hanoi and Ho Chi Minh City, where noise levels decreased with the absence of horn sounds. Our results also showed differences in the characteristic traffic noise of Vietnam and Japan.
Article
Though a number of social surveys on community responses to environmental noises have so far been conducted and accumulated in Euro-American countries, few social surveys have been conducted in Asian countries except Japan. No systematic data collected in Vietnam has been presented at the international stage. Vietnam is now in rapid economic growth and has serious environmental problems such as water, air and noise pollution from transportation and industry. Particularly data on community response to noise from Vietnam is strongly required for the international discussion on the global noise policy. Thus a small-scale socio-acoustic survey on road traffic noise was conducted in an area along a main street in Hanoi in November 2004. The sample size was 113 and the noise exposure was quite high, more than 70 dB LAeq,24h . The respondents were well balanced between male and female and mainly younger generations (age of 20s: 23%, 30s: 24%, 40s: 22%, 50s: 16% and others: 15%). A 5-point verbal scale (extremely, very, moderately, slightly and not at all) constructed by the ICBEN (International Commission on Biological Effects of Noise) method was used to measure annoyance and activity disturbances. 45% of respondents were extremely annoyed by the road traffic noise and 32% was very annoyed. 25% of respondents were extremely disturbed by being wakened up and 34% was extremely annoyed by road traffic vibration. The high annoyance and sleep interference by road traffic noise in Hanoi may be mainly caused by the frequent klaxon sounds which are not special but usually heard during all day.