Content uploaded by Thekla Morgenroth
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Thekla Morgenroth on Nov 05, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Running head: SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Should I Stay or Should I Go?
Implications of Maternity Leave Choice for Perceptions of Working Mothers
Thekla Morgenrotha (corresponding author)
aUniversity of Exeter
Washington Singer Laboratories
Perry Road
Exeter, EX4 4QG
UK
T.Morgenroth@exeter.ac.uk
Madeline E. Heilmanb
bNew York University
Department of Psychology
6 Washington Place
New York, NY 10003
USA
madeline.heilman@nyu.edu
Word count: 2498
This work was supported by the University of Exeter.
2
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Abstract
Working mothers often find themselves in a difficult situation when trying to balance work and
family responsibilities and to manage expectations about their work and parental effectiveness.
Family-friendly policies such as maternity leave have been introduced to address this issue. But
how are women who then make the decision to go or not go on maternity leave evaluated? We
presented 296 employed participants with information about a woman who made the decision to
take maternity leave or not, or about a control target for whom this decision was not relevant,
and asked them to evaluate her both in the work and the family domain. We found that both
decisions had negative consequences, albeit in different domains. While the woman taking
maternity leave was evaluated more negatively in the work domain, the woman deciding against
maternity leave was evaluated more negatively in the family domain. These evaluations were
mediated by perceptions of work/family commitment priorities. We conclude that while it is
important to introduce policies that enable parents to reconcile family and work demands,
decisions about whether to take advantage of these policies can have unintended consequences –
consequences that can complicate women’s efforts to balance work and childcare
responsibilities.
Keywords: working mothers; maternity leave; family commitment; job commitment;
family leave; parental leave
3
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Women still shoulder disproportionate amounts of childcare responsibilities (Craig &
Mullan, 2010) and, as a result, many women feel that they have to choose between having a
successful career and having a family. This is reflected both in the number of qualified women
who decide against having children (Mason & Goulden, 2002), and the fact that women
disproportionately give up their careers once they start a family (Hewlett & Luce, 2005). There
have been efforts to address this issue by introducing family-friendly policies such as maternity
leave. Most Western countries require organizations to offer paid maternity leave, and more and
more organizations in the US- where paid maternity leave is not mandatory - are introducing
such policies.
As maternity leave policies become more prevalent, it is critical to ask what
consequences they will have for those who are meant to benefit from them. It is important to
consider not only the effects of the leave itself, but also the effects of the choice about whether to
take it.
Whatever choice the working mother makes, she is vulnerable to the negative effects of
gender stereotypes. Gender stereotypes depict women as relationship-oriented and concerned
about others, not career-oriented and concerned about achievement, with the result that they are
seen as less competent and committed to work than men (Eagly & Karau, 2002, Heilman, 2001).
Motherhood makes gender stereotypes highly salient, and therefore is likely to intensify these
views of women and amplify their negative consequences. Research has indeed shown that
parenthood is more detrimental for working mothers not only than their male counterparts
(Correll, Benard, & Paik, 2007), but also than their childless female counterparts (Heilman &
Okimoto, 2008).
4
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
These findings give reason for concern. If a woman decides to take maternity leave, her
motherhood status is likely to be further highlighted, reinforcing the belief that, for her, family
takes precedence over work. In turn, the stereotype-based negativity directed at mothers in work
settings is likely to be exacerbated, with perceptions of her competence and worthiness of
rewards suffering as a consequence.
But how about the choice not to take maternity leave? The detrimental effects of being a
working mother also occur in the family domain. Gender stereotypes are prescriptive as well as
descriptive, and give rise to norms about the appropriate behavior for women and men (Heilman,
2012; Rudman & Glick, 2001). When it comes to work and family, the “should” for women is
that the family comes first (Cotter, Hermsen, & Vanneman, 2011). Pursuing a career while being
a mother is a potential violation of this norm, and can give rise to perceptions of misplaced
priorities and impaired adequacy as a parent and long-term partner (Okimoto & Heilman, 2012).
The decision not to take maternity leave is apt to worsen this problem because it is a
blatant demonstration of this gender norm violation. Refusing to stay home with one’s child
when maternity leave is offered is likely to leave little question that working mothers have their
priorities wrong and to fuel the view of them as “bad parents” and undesirable partners.
In this study we present participants with information about a woman who has either
chosen to take maternity leave or has decided against it, and examine how these decisions affect
how she is evaluated in the work and family domain. We also include a control condition of
working mothers for whom maternity leave is not currently an issue. Faced with the decision to
take or not take maternity leave, we expect women to find themselves between a rock and a hard
place:
5
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
H1: Mothers who take maternity leave will be viewed as prioritizing family over work,
and mothers who do not take maternity leave will be viewed as prioritizing work over
family, to a higher extent than women making the alternative choice or women not faced
with a maternity leave choice.
H2: Mothers who take maternity leave will be seen as less competent at their jobs and
less worthy of organizational rewards than mothers who do not take maternity leave or
women not faced with this choice.
H3: Mothers who do not take maternity leave will be seen as less competent parents and
less desirable partners than mothers who take maternity leave or women not faced with
this choice.
H3: Perceived work/family commitment priority will mediate the effects of choices to
take or not take maternity leave.
Method
We report all measures, manipulations and exclusion of participants, some of them in the
online supplement.
Participants
Employed participants from English speaking countries were recruited via the Prolific
Academic website. Data were collected in two waves within a three months period (see online
supplement). The final sample consisted of 296 participants, 205 participants from the first and
91 participants from the second data collection wave. Participants included 137 women and 157
men, and were predominantly from the USA (148) and the UK (127). The majority of
6
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
participants were working full-time (70%) and had no children (71%). The average age of
participants was 33.32 years.1
Design and Procedure
Participants were asked to form an impression of an employee based on an excerpt from
an interview between the employee and an HR professional. Participants read one of three
versions of the excerpted interview, which were identical except for a passage indicating the
employee’s decision regarding maternity leave; in a control condition, this passage was absent
(see online supplement). The study was thus a 3 condition (Taking maternity leave vs. Not taking
maternity leave vs. Control) between-participants design. Participants then completed a
questionnaire, were debriefed, and paid £0.90 or the equivalent in their currency.
Measures
Scales were constructed to measure perceptions of job commitment, family commitment,
job competence, parental competence, and desirability as a partner. Each scale was composed of
three statements to which participants rated their agreement on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree). To measure worthiness of organizational rewards participants indicated the
degree to which they would recommend a bonus, a salary increase, and a promotion on a 7-point
scale. See Table 1 for all items and reliabilities.
To create a measure of perceived work/family commitment priority, we subtracted the
job commitment rating from the family commitment rating. Positive values indicate degree of
prioritizing family over work; negative values indicate degree of prioritizing work over family.
1 There was no significant difference in the demographic composition of participants in the two data collection
waves with the exception of age – participants in data collection wave 2 were approximately 3 years younger (p =
.006; see online supplement for more information).
7
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
We included some additional exploratory measures in the first and second wave of data
collection (see online supplement for further information).
Table 1
Scales, Items, and Reliabilities
Measure Items α
Job commitment This person is committed to his or her job
This person is career-oriented
His or her job is not a high priority for this person (reversed)
.80
Family commitment This person is committed to his or her family
This person is family-oriented
Family is not a high priority for this person (reversed)
.90
Job competence This person is probably good at his or her job
This person has the qualities needed to succeed in his or her job
I don’t think this person is good at his or her job
.82
Parental competence This person is probably a good parent
I would not want my children to be raised by someone like this person
(reversed)
I don’t think this person is a good parent (reversed)
.82
Partner desirability This person is probably a good long-term partner
I would like this person to be in a committed relationship with
someone I care about
I would not like to be in a committed relationship with someone like
this person regardless of their gender (reversed)
.73
Worthiness of organizational
rewards
Bonus pay
Salary increase
Promotion
.91
Results
We conducted a series of one-way ANOVAs and LSD tests. Means, standard deviations
and statistical test results appear in Table 2.
8
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Preliminary analyses showed that there was no effects of gender, age, or parental status of
the respondent (i.e., no main effects or interactions), and controlling for these variables did not
alter the significance of our results. Data collection wave also did not affect results by itself or in
interaction with condition.
Table 2
ANOVA and LSD-test Results
Means and Standard Deviations
Taking
maternity leave
condition
Not taking
maternity leave
condition
Control
condition
Df F ηp2
Commitment priority
a
0.80a (1.13) -1.88b (1.85) -0.17c (1.22) 2, 287 85.67*** .08
Job competence 5.25a (0.88) 5.56b(1.00) 5.50b
†
(1.02) 2, 290 2.37† .02
Organizational rewards 4.24a (1.18) 4.79b (1.15) 4.41a (1.23) 2, 293 5.43** .04
Parental competence 5.64a (1.00) 4.68b (1.27) 5.47a (1.03) 2, 292 20.33*** .12
Partner desirability 5.03a (0.94) 4.56b (1.20) 5.11a (1.08) 2. 291 7.29*** .05
Note. Values in brackets refer to standard deviations. Means in the same row that do not share
subscripts differ at p < .05 based on LSD tests. † p < .10. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
aPositive values indicate prioritizing family over work and negative values indicate prioritizing
work over family.
Perceived Commitment Priority.
As predicted, we found a significant effect of condition on perceived commitment
priority. The target taking maternity leave was seen as prioritizing her family over work to a
larger extent than the target not taking maternity leave and the control target, while the target not
taking maternity leave was seen as prioritizing her work over family to a larger extent than the
other two targets.
Job-related evaluations
9
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
We found only a marginally significant effect of condition on perceived job competence,
but the pattern of the data was in line with predictions. The target choosing to take maternity
leave was seen as significantly less competent than the target choosing not to take maternity
leave and tended to be seen as less competent than the control target. In addition, there was a
significant effect of condition on perceived worthiness of organizational rewards. As predicted,
the target taking maternity leave was seen as less worthy of rewards than the target not taking
maternity leave. However, there was no difference in ratings of the targets in the take maternity
leave condition and the control condition; instead, the target not taking maternity leave was seen
as more worthy of organizational rewards than the control target.
Family-related evaluations
Analyses revealed a significant effect for condition on both family-related evaluations.
As predicted, the target not taking maternity leave was seen as a worse parent and less desirable
partner than both the target taking maternity leave and the control target.
Does nationality affect the evaluations?
Taking advantage of our multi-national sample, we conducted a series of 2 (Taking
maternity leave vs. Not taking maternity leave) X 2 (Nationality: US vs. non-US) ANOVAs to
determine whether nationality affected reactions to maternity leave choice.2
Results indicated a significant interaction between nationality and maternity leave choice
for ratings of work/family commitment priority (F (1, 186) = 9.58, p = .002, ηp2 = .05), parental
competence (F (1, 188) = 7.48, p = .007, ηp2 = .04) and partner desirability (F (1, 187) = 4.34, p
2 We chose to group all of the non-US sample together both because the small number of participants from most
countries made separate analyses impossible, and because all of the non-US countries involved have similar
maternity leave policies -policies that, unlike those in the US, are mandatory. Note that the results we report are the
same regardless of whether we only include the largest non-US sample, the UK, as a comparison group or the UK
combined with other non-US countries.
10
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
= .039, ηp2 = .02). While the pattern of means was the same in both nationality samples
participants from countries other than the US (compared to those from the US) saw the target not
taking maternity leave as prioritizing work to a higher extent (p = .013), and gave her more
negative ratings of parental competence (p = .018); they also rated the target taking maternity
leave more positively in partner desirability (p = .047) (see online supplement for descriptive
statistics). Nationality and condition did not interact for evaluations of job competence or
worthiness of rewards (both F < 1.08, p > .300).
Mediation analyses
To test the mediating role of perceived work/family commitment priority (H4), we
conducted mediation analyses using the procedure suggested by Hayes and Preacher (2014) for
multi-categorical predictor variables. Condition served as the predictor with each of the two
maternity leave choice conditions dummy coded and the control condition as the reference
category. We then performed two mediation analyses, one with each dummy variable as the
predictor (see Table 3 for correlations).
Table 3
Bivariate Correlations between All Measures
1 2 3 4
1 Commitment priority
a
-
2 Job competence -.17** -
3 Worthiness of rewards -.26** .64** -
4 Parental competence .52** .48** .25** -
5 Partner desirability .28** .56** .43** .67**
Note. * p < .05; ** p < .01
aHigher values indicate prioritizing family over work and lower values
indicate prioritizing work over family.
11
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
We expected perceptions of work/family commitment priority to mediate the effects of
the choice to take maternity leave on work-related evaluations and the effects of the choice not to
take maternity leave on family–related evaluations. In support, the indirect effect of choosing to
take maternity leave was significant in evaluations of both work competence (B = -.08; 95% CI
[-.18, -.01]) and worthiness of rewards (B = -.15; 95% CI [-.28, -.06]). Also in support, the
indirect effect of the choice not to take maternity leave was significant in evaluations of both
parental competence (B = -.55; 95% CI [-.79, -.37]) and partner desirability (B = -.27; 95% CI [-
.47, -.12]). The direct effect of maternity choice was not significant for any of the outcomes (see
Figures 1 and 2).
Figure 1. Mediation of the effects of the choice to take maternity leave on work-related
evaluations. Note. ML = Maternity leave. Higher values in commitment priority indicate
prioritizing family over work. Not taking ML is entered as a control variable. * p < .05; ** p <
.01; *** p < .001
Taking ML
Commitment
priority
Rewards
0.98***
Job
competence
0.08*
-0.16**
-0.13
0.03
12
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Figure 2. Mediation of the effects of the choice not to take maternity leave on family–related
evaluations. Note. ML = Maternity leave. Higher values in commitment priority indicate
prioritizing family over work. Not taking ML is entered as a control variable. * p < .05; ** p <
.01; *** p < .001
Discussion
This study supports the idea that gender stereotypes, which dictate how women are and
how they should be, can affect evaluations of women based on the decisions they make regarding
maternity leave. Women who chose to take maternity leave were viewed as having work/family
priorities that conformed to descriptive gender stereotypes and suffered in their work-related
evaluations, and women who chose not to take maternity leave were viewed as having
work/family priorities that violate prescriptive gender stereotypes and suffered in their family-
related evaluations. Perceived work/family priority was found to meditate the occurrence of the
negative outcomes in both the work and family domains. These effects occurred regardless of the
respondent’s gender, age, parental status, or nationality, attesting to their universality and
pervasiveness in our culture.
It is interesting to note that our results were in line with our “damned if you do, damned
if you don’t” argument: When compared with the control group, perceptions of competence,
whether in the work or family domain, were never boosted, but only impaired, by the maternity
leave decision. This pattern differed only for organizational rewards: it was the one instance in
which a maternity leave decision had positive effects. In addition to its negative consequences
Not taking ML
Commitment
priority
Partner
desirability
-1.71***
Parental
competence
0.32**
*
-0.20
-
0.25
0.16*
13
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
for evaluations in the family domain, the counter-normative choice to not take maternity leave
had a unique payoff for women.
Our analyses regarding participants’ nationality revealed a consistent pattern of results,
but responses were more extreme in the family-related evaluations in the non-US countries. This
makes sense, given that the option of paid maternity leave is mandated by law in all of the non-
US countries. In these situations, choosing not to take maternity leave is likely to be highly
unusual, underscoring normative expectations about mothers’ priorities. Further research should
examine questions of typical practices around parental leave in greater detail.
This study raises other questions as well. Because our data are cross-sectional, the
mediation results are in need of further investigation using longitudinal data or experimental
manipulation of perceived work and family commitment. Moreover, assessing the effects of
choice about family leave on perceptions of men is important for theoretical clarification of the
effects we found here, particularly as research suggests that they may be punished more harshly
for requesting family leave (Rudman & Mescher, 2013). Additionally, it is important to
determine how working mothers themselves are affected by their decisions regarding maternity
leave. Finally, it is critical to test the effects of family leave choice in natural settings where the
stakes are high and the hazards real.
What does this study tell us about our initial claim that working mothers find themselves
between a rock and a hard place when maternity leave is offered? The results support that idea
and imply that perceptions of work/family commitment priorities, and the extent to which they
conform to stereotypic expectations, lie at the heart of these negative reactions. As these
stereotypic expectations are deeply ingrained in our culture and shown to be stubbornly resistant
14
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
to change (Haines, Deaux, & Lofaro, 2016), it seems unlikely that working mothers will escape
this dilemma any time soon.
We conclude that while it is imperative to introduce policies that enable parents to
reconcile family and work demands, we should be cognizant that the availability of these policies
can inadvertently bring pressures of their own. Thus, as critical as family friendly policies may
be in helping manage the demands working mothers face, the choice about whether to make use
of them may be yet another consideration for women trying to balance their work and family
lives.
15
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
References
Correll, S., Bernard. S, & Paik, I. (2007). Getting a job: Is there a motherhood penalty? American
Journal of Sociology, 112, 1297–1338.
Cotter, D., Hermsen, J. M., & Vanneman, R. (2011). The end of the gender revolution? Gender
role attitudes from 1977 to 2008. American Journal of Sociology, 117, 259-89.
Craig, L., & Mullan, K. (2010). Parenthood, gender and work-family time in the United States,
Australia, Italy, France and Denmark. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 1344-1361
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders.
Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.
Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are a-changing ... or are they not?
A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983-2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40,
353–363.
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical
independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67,
451-470.
Heilman, M.E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women’s
ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 657-674.
Heilman, M. E. (2012). Gender stereotypes and workplace bias. Research in Organizational
Behavior, 32, 113-135.
Heilman, M. E., & Chen, J. J. (2005). Same behavior, different consequences: Reactions to
men’s and women’s altruistic citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90,
431-441.
16
SHOULD I STAY OR SHOULD I GO?
Heilman, M. E., & Okimoto, T. G. (2008). Motherhood: A potential source of bias in
employment decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 189-198.
Hewlett, S. Al., & Luce, C. B. (2005). Off-ramps and on-ramps: Keeping talented women on the
road to success. Harvard Business Review, 83, 43–54.
Mason, M. A., & Goulden, M. (2002). Do babies matter? The effect of family formation on the
lifelong careers of academic men and women. Academe, 88, 21–27.
Okimoto, T. G., & Heilman, M. E. (2012). The “bad parent” assumption: How gender
stereotypes affect reactions to working mothers. Journal of Social Issues, 68, 704-724.
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic
women. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743-762.
Rudman, L.A., & Mescher, K. (2013). Penalizing men who request a family leave: Is flexibility
stigma a femininity stigma. Journal of Social Issues, 69, 322-340.