ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The concept of intelligence encompasses the mental abilities necessary to survival and advancement in any environmental context. Attempts to grasp this multifaceted concept through a relatively simple operationalization have fostered the notion that individual differences in intelligence can often be expressed by a single score. This predominant position has contributed to expect intelligence profiles to remain substantially stable over the course of ontogenetic development and, more generally, across the life-span. These tendencies, however, are biased by the still limited number of empirical reports taking a developmental perspective on intelligence. Viewing intelligence as a dynamic concept, indeed, implies the need to identify full developmental trajectories, to assess how genes, brain, cognition, and environment interact with each other. In the present paper, we describe how a neuroconstructivist approach better explains why intelligence can rise or fall over development, as a result of a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the environmental factors involved at different times across ontogenesis.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Intelligence
Journal of
Article
Intelligence as a Developing Function: A
Neuroconstructivist Approach
Luca Rinaldi 1, 2, * and Annette Karmiloff-Smith 3
1Department of Brain and Behavioural Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia 27100, Italy
2Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano 20126, Italy
3Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, London WC1E 7HX, UK;
a.karmiloff-smith@bbk.ac.uk
*Correspondence: luca.rinaldi@unimib.it; Tel.: +39-02-6448-3775
Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis
Received: 23 December 2016; Accepted: 27 April 2017; Published: 29 April 2017
Abstract:
The concept of intelligence encompasses the mental abilities necessary to survival and
advancement in any environmental context. Attempts to grasp this multifaceted concept through a
relatively simple operationalization have fostered the notion that individual differences in intelligence
can often be expressed by a single score. This predominant position has contributed to expect
intelligence profiles to remain substantially stable over the course of ontogenetic development and,
more generally, across the life-span. These tendencies, however, are biased by the still limited number
of empirical reports taking a developmental perspective on intelligence. Viewing intelligence as a
dynamic concept, indeed, implies the need to identify full developmental trajectories, to assess how
genes, brain, cognition, and environment interact with each other. In the present paper, we describe
how a neuroconstructivist approach better explains why intelligence can rise or fall over development,
as a result of a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the environmental
factors involved at different times across ontogenesis.
Keywords:
intelligence; individual differences; development; neuroconstructivism; emergent
structure; developmental trajectory
1. Introduction
“Intelligence constitutes the state of equilibrium towards which tend all the successive adaptations
of a sensori-motor and cognitive nature, as well as all assimilatory and accommodatory interactions
between the organism and the environment.”
Jean Piaget [
1
](p. 10)
1
Attempts to grasp the construct of intelligence have quickly raised over the last century, making
this concept one of the most extensively debated in the whole history of modern psychology. Despite
its long odyssey through the psychological and neuroscience communities, however, scholars are still
looking for an exact definition of intelligence and for its suitable theoretical scaffold [
2
,
3
]. Importantly,
while our understanding of adult intelligence has rapidly progressed, our grasp of its developmental
1
Piaget was the first psychologist who theorized intelligence within a developmental framework. With admirable foresight, he
postulated that intelligence would emerge from a dynamic interplay between the individual and the environment. Ironically,
after decades of Nature–Nurture debate, we are still conceptualizing intelligence in similar terms. However, thanks to the
recent emergence of the developmental cognitive neuroscience, the neuroepigenetics and the behavioral genetics fields,
we can now more accurately define the strict interplay between the developing organism and the environment, which are
viewed as active entities exerting cascading effects on intellectual functioning.
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18; doi:10.3390/jintelligence5020018 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 2 of 26
counterpart has moved along a slightly different path, and is still under way [
4
,
5
]. The reasons for
this depend on a host of factors that make difficult capturing the dynamic nature of intelligence in
childhood and adolescence.
This paper stems from the need to better capture the developmental essence of intelligence.
After discussing the major achievements in the field, the main issues of debate, and the still open
questions, we will propose a neuroconstructivist approach to the development of human intelligence.
Neuroconstructivism, indeed, depicts development as a trajectory modulated by multiple interacting
biological and environmental constraints [
6
8
]. Intelligence is possibly the ideal construct to be
explained within such a neuroconstructivist viewpoint. Alas, it is outside our purpose to provide an
exhaustive review of the most influential forms of theorizing and testing, as no single article, chapter
or book can summarize theory and research on intelligence. Rather, we wittingly consider this paper
as an effort to gather a unique glimpse into child and adolescent intelligence.
In Section 2, we will briefly introduce some general foundations about intelligence theorizing
and testing. In Section 3, we will move on to maintain that intelligence in development is less stable
than often believed, by providing a systematic review of studies examining the alleged stability
of cognitive abilities across development. In particular, we will review three independent lines of
evidence to challenge the classic view of intelligence as a stable trait. These denote, respectively, the
difficulty in obtaining reliable intelligence assessment, the non-negligible fluctuations of intelligence
scores and their related neural changes, which occur to a greater extent in the early stages of life
compared to adolescence and adulthood. Based on the evidence reviewed, in Section 4we will
argue how a neuroconstructivist approach can more parsimoniously describe the dynamic nature of
intelligence during development than theoretical models grounded on the principle of intelligence
stability. Specifically, we will define a common set of developmental principles and mechanisms that
operate and interact at different levels (i.e., genetic, cellular, neural, behavioral and environmental)
and whose effects on cognitive abilities are being increasingly documented. Indeed, the literature is
currently scarce on theoretical perspectives that simultaneously consider all the different constraints
affecting the development of intelligence. Finally, in Section 5, we will illustrate the benefits of this
approach on public policies, especially considering that intelligence scores are a good predictor of
many life outcomes.
2. Intelligence: General Caveats
Definitions of intelligence and psychometric tests for measuring cognitive abilities have
mushroomed during the past century. In this section, we first provide some general definitions
of this extensively debated concept and then we describe the basic proprieties beyond intelligence
testing to break the ground for the core issue of this paper, i.e., the development of human intelligence.
2.1. On Some Definitions of Intelligence
Achieving consensus among influential experts about the definition of intelligence has always
been a difficult task. A comprehensive definition was expressed by the American Psychological
Association in 1995 [
9
]: “Individuals differ from one another in their ability to understand complex ideas,
to adapt effectively to the environment, to learn from experience, to engage in various forms of reasoning,
to overcome obstacles by taking thought. Although these individual differences can be substantial, they are
never entirely consistent: a given person’s intellectual performance will vary on different occasions, in different
domains, as judged by different criteria. Concepts of ‘intelligence’ are attempts to clarify and organize this
complex set of phenomena”.
Intelligence, thus, encompasses the mental abilities essential for survival and advancement in
any environmental context. The truth, however, is that behind an apparently simple definition of
intelligence lies the complexity of our cognitive architecture to make effective adaptation possible.
A number of cognitive processes, such as perception, learning, memory, reasoning and problem solving
are indeed necessary in the right combination to learn, understand and deal with new situations [
10
].
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 3 of 26
Regardless of this complexity, researchers have found various ways to measure it. In clinical and
research contexts, intelligence has almost always been computed from explicit behavior. Typically,
an individual has to respond quickly or accurately to a given stimulus or question, presented among
different possibilities. The individual’s performance is then rated for speed, accuracy or more subtle
aspects, such as learning. Individuals generally differ substantially in their performance, and those
who perform well on one test tend to do well on many other related tests. All intelligence tests, ranging
from unitary tasks to multi-faceted tasks, tend to generate a strong general factor called as “Spearman’s
g” or “general intelligence” (i.e., the first component of a factor analysis) [
11
,
12
]. This score can be
conceived as a single summary measure of cognitive ability and it is made up of a small number of
non-independent subfactors that represent more specific abilities.
Many decades after the introduction of Spearman’s g, Cattell further specified this single summary
measure of cognitive ability, differentiating between fluid intelligence (gF) and crystallized intelligence
(gC) [
13
]. In particular, gF includes a series of abilities, such as reasoning, perceptual knowledge,
and novel problem-solving ability [
14
] and, from an empirical standpoint, it is strongly associated
with working memory and with g[
15
,
16
]. On the contrary, gC refers to overlearned skills and static
knowledge acquired through educational experiences over lifetime, such as vocabulary, and it is more
resistant than gF to the effects of brain insults or other intervening events in the course of normal
development [17].
In contrast to the gfactor, intelligence quotient (IQ) has been conceived as a composite measure
that derives from performance on a variety of cognitive tasks, including working memory, verbal
comprehension, and visuospatial processing [
18
]. Even though terms like IQ,gand mental ability
have been distinguished on some occasions, in this paper, we will rather use these expressions
interchangeably to generally describe the concept of intelligence [3].
2.2. The Importance of Intelligence Testing
One of the greatest achievements in the history of psychology and, at the same time, one of its
most controversial issues, pertains to the measurement of intelligence. Different critiques, indeed,
have been advanced, especially complaining that no single test can measure the complexity of human
intelligence and that this measurement is inevitably affected by cultural factors. Although these
critiques have proper validity, there is no doubt that the measurement of intelligence has enormous
practical value, as it is a soundly good predictor of educational attainment, performance at work, and
many other aspects of success in life [3].
Individual differences in intelligence are nowadays typically measured by means of psychometric
tests, which cover a range of various cognitive domains such as problem solving, executive function,
memory, processing speed, verbal and spatial abilities. Intelligence tests should show marked
individual differences, which are considered to be quite stable in rank order throughout development
and even over long time spans [
19
]. Intelligence differences in the population nearly follow a normal
distribution, with the only exception of the slight excess at the lower end of the distribution due
to individuals with severe disorders having poor cognitive abilities [
20
]. Of particular relevance is
the fact that the coupling between intelligence testing and psychological theory has always been
moderately weak. Individual tests of intelligence are, in fact, relatively uninfluenced by theory and
this gap is even more accentuated when it comes to development [
5
], as discussed in the next section.
For instance, developmental perspectives on intelligence hark back to Jean Piaget, even though his
theory on cognitive development has been only barely used as a model for intelligence research [4].
3. Intelligence across Development: A Dynamic Concept
Section 3is devoted to introducing the dynamic essence of general cognitive ability over
development. Three types of evidence are reviewed to support the hypothesis that intelligence profiles
can rise or fall over development, thus challenging the idea that cognitive abilities are fixed and static.
First, we will discuss the reasons that make it difficult obtaining stable, reliable and valid intelligence
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 4 of 26
assessment early in life. Second, we will review the evidence pointing to substantial fluctuations of
intelligence scores over development. Third, we will pinpoint that such fluctuations are accompanied
by analogous neural changes, thus providing validity to the described behavioral changes.
3.1. Intelligence Testing across Development
Starting from early primary school years, researchers and clinicians assess intelligence through
standardized test batteries (i.e., IQ tests), as these tests are intended to be suitable for a wide age range.
The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC, which provides both Verbal and Non-Verbal IQ
scores) [
21
] and the Raven’s Progressive Matrices (i.e., both the Colored and Standard versions) [
22
]
are among the psychological tests most commonly used for the 6–17-year age range [
23
], and are taken
as a good index of general intelligence. The use of a “g” score in developmental testing, however, may
have some important limitations. For instance, a child may receive a relatively low or high composite
score, yet presenting highly dissociated skills in more specific domains.
Intelligence test instruments available for use in the preschool age differ more consistently in
terms of their normative data, the age range for which they are appropriate, the factor analytic model of
intelligence they rely on, and the types of behavior they measure. Furthermore, infant and preschooler
measures, which are typically associated with the age range of 1–6 years, differ from other intelligence
tests in scope. Infant measures are indeed generally multidimensional, as they frequently assess
cognitive and motor domains simultaneously. Consequently, until some years ago, the prevailing
opinion was that a reliable measurement of general intelligence was possible only once a child reached
five years of age [
24
]. In more recent years, however, instruments aimed at assessing intelligence in the
preschool age have improved significantly [
5
]. Common tests for this age range are the Bayley Scales
of Infant and Toddler Development [25], the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Intelligence Scale [26].
In interpreting an intelligence measurement it is fundamental to bear in mind that the inferred
score does inevitably include a certain amount of error. This is called the standard error of measurement
and it can be quantified as an estimate of a “true” score based on observed scores [
27
]. In normal
intelligence testing, the standard error of measuring is often larger for higher scores than for lower
scores, and critically, it tends to be greater in the early stages of development [
5
,
27
]. In fact, preschoolers
have often very distinct developmental and maturational trajectories, as reflected by the huge
variability in the age of acquisition of new basic skills. This obviously makes the assessment of
a young child less likely to be stable compared to the one of an older child.
Many other aspects, such as the circumstances that complicate any attempt to assess a very young
child may represent another source of error of measurement [
5
]. Conducting an assessment with
young children is indeed very complex. The examiner should create interest and motivation and keep
the child on task, but at the same time should try to follow standardized procedures [4]. Realistically,
though, the assessment of children younger than 24 months frequently implies some modifications in
the standard procedure, such as for example in the assessment order. Moreover, although caretakers
may even administer some of the test items to preschoolers and thus further undermine test reliability,
they should not be present during the evaluation of older children [4].
Additional challenges in obtaining reliable and valid assessment at the early stages of development
come from the dyadic interaction during testing. Examiner proficiency in building relationship,
eliciting optimal cooperation, maintaining target behavior, and recognizing subtle qualitative aspects
of behavior can indeed influence the validity of the testing [
5
]. Similarly, performance on intelligence
tests is also biased by the child’s comfort in separating from the parent, his/her compliance with the
examiner, temperament, motivation, health, sleep and nutritional patterns [
28
]. Parental attitudes
and socioeconomic status may further contribute to the child engagement and, consequently, to
determining the relative intelligence score [
5
]. However, the idea that intelligence tests underestimate
the cognitive abilities of children from non-supportive environments has so far received conflicting
support [29].
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 5 of 26
Together, the difficulties described above in the assessment of intelligence during development,
along with the different individual maturational trajectories, may largely undermine not only the
validity, but also the stability of the measurement itself, especially in infancy and early childhood.
3.2. Intelligence Fluctuations across Development
In most cultures, intelligence is considered as a stable trait of an individual. This common view
conceives cognitive abilities as static, with a level of intelligence that should remain relatively stable
over time, as evidenced by the fact that IQ measurements made at different points in an individual’s
life tend to correlate well [
19
,
20
]. Efforts to challenge this view, however, are not lacking. For instance,
Valsiner [
30
] contended that intelligence theories should move from being primarily internal or static
in nature to being dynamic and expressed as a sign of relationship with the context. Strong correlations
over time can indeed hide non-negligible individual variation. Over 50% of the variation remains
unexplained when two intelligence scores, measured at different times, show a correlation coefficient
of 0.7.
In the same vein, two-component theories of intellectual development (e.g., [
14
,
31
]) suggest
an alternative view to a static model of intelligence, by proposing that different components may
dissociate in the direction of their development. Indeed, gF tends to peak early in life and to show
gradual age-related declines starting from adulthood. On the contrary, gC, which is believed to
reflect cultural assimilation, generally continues to increase throughout life [
32
,
33
] (please see Box 1).
In the paragraphs below, we will provide further support to the view that intelligence is particularly
flexible over development, by discussing three apparently independent lines of evidence supporting
this hypothesis.
A first aspect concerns the stability of intelligence over time. There is broad agreement that
the stability of intelligence depends on the specific age range considered (e.g., [
34
]). Most studies
did not find good longitudinal consistency in intelligence scores between preschool years and later
stages in development (e.g., [
35
,
36
]), with cognitive abilities becoming relatively stable only from
childhood onwards (e.g., [
37
39
]). By reviewing a number of studies in the literature, Schuerger and
Witt [
40
] found that in about 13% of the 6-year-olds scores changed at least of one standard deviation
(i.e., a huge change, considering that standard intelligence tests have a mean of 100 and a standard
deviation of 15) and the same was true in 7% of the 30-year-olds. Indeed, a drastic increase in test-retest
reliability occurs as a function of age, with less stable scores in childhood and adolescence compared
to adulthood (see Figure 1a). Along these lines, a recent study revealed that, despite a good test–retest
correlation across a two-year interval (r= .81 for Full Scale IQ), 25% of typically developing children
and adolescents showed changes of nine points or more across this interval [
41
]. Similarly, correlations
between cognitive abilities at the age of 17 and measures taken during preschool years increased from
r= .16 at the age of 1 year to r= .44 at the age of 3.5 years, with coefficients becoming increasingly
stronger at the time of school enrolment (six years; r= .67) and later on during school years (eight
years; r= .77) [
42
,
43
], and especially from the age of 11 years onward [
44
]. It is worth noting, however,
that even during late childhood significant increases and decreases in individual levels of IQ are not
uncommon. These fluctuations have often been ascribed to measurement error [
45
]. However, some
of these fluctuations can also represent true changes in cognitive abilities, as evidenced by parallel
changes at the brain level (see the next section).
A second aspect concerns the stability of intelligence as a function of measurement interval.
Indeed, the longer the interval between two intelligence measurements the greater the instability [
40
]
(see Figure 1b). Accordingly, stability coefficients of WISC IQ scores drastically decline from the .80–.90
range in short-term test-retest investigations to the .50–.90 range in longer retest intervals (e.g., three
years or more) [34,46,47].
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 6 of 26
J. Intell. 2017, 5, 18 6 of 25
Figure 1. Test-retest reliability data gathered from 34 separate studies on common IQ tests (the
Standford-Binet, the WISC, the WISC-R, the WAIS, and the WAIS-R) indicate a drastic increase in
reliability with age, with less stable scores in childhood and early adolescence compared to late
adulthood (a). The interval between testing was another correlate of stability, with a drop in
reliability as interval increases (b). Adapted and reprinted with permission from John Wiley and
Sons: Journal of Clinical Psychology [40] © (1989).
A second aspect concerns the stability of intelligence as a function of measurement interval.
Indeed, the longer the interval between two intelligence measurements the greater the instability
[40] (see Figure 1b). Accordingly, stability coefficients of WISC IQ scores drastically decline from the
.80–.90 range in short-term test-retest investigations to the .50–.90 range in longer retest intervals
(e.g., three years or more) [34,46,47].
A third and final aspect pertains to the stability of intelligence as a function of IQ level (i.e.,
high, average, and low). In particular, it has been repeatedly shown that high scores in young
children are less stable than low scores. For instance, in a sample of 4-year-old children who were
followed up until the age of 23, those with initial low IQ scores showed overall more stability over
time than those with average and high IQ scores (e.g., [43,48,49]). However, intelligence fluctuations
can occur even in children with low IQ. That is, children with low intelligence scores (i.e., <85) can
subsequently reach scores above 120 at the age of 17, thus again suggesting the occurrence of drastic
changes, at least at the individual level, during development [43].
Box 1. The differentiation-dedifferentiation hypothesis.
The fact that the level of cognitive performance varies so dramatically with age has led some
theorists to propose a gradual differentiation of general ability into fairly distinct aptitudes over
the life span [50–52]. In particular, according to the differentiation-dedifferentiation hypothesis (e.g.,
[53]) intellectual skills (i.e., such as gF and gC) are rather undifferentiated in childhood. However,
the accumulation of environmental and non-cognitive (e.g., interest, motivation) influences over
time would prompt independent trajectories between gF and gC. Finally, with advancing age,
these abilities would become undifferentiated again (i.e., gF once again more closely correlated
with gC) (but see [54]).
It is worth specifying, however, that a number of behavioral genetic investigations predicts
patterns inconsistent with this hypothesis (see for a discussion [54]). According to these studies,
the proportion of individual differences attributed to genetic sources increases across childhood
and adult lifespan, as the individual selects environments that are compatible with their ability
levels, thus amplifying prior differences (e.g., [55,56]).
3.3. Neural Changes Associated with Intelligence across Development
Various neurobiological markers have been associated with individual differences in IQ. These
include total brain volume [57], cortical thickness [58,59], white matter tract integrity [60], and more
efficient brain activity both during task performance [61] and at rest [62]. Similar findings have been
Figure 1.
Test-retest reliability data gathered from 34 separate studies on common IQ tests (the
Standford-Binet, the WISC, the WISC-R, the WAIS, and the WAIS-R) indicate a drastic increase in
reliability with age, with less stable scores in childhood and early adolescence compared to late
adulthood (
a
). The interval between testing was another correlate of stability, with a drop in reliability
as interval increases (
b
). Adapted and reprinted with permission from John Wiley and Sons: Journal of
Clinical Psychology [40] © (1989).
A third and final aspect pertains to the stability of intelligence as a function of IQ level (i.e., high,
average, and low). In particular, it has been repeatedly shown that high scores in young children are
less stable than low scores. For instance, in a sample of 4-year-old children who were followed up
until the age of 23, those with initial low IQ scores showed overall more stability over time than those
with average and high IQ scores (e.g., [
43
,
48
,
49
]). However, intelligence fluctuations can occur even in
children with low IQ. That is, children with low intelligence scores (i.e., <85) can subsequently reach
scores above 120 at the age of 17, thus again suggesting the occurrence of drastic changes, at least at
the individual level, during development [43].
Box 1. The differentiation-dedifferentiation hypothesis.
The fact that the level of cognitive performance varies so dramatically with age has led some theorists
to propose a gradual differentiation of general ability into fairly distinct aptitudes over the life span [
50
52
].
In particular, according to the differentiation-dedifferentiation hypothesis (e.g., [
53
]) intellectual skills (i.e., such
as gF and gC) are rather undifferentiated in childhood. However, the accumulation of environmental and
non-cognitive (e.g., interest, motivation) influences over time would prompt independent trajectories between
gF and gC. Finally, with advancing age, these abilities would become undifferentiated again (i.e., gF once again
more closely correlated with gC) (but see [54]).
It is worth specifying, however, that a number of behavioral genetic investigations predicts patterns
inconsistent with this hypothesis (see for a discussion [
54
]). According to these studies, the proportion of
individual differences attributed to genetic sources increases across childhood and adult lifespan, as the
individual selects environments that are compatible with their ability levels, thus amplifying prior differences
(e.g., [55,56]).
3.3. Neural Changes Associated with Intelligence across Development
Various neurobiological markers have been associated with individual differences in IQ. These
include total brain volume [
57
], cortical thickness [
58
,
59
], white matter tract integrity [
60
], and more
efficient brain activity both during task performance [
61
] and at rest [
62
]. Similar findings have
been reported also in the developing brain. For instance, IQ is positively correlated with total
cerebral volume in children [
63
], and adolescents [
64
], as well as with a thicker cortex [
65
,
66
], and this
association is partly of genetic origin [
2
]. Further, subcortical grey matter contributes to variance in IQ,
although to a lesser extent than cortical grey matter [67].
Importantly, the wide-spread use of modern neuroimaging has allowed to test whether
unexpected longitudinal fluctuations in IQ may be related to brain development. In fact, despite
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 7 of 26
the human cortex mostly grows during the prenatal period [
68
], recent findings suggest that postnatal
structural brain development is substantially plastic [
69
,
70
]. Interestingly, this plasticity seems to be
related to changes in IQ scores. In a seminal longitudinal investigation, Shaw and colleagues [
67
]
showed that patterns of correlation between intelligence and brain structure (e.g., cortical thickness)
vary as a function of participant’s age. In particular, developmental trajectories of cortical thickness
(i.e., increase and subsequent thinning) appear delayed in more intelligent children [
67
]. These findings
corroborate the evidence of fluctuations in IQ behavioral scores described above and suggest that
cognitive abilities may be associated more to the magnitude and timing of developmental changes in
brain structure than to brain structure per se [71].
Another neuroimaging study illustrates the dynamic essence of intelligence-brain relations.
By combining structural and functional imaging, Ramsden and colleagues [
72
] found that in
adolescents verbal IQ fluctuations were accompanied by grey matter changes in a region that was
activated by speech, whereas non-verbal IQ fluctuations were accompanied by grey matter changes in
a region that was activated by finger movements. This speaks in favor of considerable effects of brain
plasticity associated to IQ during the teenage years (see Figure 2). The idea that changes in intelligence
measures across development can reflect meaningful changes in general cognitive abilities and in their
neuroanatomical substrate was further supported by a recent study based on a sizeable sample of
children and adolescents [73].
J. Intell. 2017, 5, 18 7 of 25
reported also in the developing brain. For instance, IQ is positively correlated with total cerebral
volume in children [63], and adolescents [64], as well as with a thicker cortex [65,66], and this
association is partly of genetic origin [2]. Further, subcortical grey matter contributes to variance in
IQ, although to a lesser extent than cortical grey matter [67].
Importantly, the wide-spread use of modern neuroimaging has allowed to test whether
unexpected longitudinal fluctuations in IQ may be related to brain development. In fact, despite the
human cortex mostly grows during the prenatal period [68], recent findings suggest that postnatal
structural brain development is substantially plastic [69,70]. Interestingly, this plasticity seems to be
related to changes in IQ scores. In a seminal longitudinal investigation, Shaw and colleagues [67]
showed that patterns of correlation between intelligence and brain structure (e.g., cortical thickness)
vary as a function of participant’s age. In particular, developmental trajectories of cortical thickness
(i.e., increase and subsequent thinning) appear delayed in more intelligent children [67]. These
findings corroborate the evidence of fluctuations in IQ behavioral scores described above and
suggest that cognitive abilities may be associated more to the magnitude and timing of
developmental changes in brain structure than to brain structure per se [71].
Another neuroimaging study illustrates the dynamic essence of intelligence-brain relations. By
combining structural and functional imaging, Ramsden and colleagues [72] found that in
adolescents verbal IQ fluctuations were accompanied by grey matter changes in a region that was
activated by speech, whereas non-verbal IQ fluctuations were accompanied by grey matter changes
in a region that was activated by finger movements. This speaks in favor of considerable effects of
brain plasticity associated to IQ during the teenage years (see Figure 2). The idea that changes in
intelligence measures across development can reflect meaningful changes in general cognitive
abilities and in their neuroanatomical substrate was further supported by a recent study based on a
sizeable sample of children and adolescents [73].
Figure 2. In the study by Ramsden and colleagues [72], 33 neurologically normal adolescents had
structural and functional brain scans, along with an IQ measurement (Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale III and WISC III), at two different times (Time 1: 12–16 year old; Time 2: 15–20 year old).
Results showed that changes in Verbal IQ, observed between the two time points, were positively
correlated with changes in grey matter density (and volume) in a region of the left motor cortex,
which is activated by the articulation of speech (a). In striking contrast, changes in Non-Verbal IQ,
observed between the two time points, were positively correlated with grey matter density in the
Figure 2.
In the study by Ramsden and colleagues [
72
], 33 neurologically normal adolescents had
structural and functional brain scans, along with an IQ measurement (Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale III and WISC III), at two different times (Time 1: 12–16 year old; Time 2: 15–20 year old). Results
showed that changes in Verbal IQ, observed between the two time points, were positively correlated
with changes in grey matter density (and volume) in a region of the left motor cortex, which is activated
by the articulation of speech (
a
). In striking contrast, changes in Non-Verbal IQ, observed between the
two time points, were positively correlated with grey matter density in the anterior cerebellum, which
is associated with motor movements of the hand (
b
). Adapted and reprinted with permission from
Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature [72] © (2011).
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 8 of 26
3.4. Following Developmental Trajectories
The evidence reviewed before challenges the classic view of intelligence as a stable trait, by
highlighting that IQ levels undergo substantial fluctuations over development. In addition to this,
it is worth specifying that recent proposals suggest that also the composition of the central core of
intelligence is not stable, but rather changes and develops through the years (see Box 2). The main
challenge for those developmental psychologists who aim to reach a full grasp of intelligence during
development is therefore to integrate individual observations into a developmental trajectory, and to
consider the multiple sources that are influencing it, in the attempt to identify possible mechanisms
driving developmental change. In this sense, a broad theoretical framework is needed to better
understand intelligence over development and to account for its dynamic essence at various levels of
analysis, possibly including biological, environmental and behavioral correlates of IQ.
Box 2. How does intelligence progress over developmental time?
Recent theorizations suggest that intellectual development is characterized by a progressive specialization
in children’s representational capacity. In particular, three fundamental processes would sustain intellectual
functioning across development by continuously generating representations of increasing inclusiveness and
resolution: abstraction,alignment, and cognizance (AACog) [
74
76
]. First, abstraction extracts similarities between
patterns of information, through mechanisms that may vary in development. Next, alignment allows to group and
relate representations in terms of their possible similarities. Finally, cognizance is the component of consciousness
focusing on the mind itself and protracts experience from past to present for abstraction and alignment. In both
theoretical and empirical terms, AACog is conceived as a common core similar to gF, with different levels
of IQ that would correspond to the different types of representations and problem-solving that a child can
master [
76
,
77
]. AACog operates from the very early stages of infancy, although the relative contribution of
each process may vary as a function of age, and progresses through four major developmental cycles, each
comprising two phases [
74
,
78
81
]. Specifically, in a first phase, new representations emerge and next they are
aligned, with each cycle terminating with insights about the cycle’s representations [
82
]. In this way, the type
of representations that the child can master, their inter-relations and their actual awareness change with age.
Importantly, conceptual development is self-propagated because AACog continuously generates new mental
content expressed in representations of increasing inclusiveness and resolution [74].
This model has also been shown to capture the dynamical relations between three principal component
of mental functioning (attention control, executive control, working memory) and gF. Results of different
studies, indeed, demonstrated that the relationship between these components and gF changes periodically with
age [
83
85
]. That is, in the first phase of each cycle the relations between scores of processing speed (i.e., indexes
of attention control and executive control) and gF tend to be high, whereas the relations between working
memory and gF tend to be low. In striking contrast, these relations become inverted in the second phase of each
cycle. Together, this evidence indicates that the developmental structure of intelligence systematically changes
across development.
4. A Neuroconstructivist Approach to Intelligence
In this section, we will argue that neuroconstructivism [
6
,
7
,
86
], which emphasizes the interrelation
between brain and cognitive development, may provide a parsimonious developmental framework to
account for the developmental essence of intelligence. In fact, the adoption of a neuroconstructivist
approach is particularly relevant for the current state of the art in intelligence research, whereby
multiple sources of constraints have been already shown to influence intellectual functioning across
developmental time. However, insofar these constraints have not been connected together by a single
theoretical framework that emphasizes the dynamic and multilevel nature of this concept. On these
grounds, here we attempt to incorporate for the first time these multiple levels into a unified model by
taking a developmental perspective.
We begin by arguing that, within this theoretical framework, we conceive intellectual development
as a progressive increase in the efficiency of representations through experience-dependent processes.
In development, indeed, new representations can progress only on the basis of previous and simpler
representations. Apparently, our view is thus similar in many respects with other theories described
before (see Box 2), whereby intelligence changes with development at various levels, including the
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 9 of 26
nature of representations and the refinement of their manipulation [
77
]. It is, nevertheless, unique in
four different respects.
First, representations are here explicitly conceived as neural activation patterns that sustain
adaptive behavior in the environment [
87
]. Within this framework, the progress through
representational efficiency takes place in the brain by means of a progressive refinement of cortical
structures. Consequently, understanding the development of intelligence implies an understanding of
how the neural substrates supporting the progress of mental representations are shaped. Variation
in brain structure and function, indeed, can be used to discriminate the intellectual functioning of
different individuals, as reviewed before [
20
]. Further, a series of neuroimaging studies have shown
a consistent negative relationship between brain activation and intelligence (see for a review [
88
]).
Specifically, brains of intelligent individuals are more functionally efficient in that they use fewer
neural resources (i.e., less energy consuming) when performing cognitive tasks compared to brains of
less intelligent individuals (the so-called “neural efficiency hypothesis” [
61
]; but see [
89
]). The need
here is therefore to identify those factors that lead to the development of efficient neural activation
patterns promoting adaptive behavior.
Second, and strictly related to the above need, we conceive the development of these increasingly
complex representations as profoundly constrained by multiple factors, which are both intrinsic
and extrinsic to the developing organism. By recognizing the role of multiple constraints, this
framework integrates different views of brain and cognitive development, including: (a) probabilistic
epigenesis, which underscores the interactions between experience and gene expression [
90
]; (b) neural
constructivism, which emphasizes the role of experience on development of small-scale neural
structures [
91
]; (c) the “interactive specialization” view of brain development, according to which the
shift from distributed to more localized processing would be due to activity-dependent interactions
between brain regions [
92
]; (d) the embodiment view, which maintains that bodily states are necessary
for cognition, especially during development [
93
,
94
]; and (e) the ensocialment view, which recognizes
a crucial role of social environment for the developing child (e.g., [
24
,
95
,
96
]). These five views thus
encompass different levels of constraints (i.e., genetic, cellular, neural, behavioral and environmental)
that would influence the development of intellectual functioning [6].
Third, we maintain that the emergence of mental representations is not influenced by each
of these levels separately. Rather, by taking a neuroconstructivist perspective, we assume a strict
interdependency between these levels [
97
]. A principle of context dependence thus operates on all levels
of analysis. In fact, the modeling of neural structures is highly dependent on the context in which these
structures develop. Representations in the brain do not develop in isolation. Rather, the constraints
that shape the developing neural system necessarily alter the actual context in which the individual
develop, affecting consequently the developmental trajectory itself and the specific outcome that is
measured [8,98]. Thus, a change at one level has the potentiality to affect all the others.
Fourth, and finally, this approach maintains that the constraints on mental representations can
be systematically varied across development. The rise and fall of intelligence profiles over time at
both behavioral and neural levels can therefore be explained by the developmental variations in the
constraints affecting intellectual functioning. For instance, gene expression in the brain changes as a
function of environmental experience over developmental time. For this very reason, it is crucial to
follow neural activation patterns that sustain adaptive behavior at multiple levels of analysis over time.
Importantly, adulthood is here viewed as a more stable state along the developmental trajectory [
20
],
as the constraints operate in a greater extent during the early phases of life. Consequently, intelligence
profiles are expected to be more stable in adulthood compared to infancy and early childhood, in line
with the evidence reviewed above.
To sum up, intelligence is here conceived as an emergent propriety originating from multiple
interactions between the constraints imposed by genes, brain, behavior, cognition and environment.
For this very reason, we suggest that it is only by simultaneously considering all these constraints that
we can better explain why intelligence can rise or fall over development. In the following paragraphs,
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 10 of 26
we make an explicit attempt to describe how the different levels of constraints shape the development
of human intelligence and, specifically, how they relate to neural representation and behavioral
outcomes. In particular, we present below a set of interpretations, along with initial available empirical
data, which provide support to a neuroconstructivist view on intelligence development.
4.1. Probabilistic Epigenesis
Until the past decade, development was mainly thought as a predetermined expression of
genes [
99
]. The view of a genetic blueprint for development has nevertheless been recently challenged,
by recognizing the primary role of environmental and behavioral influences on the developing
organism. The view of epigenesis has shifted from deterministic, unidirectional, and under tight
genetic control, to probabilistic, bidirectional, and under broad genetic control [
90
]. In particular,
this probabilistic epigenetic view of development maintains the existence of pervasive dynamic
interactions between genes, neural activity, and the physical and social environments of the developing
child [100,101].
Here we summarize findings that support the view that intelligence develops along a similar
probabilistic epigenetic route. Substantial empirical work suggests that genes may work via the
environment to shape IQ measures [
102
]. The heritability of intelligence—i.e., the proportion of
observed variance that can be ascribed to genetic factors—in any naturally occurring population is
in fact neither zero nor one. The acknowledgment of both genetic and environmental influences has
increased the study of their interplay in modulating (i.e., in terms of moderation and mediation effects)
the development of intelligence [
103
]. Indeed, the relationship between a specific gene and intelligence
is often very indirect and complex.
Unfortunately, research has not yet identified a precise genetic locus that contribute to normal
variation in intelligence scores [
104
]. Intelligence is a complex behavioral trait and, as such, is highly
polygenic, in the sense that many genes contribute to individual variation [
105
]. This is not surprising,
given that very different cognitive abilities, such as spatial ability, vocabulary, executive function and
memory, contribute in defining intelligence scores [
106
]. However, it is now time for a developmental
molecular genetics of cognition that may take advantage of recent advances in technology and
technique (such as genome-wide association study, genome-wide complex-trait analysis and DNA
resequencing studies). Insofar, only few efforts have been made in this direction (e.g., [107,108]).
The probabilistic epigenetic view has also contributed to a better understanding of the shift
from distributed to more localized processing that would occur over many months, or even years,
across development [
109
]. Indeed, gene expression in the brain would change from initial widespread
gradients across the cortex [
110
], restricting expression to more specialized cognitive-level circuits
only progressively in time. This shift may be reflected as well in the development of intelligence.
The heritability of IQ scores seem to increase over the course of development [
111
,
112
], from 20% in
infancy to 40% in adolescence, and to 60% in adulthood [103]. Importantly, this pattern is modulated
by IQ level, as individuals with high IQ show reduced heritability in adolescence (i.e., resembling
younger children), a tendency in line with the view of a an extended sensitive period for intellectual
development in more-intelligent individuals [
113
]. Overall, this speaks in favor of more constant
genetic influences on cognition in adolescence and adulthood compared to the first decade of life [
114
],
a pattern elegantly accounted by the “genetic amplification” hypothesis [
115
,
116
]. According to
this proposal, small genetic differences would be exaggerated as children select, modify and create
environments correlated with their genetic propensities [
103
]. A proposal that put into the foreground
genotype-environmental interactions views of intellectual functioning (but see for a discussion [
117
]).
4.2. Neural Constructivism
The term “neural constructivism” is opposed to selectionist theories, as it implies that our neural
architecture is extensively shaped by activity originating at various levels of the environment, ranging
from the cellular to the social environment [
118
]. By adopting a constructive process of growth, neural
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 11 of 26
constructivism conceives the protracted period of postnatal growth as essential in influencing the
resulting domain specificity of the developing neocortex [
86
]. This influence of the environment on
brain structure and function is well captured by research on environmental enrichment. Various studies
have shown that environmental enrichment may exert a variety of effects on the brain, documented in
several species of mammals [
119
]. For instance, brains in richer environments can show increases in
cortical thickness and synaptic size and number, with stronger effects during neurodevelopment than
in adulthood [119]. Together, this evidence speaks in favor of a circular loop between experience and
the development of neural networks.
The same circular loop (or context dependence) describes the bidirectional relationship between
environmental factors and neural structures subserving intellectual functioning. For instance, the
relation between children’s socioeconomic status (SES) and individual differences in intelligence is
one of those issues that often puts science in the public eye. The effect of environment on IQs and
academic achievement scores of young children can indeed be significant, with children who grow up
in poverty showing lower IQ scores [
120
,
121
]. This testifies to the wide impact of the socioeconomic
environment on various neurocognitive domains, such as working memory, cognitive control and
especially language and memory, which are highly interrelated with general intelligence [122].
However, does the socioeconomic environment have some effect on neural structures as
well? Answering the skeptics: yes, it does. A number of studies have found a relationship
between environmental factors and neurobehavioral functioning in children (e.g., [
123
125
]), with
socioeconomic status that seems to moderate patterns of age-related cortical thinning [
126
]. In
one of the largest studies to date to characterize associations between socioeconomic factors and
children’s brain structure, parental education and family income accounted for individual variation
in independent characteristics of brain structural development, independent of age, sex, and genetic
ancestry [
127
]. Children who participated in the study underwent a standardized structural MRI
protocol, provided saliva samples to assess genetic ancestry, and performed different behavioral
tests of attention, working memory, vocabulary and reading. Parental education was found to be
linearly associated with children’s total brain surface area (which is in turn associated with intelligence)
over the course of childhood and adolescence. On the contrary, surface area mediated the link
between family income and children’s performance on certain executive function tasks (see Figure 3).
These findings add to the emerging literature indicating that SES relates to structural brain variation
in the hippocampus [
128
], amygdala [
128
] and prefrontal cortex [
129
] (see for a review [
130
,
131
]).
Although the study by Noble et al. [
127
] did not directly assess children’s intellectual functioning,
intelligence has been associated with the trajectories of both cortical thickness and surface area during
development (e.g., [
67
,
73
]). As such, these findings may suggest the existence of a strict interplay
between intelligence, environmental factors and brain structure across development. However, it is
worth specifying that Noble and coworkers’ investigation [
127
] was a non-experimental cross-sectional
study and, as such, the driving link between SES and brain structure remains unclear. Indeed, this
association may be mediated by the ability of more highly educated parents to earn higher incomes.
Low-income parents face a much greater array of material hardships (housing, food, and medical costs)
that can, in turn, exacerbate environmental risks and stress (e.g., [
132
]). In this sense, understanding
the proximal interrelationships between brain functioning and more specific aspects beyond SES must
be a priority for future research.
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 12 of 26
J. Intell. 2017, 5, 18 12 of 25
stress (e.g., [132]). In this sense, understanding the proximal interrelationships between brain
functioning and more specific aspects beyond SES must be a priority for future research.
Figure 3. In a cohort of 1099 typically developing individuals between 3 and 20 years of age, parental
education was found to be significantly associated with children's total cortical surface area (a) in
different brain regions associated with language, reading, and various executive functions and
spatial skills (b). Family income was significantly logarithmically associated with children's total
cortical surface area (c) in widespread regions of children's bilateral frontal, temporal and parietal
lobes (d). Adapted and reprinted with permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature
Neuroscience [127] © (2015).
4.3. Interactive Specialization
The interactive specialization view proposes that many cortical areas start out with poorly
defined functions that become domain-specific over development, through a process of neuronal
competition and gradual specialization, location and modularization of function [92,133,134]. This
narrowing process is determined by the activity-dependent interactions between brain regions and
causes, in turn, modifications of the intraregional connectivity [92]. As a consequence, cortical areas
are much more highly interconnected in the infant brain than in the adult brain, with the ratio
between white and gray matter that changes over development [135].
The development of intelligence seems to follow a similar process of neural specialization. First,
a very recent study showed that preadolescent children with high scores in perceptual reasoning
exhibited significantly greater global efficiency of structural brain networks [136]. This finding
indicates that children with higher IQ scores have brain networks that are more highly integrated at
both global and local levels (see also [137]). Organizational efficiency of white matter is indeed
related to higher intelligence across the life-span [20]. Since brain development in childhood is
associated with large-scale changes in synaptic connectivity, gray matter thickness and myelination,
the extent of activation of this network may depend on age. Unfortunately, no study has so far
Figure 3.
In a cohort of 1099 typically developing individuals between 3 and 20 years of age,
parental education was found to be significantly associated with children
'
s total cortical surface
area (
a
) in different brain regions associated with language, reading, and various executive functions
and spatial skills (
b
). Family income was significantly logarithmically associated with children
'
s
total cortical surface area (
c
) in widespread regions of children
'
s bilateral frontal, temporal and
parietal lobes (
d
). Adapted and reprinted with permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature
Neuroscience [127] © (2015).
4.3. Interactive Specialization
The interactive specialization view proposes that many cortical areas start out with poorly defined
functions that become domain-specific over development, through a process of neuronal competition
and gradual specialization, location and modularization of function [
92
,
133
,
134
]. This narrowing
process is determined by the activity-dependent interactions between brain regions and causes, in turn,
modifications of the intraregional connectivity [
92
]. As a consequence, cortical areas are much more
highly interconnected in the infant brain than in the adult brain, with the ratio between white and gray
matter that changes over development [135].
The development of intelligence seems to follow a similar process of neural specialization. First,
a very recent study showed that preadolescent children with high scores in perceptual reasoning
exhibited significantly greater global efficiency of structural brain networks [
136
]. This finding indicates
that children with higher IQ scores have brain networks that are more highly integrated at both
global and local levels (see also [
137
]). Organizational efficiency of white matter is indeed related
to higher intelligence across the life-span [
20
]. Since brain development in childhood is associated
with large-scale changes in synaptic connectivity, gray matter thickness and myelination, the extent of
activation of this network may depend on age. Unfortunately, no study has so far explored whether
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 13 of 26
the relationship between network
'
s global efficiency and intelligence differs between childhood,
adolescence and adulthood. Similarly, although it has been shown that individual differences in
intelligence are associated with the functional connectivity between parietal and frontal brain regions
in both adults [
138
] and young children (i.e., 6–8-year olds) [
139
], it is not yet clear whether this
association varies as a function of age.
Second, some groundbreaking studies indicate that the exploration of neurodevelopmental
trajectories is crucial for understanding individual differences in intelligence [
59
,
64
]. Detecting changes
in gray and white matter volume, as well as in cortical thickness, in the first years of life may therefore
be essential for better grasping fluctuations in intelligence scores over development. Accordingly,
it has been recently demonstrated that high expanding brain regions in both postnatal development
and evolution, especially the anterior cingulate and some specific parts of the frontal cortex, tend to be
related to intellectual functions in humans, as opposed to low expanding areas [140].
4.4. Embodiment
According to the embodiment view, proactive exploration and manipulation of the environment
occupies a primary position in cognitive development [
141
]. This view is supported by increasing
research showing that development depends on bodily states and situated action (e.g., [
94
]). In general,
indeed, extensive amounts of learning proceeds through perception, action, and cognition, with the
developing body that can serve as information filter to the brain. For instance, visual orienting is
the main infants’ capacity of gathering information from the surrounding world for further study
and learning [
92
]. Similarly, motor development in infancy relies on general and basic psychological
functions necessary for survival, such as response selection, behavioral adaptation, and categorization.
It is not surprising, therefore, that the development of the attentional and sensory-motor systems
has been linked to individual differences in general cognitive abilities. In fact, infant IQ measures
heavily relay on the assessment of both attentional and motor domains. Back in the 80s, it was
suggested that visual habituation and dishabituation may be promising candidates for predicting
intelligence at later stages of life [
142
144
]. Visual habituation and dishabituation are dependent, at
various degrees, on the gradual construction of a memory trace, the recall of information from memory,
and the process of comparing the memory trace with the current visual input. The suggestion that
attentional functioning in infants may relate to cognitive abilities was then corroborated by various
studies documenting the predictive value of visual habituation and dishabituation on IQ later in life
(see for a review [
145
]) as well as on academic achievement [
146
]. In particular, the overall weighted
and normalized average correlation relating habituation/dishabituation to later IQ amounts to .37 (i.e.,
these measures can account for about 17% of the variance in later intelligence [
145
]). In line with this, a
recent study from a longitudinal cohort of children tested twice in infancy (7 and 12 months), twice in
the toddler years (24 and 36 months), and then again at 11 years, provides evidence of continuity for
four domains of core cognitive abilities (i.e., attention, processing speed, memory and representational
competence) and their relation to later IQ [
147
]. Importantly, the relationship between attentional
functioning and intelligence would be mediated by environmental factors (see for a discussion, [
148
]).
For instance, infants with caretakers who are less successful at attaining attention toward objects and
events in their environment show less optimal cognitive and language outcomes during the second and
third years of life, and lower intelligence scores at age 4, compared to infants with more encouraging
caretakers (e.g., [149]).
Infant motor development is also related to individual differences in cognitive abilities [
150
].
The rate of progression of infant motor development, especially age of walking, is associated with IQ
at 64 months [
151
], 3 years [
152
] and at 6–11 years [
153
], and even with educational attainment [
154
]
and adult brain structure [
155
]. That is, children who reach motor milestones earlier tend to have
higher IQ scores later in life, compared to those who have a more delayed motor development.
More specifically, some studies found that 4%–6% of the variance in IQ could be explained with
respect to motor development (e.g., [
150
,
152
]), even when controlling for various confounding factors,
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 14 of 26
such as mother’s cigarette consumption in the last trimester, gestational age, birth weight, and birth
length [
156
]. Further, motor coordination in adolescence tends to be related to academic outcomes [
157
]
and working memory [
158
]. Even in this case, environmental factors would play a critical role. Indeed,
the relationship between early motor development and intelligence is stronger in infants of low parental
social status than in those of high parental social status, with “sits without support” and “walks without
support” that accounted for 5.3% and 9.2% of the variance in Non-Verbal IQ, respectively [
156
] (see
Figure 4). This pattern is most likely to be explained by assuming that caretakers in high status families
are generally aware of stimulating the child appropriately. Finally, the association between body mass
index in preschool children and IQ also appears to be mediated by SES [159].
Figure 4.
In a large Danish sample, the association between several specific infant motor developmental
milestones and adult Non-Verbal IQ was moderated by parental social status (p-values immediately
after the text, represented by the symbol *, indicate the level of significance for the interaction term
with parental social class). In particular, stronger associations between milestone attainment and adult
intelligence in the subsample from low social status families were observed (standardized regression
for milestones predicting Non-Verbal IQ are reported). Adapted and reprinted with permission from
Elsevier: Early Human Development [156] © (2015).
4.5. Ensocialment
The social environment in which a child develops is another fundamental level that constrains
infants’ and children’s development. The relationship between socio-economic background and
intelligence tests is a hotly debated issue, with both parents’ education [
160
] and family income [
161
]
that have been related to IQ. Their possible interactive effects with the genetic, neural and cognitive
counterparts have been widely described before. The timing of early developmental changes is in fact
sensitive to exogenous influences, such as the quality of dyadic interaction and the level of stimulation
from the environment [
162
]. In addition to this, parental attitudes also play a determinant role in
engaging the child in those tasks that promote abilities linked to intelligence tests. For instance,
childhood neglect, i.e., a failure to provide for or supervise one’s child, has a profound impact on IQ
measures [
163
]. Furthermore, children with mothers depressed in their first year of life tend to show
low IQ scores at four years of age [
164
]. The impact of the social environment on IQ is also testified by
early childhood educational programs, which link emotional and motivational arousal with activities
designed to exercise and promote selective attention or executive functions. These programs, indeed,
have been proved to enhance various cognitive and school outcomes [
165
,
166
]. This supports the
view according to which children extensively learn through social interactions with other people. The
capacity to feel and regulate emotions is therefore another crucial aspect for understanding human
intelligence [96].
4.6. Toward an Integrated Framework: Reaching a Dynamic Balance
In the first decades of life, enormous changes occur at the cellular, neural, behavioral and
environmental levels. Despite the need of more research to disclose the interplay of these levels
over developmental time and the specific mechanisms that prompt the construction of efficient mental
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 15 of 26
representations (see for a discussion [167]), their influence on the development of intelligence cannot
be questioned. In the previous paragraphs, we made an explicit attempt to describe how the different
levels of constraints shape the development of human intelligence and, specifically, how they relate to
neural representation and behavioral outcomes from a neuroconstructivist perspective.
In particular, in our theoretical framework, intellectual development was conceived as a
progressive increase in the efficiency of mental representations, defined as the neural activation
patterns that sustain adaptive behavior in the environment. There is huge individual variability, indeed,
in the way brains of different people adapt to the environment, with more intelligent individuals
displaying more plastic brain and more efficient neural activations in a range of cognitive tasks [
20
].
Thus far, we have described how the interaction between multiple factors may lead to different
levels of phenotypical performance in IQ tests. Indeed, the shaping of neural structures that allows
the development of intelligence is dependent on the context in which the same structures develop
(i.e., principle of context dependence) (see Figure 5). In this sense, all constraints imposed interactively by
these different levels contribute to the dynamic equilibrium—characterized by alternations of stability
and instability periods—supporting intellectual functioning over development [168].
J. Intell. 2017, 5, 18 15 of 25
be questioned. In the previous paragraphs, we made an explicit attempt to describe how the
different levels of constraints shape the development of human intelligence and, specifically, how
they relate to neural representation and behavioral outcomes from a neuroconstructivist perspective.
In particular, in our theoretical framework, intellectual development was conceived as a
progressive increase in the efficiency of mental representations, defined as the neural activation
patterns that sustain adaptive behavior in the environment. There is huge individual variability,
indeed, in the way brains of different people adapt to the environment, with more intelligent
individuals displaying more plastic brain and more efficient neural activations in a range of
cognitive tasks [20]. Thus far, we have described how the interaction between multiple factors may
lead to different levels of phenotypical performance in IQ tests. Indeed, the shaping of neural
structures that allows the development of intelligence is dependent on the context in which the same
structures develop (i.e., principle of context dependence) (see Figure 5). In this sense, all constraints
imposed interactively by these different levels contribute to the dynamic equilibrium—characterized
by alternations of stability and instability periods—supporting intellectual functioning over
development [168].
Figure 5. The multiple interacting constraints that influence the development of intelligence,
conceived as the construction of efficient mental representation (i.e., neural activation patterns that
sustain adaptive behavior). The principle of context-dependence constrains intellectual development
by means of three general mechanisms: cooperation, competition and chronotopy. This last mechanism
reflects the developmental essence of intelligence, as constraints are supposed to operate mainly
during early phases of life, with intelligence profiles expected to be more stable in adulthood
compared to infancy and early childhood. Together, all constraints imposed interactively by genes,
brain, cognition, and environment are viewed as responsible for the fluctuations of intelligence over
developmental time.
However, how would intellectual development specifically progress? In line with the
neuroconstructivist approach, we maintain that three domain-general, main mechanisms would
guide the realization of more efficient representations over time: competition, cooperation and
chronotopy. In particular, competition and cooperation would contribute to the increase in efficiency of
Figure 5.
The multiple interacting constraints that influence the development of intelligence, conceived
as the construction of efficient mental representation (i.e., neural activation patterns that sustain
adaptive behavior). The principle of context-dependence constrains intellectual development by means
of three general mechanisms: cooperation,competition and chronotopy. This last mechanism reflects the
developmental essence of intelligence, as constraints are supposed to operate mainly during early
phases of life, with intelligence profiles expected to be more stable in adulthood compared to infancy
and early childhood. Together, all constraints imposed interactively by genes, brain, cognition, and
environment are viewed as responsible for the fluctuations of intelligence over developmental time.
However, how would intellectual development specifically progress? In line with the
neuroconstructivist approach, we maintain that three domain-general, main mechanisms would
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 16 of 26
guide the realization of more efficient representations over time: competition,cooperation and chronotopy.
In particular, competition and cooperation would contribute to the increase in efficiency of the mental
representations by allowing the specialization and the integration of functions, respectively. Indeed,
this process of neuronal competition and gradual specialization over development would induce the
brain to produce a more efficient activation for a certain kinds of input over others. The timing of
these mechanisms is well captured by chronotopy, which refers to the temporal dynamic in which the
context-dependence principle constrains the emergence of representations. Constraints are supposed
to operate mainly during early phases of life, with intelligence profiles that are expected to be more
stable in adulthood compared to infancy and early childhood, in line with the evidence reviewed
above (see Figure 5). We further suggest that a positive context-dependence between different levels
(i.e., genes, brain, cognition, and environment) may facilitate the functioning and the progress of the
three mechanisms in more intelligent children. In particular, the fact that high intelligence is related to
more plastic cortical development may indicate that the brain of these children adapt more efficiently
to the requests from the environment.
4.7. Predictions and Directions for Future Research
The neuroconstructivist approach outlined above maintains that all of the constraints imposed
by genes, brain, behavior, cognition and environment can simultaneously contribute to the dynamic
equilibrium of intellectual functioning. Hence, a first core prediction of this model is that changes
in IQ level should be expected over development and that these variations should be portioned out
across the different levels of constraints.
A second prediction of this approach is that the weight of each constraint in influencing
IQ should vary as a function of age. For this very reason, it is important to understand and
investigate neural activation patterns that sustain adaptive behavior at multiple levels of analysis
across development, ideally by means of longitudinal investigations. In analogy to transactional
models of gene-environment correlation and to probabilistic epigenesis, we specifically hypothesize
that the influence of genetic factors should be more variable in infancy and early childhood and become
relatively stable only over time [
168
]. Indeed, gene-environment correlations may occur when children
actively or passively seek out and choose experiences on the basis of their genetically influenced
characteristics and motivations [
168
,
169
]. Accordingly, a positive feedback process may arise, with the
child behaviors and motivations leading to experiences that reinforce those behaviors, which in turn
lead the child to further engage in similar experiences [
168
,
170
]. Hence, not only are individuals with
particular genotypes prone to select, evoke, and attend to particular environments and social contexts,
but these environments are supposed to have causal, reciprocal influences on IQ [
171
]. Because these
processes progressively accumulate over development, genetic effects should become more highly
stable with time [
168
]. In analogy to the ensocialment level, the embodiment level is also supposed to
exert its influence on cognitive abilities mainly during early stages of life.
Third, and in strict relationship with the previous prediction, because the chance to engage in
particular experiences is dependent on the proactive exploration and manipulation of the environment,
the development of the motor and attentional systems is hypothesized to mediate gene-environment
correlations early in life. In particular, the rate of progression of infant motor development and early
indices of attentional functioning are supposed to influence both the expression of genes relevant for
cognitive abilities and the relative responses from the social environment.
Fourth, we expect that the neural architecture subserving intellectual functioning should
be extensively shaped by the embodiment and the ensocialment levels. In particular, since the
development of the attentional and sensory-motor systems is linked to individual differences in
general cognitive abilities, we suggest that scores in these domains should be associated with total
cerebral volume and with trajectories of cortical thinning. In similar terms, we expect a strong
relationship between neural structures, the socioeconomic environment and IQ.
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 17 of 26
Fifth, we expect these levels to also impact on neural specialization. For instance, children who
reach motor milestones earlier or who are more attentive should have brain networks that are more
highly integrated at both global and local levels, with this association that should weaken as a function
of age. Similarly, we expect that positive social contexts and educational programs should influence
synaptic connectivity, gray matter thickness and myelination.
To sum up, we suggest that over developmental time intelligence can be subject to bidirectional
interactions between gene activity, neural activity, behavior and the environment. Hence, at a higher
level of our model, not only genes can prompt the level of efficiency of neural activations that, in
turn, promote adaptive behavior, but this neural activation occurring in a developing brain may
also lead to modifications in gene expression. In this regard, it is worth specifying that since our
model is developmental, we do hypothesize a preferential direction between our levels of constraints,
with genotypes shaping a series of endophenotypes (e.g., brain structure, white matter, etc.), which
in turn would influence behavioral phenotypes (i.e., gF) (see for a proposal based on a hierarchical
model [
172
]). However, we also hypothesize that these influences should become bidirectional over
developmental time. In fact, the direct (i.e., bodily) experience in a certain environment and the social
context in which the child is developing can further shape or mediate such a circular loop. Hence, it is
from the mutual relationship between all these constraints that efficient neural representations develop
and progress.
5. Cascading Effects on Life Outcome
A better understanding of the dynamics underlying intelligence development is fundamental
not only from a clinical and scientific standpoint, but also for its impact on educational and public
policy. Indeed, monitoring individual trajectories of IQ over time may bolster the child development
into protected routes. This is especially relevant in light of the cascading effects that intellectual
functioning exerts on many important life outcomes. For instance, individual differences in general
intelligence are one of the strongest predictors of occupational attainment, social mobility [
173
] and
job performance [
174
]. People with higher general intelligence in childhood or early adulthood also
tend to have better overall physical health, and have longer life expectancy [
175
]. Further, children
with higher childhood IQ have a lower risk of developing dementia [
176
] and of being diagnosed
with schizophrenia spectrum disorder, major depression, or any anxiety disorder in adulthood [
177
].
Understanding how differential IQ levels become associated with these life outcomes is crucial not only
from a scientific point of view, but also for developing public policy and ideating effective interventions.
The scientific significance of general intelligence and its potential for informing public policy is,
however, mainly underappreciated [
178
]. We are, in fact, still a long way from taking advantage of
scientific evidence to protect a child’s right to an open future. For instance, a large number of cognitive
interventions have been shown to exert profound effects on both gF and gC and, more generally, on
academic achievement, thus speaking against a predetermined trajectory of intelligence. In particular,
there is clear evidence that formal education has a great impact on IQ [
9
,
179
]. When children are
deprived of school for a protracted period of time they can exhibit deficits in IQ of as much as two
standard deviations. Such effects of schooling on intelligence measures can be even twice the effect of
age [
180
] and may depend on the age in which schooling begins [
181
]. Further, as reviewed above,
targeted educational programs have the potential to promote various cognitive abilities [
165
,
166
].
However, despite the documented positive effects of schooling and educational programs on IQ, we
still lack a precise identification of those variables that produce the greatest gains and, consequently,
a direct connection between policy and research. In an ideal future, we should fulfill this gap and
possibly provide teachers with specialized professional skills that help them cope with children with
different IQ levels in a scientifically appropriate way [182].
Public policies should also subsidize a better communication between families whose children
have intellectual disabilities, professionals involved in the diagnostic process, educators and teachers.
In particular, all parties should be informed about the fluctuations that characterize intelligence across
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 18 of 26
development. Viewing intelligence as a dynamic concept may indeed step up efforts toward practices
for cognitive enhancement from professionals of the health care systems and from teachers. Similarly,
families should be carefully informed about the positive effects that the social environment in which a
child develops may exert on cognitive abilities.
6. Concluding Remarks
In recent times, intelligence research has profoundly advanced by integrating different fields of
investigation. The interdisciplinary approach pursued by many recent studies has elegantly illustrated
how genes, brain, cognition, and environment may interact with each other over development to
influence intelligence profiles. This evidence indicates that intelligence is not static; rather, it undergoes
extensive developmental changes over ontogenesis. In these terms, intelligence can be conceived as the
phenotypic outcome arising from a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the
environment in which it progresses. Achieving this dynamic balance in intellectual skills is a milestone
of ontogenetic development [
183
]. A vital issue for future consideration is the fact that we know that
the microcircuitry of the brain develops enormously in the early postnatal life, followed by a period of
pruning in which only used connection weights are strengthened [
92
]. However, we know very little
about how the development of intelligence relates to this neural changes and to the progress toward
representational efficiency. For instance, how is the development of specific intellectual skills (and the
differentiation process between gF and gC) associated to the progressive modularization of the brain?
How do genes specifically modify this relationship over time? What is the exact association between
brain structure, representational efficiency and SES? Further, do the same neural and genetic aspects
that contribute to intelligence later in life also contribute to attentive processes and the attainment
of motor milestones in the first months of life? In short, there is an urgent need to longitudinally
identify the developmental processes that cause the dynamic balance between the levels implied by
the outlined neuroconstructivist approach through careful multidisciplinary investigations.
Acknowledgments:
This work was supported by a Study Visit Grant from the Experimental Psychology Society
to Luca Rinaldi. The authors would like to thank Professor Luisa Girelli, Professor Viola Macchi Cassia and four
anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments and suggestions.
Author Contributions:
Both authors were equally involved in the planning and overall conceptualization of
the article.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Piaget, J. La psychologie de l'intelligence; Armand Colin: Paris, France, 2012.
2. Deary, I.J. Intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012,63, 453–482. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3.
Nisbett, R.E.; Aronson, J.; Blair, C.; Dickens, W.; Flynn, J.; Halpern, D.F.; Turkheimer, E. Intelligence: New
findings and theoretical developments. Am. Psychol. 2012,67, 130–159. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4.
Kamphaus, R.W. Clinical Assessment of Child and Adolescent Intelligence; Springer Science+ Business Media:
Berlin, Germany, 2005.
5.
Baron, I.S.; Leonberger, K.A. Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period. Neuropsychol. Rev.
2012
,22,
334–344. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
6.
Westermann, G.; Mareschal, D.; Johnson, M.H.; Sirois, S.; Spratling, M.W.; Thomas, M.S.C.
Neuroconstructivism. Dev. Sci. 2007,10, 75–83. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7.
Mareschal, D. Neuroconstructivism: How the Brain Constructs Cognition; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK,
2007; Volume 1.
8. Karmiloff-Smith, A. An alternative to domain-general or domain-specific frameworks for theorizing about
human evolution and ontogenesis. AIMS Neurosci. 2015,2, 91–104. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
9.
Neisser, U.; Boodoo, G.; Bouchard, T.J., Jr.; Boykin, A.W.; Brody, N.; Ceci, S.J.; Halpern, D.F.; Loehlin, J.C.;
Perloff, R.; Sternberg, R.J. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am. Psychol. 1996,51, 77–101. [CrossRef]
10. Sternberg, R.J. Practical Intelligence in Everyday Life; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000.
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 19 of 26
11.
Spearman, C. “General Intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol.
1904
,15, 201–292.
[CrossRef]
12.
Gray, J.R.; Thompson, P.M. Neurobiology of intelligence: Science and ethics. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
2004
,5,
471–482. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13.
Cattell, R.B. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. J. Educ. Psychol.
1963
,54,
1–22. [CrossRef]
14. Cattell, R.B. Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1971.
15.
Kane, M.J.; Engle, R.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention,
and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev.
2002
,9, 637–671.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
16.
Deary, I.J. Human intelligence differences: Towards a combined experimental–differential approach.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001,5, 164–170. [CrossRef]
17.
Waltz, J.A.; Knowlton, B.J.; Holyoak, K.J.; Boone, K.B.; Mishkin, F.S.; de Menezes Santos, M.; Thomas, C.R.;
Miller, B.L. A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychol. Sci.
1999
,10, 119–125.
[CrossRef]
18.
Jung, R.E.; Haier, R.J. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging
neuroimaging evidence. Behav. Brain Sci. 2007,30, 135–154. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
19.
Moffitt, T.E.; Caspi, A.; Harkness, A.R.; Silva, P.A. The Natural History of Change to Intellectual Performance:
Who Changes? How Much? Is it Meaningful? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry
1993
,34, 455–506. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
20.
Deary, I.J.; Penke, L.; Johnson, W. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
2010,11, 201–211. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
21.
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1949.
22.
Raven, J. The Raven’s progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cogn. Psychol.
2000
,
41, 1–48. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23.
Evers, A.; Muñiz, J.; Bartram, D.; Boben, D.; Egeland, J.; Fernández-Hermida, J.R.; Frans, Ö.; Gintiliené, G.;
Hagemeister, C.; Halama, P. Testing practices in the 21st century. Eur. Psychol.
2012
,17, 300–319. [CrossRef]
24. Sattler, J.M. Assessment of Children: Cognitive Foundations; JM Sattler: San Diego, CA, USA, 2008.
25.
Bayley, N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Manual; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX,
USA, 1993.
26.
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY,
USA, 1967.
27. Haier, R.J. Increased intelligence is a myth (so far). Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014,8, 34. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28. Baron, I.S. Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Child; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004.
29.
Haywood, H.C.; Switzky, H.N. The malleability of intelligence: Cognitive processes as a function of
polygenic–experiential interaction. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 1986,15, 245–255.
30.
Valsiner, J. Conceptualizing intelligence: From an internal static attribution to the study of the process
structure of organism-environment relationships. Int. J. Psychol. 1984,19, 363–389. [CrossRef]
31.
Baltes, P.B.; Staudinger, U.M. Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1999,50, 471–507. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
32.
Horn, J.L.; Cattell, R.B. Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences.
J. Educ. Psychol. 1966,57, 253–270. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33.
Salthouse, T.A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol. Aging
2009
,30, 507–514. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
34.
Watkins, M.W.; Smith, L.G. Long-term stability of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth
Edition. Psychol. Assess. 2013,25, 477–483. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
35.
Bishop, E.G.; Cherny, S.S.; Corley, R.; Plomin, R.; DeFries, J.C.; Hewitt, J.K. Development genetic analysis of
general cognitive ability from 1 to 12 years in a sample of adoptees, biological siblings, and twins. Intelligence
2003,31, 31–49. [CrossRef]
36.
McCall, R.B.; Hogarty, P.S.; Hurlburt, N. Transitions in infant sensorimotor development and the prediction
of childhood IQ. Am. Psychol. 1972,27, 728–748. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 20 of 26
37.
Johnson, W.; Gow, A.J.; Corley, J.; Starr, J.M.; Deary, I.J. Location in cognitive and residential space at age
70 reflects a lifelong trait over parental and environmental circumstances: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936.
Intelligence 2010,38, 402–411. [CrossRef]
38.
Chen, Z.; Siegler, R.S. Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers’ and
older children’s thinking. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2000,65, 1–96. [CrossRef]
39.
Deary, I.J. The stability of intelligence from childhood to old age. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.
2014
,23, 239–245.
[CrossRef]
40.
Schuerger, J.M.; Witt, A.C. The temporal stability of individually tested intelligence. J. Clin. Psychol.
1989
,45,
294–302. [CrossRef]
41.
Waber, D.P.; Forbes, P.W.; Almli, C.R.; Blood, E.A. Four-year longitudinal performance of a population-based
sample of healthy children on a neuropsychological battery: The NIH MRI study of normal brain
development. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2012,18, 179–190. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
42.
Gottfried, A.W.; Gottfried, A.E.; Guerin, D.W. The Fullerton Longitudinal Study: A long-term investigation
of intellectual and motivational giftedness. J. Educ. Gift. 2006,29, 430–450. [CrossRef]
43.
Schneider, W.; Niklas, F.; Schmiedeler, S. Intellectual development from early childhood to early adulthood:
The impact of early IQ differences on stability and change over time. Learn. Individ. Differ.
2014
,32, 156–162.
[CrossRef]
44.
Deary, I.J.; Whalley, L.J.; Lemmon, H.; Crawford, J.R.; Starr, J.M. The stability of individual differences in
mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence
2000
,28,
49–55. [CrossRef]
45. Flynn, J.R. What Is Intelligence?: Beyond the Flynn Effect; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2007.
46.
Canivez, G.L.; Watkins, M.W. Long-term stability of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third
Edition. Psychol. Assess. 1998,10, 285–291. [CrossRef]
47.
Oakman, S.; Wilson, B. Stability of WISC-R intelligence scores: Implications for 3-year reevaluations of
learning disabled students. Psychol. Sch. 1988,25, 118–120. [CrossRef]
48.
Mortensen, E.L.; Andresen, J.; Kruuse, E.; Sanders, S.A.; Reinisch, J.M. IQ stability: The relation between
child and young adult intelligence test scores in low-birthweight samples. Scand. J. Psychol.
2003
,44, 395–398.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
49.
Schneider, W.; Wolke, D.; Schlagmüller, M.; Meyer, R. Pathsways to school achievement in very preterm and
full term children. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2004,19, 385–406. [CrossRef]
50.
Balinsky, B. An analysis of the mental factors of various age groups from nine to sixty. Genet. Psychol. Monogr.
1941,23, 191–234.
51.
Li, S.-C.; Lindenberger, U.; Hommel, B.; Aschersleben, G.; Prinz, W.; Baltes, P.B. Transformations in the
couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychol. Sci.
2004,15, 155–163. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
52. Garrett, H.E. A developmental theory of intelligence. Am. Psychol. 1946,1, 372–378. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
53.
Baltes, P.B.; Cornelius, S.W.; Spiro, A.; Nesselroade, J.R.; Willis, S.L. Integration versus differentiation of
fluid/crytallized intelligence in old age. Dev. Psychol. 1980,16, 625–635. [CrossRef]
54.
Tucker-Drob, E.M. Differentiation of cognitive abilities across the life span. Dev. Psychol.
2009
,45, 1097–1118.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
55.
Dickens, W.T.; Flynn, J.R. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved.
Psychol. Rev. 2001,108, 346–369. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
56.
van der Maas, H.L.J.; Dolan, C.V.; Grasman, R.P.P.P.; Wicherts, J.M.; Huizenga, H.M.; Raijmakers, M.E.J. A
dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychol. Rev.
2006,113, 842–861. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
57.
McDaniel, M.A. Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between
in vivo
brain
volume and intelligence. Intelligence 2005,33, 337–346. [CrossRef]
58.
Goh, S.; Bansal, R.; Xu, D.; Hao, X.; Liu, J.; Peterson, B.S. Neuroanatomical correlates of intellectual ability
across the life span. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011,1, 305–312. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
59.
Narr, K.L.; Woods, R.P.; Thompson, P.M.; Szeszko, P.; Robinson, D.; Dimtcheva, T.; Gurbani, M.; Toga, A.W.;
Bilder, R.M. Relationships between IQ and regional cortical gray matter thickness in healthy adults.
Cereb. Cortex 2007,17, 2163–2171. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 21 of 26
60.
Penke, L.; Maniega, S.M.; Bastin, M.E.; Hernandez, M.C.V.; Murray, C.; Royle, N.A.; Starr, J.M.; Wardlaw, J.M.;
Deary, I.J. Brain white matter tract integrity as a neural foundation for general intelligence. Mol. Psychiatry
2012,17, 1026–1030. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
61.
Haier, R.J.; Siegel, B.V.; Nuechterlein, K.H.; Hazlett, E.; Wu, J.C.; Paek, J.; Browning, H.L.; Buchsbaum, M.S.
Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission
tomography. Intelligence 1988,12, 199–217. [CrossRef]
62.
van den Heuvel, M.P.; Stam, C.J.; Kahn, R.S.; Pol, H.E.H. Efficiency of functional brain networks and
intellectual performance. J. Neurosci. 2009,29, 7619–7624. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
63.
Reiss, A.L.; Abrams, M.T.; Singer, H.S.; Ross, J.L.; Denckla, M.B. Brain development, gender and IQ in
children. Brain 1996,119, 1763–1774. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
64.
Van Leeuwen, M.; Peper, J.S.; van den Berg, S.M.; Brouwer, R.M.; Pol, H.E.H.; Kahn, R.S.; Boomsma, D.I. A
genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children. Intelligence 2009,37, 181–191. [CrossRef]
65.
Karama, S.; Colom, R.; Johnson, W.; Deary, I.J.; Haier, R.; Waber, D.P.; Lepage, C.; Ganjavi, H.; Jung, R.;
Evans, A.C. Cortical thickness correlates of specific cognitive performance accounted for by the general
factor of intelligence in healthy children aged 6 to 18. Neuroimage
2011
,55, 1443–1453. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
66.
Menary, K.; Collins, P.F.; Porter, J.N.; Muetzel, R.; Olson, E.A.; Kumar, V.; Steinbach, M.; Lim, K.O.; Luciana, M.
Associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence in children, adolescents and young adults.
Intelligence 2013,41, 597–606. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
67.
Shaw, P.; Greenstein, D.; Lerch, J.; Clasen, L.; Lenroot, R.; Gogtay, N.; Evans, A.; Rapoport, J.; Giedd, J.
Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature
2006
,440, 676–679.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
68. Rakic, P. Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 1988,241, 170–176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
69.
Giedd, J.N.; Blumenthal, J.; Jeffries, N.O.; Castellanos, F.X.; Liu, H.; Zijdenbos, A.; Paus, T.; Evans, A.C.;
Rapoport, J.L. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study.
Nat. Neurosci. 1999,2, 861–863. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
70.
Raznahan, A.; Lerch, J.P.; Lee, N.; Greenstein, D.; Wallace, G.L.; Stockman, M.; Clasen, L.; Shaw, P.W.;
Giedd, J.N. Patterns of coordinated anatomical change in human cortical development: A longitudinal
neuroimaging study of maturational coupling. Neuron 2011,72, 873–884. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
71.
Schnack, H.G.; Van Haren, N.E.M.; Brouwer, R.M.; Evans, A.; Durston, S.; Boomsma, D.I.; Kahn, R.S.;
Pol, H.E.H. Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence.
Cereb. Cortex 2015,25, 1608–1617. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
72.
Ramsden, S.; Richardson, F.M.; Josse, G.; Thomas, M.S.C.; Ellis, C.; Shakeshaft, C.; Seghier, M.L.; Price, C.J.
Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature
2011
,479, 113–116. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
73.
Burgaleta, M.; Johnson, W.; Waber, D.P.; Colom, R.; Karama, S. Cognitive ability changes and dynamics of
cortical thickness development in healthy children and adolescents. Neuroimage
2014
,84, 810–819. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
74.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M. Inference, reconceptualization, sight, and efficiency along
intellectual growth: A general theory. Enfance 2014,2014, 365–396. [CrossRef]
75.
Demetriou, A.; Shayer, M.; Efklides, A. Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development: Implications and
Applications for Education; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2016.
76.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G. Cognitive Abilities and Educational Outcomes; Springer: New York, NY, USA,
2017; pp. 39–60.
77.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M. Mapping Mind-Brain Development. In Neurotechnology Direct
Brain Communication New Insights Responsibilities Concerning Speechless But Communicative Subjects; Routledge:
New York, NY, USA, 2016; Volume 21.
78.
Demetriou, A.; Kyriakides, L. The functional and developmental organization of cognitive developmental
sequences. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006,76, 209–242. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
79.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Mouyi, A. Educating the Developing Mind: Towards an Overarching
Paradigm. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2011,23, 601–663. [CrossRef]
80.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M.; Van der Ven, S.; Brydges, C.R.; Kroesbergen, E.; Podjarny, G.;
Swanson, H.L. Relations between speed, working memory, and intelligence from preschool to adulthood:
Structural equation modeling of 14 studies. Intelligence 2014,46, 107–121. [CrossRef]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 22 of 26
81.
Žebec, M.S.; Demetriou, A.; Kotrla-Topi´c, M. Changing expressions of general intelligence in development:
A 2-wave longitudinal study from 7 to 18years of age. Intelligence 2015,49, 94–109. [CrossRef]
82.
Spanoudis, G.; Demetriou, A.; Kazi, S.; Giorgala, K.; Zenonos, V. Embedding cognizance in intellectual
development. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015,132, 32–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
83.
Makris, N.; Tahmatzidis, D.; Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G. Mapping the evolving core of intelligence:
Changing relations between executive control, reasoning, language, and awareness. Intelligence
2017
.
[CrossRef]
84.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M. Developmental intelligence: From empirical to hidden constructs.
Intelligence 2013,41, 744–749. [CrossRef]
85.
Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M.; Mouyi, A.; Kazi, S.; Platsidou, M. Cycles in speed-working
memory-G relations: Towards a developmental–differential theory of the mind. Intelligence
2013
,41, 34–50.
[CrossRef]
86.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 1998,2, 389–398. [CrossRef]
87.
Sirois, S.; Spratling, M.; Thomas, M.S.C.; Westermann, G.; Mareschal, D.; Johnson, M.H. Précis of
neuroconstructivism: How the brain constructs cognition. Behav. Brain Sci.
2008
,31, 321–356. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
88.
Neubauer, A.C.; Fink, A. Intelligence and neural efficiency. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
2009
,33, 1004–1023.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
89.
Poldrack, R.A. Is “efficiency” a useful concept in cognitive neuroscience? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.
2015
,11,
12–17. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
90. Gottlieb, G. Probabilistic epigenesis. Dev. Sci. 2007,10, 1–11. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
91.
Quartz, S.R.; Sejnowski, T.J. The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behav.
Brain Sci. 1997,20, 537–556. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
92.
Johnson, M.H.; De Haan, M. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introduction; John Wiley & Sons:
Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015.
93. Clark, A. An embodied cognitive science? Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999,3, 345–351. [CrossRef]
94.
Smith, L.B. Cognition as a dynamic system: Principles from embodiment. Dev. Rev.
2005
,25, 278–298.
[CrossRef]
95.
Bradley, R.H.; Corwyn, R.F. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol.
2002
,53,
371–399. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
96.
Meltzoff, A.N.; Kuhl, P.K.; Movellan, J.; Sejnowski, T.J. Foundations for a New Science of Learning. Science
2009,325, 284–289. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
97.
Westermann, G.; Thomas, M.S.C.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. Neuroconstructivism. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook
of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2nd ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2010.
98.
Annaz, D.; Karmiloff-Smith, A.; Thomas, M.S.C. The importance of tracing developmental trajectories for
clinical child neuropsychology. In Child Neuropsychology Concepts, Theory Practice; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford,
UK, 2008.
99.
Goswami, U. Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA,
2008.
100.
Lickliter, R.; Honeycutt, H. Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary
psychology. Psychol. Bull. 2003,129, 819–835. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
101.
Karmiloff-Smith, A.; D’Souza, D.; Dekker, T.M.; Van Herwegen, J.; Xu, F.; Rodic, M.; Ansari, D. Genetic and
environmental vulnerabilities in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
2012,109 (Suppl. 2), 17261–17265. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
102.
Caspi, A.; Williams, B.; Kim-Cohen, J.; Craig, I.W.; Milne, B.J.; Poulton, R.; Schalkwyk, L.C.; Taylor, A.;
Werts, H.; Moffitt, T.E. Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid
metabolism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007,104, 18860–18865. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
103.
Plomin, R.; Deary, I.J. Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry
2015
,20,
98–108. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
104.
Butcher, L.M.; Davis, O.S.P.; Craig, I.W.; Plomin, R. Genome-wide quantitative trait locus association scan
of general cognitive ability using pooled DNA and 500K single nucleotide polymorphism microarrays.
Genes Brain Behav. 2008,7, 435–446. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 23 of 26
105.
Davies, G.; Tenesa, A.; Payton, A.; Yang, J.; Harris, S.E.; Liewald, D.; Ke, X.; Le Hellard, S.; Christoforou, A.;
Luciano, M. Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and
polygenic. Mol. Psychiatry 2011,16, 996–1005. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
106.
Briley, D.A.; Tucker-Drob, E.M. Genetic and environmental continuity in personality development: A
meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2014,140, 1303–1331. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
107.
Deary, I.J.; Yang, J.; Davies, G.; Harris, S.E.; Tenesa, A.; Liewald, D.; Luciano, M.; Lopez, L.M.; Gow, A.J.;
Corley, J. Genetic contributions to stability and change in intelligence from childhood to old age. Nature
2012,482, 212–215. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
108.
Trzaskowski, M.; Shakeshaft, N.G.; Plomin, R. Intelligence indexes generalist genes for cognitive abilities.
Intelligence 2013,41, 560–565. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
109.
Crone, E.A.; Elzinga, B.M. Changing brains: How longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging
studies can inform us about cognitive and social-affective growth trajectories. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci.
2015,6, 53–63. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
110.
Kingsbury, M.A.; Finlay, B.L. The cortex in multidimensional space: Where do cortical areas come from?
Dev. Sci. 2001,4, 125–142. [CrossRef]
111.
Haworth, C.M.A.; Wright, M.J.; Luciano, M.; Martin, N.G.; De Geus, E.J.C.; Van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M.;
Bartels, M.; Posthuma, D.; Boomsma, D.I.; Davis, O.S.P. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases
linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol. Psychiatry 2010,15, 1112–1120. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
112.
Bartels, M.; Rietveld, M.J.H.; Van Baal, G.C.M.; Boomsma, D.I. Genetic and environmental influences on the
development of intelligence. Behav. Genet. 2002,32, 237–249. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
113.
Brant, A.M.; Munakata, Y.; Boomsma, D.I.; DeFries, J.C.; Haworth, C.M.A.; Keller, M.C.; Martin, N.G.;
McGue, M.; Petrill, S.A.; Plomin, R. The nature and nurture of high IQ: An extended sensitive period for
intellectual development. Psychol. Sci. 2013,24, 1487–1495. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
114.
Trzaskowski, M.; Yang, J.; Visscher, P.M.; Plomin, R. DNA evidence for strong genetic stability and increasing
heritability of intelligence from age 7 to 12. Mol. Psychiatry 2014,19, 380–384. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
115.
Briley, D.A.; Tucker-Drob, E.M. Explaining the increasing heritability of cognitive ability across development:
A meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychol. Sci.
2013
,24, 1704–1713. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
116. Plomin, R. Development, Genetics, and Psychology; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1986.
117.
Thomas, M.S.C. Do more intelligent brains retain heightened plasticity for longer in development? A
computational investigation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016,19, 258–269. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
118.
Elman, J.L. Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA,
USA, 1998; Volume 10.
119.
Sale, A.; Berardi, N.; Maffei, L. Enrich the environment to empower the brain. Trends Neurosci.
2009
,32,
233–239. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
120. Brooks-Gunn, J.; Duncan, G.J. The effects of poverty on children. Futur. Child. 1997,7, 55–71. [CrossRef]
121.
Gottfried, A.W.; Gottfried, A.E.; Bathurst, K.; Guerin, D.W.; Parramore, M.M. Socioeconomic Status in
Children
'
s Development and Family Environment: Infancy through Adolescence. In Socioeconomic Status,
Parenting and Child Development; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2003.
122.
Farah, M.J.; Shera, D.M.; Savage, J.H.; Betancourt, L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Malmud, E.K.; Hurt, H.
Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res.
2006
,1110, 166–174.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
123.
Tomalski, P.; Moore, D.G.; Ribeiro, H.; Axelsson, E.L.; Murphy, E.; Karmiloff-Smith, A.; Johnson, M.H.;
Kushnerenko, E. Socioeconomic status and functional brain development–associations in early infancy.
Dev. Sci. 2013,16, 676–687. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
124.
Noble, K.G.; Norman, M.F.; Farah, M.J. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten
children. Dev. Sci. 2005,8, 74–87. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
125.
Rao, H.; Betancourt, L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Korczykowski, M.; Avants, B.B.; Gee, J.C.; Wang, J.;
Hurt, H.; Detre, J.A. Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: Evidence from brain
morphology in humans. Neuroimage 2010,49, 1144–1150. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
126.
Piccolo, L.R.; Merz, E.C.; He, X.; Sowell, E.R.; Noble, K.G. Age-Related Differences in Cortical Thickness
Vary by Socioeconomic Status. PLoS ONE 2016,11, e0162511. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 24 of 26
127.
Noble, K.G.; Houston, S.M.; Brito, N.H.; Bartsch, H.; Kan, E.; Kuperman, J.M.; Akshoomoff, N.; Amaral, D.G.;
Bloss, C.S.; Libiger, O. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents.
Nat. Neurosci. 2015,18, 773–778. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
128.
Noble, K.G.; Houston, S.M.; Kan, E.; Sowell, E.R. Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing
human brain. Dev. Sci. 2012,15, 516–527. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
129.
Lawson, G.M.; Duda, J.T.; Avants, B.B.; Wu, J.; Farah, M.J. Associations between children’s socioeconomic
status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Dev. Sci. 2013,16, 641–652. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
130.
Ursache, A.; Noble, K.G. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and
implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 2016,53, 71–82. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
131.
Hackman, D.A.; Farah, M.J.; Meaney, M.J. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from
human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010,11, 651–659. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
132. Evans, G. The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 2004,59, 77–92. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
133.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science; MIT Press:
Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995.
134.
Johnson, M.H. Functional brain development in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
2001
,2, 475–483. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
135.
Johnson, M.H. Interactive specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain
development? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011,1, 7–21. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
136.
Kim, D.-J.; Davis, E.P.; Sandman, C.A.; Sporns, O.; O’Donnell, B.F.; Buss, C.; Hetrick, W.P. Children’s
intellectual ability is associated with structural network integrity. Neuroimage
2016
,124, 550–556. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
137.
Khundrakpam, B.S.; Lewis, J.D.; Reid, A.; Karama, S.; Zhao, L.; Chouinard-Decorte, F.; Evans, A.C. Brain
Development Cooperative Group. Imaging structural covariance in the development of intelligence.
Neuroimage 2017,144, 227–240. [PubMed]
138.
Song, M.; Zhou, Y.; Li, J.; Liu, Y.; Tian, L.; Yu, C.; Jiang, T. Brain spontaneous functional connectivity and
intelligence. Neuroimage 2008,41, 1168–1176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
139.
Langeslag, S.J.E.; Schmidt, M.; Ghassabian, A.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Hofman, A.; Lugt, A.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H.;
White, T.J.H. Functional connectivity between parietal and frontal brain regions and intelligence in young
children: The Generation R study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013,34, 3299–3307. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
140.
Fjell, A.M.; Westlye, L.T.; Amlien, I.; Tamnes, C.K.; Grydeland, H.; Engvig, A.; Espeseth, T.; Reinvang, I.;
Lundervold, A.J.; Lundervold, A. High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are
related to higher intellectual abilities. Cereb. Cortex 2015,25, 26–34. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
141. Barsalou, L.W. Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008,59, 617–645. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
142.
Fagan, J.F.; McGrath, S.K. Infant recognition memory and later intelligence. Intelligence
1981
,5, 121–130.
[CrossRef]
143.
Lewis, M.; Brooks-Gunn, J. Visual attention at three months as a predictor of cognitive functioning at two
years of age. Intelligence 1981,5, 131–140. [CrossRef]
144. McCall, R.B. Early predictors of later IQ: The search continues. Intelligence 1981,5, 141–147. [CrossRef]
145.
Kavšek, M. Predicting later IQ from infant visual habituation and dishabituation: A meta-analysis. J. Appl.
Dev. Psychol. 2004,25, 369–393. [CrossRef]
146.
Fagan, J.F.; Holland, C.R.; Wheeler, K. The prediction, from infancy, of adult IQ and achievement. Intelligence
2007,35, 225–231. [CrossRef]
147.
Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Jankowski, J.J.; Van Rossem, R. Information processing from infancy to 11years:
Continuities and prediction of IQ. Intelligence 2012,40, 445–457. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
148.
Scerif, G. Attention trajectories, mechanisms and outcomes: At the interface between developing cognition
and environment. Dev. Sci. 2010,13, 805–812. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
149.
Bornstein, M.H. How infant and mother jointly contribute to developing cognitive competence in the child.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1985,82, 7470–7473. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
150.
Murray, G.K.; Jones, P.B.; Kuh, D.; Richards, M. Infant developmental milestones and subsequent cognitive
function. Ann. Neurol. 2007,62, 128–136. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
151.
Hamadani, J.D.; Tofail, F.; Cole, T.; Grantham-McGregor, S. The relation between age of attainment of motor
milestones and future cognitive and motor development in Bangladeshi children. Matern. Child Nutr.
2013
,
9, 89–104. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 25 of 26
152.
Capute, A.J.; Shapiro, B.K.; Palmer, F.B.; Ross, A.; Wachtel, R.C. Cognitive-Motor Interactions The
Relationship of Infant Gross Motor Attainment to IQ at 3 Years. Clin. Pediatr. (Phila).
1985
,24, 671–675.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
153.
Piek, J.P.; Dawson, L.; Smith, L.M.; Gasson, N. The role of early fine and gross motor development on later
motor and cognitive ability. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008,27, 668–681. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
154.
Taanila, A.; Murray, G.K.; Jokelainen, J.; Isohanni, M.; Rantakallio, P. Infant developmental milestones: A
31-year follow-up. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005,47, 581–586. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
155.
Ridler, K.; Veijola, J.M.; Tanskanen, P.; Miettunen, J.; Chitnis, X.; Suckling, J.; Murray, G.K.; Haapea, M.;
Jones, P.B.; Isohanni, M.K. Fronto-cerebellar systems are associated with infant motor and adult executive
functions in healthy adults but not in schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
2006
,103, 15651–15656.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
156.
Flensborg-Madsen, T.; Mortensen, E.L. Infant developmental milestones and adult intelligence: A 34-year
follow-up. Early Hum. Dev. 2015,91, 393–400. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
157.
Rigoli, D.; Piek, J.P.; Kane, R.; Oosterlaan, J. Motor coordination, working memory, and academic achievement
in a normative adolescent sample: Testing a mediation model. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.
2012
,27, 766–780.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
158.
Rigoli, D.; Piek, J.P.; Kane, R.; Oosterlaan, J. An examination of the relationship between motor coordination
and executive functions in adolescents. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012,54, 1025–1031. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
159.
Tabriz, A.A.; Sohrabi, M.R.; Parsay, S.; Abadi, A.; Kiapour, N.; Aliyari, M.; Ahmadi, F.; Roodaki, A. Relation
of intelligence quotient and body mass index in preschool children: A community-based cross-sectional
study. Nutr. Diabetes 2015,5, e176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
160.
Lemos, G.C.; Almeida, L.S.; Colom, R. Intelligence of adolescents is related to their parents’ educational level
but not to family income. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2011,50, 1062–1067. [CrossRef]
161.
Ganzach, Y. Adolescents’ intelligence is related to family income. Pers. Individ. Dif.
2014
,59, 112–115.
[CrossRef]
162.
Karmiloff-Smith, A.; Aschersleben, G.; de Schonen, S.; Elsabbagh, M.; Hohenberger, A.; Serres, J. Constraints
on the timing of infant cognitive change: Domain-specific or domain-general? Eur. J. Dev. Sci.
2010
,4, 31–45.
163.
De Bellis, M.D.; Hooper, S.R.; Spratt, E.G.; Woolley, D.P. Neuropsychological findings in childhood neglect
and their relationships to pediatric PTSD. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009,15, 868–878. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
164.
Hay, D.F.; Kumar, R. Interpreting the effects of mothers’ postnatal depression on children’s intelligence: A
critique and re-analysis. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 1995,25, 165–181. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
165.
Blair, C.; Diamond, A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation
as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008,20, 899–911. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
166.
Neville, H.J.; Stevens, C.; Pakulak, E.; Bell, T.A.; Fanning, J.; Klein, S.; Isbell, E. Family-based training
program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013,110, 12138–12143. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
167.
Pietschnig, J.; Penke, L.; Wicherts, J.M.; Zeiler, M.; Voracek, M. Meta-analysis of associations between human
brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 2015,57, 411–432. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
168.
Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Briley, D.A. Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across
the life span: A meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychol. Bull.
2014
,140, 949–979.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
169.
Plomin, R.; DeFries, J.C.; Loehlin, J.C. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of
human behavior. Psychol. Bull. 1977,84, 309–322. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
170.
Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Harden, K.P. Intellectual Interest Mediates GenexSocioeconomic Status Interaction on
Adolescent Academic Achievement. Child Dev. 2012,83, 743–757. [PubMed]
171.
Tucker-Drob, E.M.; Briley, D.A.; Engelhardt, L.E.; Mann, F.D.; Harden, K.P. Genetically-Mediated Associations
Between Measures of Childhood Character and Academic Achievement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
2016
,111,
790–815. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
172.
Kievit, R.A.; Davis, S.W.; Griffiths, J.; Correia, M.M.; Henson, R.N. A watershed model of individual
differences in fluid intelligence. Neuropsychologia 2016,91, 186–198. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
173.
Strenze, T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research.
Intelligence 2007,35, 401–426. [CrossRef]
J. Intell. 2017,5, 18 26 of 26
174. Gottfredson, L.S. Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 1997,24, 79–132. [CrossRef]
175.
Wraw, C.; Deary, I.J.; Gale, C.R.; Der, G. Intelligence in youth and health at age 50. Intelligence
2015
,53, 23–32.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
176.
McGurn, B.; Deary, I.J.; Starr, J.M. Childhood cognitive ability and risk of late-onset Alzheimer and vascular
dementia. Neurology 2008,71, 1051–1056. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
177.
Koenen, K.C.; Moffitt, T.E.; Roberts, A.L.; Martin, L.T.; Kubzansky, L.; Harrington, H.; Poulton, R.; Caspi, A.
Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: A test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am. J. Psychiatry
2009
,
166, 50–57. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
178.
Lubinski, D.; Humphreys, L.G. Incorporating general intelligence into epidemiology and the social sciences.
Intelligence 1997,24, 159–201. [CrossRef]
179.
Ceci, S.J. How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A
reassessment of the evidence. Dev. Psychol. 1991,27, 703–722. [CrossRef]
180.
Cahan, S.; Cohen, N. Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Dev.
1989
,60, 1239–1249.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
181.
Bedard, K.; Dhuey, E. The persistence of early childhood maturity: International evidence of long-run age
effects. Q. J. Econ. 2006,121, 1437–1472. [CrossRef]
182.
Jones, C.; Tarpley, P.L.; Blancero, D. Handbook of Intelligence; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 459–468.
183.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Transforming a partially structured brain into a creative mind. Behav. Brain Sci.
1994
,17,
732–745. [CrossRef]
©
2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... Despite the absence of a consensus definition, "intelligence" can be defined based on the ability to comprehend complex concepts, adapt to varying conditions, learn from experience, employ reasoning, and to handle challenges using cognitive processes [35]. Thus, intelligence among humans is recognized as a dynamic, multi-dimensional construct which can be viewed as a broad reflection of human cognitive abilities [36]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The introduction and rapid evolution of generative artificial intelligence (genAI) models necessitates a refined understanding for the concept of “intelligence”. The genAI tools are known for its capability to produce complex, creative, and contextually relevant output. Nevertheless, the deployment of genAI models in healthcare should be accompanied appropriate and rigorous performance evaluation tools. In this rapid communication, we emphasizes the urgent need to develop a “Generative AIQ Test” as a novel tailored tool for comprehensive benchmarking of genAI models against multiple human-like intelligence attributes. A preliminary framework is proposed in this communication. This framework incorporates miscellaneous performance metrics including accuracy, diversity, novelty, and consistency. These metrics were considered critical in the evaluation of genAI models that might be utilized to generate diagnostic recommendations, treatment plans, and patient interaction suggestions. This communication also highlights the importance of orchestrated collaboration to construct robust and well-annotated benchmarking datasets to capture the complexity of diverse medical scenarios and patient demographics. This communication suggests an approach aiming to ensure that genAI models are effective, equitable, and transparent. To maximize the potential of genAI models in healthcare, it is important to establish rigorous, dynamic standards for its benchmarking. Consequently, this approach can help to improve clinical decision-making with enhancement in patient care, which will enhance the reliability of genAI applications in healthcare.
... However, unlike adults, the ongoing developmental processes represent a challenge when investigating cognitive performance in the pediatric population. Although adult intelligence is viewed as a stable trait, intelligence research has profoundly advanced in recent times, pointing to more dynamic mechanisms undergoing extensive developmental changes (179). There are age differences in the development of fluid and crystallized intelligence, with fluid peaking earlier in life than crystallized (146,180) and young children possess less established skills and knowledge. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Pediatric acquired brain injury (pABI) profoundly affects cognitive functions, encompassing IQ and executive functions (EFs). Particularly, young age at insult may lead to persistent and debilitating deficits, affecting daily-life functioning negatively. This study delves into the intricate interplay of age at insult, time post-insult, and their associations with IQ and EFs during chronic (>1 year) pABI. Additionally, we investigate cognitive performance across different levels of global function, recognizing the multifaceted nature of developmental factors influencing outcomes. Methods Drawing upon insult data and baseline information analyzing secondary outcomes from a multicenter RCT, including comprehensive medical and neuropsychological assessments of participants aged 10 to 17 years with pABI and parent-reported executive dysfunctions. The study examined associations between age at insult (early, EI; ≤7y vs. late, LI; > 7y) and time post-insult with IQ and EFs (updating, shifting, inhibition, and executive attention). Additionally, utilizing the Pediatric Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended, we explored cognitive performance across levels of global functioning. Results Seventy-six participants, median 8 years at insult and 5 years post-insult, predominantly exhibiting moderate disability (n = 38), were included. Notably, participants with LI demonstrated superior IQ, executive attention, and shifting compared to EI, [adjusted mean differences with 95% Confidence Intervals (CIs); 7.9 (1.4, 14.4), 2.48 (0.71, 4.24) and 1.73 (0.03, 3.43), respectively]. Conversely, extended post-insult duration was associated with diminished performances, evident in mean differences with 95% CIs for IQ, updating, shifting, and executive attention compared to 1–2 years post-insult [−11.1 (−20.4, −1.7), −8.4 (−16.7, −0.1), −2.6 (−4.4, −0.7), −2.9 (−4.5, −1.2), −3.8 (−6.4, −1.3), −2.6 (−5.0, −0.3), and −3.2 (−5.7, −0.8)]. Global function exhibited a robust relationship with IQ and EFs. Conclusion Early insults and prolonged post-insult durations impose lasting tribulations in chronic pABI. While confirmation through larger studies is needed, these findings carry clinical implications, underscoring the importance of vigilance regarding early insults. Moreover, they dispel the notion that children fully recover from pABI; instead, they advocate equitable rehabilitation offerings for pABI, tailored to address cognitive functions, recognizing their pivotal role in achieving independence and participation in society. Incorporating disability screening in long-term follow-up assessments may prove beneficial.
... Intellectual functioning, as measured by IQ, is predictive of global functioning across multiple domains, including academic achievement and quality of life. 1 Concussion (ie, mild traumatic brain injury [TBI]) is highly prevalent, affecting millions of children in North America each year. 2 In contrast to early and persistent intellectual impairment after moderate to severe TBI in children, [3][4][5][6][7] the effect of concussion on IQ is unclear. 7 Evidence for IQ differences after pediatric concussion is mixed in individual studies. ...
Article
d on behalf of the Pediatric Emergency Research Canada A-CAP Study Group* abstract OBJECTIVES: This study investigated IQ scores in pediatric concussion (ie, mild traumatic brain injury) versus orthopedic injury. METHODS: Children (N 5 866; aged 8-16.99 years) were recruited for 2 prospective cohort studies from emergency departments at children's hospitals (2 sites in the United States and 5 in Canada) #48 hours after sustaining a concussion or orthopedic injury. They completed IQ and performance validity testing postacutely (3-18 days postinjury; United States) or 3 months postinjury (Canada). Group differences in IQ scores were examined using 3 complementary statistical approaches (linear modeling, Bayesian, and multigroup factor analysis) in children performing above cutoffs on validity testing. RESULTS: Linear models showed small group differences in full-scale IQ (d [95% confidence in-terval] 5 0.13 [0.00-0.26]) and matrix reasoning (0.16 [0.03-0.30]), but not in vocabulary scores. IQ scores were not related to previous concussion, acute clinical features, injury mechanism , a validated clinical risk score, pre-or postinjury symptom ratings, litigation, or symptomatic status at 1 month postinjury. Bayesian models provided moderate to very strong evidence against group differences in IQ scores (Bayes factor 0.02-0.23). Multigroup factor analysis further demonstrated strict measurement invariance, indicating group equivalence in factor structure of the IQ test and latent variable means. CONCLUSIONS: Across multisite, prospective study cohorts, 3 complementary statistical models provided no evidence of clinically meaningful differences in IQ scores after pediatric concussion. Instead, overall results provided strong evidence against reduced intelligence in the first few weeks to months after pediatric concussion. WHAT'S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Pediatric concussion (ie, mild traumatic brain injury) is highly prevalent, but whether concussion results in lower IQ scores is controversial. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: This study revealed no evidence of clinically meaningful differences in IQ scores postacutely (ie, 2 weeks) or at 3 months post-injury between pediatric concussion and mild orthopedic injury, suggesting that pediatric concussions do not alter IQ.
... Intellectual functioning, as measured by IQ, is predictive of global functioning across multiple domains, including academic achievement and quality of life. 1 Concussion (ie, mild traumatic brain injury [TBI]) is highly prevalent, affecting millions of children in North America each year. 2 In contrast to early and persistent intellectual impairment after moderate to severe TBI in children, [3][4][5][6][7] the effect of concussion on IQ is unclear. 7 Evidence for IQ differences after pediatric concussion is mixed in individual studies. ...
Article
Objectives: This study investigated IQ scores in pediatric concussion (ie, mild traumatic brain injury) versus orthopedic injury. Methods: Children (N = 866; aged 8-16.99 years) were recruited for 2 prospective cohort studies from emergency departments at children's hospitals (2 sites in the United States and 5 in Canada) ≤48 hours after sustaining a concussion or orthopedic injury. They completed IQ and performance validity testing postacutely (3-18 days postinjury; United States) or 3 months postinjury (Canada). Group differences in IQ scores were examined using 3 complementary statistical approaches (linear modeling, Bayesian, and multigroup factor analysis) in children performing above cutoffs on validity testing. Results: Linear models showed small group differences in full-scale IQ (d [95% confidence interval] = 0.13 [0.00-0.26]) and matrix reasoning (0.16 [0.03-0.30]), but not in vocabulary scores. IQ scores were not related to previous concussion, acute clinical features, injury mechanism, a validated clinical risk score, pre- or postinjury symptom ratings, litigation, or symptomatic status at 1 month postinjury. Bayesian models provided moderate to very strong evidence against group differences in IQ scores (Bayes factor 0.02-0.23). Multigroup factor analysis further demonstrated strict measurement invariance, indicating group equivalence in factor structure of the IQ test and latent variable means. Conclusions: Across multisite, prospective study cohorts, 3 complementary statistical models provided no evidence of clinically meaningful differences in IQ scores after pediatric concussion. Instead, overall results provided strong evidence against reduced intelligence in the first few weeks to months after pediatric concussion.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive abilities, including general intelligence and domain-specific abilities such as fluid reasoning, comprehension knowledge, working memory capacity, and processing speed, are regarded as some of the most stable psychological traits, yet there exist no large-scale systematic efforts to document the specific patterns by which their rank-order stability changes over age and time interval, or how their stability differs across abilities, tests, and populations. Determining the conditions under which cognitive abilities exhibit high or low degrees of stability is critical not just to theory development but to applied contexts in which cognitive assessments guide decisions regarding treatment and intervention decisions with lasting consequences for individuals. In order to supplement this important area of research, we present a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies investigating the stability of cognitive abilities. The meta-analysis relied on data from 205 longitudinal studies that involved a total of 87,408 participants, resulting in 1,288 test–retest correlation coefficients among manifest variables. For an age of 20 years and a test–retest interval of 5 years, we found a mean rank-order stability of ρ = .76. The effect of mean sample age on stability was best described by a negative exponential function, with low stability in preschool children, rapid increases in stability in childhood, and consistently high stability from late adolescence to late adulthood. This same functional form continued to best describe age trends in stability after adjusting for test reliability. Stability declined with increasing test–retest interval. This decrease flattened out from an interval of approximately 5 years onward. According to the age and interval moderation models, minimum stability sufficient for individual-level diagnostic decisions (rtt = .80) can only be expected over the age of 7 and for short time intervals in children. In adults, stability levels meeting this criterion are obtained for over 5 years.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to explore the prospect of using neurophenomenology to understand, design and test phygital consumer experiences. It aims to clarify interpretivist approaches to consumer neuroscience, wherein theoretical models of individual phenomenology can be combined with modern neuroimaging techniques to detect and interpret the first-person accounts of phygital experiences. Design/methodology/approach The argument is conceptual in nature, building its position through synthesizing insights from phenomenology, phygital marketing, theoretical neuroscience and other related fields. Findings Ultimately, the paper presents the argument that interpretivist neuroscience in general, and neurophenomenology specifically, provides a valuable new perspective on phygital marketing experiences. In particular, we argue that the approach to studying first-personal experiences within the phygital domain can be significantly refined by adopting this perspective. Research limitations/implications One of the primary goals of this paper is to stimulate a novel approach to interpretivist phygital research, and in doing so, provide a foundation by which the impact of phygital interventions can be empirically tested through neuroscience, and through which future research into this topic can be developed. As such, the success of such an approach is yet untested. Originality/value Phygital marketing is distinguished by its focus on the quality of subjective first-personal consumer experiences, but few papers to date have explored how neuroscience can be used as a tool for exploring these inner landscapes. This paper addresses this lacuna by providing a novel perspective on “interpretivist neuroscience” and proposes ways that current neuroscientific models can be used as a practical methodology for addressing these questions.
Article
El objetivo de este trabajo consiste en identificar guiones culturalmente relevantes en la narrativa materna lúdica que permitan evidenciar posibles intenciones de las madres y derivar supuestos acerca del efecto de las narrativas maternas en la socialización temprana. Se realizó un microanálisis inductivo de interacciones que codificó las secuencias del juego naturalista de ocho díadas a las seis semanas, seis meses y un año del bebé. La Teoría Fundamentada revela los patrones más frecuentes. Se concluye que las narrativas maternas que se articulan con objetos cotidianos apuntan a significadosculturales para hacerlos accesibles al infante, al apelar a guiones cotidianos y así conformar orientaciones sociales.
Article
Full-text available
Two issues that complicate behavioral genetic analyses are the interaction and correlation between genetic and environmental influences. In the present report, the effects of genotype-environment interaction and correlation on behavioral genetic studies (twin and adoption studies) are examined. The analysis suggests that genotype-environment interaction may bias twin study estimates of genetic and environmental influence but need not affect adoption studies. On the other hand, genotype-environment correlation may affect both twin and adoption study estimates of genetic and environmental influence, the direction of the effect depending on the sign of the correlation. New tests of genotype-environment interaction and correlation, using adoption data, are proposed. (27 ref)
Article
Full-text available
Fluid intelligence is a crucial cognitive ability that predicts key life outcomes across the lifespan. Strong empirical links exist between fluid intelligence and processing speed on the one hand, and white matter integrity and processing speed on the other. We propose a watershed model that integrates these three explanatory levels in a principled manner in a single statistical model, with processing speed and white matter figuring as intermediate endophenotypes. We fit this model in a large (N=555) adult lifespan cohort from the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) using multiple measures of processing speed, white matter health and fluid intelligence. The model fit the data well, outperforming competing models and providing evidence for a many-to-one mapping between white matter integrity, processing speed and fluid intelligence. The model can be naturally extended to integrate other cognitive domains, endophenotypes and genotypes.
Article
Full-text available
Recent findings indicate robust associations between socioeconomic status (SES) and brain structure in children, raising questions about the ways in which SES may modify structural brain development. In general, cortical thickness and surface area develop in nonlinear patterns across childhood and adolescence, with developmental patterns varying to some degree by cortical region. Here, we examined whether age-related nonlinear changes in cortical thickness and surface area varied by SES, as indexed by family income and parental education. We hypothesized that SES disparities in age-related change may be particularly evident for language-and literacy-supporting cortical regions. Participants were 1148 typically-developing individuals between 3 and 20 years of age. Results indicated that SES factors moderate patterns of age-associated change in cortical thickness but not surface area. Specifically, at lower levels of SES, associations between age and cortical thickness were curvilinear, with relatively steep age-related decreases in cortical thickness earlier in childhood, and subsequent leveling off during adolescence. In contrast, at high levels of SES, associations between age and cortical thickness were linear, with consistent reductions across the age range studied. Notably, this interaction was prominent in the left fusi-form gyrus, a region that is critical for reading development. In a similar pattern, SES factors significantly moderated linear age-related change in left superior temporal gyrus, such that higher SES was linked with steeper age-related decreases in cortical thickness in this region. These findings suggest that SES may moderate patterns of age-related cortical thinning , especially in language-and literacy-supporting cortical regions.
Book
Taking a stand midway between Piaget's constructivism and Fodor's nativism, Annette Karmiloff-Smith offers an exciting new theory of developmental change that embraces both approaches. She shows how each can enrich the other and how both are necessary to a fundamental theory of human cognition. Karmiloff-Smith shifts the focus from what cognitive science can offer the study of development to what a developmental perspective can offer cognitive science. In Beyond Modularity she treats cognitive development as a serious theoretical tool, presenting a coherent portrait of the flexibility and creativity of the human mind as it develops from infancy to middle childhood. Language, physics, mathematics, commonsense psychology, drawing, and writing are explored in terms of the relationship between the innate capacities of the human mind and subsequent representational change which allows for such flexibility and creativity. Karmiloff-Smith also takes up the issue of the extent to which development involves domain-specific versus domain-general processes. She concludes with discussions of nativism and domain specificity in relation to Piagetian theory and connectionism, and shows how a developmental perspective can pinpoint what is missing from connectionist models of the mind. Bradford Books imprint
Article
We explored relations between attention control, shifting flexibility, working memory, reasoning in different domains, awareness about reasoning, and language from 9 to 15 years of age. For this aim 198 9-, 11-, 13-, and 15-years old participants were examined with tasks addressed to all processes. All processes developed systematically throughout the period studied. Structural equation modeling revealed a powerful common construct underlying reasoning and language processes. All domain-specific cognitive, language, or awareness processes represented this common factor equally well. This factor was related to attention control, shifting flexibility, and working memory but this relation varied with development, being dominated by attention control at 9-11, inferential-representational processes at 11-13, and awareness-symbolic processes at 13-15. Piecewise linear modeling showed that transition points between phases are marked by phase-dominating processes. Modeling ability and age differentiation with increasing g suggested some ability differentiation at the end of developmental cycles suggesting that g-ability relations are re-worked anew in successive developmental cycles. Implications for developmental, cognitive, and brain science are discussed. Acknowledgement: This paper is based on a study designed and monitored by the
Article
Test‐retest reliability data gathered from 79 sources (34 separate studies) were analyzed by a multiple‐regression method in an attempt to estimate the effects of several factors on the temporal stability of individually tested intelligence. Five intelligence tests were examined: the Standford‐Binet (except the fourth edition), the WISC, the WISC‐R, the WAIS, and the WAIS‐R. Samples encompassed a wide range of subjects divergent on status, age, and sample size. Subject age and status, gender, and test‐retest interval were evaluated, and age and interval were found to be significant predictors of reliability. Subject sex and specific instrument were not found to have a significant effect on reliability. A summary table provides expected reliability coefficients, standard error, and percent of persons with IQ change in excess of 15 points, tabulated for combinations of each of the two predictors.