ArticlePDF Available

When Feelings Lack Precision: Low Positive and Negative Emotion Differentiation and Depressive Symptoms in Daily Life

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Research suggests the ability to differentiate discrete emotions protects against psychopathology. Little is known about daily processes through which negative and positive emotion differentiation (NED, PED) influence depressive symptomatology. We examined NED and PED as moderators of associations between daily processes (negative/positive experiences, brooding, and savoring) and daily depressive symptoms. Hypotheses were tested using intensive longitudinal techniques in two samples oversampled for depression: 157 young adults (Study 1) and 50 veterans recruited from VA primary care (Study 2). In Study 1, low NED predicted stronger associations between daily brooding and depressive symptoms. In Study 2, low NED predicted stronger reactivity to daily negative events. In both studies, low PED strengthened salutary effects of positive experiences and savoring on symptoms. Largely consistent across demographically divergent samples, results suggest both low NED and PED enhance effects of daily events and pers...
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running Head: EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
1
When Feelings Lack Precision: Low Positive and Negative Emotion Differentiation and
Depressive Symptoms in Daily Life
Lisa R. Starr1
Rachel Hershenberg2
Y. Irina Li1
Zoey A. Shaw1
1University of Rochester
2Emory University
To appear in Clinical Psychological Science. Accepted for publication January 26, 2017.
Published Online First on April 30, 2017. For the publisher’s version of this manuscript, please
see http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/2167702617694657
Correspondence can be directed to Lisa R. Starr, Ph.D., Department of Clinical and Social
Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, 491 Meliora Hall, P.O. Box 270266,
Rochester, NY 14627-0266. Telephone: (585) 276-6862, Fax: (585) 273-1100, Email:
lisa.starr@rochester.edu
KEY WORDS: negative emotion differentiation, positive emotion differentiation, depression,
brooding, savoring, ecological momentary assessment, daily diary
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
2
Abstract
Research suggests the ability to differentiate discrete emotions protects against
psychopathology. Little is known about daily processes through which negative and positive
emotion differentiation (NED, PED) influence depressive symptomatology. We examined NED
and PED as moderators of associations between daily processes (negative/ positive experiences,
brooding, and savoring) and daily depressive symptoms. Hypotheses were tested using intensive
longitudinal techniques in two samples oversampled for depression: 157 young adults (Study 1)
and 50 Veterans recruited from VA primary care (Study 2). In Study 1, low NED predicted
stronger associations between daily brooding and depressive symptoms. In Study 2, low NED
predicted stronger reactivity to daily negative events. In both studies, low PED strengthened
salutary effects of positive experiences and savoring on symptoms. Largely consistent across
demographically divergent samples, results suggest both low NED and PED enhance effects of
daily events and perseverative self-focus on fluctuations in depressive symptoms.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
3
When Feelings Lack Precision: Low Positive and Negative Emotion Differentiation and
Depressive Symptoms in Daily Life
Emotion differentiation (ED) refers to the ability to identify and precisely label discrete
emotional states (Barrett, Gross, Christensen, & Benvenuto, 2001; Kashdan, Barrett, &
McKnight, 2015). Those who are low on this ability tend to report their emotions in broad terms
of valence (“I feel good,” or “I feel upset”) rather than pinpointing concrete emotions (“I feel
excited,” or “I feel afraid”). Increasing research suggests that ED has consequences for
psychopathology and well-being. Emotions communicate critical information about the need to
employ attentional and behavioral resources. Thus, when people are better able to discriminate
between discrete emotional states they are more prepared to extract relevant information about
the causes and consequences of their emotions, such as the eliciting context, cognitive and
physiological correlates, and behavioral urges. This awareness also provides information needed
to effectively select and deploy appropriate emotion regulation strategies (Barrett et al., 2001).
Supporting this theoretical model, ED is associated with less impulsive emotional responding
and more effective use of emotion regulation strategies (Barrett et al., 2001; Kashdan et al.,
2015; Tugade, Fredrickson, & Barrett, 2004).
Given that appropriately experiencing and flexibly expressing emotions is central to well-
being and represents a transdiagnostic process in psychopathology (Kring & Sloan, 2011), it is
not surprising that difficulty discriminating between concrete emotional states that is, low
EDhas been linked to critical processes and behaviors across a wide range of disorders,
including substance abuse, eating disorders, borderline personality disorder, autism spectrum
disorder, and social anxiety disorder (Dixon-Gordon, Chapman, Weiss, & Rosenthal, 2014;
Erbas, Ceulemans, Boonen, Noens, & Kuppens, 2013; Kashdan & Farmer, 2014; Kashdan,
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
4
Ferssizidis, Collins, & Muraven, 2010; O’Toole, Jensen, Fentz, Zachariae, & Hougaard, 2014;
Selby et al., 2013; Zaki, Coifman, Rafaeli, Berenson, & Downey, 2013). Some evidence also
specifically links poor differentiation of negative emotions [NEs] (referred to as Negative
Emotion Differentiation [NED]) to depression, including self-reported depressive symptoms,
major depressive disorder (MDD), and symptom severity within depressive episodes (Demiralp
et al., 2012; Erbas, Ceulemans, Lee Pe, Koval, & Kuppens, 2014; Golston, Gara, & Woolfolk,
1992). This growing body of work suggests that NED could be an important contributor to
depression’s etiology and maintenance. However, research on ED and depression remains
limited, and no research has teased apart specific daily processes through which ED influences
depressive symptoms.
Most research on ED has focused on NED. A much smaller set of studies has explored
positive emotion differentiation (PED), perhaps because the limited existing research suggests
that compared to NED, PED is less consistently linked to emotion regulation deficits and well-
being (Barrett et al., 2001; Demiralp et al., 2012; Kashdan & Farmer, 2014; Pond et al., 2012).
NE and positive emotion (PE) serve different purposes; whereas PEs build long-term resources
and broaden one’s response repertoire (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001), NEs primarily function to
allocate resources to avoid or mitigate immediate threats (Parrott, 2002). Thus, failure to regulate
PEs may be much less costly than failure to regulate NEs (Barrett et al., 2001; Quigley & Barrett,
1999). That said, several studies have suggested that PED does play a role in psychopathological
processes and coping behaviors (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014; Selby et al., 2013; Tugade et al.,
2004). However, PED has rarely been explored in the context of depression, despite the fact that
there has been a sharpening focus on the role of PEs and rewarding experiences within the
depression literature (Treadway & Zald, 2011). Indeed, the absence of PEs and failure to
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
5
anticipate, seek, and benefit from positive experiences are increasingly considered a central and
perhaps defining feature of depression (Kovacs et al., 2016; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Rottenberg,
2007; Treadway & Zald, 2011; Watson & Naragon-Gainey, 2010; Weinberg, Liu, Hajcak, &
Shankman, 2015). Although one prior study failed to document a basic association between PED
and MDD (Demiralp et al., 2012), PED may still influence critical daily processes that influence
depressive symptoms.
The limited previous research has assumed that, like low NED, low PED would confer
risk for maladaptive outcomes (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014; Hill & Updegraff, 2012; Selby et al.,
2013; Tugade et al., 2004). This assumption is based on the logic that the ability to perceive
emotions in a more sophisticated, granular manner is adaptive, regardless of whether the
emotions are positive (high PED) or negative (high NED). However, it is also possible that
excessively differentiating between positive emotional states may lead to a more constrained,
narrower experience of positive emotions. As a result, high PED may make some behaviors and
experiences less emotionally rewarding. On the one hand, this may reduce risk for maladaptive
behaviors that are typically reinforced by emotional rewards; supporting this notion, high PED
has been associated with a reduced rate of self-destructive behaviors such as disordered eating
and self-injurious behavior (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014; Selby et al., 2013). On the other hand,
PED may also constrain the emotional benefits of adaptive experiences, such as everyday uplifts
and positive experiences. Notably, a core feature of depression is the absence of PE and
reactivity to rewarding experiences. Thus, high PED could contribute to anhedonia and
depressive symptom risk by reducing the antidepressant effects of positive activities.
In the current research, we first focus on the impact of ED on levels of depressive
symptoms when positive and negative events happen in everyday life. In addition, we examine
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
6
the role of ED in amplifying or decreasing depressive symptoms when employing two common
emotion regulation strategies: brooding (on NE) and savoring (PE).
NED and Daily Negative Experiences. We expect that low NED will predict a stronger
association between everyday negative experiences and daily depressive symptoms. As noted
previously, those with poor NED have more difficulty selecting and implementing effective
emotion regulation strategies (Barrett et al., 2001; Kashdan et al., 2015) and are more likely to
resort to destructive behaviors when confronted with NE (Kashdan et al., 2010; Pond et al.,
2012; Zaki et al., 2013). Lacking appropriate coping skills, these individuals may be more
vulnerable to exacerbation of depressive symptoms when negative experiences occur. Moreover,
a wide body of evidence suggests that the act of affect labeling may be regulatory in itself. Affect
labelling has been linked to diminished emotional reactivity (as measured by self-report, neural
activation, and autonomic response) following exposure to negative stimuli (Kircanski,
Lieberman, & Craske, 2012; Lieberman et al., 2007; Lieberman, Inagaki, Tabibnia, & Crockett,
2011). As NED suggests a greater propensity toward precise labeling of emotions, those with
higher NED may be protected against depressive responses following negative everyday
experiences.
To our knowledge no studies have examined whether NED confers reactivity to daily
stressors. However, Kashdan et al. (2014) did find that, among those with low self-esteem, low
NED predicted greater neural reactivity in response to social rejection, consistent with the
hypothesis that poor NED serves as a vulnerability factor that exacerbates the consequences of
stressors or other kinds of negative environmental events. We thus hypothesized that lower NED
would predict stronger associations between daily negative experiences and daily depressive
symptoms.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
7
PED and Daily Positive Experiences. As a corollary, we also examined whether PED
would impact the association between everyday positive experiences and daily depressive
symptoms. Although the depression literature has traditionally focused far more on stressors,
positive experiences have long been considered important agents in reducing depressive
symptoms (Lewinsohn & Graf, 1973; Lewinsohn, Sullivan, & Grosscup, 1980), and reactivity to
positive experiences is being increasingly explored in the context of depression, both in daily life
and within the laboratory (Bylsma, Morris, & Rottenberg, 2008; Bylsma, Taylor-Clift, &
Rottenberg, 2011; Peeters, Nicolson, Berkhof, Delespaul, & deVries, 2003; Starr & Hershenberg,
in press; Thompson et al., 2012). As previously explained, low PED may amplify salutatory
effects of positive events on depressive symptoms by allowing for a more diffuse, less
constrained positive emotional experience when “good things” happen. Moreover, research
suggests that positive affect labeling is associated with diminished self-reported pleasure
(Lieberman et al., 2011). Therefore, individuals with low PED, who are not prone to labeling
positive emotions, may be more reactive to daily positive experiences. We thus hypothesized that
lower PED would predict stronger associations between daily positive experiences and lower
levels of daily depressive symptoms.
Emotion Differentiation and Positive and Negative Rumination. Next, we considered
how NED and PED might interact with, respectively, negative rumination (brooding) and
positive rumination (savoring). In a wide body of research that includes longitudinal, daily diary,
and experimental evidence, depressive rumination robustly predicts the onset and maintenance of
negative mood and depressive episodes (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008).
Brooding, or passive focus on negative consequences of symptoms, has been identified as the
most depressogenic component of rumination (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003).
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
8
Low NED may exacerbate the tendency to engage in, and the negative consequences of,
brooding. Indeed, those with difficulty understanding their NEs may feel more compelled to
dwell on them, and low NED may amplify the negative effects of brooding on daily depressive
symptoms. For example, having a more diffuse, generalized perception of negative emotions
may mean that intense focus on one negative feeling (e.g., I feel disappointed) may quickly
spread to other NEs (I feel sad, I feel guilty, I feel anxious). In turn, brooding about a wider
range of NEs may provoke correspondingly broad negative inferences about the self, world, and
future, triggering or exacerbating depressive symptoms. Little previous work has examined the
effects of NED and rumination, though one study showed that high NED protects against the
effects of trait rumination on non-suicidal self-injury in borderline personality disorder (Zaki et
al., 2013). We thus hypothesized that low NED would be associated with trait rumination, and
that low NED would strengthen the association between daily brooding and depressive
symptoms.
Although rumination is typically shorthand for brooding and other forms of perseverative
focus on NE, rumination can also be in response to PE. For example, savoring refers to PE-
focused cognitive responses that serve to increase or maintain one’s PEs (Martin & Tesser, 1996;
Quoidbach, Berry, Hansenne, & Mikolajczak, 2010; Wood, Heimpel, & Michela, 2003).
Although less widely studied than depressive rumination, growing research suggests that
savoring may be protective against depression. Higher savoring beliefs are negatively correlated
with depression, and savoring predicts decreased daily depressive symptoms (Bryant, 2003;
Hurley & Kwon, 2012; Li, Starr, & Hershenberg, 2016). Analogous to our predictions for NED
and brooding, we expect that the association between savoring and lower depressive symptoms
will be amplified among those with low PED. For example, for those with a less differentiated
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
9
perception of PE, savoring one PE (e.g., I feel cheerful) may intensify a broad range of PEs (I
feel enthusiastic, I feel confident, etc.), which may translate into reduced depressive symptoms.
Thus, we hypothesized that the association between savoring and lower depressive symptoms
will be amplified among those with low PED.
The Present Research. We tested these hypotheses across two studies, one in a sample
of young adults and another in a sample of Veterans recruited from a primary care cohort being
evaluated for behavioral health symptoms. Both were oversampled for depressive symptoms to
allow for significant variation in daily depressive symptoms over the course of the study. Both
studies relied on intensive longitudinal techniques for calculation of PED/ NED and testing of
hypotheses. These techniques produce real-time data collected in naturalistic settings,
minimizing the need for retrospective recall and increasing generalizability. Further, objectively
calculating ED from momentary affect ratings, rather than asking participants to self-report on
their perceived ability to differentiate emotions, reduces reliance on introspection and self-
awareness. In Study 1, ecological momentary assessment (EMA) and daily diary surveys were
both administered over overlapping time periods. EMA data were used to calculate ED, as EMA
is better suited to capture discrete emotional states, and daily diary data were used to test study
hypotheses (because daily diary allows for longer surveys and more careful assessment of
hassles, uplifts, savoring, and brooding). Study 2 relied exclusively on EMA data, allowing for
replication of findings across intensive longitudinal approaches.
After examining basic associations between ED and baseline depression and rumination,
we tested the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 1) Low NED will predict stronger associations
between daily negative experiences and daily depressive symptoms. Hypothesis 2) Low PED
will predict stronger associations between daily positive experiences and reductions in daily
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
10
depressive symptoms. Hypothesis 3) Low NED will predict stronger associations between daily
brooding and daily depressive symptoms. Hypothesis 4) Low PED will predict stronger
associations between daily savoring and reduced daily depressive symptoms. With the exception
of Hypothesis 3, which was examined only in Study 1, all hypotheses were tested in both
samples, allowing for direct replication across two samples that varied considerably in
depression risk, demographic characteristics, and experience sampling methodology.
Study 1
Method
Participants. We recruited 160 undergraduate psychology students. Although they
should not be considered representative of clinical populations, college students are vulnerable to
depressive symptoms (e.g., Garlow et al., 2008), and research suggests that findings generated in
undergraduate samples generalize to clinical samples (Vredenburg, Flett, & Krames, 1993).
Eligibility criteria for participation were minimum age of 18 years, access to Internet and a
personal cell phone, and no English comprehension difficulties. To ensure we recruited a sample
with a broad range of depressive symptoms, we conducted a screening study where potential
participants completed a self-report depression measure, the Quick Inventory of Depressive
Symptomatology (QIDS; Rush et al., 2003). Participants were then preferentially recruited to
achieve approximately equal distribution across three categories (based on published clinical
thresholds, Rush et al., 2003): no symptoms (QIDS < 6, 31% of sample), mild symptoms (QIDS
score of 6-10, 33% of sample) and moderate to severe symptoms (QIDS > 10, 36% of sample).
Students received extra credit and were entered into raffles based on diary compliance. Table 1
reports sample characteristics. Of the participants recruited, one did not complete diaries, five
provided too few valid EMA surveys for ED calculation, and two were excluded from analyses
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
11
after failing inattention checks (described below), resulting in a final sample of 152 participants.
This research was approved by the University of Rochester’s Research Subjects Review Board.
Procedure. Participants completed an initial baseline visit and once-a-day daily diaries
for 14 consecutive days, beginning the evening of the baseline visit. Diaries were completed as
close to bedtime as convenient. Participants completed 10.97 (78.3%) diaries on average.
Number of missed diaries was not significantly related to baseline depressive symptoms, NED,
or PED.
In addition to the daily diary, participants also completed a brief ecological momentary
assessment protocol by completing short, telephone-based surveys five times per day for five
days. In the current study, EMA data was used for the computation of NED/ PED only; all
hypotheses were tested using daily diary data. EMA surveys were administered using the
telephone-based platform telEMA (Fernandez, Johnson, & Rodebaugh, 2013). Participants
designated time-of-day windows (typically 12-hours) in which they were available to complete
surveys. The daily window was divided into five equal intervals, and one call was placed at a
random time within each of the five intervals, with the additional constraint that no two calls
could be less than 1.5 hours apart (to minimize burden). Participants were able to designate
multiple phone numbers and specify the number of repeat calls they would receive if they missed
a call. Participants had up to 30 minutes to call a designated number to complete a survey if they
missed a call. EMAs were typically started the day after baseline participation (thus, EMA and
daily diary periods overlapped) and were always timed so that the five-day period included three
weekdays and two weekend days. Item order was randomized within blocks. During the data
cleaning process, EMA data were inspected for evidence of invalid response patterns (e.g.,
repeatedly entering identical numbers), and suspicious data were excluded. Participants
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
12
completed an average of 19.44 EMAs (78%), and 80% of the sample completed at least 18/25
EMAs. Four participants completed three or fewer valid EMAs and were excluded.
In line with Maniaci and Rogge’s (2014) recommendation, we included six inattention
items in the baseline measure to identify inattentive respondents. Exclusion of inattentive
respondents improves statistical power (Maniaci & Rogge, 2014). Two participants failed
inattention checks consistently and were excluded from all analyses.
Measures. Baseline. Baseline depressive symptom severity was measured using the
QIDS (Rush et al., 2003), a 16-item self-report questionnaire assessing the nine criterion
symptom domains of MDD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders4th edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The QIDS was administered in
the screening study for recruitment purposes and then re-administered at baseline and utilized as
a continuous score. Previous research has supported the psychometric properties of the QIDS
(Rush et al., 2006; Rush et al., 2003). For example, the QIDS has demonstrated construct validity
through correlations with depressive symptoms and diagnoses, as well as sensitivity to symptom
change (Gonzalez, Boals, Jenkins, Schuler, & Taylor, 2013; Trivedi et al., 2004). Cronbach’s
alpha in this study was .84. Baseline rumination was assessed using the Ruminative Response
Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991), a widely used 22-item scale prompting
respondents to rate the frequency of 22 ruminative thoughts or behaviors. The RRS has excellent
internal consistency and external validity (Butler & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1994; Nolen-Hoeksema &
Morrow, 1991); in this sample, Cronbach’s alpha= .94.
Diary Items. Daily Negative and Positive Experiences. Daily negative (“hassles”) and
positive (“uplifts”) experiences were assessed based on methods of Totenhagen and colleagues
(2012). Participants were given a list of items and asked to indicate how much of a hassle and an
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
13
uplift each item was on that day on a scale of zero (none) to three (a great deal). Items ranged
across 15 general life domains: 1) parents and family members, 2) romantic life, 3) close friends,
4) other peers, 5) social events, 6) career, 7) finances, 8) exercise, 9) health, 10), chores, 11)
hobbies, 12) extra-curricular activities, 13) recreation, 14) online activities, and 15) other. An
average total score was then computed for hassles and uplifts, respectively. Internal consistency
for these and other daily measures was computed by separately computing Cronbach’s alphas for
each of the fourteen days and then calculating the mean. Mean internal consistency for hassles
and uplifts were .83 and .84, respectively. Hassles and uplifts scales in this sample were
significantly associated with concurrent daily depressive symptoms, PE, and NE in expected
directions, supporting construct validity (see also Li et al., 2016; Starr & Hershenberg, in press).
Daily Depressive Symptoms. We assessed daily depressive symptoms using a modified
version of the seven-item depression subscale of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS;
Antony, Bieling, Cox, Enns, & Swinson, 1998). Items are rated on a Likert-type scale from zero
to three. The DASS has demonstrated strong psychometric properties (Brown, Chorpita,
Korotitsch, & Barlow, 1997; Clara, Cox, & Enns, 2001). The original items were modified so
that the time frame indicates the current day (e.g. “Indicate how much the statement applied to
you today”). Supporting validity, aggregated mean of daily DASS depressive symptoms was
significantly correlated with baseline depressive symptoms (r= .61, p < .001). Mean internal
consistency across individual days was .93.
Daily Brooding and Savoring. Daily brooding was assessed using the 5-item brooding
subscale of the RRS (Treynor et al., 2003), with instructions modified to cover brooding over the
course of the current day. Each item was rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale. This scale, which
has shown excellent psychometric properties in between-persons studies (Miranda & Nolen-
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
14
Hoeksema, 2007 ; Moberly & Watkins, 2008; Treynor et al., 2003), has been previously adapted
to assess daily brooding in daily diary research (Starr, 2015). Reports of daily brooding using
this measure have been associated with baseline rumination measures and concurrent depressed
mood (Li, Hershenberg, & Starr, 2016; Starr, 2015), supporting construct validity of the daily
measure. Savoring was assessed using a shortened version of the Response to Positive Affect
scale (RPA; Feldman, Joormann, & Johnson, 2008), with instructions prompting participants to
consider how they have responded to feeling “happy, excited, or enthused” modified to apply to
the current day only. Consistent with previous research suggesting that emotion-focused (EF)
and self-focused (SF) positive rumination comprise a single factor (Nelis et al., 2016), two items
with the highest factor loadings were taken from the EF and SF subscales respectively to create a
4-item daily savoring scale (e.g., “Think about how happy you feel”), each rated on a 4-point
Likert-type scale. The full RPA scale has shown adequate internal consistency as well as
convergent and incremental validity (Feldman et al., 2008; Raes, Daems, Feldman, Johnson, &
Van Gucht, 2010). In support of the validity of the daily application of the RPA, Li, Starr, and
Hershenberg (2016) showed that baseline savoring was significantly associated with aggregated
mean ratings of daily savoring (r = .50, p < .01). Mean daily Cronbach’s alpha for brooding and
savoring were .86 and .83, respectively.
Positive and Negative Emotion Differentiation. In the EMA survey, we calculated
current PE and NE based on ratings on standard mood adjectives used in emotion reactivity
research (replicating Byslma et al., 2011; also see Hershenberg, Mavandadi, Wright, & Thase,
2017). Seven positive mood ratings (talkative, enthusiastic, confident, cheerful, energetic,
satisfied, and happy) and seven negative mood ratings (tense, anxious, distracted, restless,
irritated, depressed, guilty) were assessed on each call. To calculate ED, average intraclass
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
15
correlation coefficients (ICCs) for either PE or NE items were calculated for each participant
across all assessments (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979; Tugade et al., 2004). For ease of interpretation,
ICCs were subtracted from 1.0 to reverse the score, so that higher NED/ PED scores reflect
greater emotion differentiation ability and lower scores reflected lower differentiation of discrete
emotions. This established, well-validated method of calculating ED has been used in multiple
previous studies (e.g., Hill & Updegraff, 2012; Selby et al., 2013; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2007).
Mean emotion intensity (used as a covariate) was calculated by taking the mean levels of NE and
PE for each participant across all EMA observations.
Data Analytic Approach
We used multilevel modeling (MLM) using SPSS 23.0 MIXED. MLM is a powerful
statistical approach that accounts for the nested, non-independent nature of intensive longitudinal
data. Repeated measures were nested within participants. Most hypotheses were tested using
cross-level interactions (known as slopes-as-outcomes models) between a level-one (within-
subjects) predictor (daily hassles, uplifts, brooding, or savoring) and a level-two (between-
subjects) moderator (NED or PED). MLM copes well with missing data and has greater
statistical power compared to traditional analytic approaches.
All predictors were entered as fixed effects, with level-one variables and the intercept
also modeled as random effects. Level-two predictors were mean centered. In addition, following
the recent recommendations of Bolger and Laurenceau (2013), we partitioned each level-one
predictor into two orthogonal components: a between-subjects component, represented by the
person’s grand-mean-centered aggregated mean score over the course of the full diary period
(i.e., a means component,
), and a within-subjects component, represented by the person-
mean-centered score (i.e., the daily deviation from the means component,
  
). Both
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
16
within- and between-subjects components were included for all level-one predictors, both as
main effects and in cross-level interactions. The inclusion of the between-subjects components of
level-one variables enhances the interpretability of models by ensuring that results are not
artifacts of individual differences in the average level of the variable (e.g., the tendency for some
people to brood more than others). However, results for the between-subjects variables are not
themselves considered interpretable (Bolger & Laurenceau, 2013). Thus, although results for
between-subjects variables are included in Table 1 for the sake of comprehensiveness, readers
should focus primarily on results for the “within” variables (e.g., broodingwithin); for simplicity,
“between” variables are not described in the Results section text. Importantly, this caveat only
applies to the between-subjects component of level-one variables; level two variables (e.g.,
NED/ PED) are inherently between-subjects and are interpretable. Note that use of this relatively
new analytic approach did not substantially change results, as compared to a more traditional
approach where between-subjects components of level-one variables were not included.
We also controlled for time in all models to ensure that effects were not artifacts of
temporal change. We applied a first-order autoregressive (AR[1]) model to correct for
autocorrelation of residuals and an unstructured covariance matrix for random effects. Daily
depressive symptoms (concurrent to predictors) were entered as the outcome in all MLM models.
Following the notation of Bolger and Laurenceau (2013), moderation models
(Hypotheses 1-4) can be described with the following equations:
Level 1:
  
  
     (1)
Level 2:
    
 
  
  (2)
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
17
    
 (3)
   (4)
These equations can be simplified into the following equation, in which the first seven
terms denote fixed effects and the last three terms represent random effects (see Bolger &
Laurenceau, 2013):
   
 
  
  
  
  

  
  
 
  
   (5)
For example, in the model constructed to test Hypothesis 3,
represents NED (the level-2
moderator),
 and
  
 respectively represent main effects for broodingbetween and
broodingwithin (the level-1 predictor), and
 and

  
 respectively denote NED
broodingbetween and NED broodingwithin interactions. In all moderation models, of primary
interest is the significance of the sixth term in equation 5,

  
, the interaction between
ED and the within-subjects component of the level-1 variable. Significant interactions were
probed using simple slope tests (Aiken & West, 1991; Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006) at one
standard deviation above and below the moderator’s mean.
Finally, because individuals prone to more extreme (i.e., higher or lower) levels of
negative and positive affect will have a restricted range of emotion ratings, we also controlled for
mean emotion intensity (NE intensity in Hypotheses 1 and 3, PE intensity in Hypotheses 2 and
4). Doing so was a conservative approach to rule out any concerns related to a restricted range.
Note that results remained unchanged when controlling for mean emotion intensity; thus, for
parsimony, we do not present the models including this additional covariate.
Results
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
18
The Study 1 EMA dataset (used for the computation of ED) included 3,029 valid
observations. Average ICC levels were .17 for NE and .34 for PE, resulting in mean NED and
PED (1-ICC) of .83 and .66 respectively.
The Study 1 daily diary dataset included 1,723 valid observations. Descriptive data for
study variables are reported in Table 1.
As a preliminary step, we examined bivariate correlations between NED, PED, baseline
depressive symptoms (QIDS), and baseline rumination (RRS). We found a marginally
significant, negative association between NED and QIDS (r= -.16, p = .056) but no correlation
between PED and QIDS (p > .05). NED was significantly correlated with baseline RRS (r= .19,
p= .017). NED and PED were also significantly correlated with each other (r = .19, p = .017).
Hypothesis 1. Next, we examined whether NED moderated the association between
fluctuations in daily hassles and daily depressive symptoms. As described in the Data Analytic
Approach section, a model was constructed that included main effects for daily hassles and NED
as well as their interaction and the effects of time. Full results are reported in Table 2. The
interaction between daily hassles and NED were not significant (p > .05). Although non-
significant, the interaction is illustrated in Figure 1a-i to facilitate comparison with Study 2.
Hypothesis 2. Next, we tested whether low PED moderated the association between
uplift fluctuations and daily depressive symptoms. Following the approach described above, we
constructed a multilevel model with daily uplifts, NED, and their interaction, as well as time,
with daily depressive symptoms entered as the outcome. Full results are reported in Table 2. As
shown there, the main effect for uplifts (but not PED) was significant, p < .001. Of note, the
interaction term was significant, p= .004. The significant interaction was decomposed using a
simple slope tests (Aiken & West, 1991; Preacher et al., 2006). Supporting Hypothesis 2, as
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
19
illustrated in Figure 2a-i, the negative association between daily uplifts and daily depressive
symptoms was stronger when PED was low (M -1 SD), b= -.16, SE= .03, p < .001; in contrast,
uplifts did not significantly predict reductions in depressive symptoms when PED was high (M +
1 SD), b= -.03, SE= .03, p= .403.
Hypothesis 3. We next examined whether NED predicted stronger positive associations
between daily brooding and daily depressive symptoms. NED and daily brooding were entered
into a multilevel model as both main effects and an interactive effect, along with time, with daily
depressive symptoms as the outcome. Full results are displayed in Table 2. The main effect for
brooding was significant, p< .001, but not for NED. Supporting Hypothesis 3, the interaction
between daily brooding fluctuations and NED was significant, p= .028. Decomposition is
illustrated in Figure 1b. As expected, reports of daily brooding were more predictive of same-day
depressive symptoms at low levels of NED, b= .56, SE= .06, t(136.65)= 9.21, p< .001, as
compared to high levels, b= .37, SE = .06, p< .001.
Hypothesis 4. Finally, we examined whether PED likewise predicted stronger
associations between daily savoring and reduced depressed mood. Analogous to the above
models, we constructed a model that included PED, daily savoring, PED daily savoring, and
time. As shown in Table 2, the main effect for savoring (but not for PED) was significant, p <
.001. Moreover, supporting Hypothesis 4, the interaction was significant, p= .001 and is
presented in Figure 2b-i. As predicted, daily savoring more strongly predicted lower depressive
symptoms for those with low PED, b= -2.26, SE= .30, p< .001, compared to those with high
PED, b= -.81, SE= .30, p = .008.
Conclusions and Limitations
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
20
These results provide broad support for the majority of hypotheses (with the notable
exception of Hypothesis 1; we found no support for NED conferring stress reactivity in this
sample). Results should be interpreted in the context of study limitations. First, this study utilized
an undergraduate sample. On one hand, young adults are at high risk for first onset of depression
(Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, & Walters, 2005), and research suggests that undergraduate
samples generate comparable findings to clinical samples (e.g., Vredenburg et al., 1993). That
said, some have criticized the overreliance on college samples to study clinical phenomena
(Coyne, 1994). A higher risk sample could arguably produce findings more applicable to clinical
depression. From a demographic standpoint, as with most studies recruiting from psychology
courses, our sample had a limited age range and was predominantly female. This is potentially
problematic because ED increases with age (Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000),
and depression risk (and rumination) varies considerably by gender (Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson,
& Grayson, 1999). Replicating findings in a sample with markedly different demographics and
higher clinical risk would provide greater confidence in the generalizability of results.
Further, all Study 1 models reflect concurrent associations (i.e., the level-one predictor
variable was reported at the same time as the outcome variable. The daily diary design (with a
full-day interval between surveys), while offering numerous benefits, was not well-suited for
testing lagged effects. The effects of daily events tend to quickly dissipate and are sometimes
countered by mood rebound effects, making lagged findings often elusive when using daily diary
designs (Bolger, DeLongis, Kessler, & Schilling, 1989; Stone, Neale, & Shiffman, 1993).
Because we only tested concurrent models in Study 1, it is unclear if predictor variables preceded
outcomes; the reverse direction of causality remains a possibility (for example, those with low
NED may be more prone to generating negative experiences when depressed).
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
21
To address these limitations, we tested hypotheses in a second study, which included a
sample of older, largely male Veterans recruited from a primary care cohort being evaluated for
behavioral health symptoms. Veterans are at substantially higher risk for mental health problems
compared to the general population (Dohrenwend et al., 2006; Seal, Bertenthal, Miner, Sen, &
Marmar, 2007), thus, depressive symptoms reported in this sample may be more reflective of
clinically significant pathology. This sample used an experience sampling/ ecological
momentary assessment (EMA) approach in which participants were signaled multiple times per
day to complete surveys in naturalistic settings at random intervals. We tested the same
hypotheses as in Study 1, with the exception of Hypothesis 3 (NED brooding), for which data
were not available. In addition, because the EMA approach allowed for shorter intervals between
surveys, we were able to explore lagged models (predictor variables temporally preceding
outcomes) in addition to testing concurrent models, analogous to the diary design.
Study 2
Method
Participants and procedure. We recruited Veterans with a range of depression severity
based on scores on the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke, Spitzer, & Williams,
2001) from a primary care cohort being evaluated for behavioral health symptoms at a
northeastern Veteran Affairs Medical Center. The Behavioral Health Laboratory (BHL) within
the VAMC collects screening data on new Veteran patient referrals on an ongoing basis.
Potentially eligible Veterans were identified by their responses to the PHQ-9 included in the
BHL assessment; those eligible to participate and interested in hearing more about the study
were contacted by study staff. Veterans were selectively recruited to achieve an approximate
distribution of one-third no depressive symptoms, one-third minor depression, and one-third
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
22
major depression, according to PHQ-9 cut-offs (Kroenke et al., 2001). Exclusion criteria
included psychotic disorders and current mania. Although sampling methods specifically
targeted an over-selection of depression, the sample was psychiatrically and medically
heterogeneous; one participant (2%) had a probable past manic episode, 25 participants (50%)
received a probable diagnosis of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, 34 participants (68%) indicated
significant interference from pain, and four participants (8%) were considered at risk for alcohol
problems.
Interested Veterans were invited to a laboratory session, during which they provided a
baseline measure of depressive symptoms with repeated administration of the PHQ-9 and
received instructions on the EMA portion of the study, which began the following day. We
collected EMA data using Interactive Voice Recording, a phone-based system for collecting data
via keypad press. Participants were called six times per day for seven days. Calls occurred on a
random basis within a 12-hour block of participant designated time (e.g., 9am to 9pm). If they
missed the call, participants were given 25 minutes to call back a toll-free number to complete
the survey. We worked closely with participants during the baseline assessment to make sure that
they understood the items being asked of them on the phone surveys. Adherence to the protocol
was monitored, and we reached out to participants to troubleshoot noncompliance. Participants
received $35 for the lab session and $2.50 per EMA call (max payment $140). Participants
completed an average of 69% of calls (M = 29.10, sd = 10; modal number of calls 39 out of 42),
which is comparable to other EMA studies (Byslma et al., 2011). Rate of missed surveys was
unrelated to baseline depression, NED, or PED. Demographic information is displayed in Table
1; as shown, Veterans were predominantly male and racially diverse. The majority (72%) were
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
23
above 50 years of age. All study procedures were approved by the Corporal Michael J. Crescenz
VAMC Institutional Review Board.
Baseline. Depressive Symptoms. Participants rated the frequency over the past two
weeks with which they experienced each of nine symptoms of depression using the PHQ-9
(Kroenke et al., 2001). Psychometric properties of the PHQ-9 are well-established (Spitzer,
Kroenke, Williams, & Group, 1999; Spitzer, Williams, Kroenke, Homyak, & McMurray, 2000),
and Cronbach’s alpha in this study was .83. Baseline rumination was measured with the RRS
(see Study 1).
Momentary Positive and Negative Experiences. At the time of each phone call,
participants were asked to report “how you were spending your time before you took a break to
take this survey”. To rate the valence of their current activity, participants used a face-valid,
continuous scale, ranging from 1 (most unpleasant) to 5 (most pleasant). We refer to this scale as
“pleasant activities.” To facilitate comparisons with Study 1, for Hypothesis 1 only we reverse-
coded the scale, so that greater scores reflect more unpleasant recent experiences (which we refer
to as “unpleasant activities”).
NED and PED. On each phone call, after rating the pleasantness of their current
activity, participants were asked to “Keep thinking about how you felt before you took a break to
take this survey. Use a 1 to 5 scale, where 1 is ‘I didn’t feel this way at all’ and 5 is I felt this
way a great deal.We used the same adjectives as in Study 1, and PED and NED were
computed using identical procedures as in Study 1.
Momentary Depressed Mood. Depressed mood was assessed using the rating for the
single item, “I felt depressed,” on the 1-5 continuous scale described above. The use of single-
item indicators of mood is relatively common in EMA/ diary research (as brevity is critical for
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
24
survey compliance) and is psychometrically justifiable for non-complex constructs (Burisch,
1997; Laurenceau, Barrett, & Rovine, 2005; Starr, 2015; Starr & Davila, 2012a).
Savoring. Similar to Study 1, savoring items were adapted from Feldman, Joorman, &
Johnson’s (2008) RPA scale, although in this study, all items were taken from the EF scale of the
RPA (no items from the SF scale were administered; we still refer to this scale as savoring for
consistency with Study 1, but note that in this study it reflects the somewhat narrower construct
of emotion-focused savoring). Participants were asked, “How you have responded to these
feelings?” (i.e., their emotion ratings) using a 1 (not at all) to 5 (a great deal) scale. We
administered three emotion-focused items: “I started to think about how happy I feel”; “I started
to think about how strong I feel”; and “I savored this moment, and took their average for a total
savoring score. Mean Cronbach’s alpha (computed separately for each prompt, and then
averaged) was .77.
Data Analytic Approach
Prior to data analysis, EMA data were inspected and suspicious response patterns (e.g.,
large numbers of identical numeric responses) were flagged for exclusion (see McCabe, Mack, &
Fleeson, 2012). The analytic approach for Study 2 was similar to that in Study 1, with the
following changes to accommodate the EMA design. A continuous variable representing time
passed since the first completed survey was used as the repeated measures variable and was
entered as a covariate to account for temporal artifacts. To control for possible effects of diurnal
mood variation, time of day was also included in all concurrent models (as shown in Table 2, this
variable was not significant in any models and was dropped from lagged models to for
parsimony). As in Study 1, we ran additional models including mean emotion intensity as a
covariate in tests of all hypotheses; findings were not substantially impacted, and results
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
25
presented here exclude this covariate for simplicity. Depressed mood was entered as the outcome
variable in all MLM analyses.
We tested both concurrent and lagged models in this sample. To create lagged variables,
data for each signal were shifted in our dataset, so that depressed mood at each signal t could be
predicted by variables at signal t-1. To prevent overnight lags, the first signal of each day was
excluded as outcomes; because this reduced the amount of data available for analysis, power was
correspondingly reduced in lagged analyses. Moreover, effects are generally weaker in lagged
analyses (because effects of within-day events are typically short-lived; Marco & Suls, 1993).
Consequently, we consider lagged models exploratory, as they may be somewhat underpowered
and should be interpreted in conjunction with concurrent models. Time lag between observations
was included as a covariate in lagged models to account for non-equal intervals.
Results
The Study 2 dataset included 1,455 valid observations. Table 1 displays descriptive data
for major study variables. In this dataset, mean NE ICC was .26 (M NED = 1-ICC = .74, SD=
.20) and mean PE ICC was .44 (M PED= .56, SD= .44). Baseline depressive symptoms were
significantly, negatively correlated with NED (r= -.29, p= .040), but not with PED (r= -.23, p =
.116). NED was significantly, negatively correlated with baseline rumination (r= -.31, p= .033).
NED and PED were also significantly correlated with each other (r= .40, p= .004).
Hypothesis 1. We next examined whether low NED predicted stronger associations
between unpleasantness ratings of recent activities and depressed mood. Following the
prescribed data analytic plan, we tested a model that included unpleasant activities, NED, and
their interaction, as well as total elapsed time, time of day, and between-subjects components of
unpleasant activities as covariates. Full results appear in Table 2. As shown, there was a main
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
26
effect for negative experiences, but not for NED. Importantly, the NED unpleasant experiences
interaction was significant (p < .001). Decomposition revealed that, in line with expectations, at
low levels of NED, unpleasant activities were strongly related to depressed mood, b= .35, SE=
.04, p < .001, whereas at high levels of NED, unpleasant activities were not significantly
predictive of depressed mood, b= .07, SE= .05, p= .150 (see Figure 1a-ii for illustration).
We also tested this hypothesis using lagged data, where NED was examined as a
moderator of the lagged association between unpleasant activities at the previous signal and
current depressed mood.
1
The interaction was again significant, b= -.50, SE= .22, p= .031. At
low levels of NED, negative experiences predicted non-significant increases in depressed mood,
b= .08, SE= .06, p= .171, whereas at high levels, negative experiences actually predicted
marginal decreases in depressed mood, b= -.12, SE= .07, p= .096.
Hypothesis 2. We next tested PED as a moderator of the association between
pleasantness ratings of recent activities and depressed mood, by testing a model that included
pleasant activities, PED, PED pleasant activities, and time and between-subjects component
covariates. As shown in Table 2, the main effect for positive experiences (but not PED) was
significant. Notably, the focal interaction term was significant. We probed the significant
interaction, as shown in Figure 2a-ii, and found that, consistent with hypotheses and Study 1
1
For lagged analyses, we did not control for depressed mood at signal t1 because including
lagged dependent variables as predictors in multilevel models introduces severe bias (see
Allison, 2015). To allow for a more conservative approach that is appropriate within a multilevel
framework, we also examined whether the lagged model would remain significant after
controlling for the concurrent model (see Starr & Davila, 2012b for an example of this
approach). To do so, we included as predictors (in addition to the NED main effect, random
effects, time, and other covariates) a) unpleasant activities at t1, b) NED unpleasant activities
at t1, c) unpleasant activities at t and d) NED unpleasant activities at t. Terms a) and b)
represent the lagged model, and terms c) and d) represent the concurrent model. The interaction
terms for the concurrent model and the lagged model were both significant (concurrent p < .001,
lagged p = .029). Although this approach may be unnecessarily conservative, the significance of
the lagged model provides greater confidence in the robustness of results.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
27
results, pleasant activities sharply predicted reduced depressed mood at low levels of PED, b =-
.33, SE= .05, p < .001 but only marginally predicted lower depressed mood at high levels of
PED, b= -.10, SE= .05 p= .071. Testing the same hypothesis using lagged data, we again found a
significant interaction, b= .58, SE= .20, p= .020, with a similar pattern of results, with positive
experiences predicting decreased later depressed mood only at low levels of PED.
2
Hypothesis 4. Finally, we examined whether PED predicts strengthened associations
between daily savoring and depressed mood. We constructed a multilevel model where daily
savoring, PED, and their interaction predicted concurrent depressed mood, with total elapsed
time, time of day, and between-subjects savoring components as covariates. Full results are listed
in Table 2; as displayed there, the main effect for savoring was significant. The interaction
between PED and savoring was marginally significant, p = .072 (interestingly, this interaction
was significant in supplemental analyses controlling for the covariate of mean emotional
intensity, p= .046). Aligning with predictions and with Study 1 findings, daily savoring predicted
significantly decreased concurrent depressed mood among those with low PED, b= -.27, SE=
.07, p= .001, but not among those with high PED, b= -.06, SE= .08, p= .459 (see Figure 2b-ii).
When substituting lagged depressed mood as the outcome, the interaction was non-significant
(p= .113) though trended similarly such that momentary savoring was predictive of decreases in
depressed mood among those with lower levels of PED.
General Discussion
Drawing from two intensive longitudinal studies with complementary designs and
samples, our findings provide evidence for roles of both NED and PED in modulating within-
2
As with the NED unpleasant activities model, we again re-ran lagged analyses using the very
conservative approach of controlling for concurrent main and interactive effects (see Footnote 1).
The PED pleasant activities interaction term was significant for both the concurrent (p < .001)
and lagged (p= .018) pleasant activity variables.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
28
person variations in depressive symptoms in daily life. Results largely replicated across both
studies, despite substantial differences in sample demographics; although both samples were
oversampled for depressive symptoms, Study 1 featured undergraduate, predominately female
young adults and Study 2 included Veterans recruited from a primary care cohort being
evaluated for behavioral health symptoms who were predominantly males over 50. The
consistency of findings across such divergent samples supports the robustness and
generalizability of our pattern of results.
First, we found evidence that NED (but not PED) was correlated with depressive
symptoms, aligning with other studies that have linked low NED to depressive symptoms and
disorders (Demiralp et al., 2012; Erbas et al., 2014; Golston et al., 1992). This finding is
consistent with research linking depression to alexithymia, a personality trait marked in part by
blunted ability to identify and describe one’s emotions (Honkalampi, Hintikka, Tanskanen,
Lehtonen, & Viinamäki, 2000). Individuals with depression show a broad range of deficits in
cognitive functioning, including overgeneralized autobiographical memory, negative attentional
biases, reduced prospective imagery, and impaired executive functioning (for a review see
Joormann & Arditte, 2014), which may cause depressed people to perceive negative emotion in a
blunt, overgeneralized manner. Discrete emotional states provide a wealth of information about
the nature of the emotional situation, its potential consequences, and effective regulation
strategies (Parrott, 2002); decreased ability to discern nuance in negative emotional experiences
puts depressed individuals at an inherent disadvantage for managing these emotions. Further, the
act of affect labeling may be implicitly regulatory (e.g., Kircanski et al., 2012; Lieberman et al.,
2011). That said, our data (as with other existing studies) cannot distinguish between two
important possibilities: that low NED is a pre-existing risk factor for the emergence of
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
29
depression, or that low NED is a feature of depression that emerges with onset and improves
with remission. Future research using long-term longitudinal design can help to resolve this
important question.
The current research does provide evidence that ED (both negative and positive)
influences the ebb and flow of depressive symptoms. First, the present study is the first to show
that poor NED predicts a stronger association between daily negative experiences and daily
depressive symptoms. This suggests that the effects of negative emotion differentiation may be
more salient in the context of negative experiences. This finding is consistent with the affect-as-
information theory, which posits that specific, differentiated negative emotional experiences are
more adaptive than global affective states because they are subject to fewer misattribution errors
(Schwarz & Clore, 1996). When people have the ability to discriminate between discrete
emotional states elicited by a negative event, they are better able to identify the cause of the
emotional experience and generate an adaptive response, whereas those who experience
undifferentiated, global affective states cannot (Russell & Barrett, 1999). This capacity may be
particularly relevant to depressive symptoms, since the meaning attributed to adverse events
(e.g., interpreting them as internal, global, and stable) is often associated with the etiology and
maintenance of symptoms (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989; Hankin, Fraley, & Abela, 2005;
Weiner, 1974). Moreover, greater negative emotional differentiation is correlated with the
selection and implementation of effective emotion regulation strategies (Barrett et al., 2001;
Kashdan et al., 2015), and the act of labeling emotions may itself help regulate emotions
(Kircanski et al., 2012; Lieberman et al., 2007; Lieberman et al., 2011). Accordingly, decreased
differentiation may lead to more difficulties coping with negative experiences and their resulting
emotions and increased daily depressive symptoms.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
30
Moreover, NED moderated the associations between daily negative events and depressive
symptoms in our Veteran sample (Study 2), but not in our sample of young adults (Study 1). This
discrepant finding may be associated with the demographic differences of the two samples.
Previous research suggests that emotion differentiation improves as adults age, so that poor
differentiation of negative emotions may confer more risk for depressive symptoms in older
adults (Carstensen et al., 2000). This explanation is speculative, and future work will help to
address if this difference reflects the different demographics of the study samples.
Low NED also predicted stronger associations between daily brooding and concurrent
depressive symptoms. Coupled with the negative correlation between NED and trait rumination,
findings suggest that people with low NED suffer a rumination “double whammy”, with a
greater tendency towards rumination as well as greater depressive reactivity to it. A number of
studies have suggested that low NED is related to the selection and deployment of ineffective or
destructive coping strategies. The current study is the first to show that maladaptive emotion
regulation techniques also have more deleterious affective consequences among those with low
NED. Findings align with those of Zaki et al. (2013), who showed that trait rumination was more
predictive of non-suicidal self-injury in borderline personality disorder for those with low NED.
People who have trouble understanding their emotions may be more likely to get especially
“stuck” when brooding about their NE, which may lead the brooding to have more of a
deleterious impact on effective problem solving. In contrast, it could be the case that those with
high NED may be more effective at identifying the source of their emotions, which may make
ruminative self-focus more productive and less impactful on depressive symptoms.
Paralleling our NED findings, we also showed that, across both samples, low PED
enhanced the ameliorative effects of both positive experiences and positive rumination
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
31
(savoring) on depressive symptoms. Most of the limited previous research on PED has focused
on the role of low PED in predicting problematic behaviors (e.g., Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014;
Selby et al., 2013); we showed that low PED might be associated with adaptive outcomes in the
context of depressive symptoms. Indeed, the extent to which low PED is harmful versus helpful
may largely depend on the outcome being considered. Among those with low PED, positive
experiences may trigger a wealth of PEs, rather than a specific, constrained emotion. In some
contexts, these broad, undifferentiated PEs may motivate and reinforce destructive behaviors
(e.g., Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014; Selby et al., 2013). However, in the context of depression, these
PEs may motivate more behavioral activation, culminating in reduced depressive symptoms.
Thus, PED may have trade-offs; it may be protective in some clinical contexts while adding to
vulnerabilities in others. That said, research on PED remains very limited, and more
investigation is decidedly needed.
Theoretical Considerations, Future Directions, and Conclusions
Can emotion differentiation be differentiated? Notably, although research has
typically assumed that low ED scores reflect an inability to cognitively discriminate between
emotions, they may also reflect a genuine propensity towards experiencing multiple emotions in
clusters rather than individually. If so, perseverative focus on one NE (through brooding) or PE
(through savoring) may activate a network of related emotions, amplifying the affective
experience and influencing depressive symptoms; likewise, positive and negative events may
trigger a broader (and perhaps more intense) affective response in individuals labeled as low
emotion differentiators. This may be one reason why the literature on PED is somewhat mixed;
low PED may actually be comprised of two counteractive components, one potentially beneficial
(the propensity to experience multiple concurrent positive emotions) and one potentially
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
32
maladaptive (deficient ability to discern discrete emotional states). In contrast, low NED may be
more consistently maladaptive because both the tendency to experience large clusters of NEs and
the inability to differentiate these NEs are likely to have negative consequences (and may
therefore have additive effects). Although our approach to assessing NED and PED (using EMAs
to calculate intraclass correlations among momentary emotions) is a widely accepted method
(Selby et al., 2013; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979; Tugade et al., 2004), future researchers should
develop techniques that better discriminate between the experience and the discernment of
multiple concurrent emotions.
Clinical implications. Our findings on how NED and PED influence fluctuations of
depressive symptoms in daily life may be helpful in increasing understanding of the dynamics of
emotional experience in depression and inform the application of effective treatment targets.
Evidence has pointed toward the efficacy of interventions that help individuals expand their
emotion vocabulary to better identify and precisely label discrete emotions (Cameron, Payne, &
Doris, 2013; Kircanski et al., 2012). This approach, known as affect labeling, suggests that better
recognizing and naming discrete emotional states reduces emotional reactivity and maladaptive
emotion regulation strategies (Lieberman et al., 2007). This, in turn, may facilitate the ability to
manage one’s behavior or distress in response to negative experiences, thereby reducing
depressive symptoms. Moreover, while working to improve NED, clinicians might help patients
harness the effects of low PED as they work to increase patients’ awareness of positive
emotional states and increased sensitivity to rewards, though future empirical work is needed to
see if this could be a useful application of our basic research findings.
Study limitations. When considered in isolation, each study has several limitations.
Study 1 utilized an undergraduate sample; although participants were oversampled for depressive
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
33
symptoms, the pathology captured is likely a non-ideal proxy for clinical depression. Study 1
also used a daily diary design that relied on recall of experiences, perseverative thought, mood,
and symptoms over the course of the day, and all analyses tested concurrent associations. Study
2 relied on single-item measures and a relatively small sample. However, these limitations were
well-balanced across the two studies. Study 2 included a sample of Veterans (a higher-risk
population with a wider and older age range) and shorter interval EMA sampling which allowed
lagged analyses; Study 1 included more detailed daily measures, a larger sample, and a greater
representation of women. Thus, the replication of results across the two studies provides greater
confidence in findings. That said, several limitations remain across our study as a whole.
Although both studies oversampled for self-reported depressive symptoms, neither utilized
clinical interviews to assess clinical depression. Likewise, as Study 1 utilized a college sample
and Study 2 included a psychiatrically heterogeneous group of Veterans, neither sample can be
considered exclusively representative of major depression, and future research can help to
establish if results generalize. Moreover, as explained previously, our operationalization of
emotion differentiation cannot tease apart if low ED reflects an inability to discriminate between
emotions or a genuine propensity to experience multiple emotions concurrently. In addition, both
studies relied on the same emotion adjectives (derived from Bylsma et al., 2011), which were not
explicitly designed to assess ED and may capture non-discrete emotional states. Recent evidence
underscores the importance of careful selection of emotion adjectives in momentary research,
and this should be an important consideration in future ED research. As noted previously, our
design cannot determine whether NED contributes to risk for depression or simply emerges
concomitant with symptoms. Finally, our measures of daily preservative self-focus, particularly
savoring, were brief and may have conceptual overlap with other related constructs (e.g.,
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
34
mindfulness), suggesting that future research will benefit from testing the specificity of these
established associations.
Despite these limitations, the current study adds to a growing literature highlighting the
importance of ED to emotional health. Indeed, evidence points to low NED as a transdiagnostic
factor that contributes to a range of psychopathological conditions and behaviors including
depression, eating disorders, social anxiety disorder, alcohol abuse, and aggression. In contrast,
low PED may have trade-offs for mental health, depending on the clinical context. Future
research is needed to better understand the nature and consequences of these intriguing
constructs.
AUTHORSHIP. L.R.S. and R.H. conceptualized the manuscript collaboratively. L.R.S. was
principal investigator of Study 1 and R.H. was principal investigator of Study 2. L.R.S.
conducted all analyses and led manuscript preparation. R.H., Y.I.L., and Z.A.S. made substantial
contributions to manuscript preparation. All authors approved the final version of the paper for
submission.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. Study 1 was completed with funds provided by the University of
Rochester. We thank the members of our research team, particularly Fanny Mlawer and
Chistopher Anzalone, who managed data collection for this study, as well as the participants who
generously contributed their time. Study 2 was completed with funds provided by the VISN 4
Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC Director: D. Oslin) Pilot
Project Funds, Corporal Michael J. Crescenz Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Philadelphia, PA.
The views expressed in the article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
35
position or policy of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States government. We
thank Erin Wright and Sara Mooar for help with data collection, Dorothy McDougall and Joan
Havey for administrative support, Christopher Petro for technical support, and the Veterans who
generously participated in this research.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
36
References
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-
based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96(2), 358-372. doi:0033-
295X/89/J00.15
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Allison, P. (2015). Don't Put Lagged Dependent Variables in Mixed Models. Retrieved from
http://statisticalhorizons.com/lagged-dependent-variables
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, D.C.: Author.
Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric
properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in
clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176-181.
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you're
feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion
differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion, 15(6), 713-724.
Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., & Schilling, E. A. (1989). Effects of daily stress on
negative mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(5), 808-818.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.808
Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J.-P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary
and experience sampling research. New York: Guilford.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
37
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric properties
of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 35(1), 79-89.
Bryant, F. (2003). Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about
savouring. Journal of Mental Health, 12(2), 175-196.
Burisch, M. (1997). Test length and validity revisited. European Journal of Personality, 11(4),
303-315. doi:Doi 10.1002/(Sici)1099-0984(199711)11:4<303::Aid-Per292>3.0.Co;2-#
Butler, L. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1994). Gender differences in responses to depressed mood
in a college sample. Sex Roles, 30(5), 331-346.
Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity
in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676-691.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001
Bylsma, L. M., Taylor-Clift, A., & Rottenberg, J. (2011). Emotional reactivity to daily events in
major and minor depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(1), 155-167.
doi:10.1037/a0021662
Cameron, C. D., Payne, B. K., & Doris, J. M. (2013). Morality in high definition: Emotion
differentiation calibrates the influence of incidental disgust on moral judgments. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 719-725. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.014
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in
everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
79(4), 644-655.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
38
Clara, I. P., Cox, B. J., & Enns, M. W. (2001). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Depression-
Anxiety-Stress Scales in depressed and anxious patients. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 23(1), 61-67. doi:10.1023/A:1011095624717
Coyne, J. C. (1994). Self-reported distress: Analog or ersatz depression? Psychological Bulletin,
116(1), 29-45.
Demiralp, E., Thompson, R. J., Mata, J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Barrett, L. F., . . .
Jonides, J. (2012). Feeling Blue or Turquoise? Emotional Differentiation in Major
Depressive Disorder. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1410-1416.
doi:10.1177/0956797612444903
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Chapman, A. L., Weiss, N. H., & Rosenthal, M. Z. (2014). A Preliminary
Examination of the Role of Emotion Differentiation in the Relationship Between
Borderline Personality and Urges for Maladaptive Behaviors. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36(4), 616-625. doi:10.1007/s10862-014-
9423-4
Dohrenwend, B. P., Turner, J. B., Turse, N. A., Adams, B. G., Koenen, K. C., & Marshall, R.
(2006). The Psychological Risks of Vietnam for U.S. Veterans: A Revisit with New Data
and Methods. Science, 313(5789), 979-982. doi:10.1126/science.1128944
Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Boonen, J., Noens, I., & Kuppens, P. (2013). Emotion differentiation
in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(10), 1221-1227.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.007
Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Lee Pe, M., Koval, P., & Kuppens, P. (2014). Negative emotion
differentiation: Its personality and well-being correlates and a comparison of different
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
39
assessment methods. Cognition & emotion, 28(7), 1196-1213.
doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875890
Feldman, G. C., Joormann, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2008). Responses to positive affect: A self-
report measure of rumination and dampening. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(4),
507-525.
Fernandez, K. C., Johnson, M. R., & Rodebaugh, T. L. (2013). TelEMA: a low-cost and user-
friendly telephone assessment platform. Behavior research methods, 45(4), 1279-1291.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of general psychology,
2(3), 300.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-
and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218.
Garlow, S. J., Rosenberg, J., Moore, J. D., Haas, A. P., Koestner, B., Hendin, H., & Nemeroff, C.
B. (2008). Depression, desperation, and suicidal ideation in college students: results from
the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention College Screening Project at Emory
University. Depression and Anxiety, 25(6), 482-488. doi:10.1002/da.20321
Golston, R. B., Gara, M. A., & Woolfolk, R. L. (1992). Emotion Differentiation: A Correlate of
Symptom Severity in Major Depression. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
180(11), 712-718.
Gonzalez, D. A., Boals, A., Jenkins, S. R., Schuler, E. R., & Taylor, D. (2013). Psychometrics
and latent structure of the IDS and QIDS with young adult students. Journal of affective
disorders, 149(1), 217-220.
Hankin, B. L., Fraley, R. C., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2005). Daily Depression and Cognitions About
Stress: Evidence for a Traitlike Depressogenic Cognitive Style and the Prediction of
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
40
Depressive Symptoms in a Prospective Daily Diary Study. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88(4), 673-685.
Hershenberg, R., Mavandadi, S., Wright, E., & Thase, M. E. (2017). Anhedonia in the daily lives
of depressed Veterans: A pilot report on experiential avoidance as a moderator of
emotional reactivity. Journal of affective disorders, 208, 414-417.
Hill, C. L., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation.
Emotion, 12(1), 81.
Honkalampi, K., Hintikka, J., Tanskanen, A., Lehtonen, J., & Viinamäki, H. (2000). Depression
is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general population. Journal of
psychosomatic research, 48(1), 99-104.
Hurley, D. B., & Kwon, P. (2012). Results of a study to increase savoring the moment:
Differential impact on positive and negative outcomes. Journal of Happiness Studies,
13(4), 579-588.
Joormann, J., & Arditte, K. (2014). Cognitive aspects of depression. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L.
Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of Depression (3rd ed., pp. 259-276). New York: Guilford.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking Emotion Differentiation:
Transforming Unpleasant Experience by Perceiving Distinctions in Negativity. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 10-16. doi:10.1177/0963721414550708
Kashdan, T. B., DeWall, C. N., Masten, C. L., Pond, R. S., Jr., Powell, C., Combs, D., . . .
Farmer, A. S. (2014). Who Is Most Vulnerable to Social Rejection? The Toxic
Combination of Low Self-Esteem and Lack of Negative Emotion Differentiation on
Neural Responses to Rejection. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e90651.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0090651
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
41
Kashdan, T. B., & Farmer, A. S. (2014). Differentiating Emotions Across Contexts: Comparing
Adults With and Without Social Anxiety Disorder Using Random, Social Interaction, and
Daily Experience Sampling. Emotion, 14(3), 629-638.
Kashdan, T. B., Ferssizidis, P., Collins, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2010). Emotion differentiation as
resilience against excessive alcohol use an ecological momentary assessment in underage
social drinkers. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1341-1347.
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime Prevalence
and Age-of-Onset Distributions of DSM-IV Disorders in the National Comorbidity
Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593-602.
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593
Kircanski, K., Lieberman, M. D., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Feelings Into Words: Contributions of
Language to Exposure Therapy. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1086-1091.
Kovacs, M., Bylsma, L. M., Yaroslavsky, I., Rottenberg, J., George, C. J., Kiss, E., . . . Kapornai,
K. (2016). Positive Affectivity Is Dampened in Youths With Histories of Major
Depression and Their Never-Depressed Adolescent Siblings. Clinical Psychological
Science, 4(4), 661-674. doi:10.1177/2167702615607182
Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2011). Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology: A
Transdiagnostic Approach to Etiology and Treatment New York: Guilford.
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression
severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 606613.
Laurenceau, J.-P., Barrett, L. F., & Rovine, M. J. (2005). The interpersonal process model of
intimacy in marriage: A daily-diary and multilevel modeling approach. Journal of Family
Psychology, 19(2), 314-323. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.314
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
42
Lewinsohn, P. M., & Graf, M. (1973). Pleasant activities and depression. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 41(2), 261-268. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0035142
Lewinsohn, P. M., Sullivan, J. M., & Grosscup, S. J. (1980). Changing reinforcing events: An
approach to the treatment of depression. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice,
17(3), 322-334. doi:10.1037/h0085929
Li, Y. I., Starr, L. R., & Hershenberg, R. (2016). Responses to Positive Affect in Daily Life:
Positive Rumination and Dampening Moderate the Association Between Daily Events
and Depressive Symptoms.
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Corckett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M.
(2007). Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response
to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18(5), 421-428.
Lieberman, M. D., Inagaki, T. K., Tabibnia, G., & Crockett, M. J. (2011). Subjective responses
to emotional stimuli during labeling, reappraisal, and distraction. Emotion, 8(3), 307-301.
Maniaci, M. R., & Rogge, R. D. (2014). Caring about carelessness: Participant inattention and its
effects on research. Journal of Research in Personality, 48(0), 61-83.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.008
Marco, C. A., & Suls, J. (1993). Daily stress and the trajectory of mood: spillover, response
assimilation, contrast, and chronic negative affectivity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 64(6), 1053.
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. Advances in social cognition, 9,
1-47.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
43
McCabe, K. O., Mack, L., & Fleeson, W. (2012). A guide for data cleaning in experience
sampling studies. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of Research Methods
for Studying Daily Life (pp. 321-338). New York: Guilford.
Miranda, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Brooding and reflection: Rumination predicts
suicidal ideation at 1-year follow-up in a community sample. Behaviour research and
therapy, 45(12), 3088-3095.
Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: an
experience sampling study. Journal of abnormal psychology, 117(2), 314.
Nelis, S., Luyckx, K., Feldman, G., Bastin, M., Raes, F., & Bijttebier, P. (2016). Assessing
response styles to positive affect: One or two dimensions of positive rumination in the
Responses to Positive Affect questionnaire? Personality and individual differences, 89,
40-46.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in
depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 1061-1072.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic
stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115-121.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400-424.
O’Toole, M. S., Jensen, M. B., Fentz, H. N., Zachariae, R., & Hougaard, E. (2014). Emotion
Differentiation and Emotion Regulation in High and Low Socially Anxious Individuals:
An Experience-Sampling Study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(4), 428-438.
doi:10.1007/s10608-014-9611-2
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
44
Parrott, W. G. (2002). The functional utility of negative emotions. In L. F. Barrett & P. Salovey
(Eds.), The Wisdom in Feeling: Psychological Processes in Emotional Intelligence (pp.
341-359). New York: Guilford.
Peeters, F., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J., Delespaul, P., & deVries, M. (2003). Effects of daily
events on mood states in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
112(2), 203-211. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.203
Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., Goetz, E. L., Birk, J. L., Bogdan, R., . . . Fava, M.
(2009). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens Response to rewards in unmedicated
individuals with major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(6), 702-
710. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201
Pond, R. S. J., Kashdan, T. B., DeWall, C. N., Savostyanova, A., Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F.
D. (2012). Emotional Differentiation Moderates Aggressive Tendencies in Angry People:
A Daily Diary Analysis. Emotion, 12(2), 326-337.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction
effecs in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis.
Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437-448.
Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Emotional learning and mechanisms of intentional
psychological change.
Quoidbach, J., Berry, E. V., Hansenne, M., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Positive emotion
regulation and well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening
strategies. Personality and individual differences, 49(5), 368-373.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
45
Raes, F., Daems, K., Feldman, G., Johnson, S., & Van Gucht, D. (2010). A psychometric
evaluation of the Dutch version of the responses to positive affect questionnaire.
Psychologica Belgica, 49(4).
Rottenberg, J. (2007). Major depressive disorder: Emerging evidence for emotion context
insensitivity. In J. Rottenberg & S. L. Johnson (Eds.), Emotion and psychopathology:
Bridging affective and clinical science (1 ed., pp. 151-166): American Psychological
Association.
Rush, A. J., Bernstein, I. H., Trivedi, M. H., Carmody, T. J., Wisniewski, S., Mundt, J. C., . . .
Nierenberg, A. A. (2006). An evaluation of the Quick Inventory of Depressive
Symptomatology and the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression: A sequenced treatment
alternatives to relieve depression trial report. Biological Psychiatry, 59(6), 493-501.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.022
Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Ibrahim, H. M., Carmody, T. J., Arnow, B., Klein, D. N., . . .
Manber, R. (2003). The 16-Item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology
(QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): A psychometric
evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 54(5), 573-
583. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01866-8
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other
things called emotion: Dissecting the elephan. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(5), 805-819.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. Social psychology: A
handbook of basic principles, New York: Guilford.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
46
Seal, K. H., Bertenthal, D., Miner, C. R., Sen, S., & Marmar, C. (2007). Bringing the war back
home: Mental health disorders among 103 788 us veterans returning from iraq and
afghanistan seen at department of veterans affairs facilities. Archives of Internal
Medicine, 167(5), 476-482. doi:10.1001/archinte.167.5.476
Selby, E. A., Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., Panza, E., Mitchell, J. E., . . . Le
Grange, D. (2013). Nothing tastes as good as thin feels low positive emotion
differentiation and weight-loss activities in anorexia nervosa. Clinical Psychological
Science, 2167702613512794.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability.
Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420.
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., & Group, P. H. Q. P. C. S. (1999). Validation and
utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. JAMA: the
journal of the American Medical Association, 18, 1737-1744.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Kroenke, K., Homyak, R., & McMurray, J. (2000). Validity and
utility of the PRIME-MD patient health questionnaire in assessment of 3000 obstetric-
gynecologic patients: the PRIME-MD Patient Health Questionnaire Obstetrics-
Gynecology Study. American journal of obsetrics and gynecology, 183(3), 759-769.
Starr, L. R. (2015). When Support Seeking Backfires: Co-Rumination, Excessive Reassurance
Seeking, and Depressed Mood in the Daily Lives of Young Adults. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 34(5), 436-457. doi:10.1521/jscp.2015.34.5.436
Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2012a). Cognitive and Interpersonal Moderators of Daily Co-
Occurrence of Anxious and Depressed Moods in Generalized Anxiety Disorder.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(6), 655-669. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9434-3
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
47
Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2012b). Temporal patterns of anxious and depressed mood in
generalized anxiety disorder: A daily diary study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50,
131-141. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.11.005
Starr, L. R., & Hershenberg, R. (in press). Depressive Symptoms and the Anticipation and
Experience of Uplifting Events in Everyday Life. Journal of Clinical Psychology.
Stone, A. A., Neale, J. M., & Shiffman, S. (1993). Daily assessments of stress and coping and
their association with mood. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 15(1), 8-16.
Thompson, R. J., Mata, J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). The
everyday emotional experience of adults with major depressive disorder: Examining
emotional instability, inertia, and reactivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4),
819-829. doi:10.1037/a0027978
Totenhagen, C. J., Serido, J., Curran, M. A., & Butler, E. A. (2012). Daily hassles and uplifts: A
diary study on understanding relationship quality. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(5),
719-728. doi:10.1037/a0029628
Treadway, M. T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from
translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 537-555.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination Reconsidered: A
Psychometric Analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247-259.
Trivedi, M. H., Rush, A., Ibrahim, H., Carmody, T., Biggs, M., Suppes, T., . . . Dennehy, E.
(2004). The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rating (IDS-C) and
Self-Report (IDS-SR), and the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology,
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
48
Clinician Rating (QIDS-C) and Self-Report (QIDS-SR) in public sector patients with
mood disorders: a psychometric evaluation. Psychological medicine, 34(01), 73-82.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2007). Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion
regulation strategies that promote resilience. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8(3), 311-333.
Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Barrett, L. F. (2004). Psychological Resilience and
Positive Emotional Granularity: Examining the Benefits of Positive Emotions on Coping
and Health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1161-1190. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
6494.2004.00294.x
Vredenburg, K., Flett, G. L., & Krames, L. (1993). Analogue versus clinical depression: A
critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 327-344. doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.113.2.327
Watson, D., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2010). On the specificity of positive emotional dysfunction
in psychopathology: Evidence from the mood and anxiety disorders and
schizophrenia/schizotypy. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 839-848.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.002
Weinberg, A., Liu, H., Hajcak, G., & Shankman, S. A. (2015). Blunted neural response to
rewards as a vulnerability factor for depression: Results from a family study.
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory, Morriston, NJ: General
Learning Press.
Wood, J. V., Heimpel, S. A., & Michela, J. L. (2003). Savoring versus dampening: self-esteem
differences in regulating positive affect. Journal of personality and social psychology,
85(3), 566.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
49
Zaki, L. F., Coifman, K. G., Rafaeli, E., Berenson, K. R., & Downey, G. (2013). Emotion
Differentiation as a Protective Factor Against Nonsuicidal Self-Injury in Borderline
Personality Disorder. Behavior Therapy, 44(3), 529-540.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.008
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
50
Table 1
Demographic Information and Descriptive Data for Study 1 and Study 2
Study 1
Study 2
N
157
50
Age
20.10 (1.23)
56.48 (14.06)
% Female
81%
12%
Race
% Caucasian
% African American
% Asian
% Other
57%
6%
29%
7%
44%
52%
0%
6%?
Baseline Depressive Symptoms
8.81 (5.19)
12.92 (6.00)
Baseline Rumination
56.56 (15.03)
53.90 (15.58)
NED
.83 (.13)
.74 (.20)
PED
.66 (.18)
.56 (.21)
Aggregated Daily/ Momentary Variables
Depressive Sx/ Depressed Mood
4.00 (4.29)
1.85 (.81)
Negative Experiences/ Hassles
7.83 (5.45)
2.41 (.70)
Positive Experiences/ Uplifts
10.71 (6.29)
3.59(.70)
Brooding
9.41 (3.18)
---
Savoring
1.97 (.56)
2.53(.87)
Notes. All variables (except rumination) were assessed using different measures in Study 1 and
Study 2, and therefore descriptive data should not be directly compared across studies. Baseline
depressive symptoms assessed with QIDS (Rush et al., 2003) in Study 1 and PHQ-9 (Kroenke et
al., 2001) in Study 2. NED and PED were computed using different indicators of NEs and PEs
and may also not be directly comparable. See Method sections for Study 1 and Study 2 for more
detail.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
51
Table 2
Results of Multilevel Models Predicting Daily Depressive Symptoms from Daily Variables, as
Moderated by Negative and Positive Emotion Differentiation
Study 1
Study 2
Hypothesis 1
b (SE)
p
95% CI
b (SE)
p
95% CI
Intercept
4.32(0.35)
< .001
[3.63, 5.00]
1.77(.15)
< .001
[1.48, 2.07]
Neg. Experienceswithin
.20(0.03)
< .001
[.14, .25]
.21(.03)
< .001
[.14, .28]
NED
-4.16(2.48)
.096
[-9.06, .74]
-.49(.51)
.357
[-1.49, .56]
NED Neg. Experienceswithin
-.16(0.22)
.468
[-.60, .28]
-.69(.16)
<.001
[-1.02, -.41]
Neg. Experiencesbetween
.30(0.06)
< .001
[.18, .41]
.70(.19)
.001
[.32, 1.07]
NED Neg. Experiencesbetween
.85(0.38)
.027
[.10, 1.60]
-2.12(1.31)
.112
[-4.76, 0.52]
Time
-.06(0.02)
.006
[3.63, 5.00]
.00(.00)
.027
[.00, .00]
Time of Daya
n/a
.00(.00)
.902
[.00, .00]
Hypothesis 2
Intercept
4.81(.38)
< .001
[4.07, 5.55]
1.81(.15)
< .001
[1.52, 2.10]
Pos. Experienceswithin
-.10(.02)
< .001
[-.14, -.05]
-.22(.04)
< .001
[-.29, -.14]
PED
1.99(1.97)
.313
[-1.89, 5.88]
-.07(.50)
.883
[-1.08, .94]
PED Pos. Experienceswithin
.38(.13)
.004
[.12, .64]
.56(.17)
.002
[.21, .90]
Pos. Experiencesbetween
-.03(.05)
.552
[-.14, .07]
-.52(.17)
.003
[-.86, -.19]
PED Pos. Experiencesbetween
.07(.27)
.803
[-.46, .59]
-.29(.65)
.657
[-1.58, 1.01]
Time
-.14(.02)
< .001
[-.19, -.09]
.00(.00)
.029
[.00, .00]
Time of Daya
n/a
.00(.00)
.778
[.00, .00]
Hypothesis 3b
Intercept
4.24(.28)
< .001
[3.69, 4.79]
n/a
Daily Broodingwithin
.46(.04)
< .001
[.38, .55]
NED
-1.80(1.95)
.357
[-5.65, 2.05]
NED Broodingwithin
-.73(.33)
.028
[-1.38, -.08]
Daily Broodingbetween
.80(.07)
< .001
[.65, .94]
NED Broodingbetween
-.45(.50)
.368
[-1.43, .54]
Time
-.07(.02)
< .001
[-.11, -.04]
Hypothesis 4
Intercept
4.73(.35)
< .001
[4.04, 5.42]
1.94(.15)
<.001
[1.64, 2.24]
Daily Savoringwithin
-1.54(.22)
< .001
[-1.97, -1.11]
-.16(.05)
.004
[-.27, -.05]
PED
2.74(1.84)
.139
[-0.9, 6.39]
-.09(.52)
.860
[-1.15, .96]
PED Daily Savoringwithin
4.15(1.19)
.001
[1.79, 6.51]
.48(.26)
.072
[-.05, 1.01]
Daily Savoringbetween
-2.28(.58)
< .001
[-3.43, -1.12]
-.08(.13)
.541
[-.19, .35]
PED Daily Savoringbetween
-1.21(.26)
.672
[-7.50, 4.85]
-1.21(.58)
.029
[-2.30, .-.13]
Time
-.13(0.02)
< .001
[-0.17, -0.08]
.00(.00)
.016
[.00, .00]
Time of Daya
n/a
.00(.00)
.279
[.00, .00]
aStudy 2 only. bHypothesis 3 was not evaluated in Study 2.
Notes. Focal moderation findings are bolded. Daily depressive symptoms (depressed mood in Study
2) were the outcome variable in all models. Positive and negative experiences defined respectively
as uplifts and hassles in Study 1 and pleasantness/ unpleasantness of recent activities in Study 2.
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
52
1
1.2
1.4
1.6
1.8
2
2.2
2.4
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Momentary Depressed Mood
Negative Experiences
2
2.5
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
6.5
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Daily Depressive Symptoms
Negative Experiences
2
2.5
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
6.5
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Daily Depressive Symptoms
Daily Brooding
Study 1 Study 2
a) i. ii.
(note: non-significant interaction)
b)
Figure 1
Daily depressive symptoms as a function of a) recent negative experiences (Hypothesis 1, with results from i) Study 1 and ii) Study 2)
and b) daily brooding (Hypothesis 3, Study 1 only), as moderated by negative emotion differentiation
EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS
53
1.3
1.4
1.5
1.6
1.7
1.8
1.9
2
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Momentary Depressed Mood
Positive Experiences
1.3
1.4
1.5
1.6
1.7
1.8
1.9
2
2.1
2.2
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Momentary Depressed Mood
Momentary Savoring
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Daily Depressive Symptoms
Positive Experiences
3
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
Low (M -1 SD) High (M +1 SD)
Daily Depressive Symptoms
Daily Savoring
Study 1 Study 2
a) i. ii.
b) i. ii.
Figure 2
Findings from i) Study 1 and ii) Study 2 showing daily depressive symptoms as a function of a) recent positive experiences
(Hypothesis 2) and b) recent savoring (Hypothesis 4), as moderated by positive emotion differentiation
... Low negative emotion differentiation (NED) has been associated with negative outcomes, including lower self-esteem (Willroth et al., 2020), rumination (Seah et al., 2020), and poor emotion regulation (Barrett et al., 2001;Tong & Keng, 2017). Notably, low NED has consistently been associated with depressive symptoms (e.g., Erbas et al., 2014Erbas et al., , 2018Plonsker et al., 2017;Starr, Hershenberg, et al., 2017). In contrast, positive emotion differentiation (PED) and depression have been unrelated in most studies (e.g., Demiralp et al., 2012;Lennarz et al., 2018;Starr, Hershenberg, et al., 2017). ...
... Notably, low NED has consistently been associated with depressive symptoms (e.g., Erbas et al., 2014Erbas et al., , 2018Plonsker et al., 2017;Starr, Hershenberg, et al., 2017). In contrast, positive emotion differentiation (PED) and depression have been unrelated in most studies (e.g., Demiralp et al., 2012;Lennarz et al., 2018;Starr, Hershenberg, et al., 2017). ...
... While the lexical facilitation hypothesis is aligned with current theory, it is at odds with some existing data. Although the EV-ED association has never been tested, the constructs have demonstrated opposite relationships with depression: NED has been shown to be protective against depression (e.g., Erbas et al., 2018;Starr, Hershenberg, et al., 2017;Willroth et al., 2020), whereas NEV has been associated with greater depressive symptoms (Entwistle et al., 2023;Vine et al., 2020). Moreover, emerging data suggest that the relationship between ED (and adjacent constructs) and language abilities may be more complex than previously assumed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion differentiation (ED; the ability to distinguish discrete internal emotion states) may reflect or benefit from knowledge of linguistic labels. The present study uses natural language processing to examine how emotion vocabulary (EV; diversity of unique emotion terms within active vocabulary) relates to ED and depression in an adolescent sample. We tested two competing preregistered (https://osf.io/4j75w/) models regarding the EV–ED link. In the lexical facilitation hypothesis, we posited that larger EV may inform ED, perhaps resulting in larger EVs being associated with greater ED. In the emotional concision hypothesis, we theorized that ED may reflect narrower emotional experiences that are more succinctly labelled, which could result in larger EV being associated with lower ED. A community sample of adolescents (N = 241, ages 14–17, predominantly White) completed interviews, self-report measures, and ecological momentary assessments as part of a larger study conducted between 2014 and 2016. EV was derived using speech samples from transcribed recordings of life stress interviews. In line with the emotion concision hypothesis, EV and ED were inversely related for negative emotions. Moreover, larger negative EV and lower negative ED were each uniquely associated with depression, casting further doubt on whether diverse negative EVs within spontaneous language are fundamentally adaptive for emotional functioning. Replication in more diverse samples is needed to extend generalizability.
... Une seconde série d'observations a révélé le lien entre la granularité, l'ajustement psychologique et le bien-être. En effet, des niveaux élevés de granularité sont associés à une meilleure capacité à gérer les stresseurs et émotions négatives 13,29,31,32,33,34 . Par conséquent, de tels individus font preuve de plus d'adaptation, de moins d'anxiété et d'une meilleure santé mentale 20,23,29,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44 . ...
... En effet, des niveaux élevés de granularité sont associés à une meilleure capacité à gérer les stresseurs et émotions négatives 13,29,31,32,33,34 . Par conséquent, de tels individus font preuve de plus d'adaptation, de moins d'anxiété et d'une meilleure santé mentale 20,23,29,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44 . Entre autres, les personnes avec de hauts niveaux de granularité ont moins tendance à s'adonner à des comportements inadaptés comme la consommation excessive d'alcool et de nourriture 45,46 , l'utilisation de drogues 47 ou l'agressivité 15 . ...
... Evidence collected in the last two decades provides consistent support for a link between emotional granularity and mental health (Erbas et al., 2014(Erbas et al., , 2018Starr et al., 2017;Seah et al., 2020), physical health (Hoemann et al., 2021b;Bonar et al., 2023), and adaptive health behavior (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014;Kashdan et al., 2015). Note that this is different from other findings that study how the prevalence of specific emotions varies with mental health, (for example, people with depression tend to use more sadness-associated words). ...
... Individuals with higher emotion granularity tend to use a broader range of strategies to deal with negative emotions, and are more successful at doing so (Barrett et al., 2001). Several studies have shown that emotion granularity is lower in individuals with mental health conditions like bipolar disorder (Suvak et al., 2011;Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014), manic depressive disorder (Demiralp et al., 2012), schizophrenia (Kring et al., 2003, autism spectrum disorder (Erbas et al., 2013), and affective disorders like anxiety (Seah et al., 2020) and depression (Starr et al., 2017;Willroth et al., 2020). Lower granularity is also associated with increased tendencies to engage in maladaptive behaviour, such as alcohol consumption (Kashdan et al., 2015;Emery et al., 2014) and aggression (Pond Jr et al., 2012). ...
... In adults, both higher emotional clarity and negative emotion differentiation were associated with fewer selfreported emotion regulation difficulties (Jones & Herr, 2018;Vine & Aldao, 2014). More specifically, higher clarity and more negative differentiation were associated with less use of repetitive negative thinking, less use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, and more effective implementation of regulatory behavior (Brown et al., 2021;Cabello et al., 2013;Gross & John, 2003;Kalokerinos et al., 2019;Starr et al., 2017;Vine & Aldao, 2014;Vine & Marroquín, 2018). However, all of these studies focused either on the associations of emotional clarity or emotion differentiation with emotion regulation. ...
... Regarding positive emotion differentiation, youth reported decreased use of rumination on days with higher-than-usual emotion differentiation. This finding is in line with past work emphasizing the protective role of positive emotion differentiation in emotion regulation across different age groups (e.g., Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014;Nook et al., 2021;Starr et al., 2017). In addition, it further extends prior research on youth and thereby emphasizes the important role of positive affect in this specific population (Gilbert, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional clarity and emotion differentiation (ED) are two core aspects of the application of emotional knowledge. During adolescence, novel emotional experiences result in temporary decreases of differentiation and clarity. These temporary difficulties might profoundly impact choices of regulatory strategies. And indeed, prior research has shown that lower emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are each associated with higher use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in youth. The two constructs, however, are rarely examined together, and it remains unclear how they are associated in daily life, particularly in children and adolescents. In addition, previous studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. To address these gaps, the present study used an intensive longitudinal design in youth. Between June 2021 and March 2022, 172 children and adolescents (M = 12.99 years) completed a 28-day diary (>3,500 entries in total) reporting daily affect, emotional clarity, and the use of five emotion regulation strategies in response to negative and positive affect (i.e., rumination, dampening, behavioral avoidance, negative and positive suppression). As predicted, on both between- and within-person levels, higher emotional clarity was associated with decreased use of all maladaptive emotion regulation strategies after adjusting for mean affect intensity. Results for emotion differentiation were mostly nonsignificant. Only higher daily positive emotion differentiation was associated with decreased rumination. In sum, this innovative study explores multiple aspects of emotional knowledge usage and regulation during a critical developmental stage and emphasizes the role of emotional clarity in the regulation of negative and positive affect.
... The consistently larger effect sizes related to negative sentiment, compared to positive sentiment, suggest that negative emotions (such as shame, anxiety, anger, and disgust) played a more dominant role in the lives of high-risk participants in this sample, than the mere absence of positive emotions or anhedonia. This may reflect a characteristic manifestation of depression among young adults 32 . Studies show that depression is highly prevalent among this age group: ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Despite the growing prevalence of depression, current diagnostic methods sometimes fail to identify individual cases, particularly in the presence of comorbid conditions. This study explores the potential of AI-assisted generative art as a complementary tool for assessing depression risk. A sample of 230 participants (Mage = 21.54 years) used Midjourney, a text-to-image software, to create images of themselves using natural language prompts. Depression risk was measured using the 9-item Beck Depression Inventory, and prompt sentiment was analyzed with a pretrained RoBERTa model. Multiple significant correlations of moderate strength were identified, with negative sentiment emerging as the strongest predictor of depression scores. A random forest classifier achieved 80% sensitivity (AUC = 0.83) in identifying high-risk individuals, with even stronger metrics observed under a stricter cutoff criterion, though this introduces methodological challenges related to class imbalance. These findings suggest that cognitive and emotional biases associated with depression can influence self-expression in generative art, highlighting its potential for capturing depressive symptoms and advancing machine-learning-based screening algorithms in the future.
... Past research provides broad support for the benefits of ED, particularly negative ED (NED), on psychological health and functioning across clinical and non-clinical samples. NED is associated with less negative affect Erbas et al., 2014), adaptive emotion regulation (Barrett et al., 2001;Kalokerinos et al., 2019), lower depression (Erbas et al., 2014;Starr et al., 2017), less anxiety (Matt et al., 2016), greater well-being , and decreased maladaptive behaviours like nonsuicidal self-injury (Zaki et al., 2013), problematic alcohol use (Kashdan et al., 2010), and avoidance of social activities (Seah et al., 2020). NED is also associated with adaptive health behaviours in patients with chronic illnesses (Coifman et al., 2014; and lower likelihood of engaging in substance use and binge eating during the COVID-19 pandemic (Seah et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion differentiation (ED) – complexity in the mental representation and description of one’s emotional experiences – is important for mental health. However, less is known whether ED can be enhanced in adults. We investigated if scaffolding emotion language use during affect labelling – initial provision of emotion word prompts (close-ended) followed by free response (open-ended) – impacts ED and psychological health. Utilising a crossover design, 92 college students completed questionnaires assessing psychological health at three time-points and ecological momentary assessment of emotions, affect valence and emotional self-efficacy for 14 days. Participants were randomised to the “scaffolding” group, where they reported emotions using the close-ended (7 days) followed by open-ended (7 days) approach, or the reverse sequence (control group). We extracted two ED indices: traditional intraclass correlation coefficient from close-ended reports and novel specificity index from open-ended reports. Primary analyses examined group differences across weeks while exploratory analyses examined moment-to-moment differences using multilevel modelling. Relative to controls, the scaffolding group demonstrated greater ED during open-ended emotion reporting of negative emotions and associated shifts in negative affect and emotional self-efficacy. There were no significant group differences in psychological symptoms. Results provide preliminary evidence that scaffolding may enhance ED and have implications for psychological intervention.
... Measures of label uniqueness were associated with mental ill-health differently than they were in Study 1. Namely, participants who used more unique words for positive emotion over the course of experience sampling also reported more symptoms of anxiety and depression. This finding is broadly in keeping with work showing that reporting on pleasant experiences with greater precision inhibits appreciation and enjoyment (i.e., savoring; e.g., Starr et al., 2017). It is possible that diversity of emotion word use reflects regular attempts to identify the 'correct' label; these attempts may indicate (or induce) a more abstract or psychologically distanced mode of processing, which in turn may signal dampening or avoidance of positive emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
To measure emotion in daily life, studies often prompt participants to repeatedly rate their feelings on a set of prespecified terms. This approach has yielded key findings in the psychological literature yet may not represent how people typically describe their experiences. We used an alternative approach, in which participants labeled their current emotion with at least one word of their choosing. In an initial study, estimates of label positivity recapitulated momentary valence ratings and were associated with self-reported mental health. The number of unique emotion words used over time was related to the balance and spread of emotions endorsed in an end-of-day rating task, but not to other measures of emotional functioning. A second study tested and replicated a subset of these findings. Considering the variety and richness of participant responses, a free-label approach appears to be a viable as well as compelling means of studying emotion in everyday life.
... This finding is in line with past work emphasizing the protective role of positive emotion differentiation in emotion regulation across different age groups (e.g., Dixon-Gordon et al., 2014;Nook et al., 2021;Starr et al., 2017). In addition, it further extends prior research on youth and thereby emphasizes the important role of positive affect in this specific population (Gilbert, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are two core aspects of the application of emotional knowledge. During adolescence, novel emotional experiences result in temporary decreases of differentiation and clarity. These temporary difficulties might profoundly impact choices of regulatory strategies. And indeed, prior research has shown that lower emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are each associated with higher use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in youth. The two constructs, however, are rarely examined together and it remains unclear how they are associated in daily life, particularly in children and adolescents. In addition, previous studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. To address these gaps, the current study used an intensive longitudinal designin youth. Between June 2021 and March 2022, 172 children and adolescents (M= 12.99 years) completed a 28-day diary (>3500 entries in total) reporting daily affect, emotional clarity, and use of five emotion regulation strategies in response to negative and positive affect (i.e., rumination, dampening, behavioral avoidance, negative and positive suppression). As predicted, on both between- and within-person levels, higher emotional clarity was associated with decreased use of all maladaptive emotion regulation strategies after adjusting for mean affect intensity. Results for emotion differentiation were mostly non-significant. Only higher daily positive emotion differentiation was associated with decreased rumination. In sum, this innovative study explores multiple aspects of emotional knowledge usage and regulation during a critical developmental stage and emphasizes the role of emotional clarity in the regulation of negative and positive affect.
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to systematically examine the relationship between emotion differentiation (ED) and depressive symptoms. Cochrane Library, Web of Science, Embase, EBSCO, PubMed and Science Direct databases were searched from the time of build to March 25, 2023 for studies that: (i) were in English, (ii) peer-reviewed, (iii) reported correlations between PED/NED and depressive symptoms, and (iv) NED, PED, and NA were derived from emotional scores using ecological momentary assessment, daily diaries, and standardized stimuli ratings. Exclusions included duplicates, non-empirical research, studies not focused on PED/NED or depressive symptoms, conference abstracts, review articles, and studies lacking correlations between PED/NED and depressive symptoms. Review Manager 5.4 and Stata 17.0 software were used to perform meta-analysis, subgroup analysis, sensitivity analysis, meta-regression analysis and publication bias detection of the extracted data. A total of 16 samples with 2515 participants were included. Some evaluated studies found a negative correlation between negative emotion differentiation (NED) and depressive symptoms, while others reported positive association. No significant correlation was observed between positive emotion differentiation (PED) and depressive symptoms. Meta-analysis revealed a significantly negative association between NED and depressive symptoms (r = -0.14, 95% CI= [-0.21, -0.06], p < 0.01). However, PED was not significantly related to depressive symptoms (r = -0.02, 95% CI = [-0.07, 0.03], p = 0.41). Critically, the correlation between NED and depressive symptoms was moderated by mean negative affect (NA) (t = 3.50, p< 0.01, 95% CI = [0.297, 1.377]), whereas diagnostic status, assessment of NED, age and proportion of female did not emerge as moderators. This study confirmed the negative association between NED and depressive symptoms, in particular when experiencing more intensive negative emotions, whereas the null association between PED and depressive symptoms. In addition, the diagnostic status, assessment of NED, age and proportion of female did not affect the link. Our findings have important clinical implications for facilitating the comprehending, prevention and treatment of depression. We discuss limitations as well as suggestions for future research.
Article
Full-text available
p style="text-align: justify;">The article reviews of a number of modern foreign studies of emotion differentiation and its role in emotion regulation processes. An attempt is made to generalise the currently existing results on the connection of emotional differentiation with various personality and behavioural constructs, as well as about its role of in psychological well-being. The main advantages of high negative emotional differentiation and disadvantages of low negative emotional differentiation are identified. Trait and momentary emotional differentiation was distinguished, difficulties in conceptualising and measuring the momentary emotional differentiation were outlined, the lines of future studies of the relationship between both measures of emotional differentiation and emotion regulation were described. Several studies of the role of emotional differentiation in the choice and effectiveness of putatively adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies were analysed. It was found that higher levels of negative emotional differentiation were related to the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies rather than to their choice. Similarities and differences in the methodology and obtained results of the analysed studies are identified. Issues requiring further research are formulated, prospects for future research are described. The practical significance of the research findings for the use in psychotherapy and treatment of psychological disorders is outlined.</p
Article
Full-text available
Age differences in emotional experience over the adult life span were explored, focusing on the frequency, intensity, complexity, and consistency of emotional experience in everyday life. One hundred eighty-four people, age 18 to 94 years, participated in an experience-sampling procedure in which emotions were recorded across a 1-week period. Age was unrelated to frequency of positive emotional experience. A curvilinear relationship best characterized negative emotional experience. Negative emotions declined in frequency until approximately age 60, at which point the decline ceased. Individual factor analyses computed for each participant revealed that age was associated with more differentiated emotional experience. In addition, periods of highly positive emotional experience were more likely to endure among older people and periods of highly negative emotional experience were less stable. Findings are interpreted within the theoretical framework of socioemotional selectivity theory.
Article
Full-text available
What is the structure of emotion? Emotion is too broad a class of events to be a single scientific category, and no one structure suffices. As an illustration, core affect is distinguished from prototypical emotional episode. Core affect refers to consciously accessible elemental processes of pleasure and activation, has many causes, and is always present. Its structure involves two bipolar dimensions. Prototypical emotional episode refers to a complex process that unfolds over time, involves causally connected subevents (antecedent; appraisal; physiological, affective, and cognitive changes; behavioral response; self-categorization), has one perceived cause, and is rare. Its structure involves categories (anger, fear, shame, jealousy, etc.) vertically organized as a fuzzy hierarchy and horizontally organized as part of a circumplex.
Article
Full-text available
Experience sampling methodology was used to examine the effects of current and prior problems on negative mood within and across days. Forty male community residents wore signal watches and kept diary records of problem occurrence and mood 8 times a day for 8 consecutive days. Trait negative affectivity (NA), prior mood, and concurrent stress were related to mood during the day. Mood in response to a current problem was worse if the prior time had been problem free than if the prior time had been stressful. High NA Ss were more reactive to concurrent stressors than were low NAs, but the effect was small. NA and current-day stress were the major influences of mood across days. High NAs were more distressed by current-day problems and recovered more slowly from problems of the preceding day. The benefits of conceptualizing the effects of daily stressors on mood in terms of spillover, response assimilation, habituation, and contrast are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Despite proliferation of laboratory-based studies examining reward functioning in depression, few studies have examined these processes in everyday life. We addressed this shortcoming by exploring experience and anticipation of uplifting experiences under ecologically valid conditions METHOD: One hundred fifty-seven young adults, oversampled for depressive symptoms, completed a 14-day diary tracking mood in relation to recent and anticipated positive events RESULTS: Consistent with studies supporting "mood-brightening" effects in depression, participants with greater baseline dysphoria showed stronger associations between elevated daily uplifts and lower daily depressive symptoms, particularly when events were interpersonal in nature. Baseline dysphoria was associated with lower daily anticipation of positive next-day experiences; however, when dysphoric individuals did anticipate positive experiences, they experienced greater reductions in depressed mood CONCLUSION: Results suggest that despite reward processing deficits found in laboratory studies, dysphoric individuals show improvements in mood in conjunction with anticipation and consumption of uplifting events in daily life.
Article
Full-text available
Depressive rumination has been strongly linked to the development and maintenance of depression; however, less attention has been paid to ruminative processes in response to positive affect, and fewer have examined these processes in daily life. The current study sought to address these gaps by exploring depressive rumination and two forms of responses to positive affect, dampening and positive rumination, under ecologically valid conditions using daily diary methodology. One hundred fifty-seven young adults completed 14-day end-of-day diaries assessing positive affect and depressive symptoms in relation to depressive rumination, responses to positive affect, and daily positive and negative events. Daily depressive rumination predicted stronger associations between negative events and daily depressive symptoms. Higher daily dampening was associated with higher daily depressive symptoms and decreased positive affect and predicted lower associations between daily positive events and improvements in mood (including reduced daily positive affect and increased daily depressive symptoms). Higher daily positive rumination was negatively associated with daily depressive symptoms and interacted with daily positive events such that positive rumination had a greater impact on depressed mood on days when positive experiences were low. Results indicate that both depressive rumination and responses to positive affect play a role in influencing daily mood and depressive symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
It was hypothesized that women are more vulnerable to depressive symptoms than men because they are more likely to experience chronic negative circumstances (or strain), to have a low sense of mastery, and to engage in ruminative coping. The hypotheses were tested in a 2-wave study of approximately 1,100 community-based adults who were 25 to 75 years old. Chronic strain, low mastery, and rumination were each more common in women than in men and mediated the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Rumination amplified the effects of mastery and, to some extent, chronic strain on depressive symptoms. In addition, chronic strain and rumination had reciprocal effects on each other over time, and low mastery also contributed to more rumination. Finally, depressive symptoms contributed to more rumination and less mastery over time.
Article
Background: Decreased enjoyment from pleasant events is a key component of anhedonia, but evidence has been inconsistent demonstrating its association across levels of depressive symptom severity. We test the hypothesis that depressed participants who engage in greater (rather than lower) concurrent use of experiential avoidance strategies will demonstrate impaired positive (PA) and negative (NA) emotional reactivity when pleasant events take place. Methods: 50 Veterans with a range of depression severity completed a 7-day phone-based ecological momentary assessment protocol that assessed the pleasantness of their recent activity, level of PA and NA, and concurrent use of experiential avoidance strategies. Results: As events were rated as more pleasant, depressed Veterans using less experiential avoidance were distinguished from depressed Veterans using greater experiential avoidance, such that greater experiential avoidance interfered with PA and NA reactivity. Limitations: Small sample of primarily older men, all were Veterans, and assessments relied on self-reports of event pleasantness and depression; we did not include a control group. Conclusions: It is critical to understand how depressed individuals experience potentially rewarding aspects of their environments. Our study provides preliminary data that depressed individuals may benefit from positive events in daily life when experiential avoidance is low and may demonstrate impaired reactivity when avoidance is high. This study may help clinicians to identify the contexts that support hedonic responses to potentially rewarding aspects of their depressed patients' environments.