ArticlePublisher preview available

The Repeated Recording Illusion: The Effects of Extrinsic and Individual Difference Factors on Musical Judgments

University of California Press
Music Perception: an interdisciplinary journal
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The repeated recording illusion refers to the phenomenon in which listeners are under the impression that they hear different musical stimuli while they are in fact identical. This phenomenon has not yet been studied systematically. Thus, the present paper aims to construct an experimental paradigm to enable the systematic measurement of the repeated recording illusion, investigating individual difference factors that contribute to it as well as extrinsic factors responsible for differences in musical judgements when the acoustic input remains the same. Seventy-two participants were misled to think that they had heard three different musical performances of an original piece when in fact they were exposed to the same repeated recording. Each time, the recording was accompanied by a different text suggesting a low, medium or high prestige of the performer. Most participants (75 %) believed that they had heard different musical performances. High levels of neuroticism and openness made it significantly more likely that an individual would fall for the illusion. Musicians were not any more or any less susceptible to the illusion than nonmusicians. For participants who fell for the illusion, the explicit prestige texts influenced evaluations of the music significantly. The effect of repeated exposure was only significant in the more familiar music condition. These results suggest that musical judgements are sometimes not based on musical cues but are influenced by factors that do not depend on the music itself. The repeated recording illusion can constitute a paradigm for investigating psychological biases and individual differences in aesthetic and musical judgements because the illusion allows for the study of their effects while the music remains the same. Results are interpreted within Tversky and Kahneman’s framework of judgements and decision-making. Pre-press manuscript (accepted in 2017, Music Perception, http://mp.ucpress.edu)
THE REPEATED RECORDING ILLUSION:THE EFFECTS OF EXTRINSIC
AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCE FACTORS ON MUSICAL JUDGMENTS
MANUEL ANGLADA-TORT &DANIEL MU
¨LLENSIEFEN
Goldsmiths, University of London, London, United
Kingdom
T
HE REPEATED RECORDING ILLUSION REFERS TO
the phenomenon in which listeners believe to hear
different musical stimuli while they are in fact identical.
The present paper aims to construct an experimental
paradigm to enable the systematic measurement of this
phenomenon, investigating potentially related extrinsic
and individual difference factors. Participants were told
to listen to ‘‘different’ musical performances of an orig-
inal piece when in fact they were exposed to the same
repeated recording. Each time, the recording was
accompanied by a text suggesting a low, medium, or
high prestige of the performer. Most participants
(75%) believed that they had heard different musical
performances. Participants with high levels of neuroti-
cism and openness were significantly more likely to fall
for the illusion. While the explicit information pre-
sented with the music influenced participants’ ratings
significantly, the effect of repeated exposure was only
significant in the more familiar music condition. These
results suggest that like many other human judgments,
evaluations of music also rely on cognitive biases and
heuristics that do not depend on the stimuli themselves.
The repeated recording illusion can constitute a useful
paradigm for investigating nonmusical factors because
it allows for the study of their effects while the music
remains the same.
Received: September 22, 2016, accepted February 24,
2017.
Key words: aesthetics, individual differences, explicit
information, music performance, judgments and
preferences
I
N
1977,
THE
G
ERMAN RADIO STATION
WDR 3
conducted an audience participation experiment
during a live program (see the description in Behne,
1987). The radio broadcaster misled the audience to
think that they would hear three different performances
ofthesameexcerptofBrucknersSymphonyNo.4,
providing brief information about three different con-
ductors (Karl Bo¨hm, Leonard Bernstein, and Herbert
von Karajan) just before each recording was played.
However, the radio broadcaster played the same record-
ing three times. The radio station received 536 calls.
81.7%of the callers were misled and reported differ-
ences between the identical music recordings. Only the
remaining 18.3%of the listeners who called in reported
that there were no differences between the three perfor-
mances. Nevertheless, we note that the audience partic-
ipation experiment had several shortcomings, such as
a lack of control over experimental conditions and
a potential sampling bias for those listeners who
believed they had heard different musical performances
to call the radio station. Therefore, one of the main
motivations of the present paper was the replication
of this phenomenon in an experimental setting.
We will refer to this phenomenon, where listeners are
under the impression that they hear different musical
performances while in fact they are identical, as the
repeated recording illusion. Duerksen (1972) was
amongstthefirstacademicstudiestouseasimilar
approach. He played two tape recordings of an identical
piano performance to music major and nonmusic major
students. Participants were told that one performance
was by an eminent professional pianist and the other
one by a student. Both groups rated technical and musi-
cal characteristics of the music recording consistently
lower when told the performance was by a student than
when told it was by a professional. However, Duerksen
(1972) merely attributed the findings to an effect of
expectations and did not investigate whether partici-
pants believed that they had heard the same or different
musical performances.
There are a number of studies that used similar exper-
imental paradigms, presenting participants with identi-
cal recordings in succession (Behne & Wo¨llner, 2011;
Cavitt, 1997, 2002; Elliott, 1995; Griffiths, 2008; Juch-
niewicz, 2008; Radocy, 1976; Silvey, 2009). The main
purpose of these studies was to investigate nonmusical
factors that influence evaluations of musical perfor-
mances, such as the effect of expectations (Cavitt,
1997, 2002; Duerksen, 1972), authority (Radocy,
1976), musicians’ body movements (Behne & Wo¨llner,
Music Perception,VOLUME 35, ISSUE 1, PP. 94–117, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312. ©2017 BY THE R EGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA ALL
RIGHTS RESERVED.PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESSS
REPRINTS AND PERMISSIONS WEB PAGE,HTTP://WWW.UCPRESS.EDU/JOURNALS.PHP?P¼REPRINTS. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2017.35.1.94
94 Manuel Anglada-Tort & Daniel Mu¨llensiefen
... Existing literature has revealed an important role of "context" or "source" information in influencing music appreciation (Thompson et al., 2023). For instance, by manipulating details about the performer, program notes, titles or origins of the music pieces, previous studies have shown that such contextual cues significantly affect listeners' evaluation of the same music performance (e.g., Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Anglada-Tort et al., 2021;Aydogan et al., 2018;Kroger & Margulis, 2017). While these studies demonstrate the effect of contextual information on music judgment in general, no study so far has systematically examined the distinction between analytical (i.e., about core music elements and emotional expressions) versus historical (i.e., about stylistic contexts and backgrounds) types of musical knowledge. ...
... Our finding showing enhanced music liking after analytical knowledge acquisition also aligns with and extends previous evidence on the influential role of source information in aesthetic evaluation (e.g., Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Anglada-Tort et al., 2019). Importantly, we reveal that analytical knowledge targeting core aspects of the music has a greater appreciative effect than historical knowledge, highlighting the significance of this type of knowledge distinction. ...
Article
Full-text available
People possessing musical knowledge tend to enjoy music more, but the linkage remains to be determined. Based on the shared affective motion experience model for music appreciation, we hypothesized that acquiring musical knowledge about the music itself, for example, an analytical understanding of music elements and the related emotional expressions, would increase music liking. To test the hypothesis, we asked 48 participants to learn analytical or historical information about a piece of music by watching a pre‐recorded teaching video. Learners' physiological responses, such as skin conductance and heart rate, were recorded during learning. The increase of music liking was observed after both types of knowledge acquisition, but more so for analytical knowledge. Notably, acquiring analytical knowledge made learners' skin conductance more similar, indicating the alignment of physiological responses. This physiological similarity, correlated with analytical knowledge similarity, could mediate the effect of knowledge acquisition on music liking. In sum, this study reveals the impact of analytical knowledge on music enjoyment and the associated neurophysiological mechanism. It extends the theoretical framework of shared affective motion experience to explain how musical knowledge influences music appreciation.
... Simply providing a program note-either dramatic or purely descriptive-has been shown to increase the level of attention paid to a performance (Margulis et al., 2015). Furthermore, listeners' perception, preferences, and evaluation of music can be altered by explicitly manipulating a program note (Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Fischinger et al., 2020). For example, explicitly emotional programs can intensify the emotions invoked by music (Vuoskoski & Eerola, 2015), whereas information regarding a composer's expressive intent has the power to alter music's perceived expressive content . ...
... It has been shown that listeners are capable of perceiving distinct emotional categories within music from unfamiliar foreign sources (Balkwill & Thompson, 1999). There is also evidence that listeners are more likely to engage with an imagined extramusical narrative in a piece of music from a familiar cultural tradition and that this, in turn, leads to greater enjoyment (Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Margulis et al., 2019). Furthermore, listeners struggle to remember music from an unfamiliar culture, as they rely on encultured schematic understandings of music when processing novel stimuli (Demorest et al., 2016;Demorest & Morrison, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Music appreciation is a complex process that involves responses to surface-level structure, personal associations, and source sensitivity. Source sensitivity is an understanding of the context in which a musical artifact was created. This article joins a growing body of literature in which program notes are manipulated to highlight the importance of source sensitivity, shifting the focus onto cultural context. Two hundred eighty-four participants formed six groups in a 2×3 between-subjects design. Western and non-Western participants (ethnicity condition) listened to an original percussion composition accompanied by a short text providing a Western, Indian, or blended cultural context (cultural context condition). They then evaluated the music across a range of measures. Exploratory factor analysis revealed two factors of appreciation: Hedonic and Eudaimonic. Results revealed significant ethnicity–cultural context interactions for both. In the Indian context, Western participants exhibited high appreciation, whereas non-Western listeners exhibited low appreciation. Among non-Westerners, appreciation was highest in the blended context. Our results demonstrate an association between cultural source information and music appreciation. We propose that Western participants experience a proteophilic response to Indian music due to their secure status as members of a dominant social group. Non-Western participants, however, require a neutralizing Western context to similarly appreciate Indian music content.
... Possibly related to esthetic experience, music listening causes affective and physiological reactions, such as thrills, awe, and the state of being moved [24,25]. Even though there is evidence suggesting that musical training has very little effect on the perception of emotion in music [25] and that evaluations of music also rely on cognitive biases and heuristics that do not depend on the stimuli themselves [26], musicians and non-musicians react differently to sadness and fear, both at behavioral and physiological levels [27]. More precisely, sadness and fear conveyed by music would be more arousing for musicians, suggesting an association between musical training and the processing of "negative" emotions, while non-specific activations would be brought about by happiness. ...
... The study aimed then at assessing the possible role of regions involved in the neuroesthetics of art [1], in that of music [38,39]. [19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26] who reported no previous training or special interest in music (e.g., music bloggers). The musician group included 20 musicians (F = 10; mean age = 21.5, S.D. 3.26; range 18-31 years old) with musical education (graduating or graduated students of the Italian Musical Conservatory). ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans are the only species capable of experiencing pleasure from esthetic stimuli, such as art and music. Neuroimaging evidence suggests that the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) plays a critical role in esthetic judgments, both in music and in visual art. In the last decade, non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) has been increasingly employed to shed light on the causal role of different brain regions contributing to esthetic appreciation. In Experiment #1, musician (N = 20) and non-musician (N = 20) participants were required to judge musical stimuli in terms of “liking” and “emotions”. No significant differences between groups were found, although musicians were slower than non-musicians in both tasks, likely indicating a more analytic judgment, due to musical expertise. Experiment #2 investigated the putative causal role of the left dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) in the esthetic appreciation of music, by means of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Unlike previous findings in visual art, no significant effects of tDCS were found, suggesting that stimulating the left DLPFC is not enough to affect the esthetic appreciation of music, although this conclusion is based on negative evidence.
... Programming also influences the evaluation of music (e.g. liking or virtuosity) (Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Fischinger et al., 2020) and perception of its basic characteristics (Chapman, 1981;North & Hargreaves, 2010;Fischinger et al., 2020). The typical atmosphere of a classical concert hall as a priming or framing factor of music experience has not been analyzed to a comprehensive extent so far . ...
Chapter
Digital Concert Experience (DCE) is a research project designed and conducted to contribute to a better understanding of audience experience in online streams of Western classical music in ecologically valid settings. Based on an online survey, a latent profile analysis identified three different audience segments of classical concert streams. These are differentiated by different preferences for stream characteristics. In a subsequent experiment, participants watched one out of four different stream variations and were asked to rate different aspects of their concert experience. Seven main dimensions of concert experience were derived from the questionnaire answers and subsequently analyzed with respect to presented stream variations. Concert streams were found to have the potential to evoke high appreciation of music, but to evoke lower degrees of emotional and immersive experience, according to our results. Social interaction opportunities available during the stream led to increased feelings of social connection. Additional information about a certain piece in the programme had a strong effect on the understanding of it. Moreover, we observed differentiated forms of concert experience within each preference group dependent on presented stream variation. We close the chapter by providing an outlook on our Experimental Concert Research (ECR) project, which aims to holistically explore the concert experience in conventional live concerts with physical co-presence of musicians and audience in an ecologically valid way. Both projects contribute to the empirical study of the classical concert experience with regard to current challenges of classical music in the cultural sector.
... [11][12][13]), or factors related to the context and social situation where the listening is taking place (e.g. [14][15][16][17]). Very little research on musical behaviour has considered broad environmental influences that exist over extended periods of time, such as seasonal patterns and prevailing weather conditions. ...
Article
Full-text available
We examine associations between prevailing weather conditions and music features in all available songs that reached the United Kingdom weekly top charts throughout a 67-year period (1953-2019), comprising 23 859 unique entries. We found that music features reflecting high intensity and positive emotions were positively associated with daily temperatures and negatively associated with rainfall, whereas music features reflecting low intensity and negative emotions were not related to weather conditions. These results held true after controlling for the mediating effects of year (temporal patterns) and month (seasonal patterns). However, music-weather associations were more nuanced than previously assumed by linear models, becoming only meaningful in those months and seasons when changes in weather were the most notable. Importantly, the observed associations depended on the popularity of the music: while songs in the top 10 of the charts exhibited the strongest associations with weather, less popular songs showed no relationship. This suggests that a song's fit with prevailing weather may be a factor pushing a song into the top of the charts. Our work extends previous research on non-musical domains (e.g. finance, crime, mental health) by showing that large-scale population-level preferences for cultural phenomena (music) are also influenced by broad environmental factors that exist over long periods of time (weather) via mood-regulation mechanisms. We discuss these results in terms of the limited nature of correlational studies and cross-cultural generalizability.
... One reason why source sensitivity impacts upon appraisals of music is because listeners-even trained adjudicators-are surprisingly unreliable at evaluating performances based on acoustic input alone. When people were told they would hear two different performances of the same piece, but were then played the same recording twice, three quarters of them believed they had heard different performances (Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017). Similarly, when participants heard the same audio recording paired with visual footage of two different performers playing it, musically trained and untrained judges perceived differences in the expressive approach between versions, presumably because they attributed them to different sources (Behne, 1999;Behne & Wöllner, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research has investigated psychological processes in an attempt to explain how and why people appreciate music. Three programs of research have shed light on these processes. The first focuses on the appreciation of musical structure. The second investigates self-oriented responses to music, including music-evoked autobiographical memories, the reinforcement of a sense of self, and benefits to individual health and wellbeing. The third seeks to explain how music listeners become sensitive to the causal and contextual sources of music making, including the biomechanics of performance, knowledge of musicians and their intentions, and the cultural and historical context of music making. To date, these programs of research have been carried out with little interaction, and the third program has been omitted from most psychological enquiries into music appreciation. In this paper, we review evidence for these three forms of appreciation. The evidence reviewed acknowledges the enormous diversity in antecedents and causes of music appreciation across contexts, individuals, cultures, and historical periods. We identify the inputs and outputs of appreciation, propose processes that influence the forms that appreciation can take, and make predictions for future research. Evidence for source sensitivity is emphasized because the topic has been largely unacknowledged in previous discussions. This evidence implicates a set of unexplored processes that bring to mind causal and contextual details associated with music, and that shape our appreciation of music in important ways. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
... We use the term valence simply to denote positive versus negative aspects of framing, and this does not relate to specific moods or emotions. framing and preference have labelled identical musical excerpts as performances by different artists with a varying associated skill level, such as labels of student performers versus world-renowned performers (Anglada-Tort & Müllensiefen, 2017;Aydogan et al., 2018;Duerksen, 1972;Kroger & Margulis, 2017). Other studies have presented identical excerpts that are incorrectly labelled as separate human performances, or as human-created compositions versus computer-created compositions Steinbeis & Koelsch, 2009;Ziv & Moran, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
While accompanying or contextualizing information (“framing”) is often included alongside music, prior research on the impact of framing on music preference has produced heterogeneous results. The majority of these studies have examined historical framing, although a small subset has suggested that imaginative framing may have an understudied potential to increase preference. In such studies the participants are encouraged to freely use their imagination while listening. The present work directly compared these two approaches to framing to examine which has greater positive impact on preference. One-hundred and fifty-two participants were exposed to five varied music excerpts (Pop/Rock and Classical), with participants placed into one of three conditions. Those in the historical condition were supplied with initial historical framing, and were encouraged to freely search online for information related to the piece while listening to it (hence guided self- framing), whereas those in the imaginative condition were encouraged to freely use their imagination while listening. Those in a third, “unrelated” control condition were encouraged to choose an online game (selected from a provided list) while listening. Bayesian modelling was used to examine preference ratings by piece and condition. Historical framing led to higher preference than the control for four of five pieces, while imaginative framing led to higher preference than the control for two pieces. Additionally, those receiving historical framing rated preference higher for pieces that had a greater amount of information readily available online. Thus, historical framing most benefited preference, although we discuss limitations and future directions for research on guided self-framing.
... That these effects also work in the contexts of the arts in general (Tröndle et al., 2014;Tröndle and Tschacher, 2016), and in music has been shown by a number of studies (for a recent overview, see Fischinger et al., 2020). Effects of program notes and other additional information in the form of texts or images have been found for emotions induced by music (Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2015), enjoyment of music (Margulis, 2010), children's attention and comprehension (Margulis et al., 2015), evaluation (Anglada-Tort and Müllensiefen, 2017;Fischinger et al., 2020), and even perception of basic musical characteristics (Chapman, 1981;North and Hargreaves, 2010;Fischinger et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Performing and listening to music occurs in specific situations, requiring specific media. Empirical research on music listening and appreciation, however, tends to overlook the effects these situations and media may have on the listening experience. This article uses the sociological concept of the frame to develop a theory of an aesthetic experience with music as the result of encountering sound/music in the context of a specific situation. By presenting a transdisciplinary sub-field of empirical (concert) studies, we unfold this theory for one such frame: the classical concert. After sketching out the underlying theoretical framework, a selective literature review is conducted to look for evidence on the general plausibility of the single elements of this emerging theory and to identify desiderata. We refer to common criticisms of the standard classical concert, and how new concert formats try to overcome alleged shortcomings and detrimental effects. Finally, an empirical research program is proposed, in which frames and frame components are experimentally manipulated and compared to establish their respective affordances and effects on the musical experience. Such a research program will provide empirical evidence to tackle a question that is still open to debate, i.e., whether the diversified world of modern-day music listening formats also holds a place for the classical concert – and if so, for what kind of classical concert.
Article
Music assessment is critical to delivering thoughtful, frequent music instruction. The assessment provides information to both instructors and students regarding student achievement and serves as a guide for instructional practices. In China, the teaching system is affected by an exam-oriented habitus, which has resulted in the long-term use of summative assessment to evaluate learning outcomes in the music classroom. Traditional assessment methods have the disadvantage of needing more real-world context. Students answer questions one by one without the need to apply long-term critical reasoning skills. Peer assessment is related to both the processes and the outcome. It is a technique where students assess their performance. Instructors must relinquish their dependence on traditional metrics of technique usage to accommodate this new concept. Peers evaluate student work on projects, assignments, and reports. Innovative teaching techniques and assessments are not only the exterior expression of innovation in music education but also the focal point of this innovation. When it comes to adapting to the demands of the new millennium, music education assessment is a strategic decision for the twenty-first century. The purpose of this study is to review the music assessment methods in Chinese higher education through a bibliometric analysis.
Article
Full-text available
Music-related decision making encompasses a wide range of behaviours including those associated with composition and performance, listening choices, and decisions involving music education and therapy. Although research programmes in psychology and economics have contributed to an improved understanding of music-related behaviour, historically these disciplines have been unconnected. Recently however, researchers have begun to bridge this gap by employing tools from behavioural economics. This paper contributes to the literature by providing a discussion about the benefits of using behavioural economics in music-decision research. We achieve this in two ways. First, through a systematic review, we identify the current state of the literature within four key areas of behavioural economics – heuristics and biases, social decision making, behavioural time preferences, and dual-process theory. Second, taking findings of the literature as a starting point, we demonstrate how behavioural economics can inform future research. Based on this, we propose the Behavioural Economics of Music (BEM), an integrated research programme that aims to break new ground by stimulating interdisciplinary research in the intersection between music, psychology, and economics.
Article
Full-text available
The authors examined the effects of age, musical experience, and characteristics of musical stimuli on a melodic short-term memory task in which participants had to recognize whether a tune was an exact transposition of another tune recently presented. Participants were musicians and nonmusicians between ages 18 and 30 or 60 and 80. In 4 experiments, the authors found that age and experience affected different aspects of the task, with experience becoming more influential when interference was provided during the task. Age and experience interacted only weakly, and neither age nor experience influenced the superiority of tonal over atonal materials. Recognition memory for the sequences did not reflect the same pattern of results as the transposition task. The implications of these results for theories of aging, experience, and music cognition are discussed.
Article
The social contexts in which people create, perform, perceive, understand and react to music have been neglected by psychologists. This book provides a comprehensive and up-to-date account of the social contexts in which people create, perform, perceive, understand, and react to music. It represents the first attempt to define the field since Farnsworth's book of the same title published in 1969, including the newer areas of medicine, marketing, and education in which the social psychology of music has direct applications in the real world. After an opening review chapter, the remaining 14 chapters are divided into six sections: individual differences; social groups and situations; social and cultural influences; developmental issues; musicianship; real world applications. Several of these chapters are ground-breaking reviews published for the first time. Aside from psychologists and music educators, The Social Psychology of Music will appeal to musicians, communications researchers, broadcasters, and commercial companies.
Article
This study investigated the performance preferences of listeners without formal training in music. Specifically, it asked whether the quality of the performance (as represented by the status of the performer), the order of presentation of the performances, and extrinsic information about the quality of the performance impacted preferences. In Experiment 1, participants heard pairs of performances of solo piano music and were informed that one was played by a conservatory student, and one by a world-renowned professional. After each pair, they selected the one they thought had been performed by the professional. Their responses seem to have been driven by a combination of a preference for the performance actually played by the professional and a preference for the second performance in the pair. In Experiment 2, they heard the same performance pairs, but this time were informed, correctly or incorrectly, before each performance whether it was played by a student or by a professional. After each pair, they selected the performance they preferred. This time, their responses were influenced not just by the actual performer identity and the order of presentation, but also by the priming condition. Listener preferences seem to be driven by a combination of factors intrinsic and extrinsic to the performance itself.
Article
This article explores our current understanding of why we like and choose to listen to the music that we do. It begins by defining terms and considering methods, moving on to discuss the biological influences of arousal and other personality traits on music preference, questions of style discrimination, and finally the cultural influences of experience upon preference. The article evaluates existing models of music preference and considers further directions and challenges in the field.
Article
We discuss the cognitive and the psy- chophysical determinants of choice in risky and risk- less contexts. The psychophysics of value induce risk aversion in the domain of gains and risk seeking in the domain of losses. The psychophysics of chance induce overweighting of sure things and of improbable events, relative to events of moderate probability. De- cision problems can be described or framed in multiple ways that give rise to different preferences, contrary to the invariance criterion of rational choice. The pro- cess of mental accounting, in which people organize the outcomes of transactions, explains some anomalies of consumer behavior. In particular, the acceptability of an option can depend on whether a negative outcome is evaluated as a cost or as an uncompensated loss. The relation between decision values and experience values is discussed. Making decisions is like speaking prose—people do it all the time, knowingly or unknowingly. It is hardly surprising, then, that the topic of decision making is shared by many disciplines, from mathematics and statistics, through economics and political science, to sociology and psychology. The study of decisions ad- dresses both normative and descriptive questions. The normative analysis is concerned with the nature of rationality and the logic of decision making. The de- scriptive analysis, in contrast, is concerned with peo- ple's beliefs and preferences as they are, not as they should be. The tension between normative and de- scriptive considerations characterizes much of the study of judgment and choice. Analyses of decision making commonly distin- guish risky and riskless choices. The paradigmatic example of decision under risk is the acceptability of a gamble that yields monetary outcomes with specified probabilities. A typical riskless decision concerns the acceptability of a transaction in which a good or a service is exchanged for money or labor. In the first part of this article we present an analysis of the cog- nitive and psychophysical factors that determine the value of risky prospects. In the second part we extend this analysis to transactions and trades. Risky Choice Risky choices, such as whether or not to take an umbrella and whether or not to go to war, are made without advance knowledge of their consequences. Because the consequences of such actions depend on uncertain events such as the weather or the opponent's resolve, the choice of an act may be construed as the acceptance of a gamble that can yield various out- comes with different probabilities. It is therefore nat- ural that the study of decision making under risk has focused on choices between simple gambles with monetary outcomes and specified probabilities, in the hope that these simple problems will reveal basic at- titudes toward risk and value. We shall sketch an approach to risky choice that