ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

The term “environmental problem” exposes a fundamental misconception: Disruptions of Earth’s ecosystems are at their root a human behavior problem. Psychology is a potent tool for understanding the external and internal drivers of human behavior that lead to unsustainable living. Psychologists already contribute to individual-level behavior-change campaigns in the service of sustainability, but attention is turning toward understanding and facilitating the role of individuals in collective and collaborative actions that will modify the environmentally damaging systems in which humans are embedded. Especially crucial in moving toward long-term human and environmental well-being are transformational individuals who step outside of the norm, embrace ecological principles, and inspire collective action. Particularly in developed countries, fostering legions of sustainability leaders rests upon a fundamental renewal of humans’ connection to the natural world.
Content may be subject to copyright.
REVIEW
Beyond the roots of human inaction:
Fostering collective effort toward
ecosystem conservation
Elise Amel,
1
*Christie Manning,
2
Britain Scott,
1
Susan Koger
3
The term environmental problemexposes a fundamental misconception: Disruptions
of Earths ecosystems are at their root a human behavior problem. Psychology is a
potent tool for understanding the external and internal drivers of human behavior that
lead to unsustainable living. Psychologists already contribute to individual-level
behavior-change campaigns in the service of sustainability, but attention is turning toward
understanding and facilitating the role of individuals in collective and collaborative
actions that will modify the environmentally damaging systems in which humans are
embedded. Especially crucial in moving toward long-term human and environmental
well-being are transformational individuals who step outside of the norm, embrace
ecological principles, and inspire collective action. Particularly in developed countries,
fostering legions of sustainability leaders rests upon a fundamental renewal of humans
connection to the natural world.
The ecological systems upon which humans
rely for life support are in crisis, and human
behavior is the root cause. These problems
are thus not environmental, but rather re-
lated to how humans meet their needs and
wants in ecologically disruptive ways. Manipulat-
ing, exploiting, and destroying nonhuman nature
are not new activities for our species, but today
these occur at an unprecedented scale and escalat-
ing rate.
Asthedecadessincethe1970shaverevealed,
merely educating people about what actions they
can take does not dramatically shift behavior; nor
does inspiring fear or guilt. Despite widespread
awareness and concern, many people continue to
engage in behaviors that further environmental
destruction, both mindlessly and consciously. For
example, nearly half of Americans are concerned
or alarmedabout global warming (1), yet those
who can afford it routinely fly to vacation des-
tinations, drive solo, and keep their homes at a
constant 72°F (22°C). Further, messages about
predicted environmental catastrophes may ac-
tually increase anti-environmental behavior as
individuals attempt to soothe their anxiety through
materialistic pursuits (2). Psychological science
can shed light on many such counterintuitive and
counterproductive responses to our ecological
predicament.
Human behavior is determined by forces both
inside and outside of the individual. Internal fac-
tors such as emotions, beliefs, attitudes, and val-
ues influence behavior to some extent (35), but
behavior occurs within a powerful context com-
prising cultural worldviews, social networks, sta-
tus inequalities, policies, scripts, roles, and rules.
Situations are such potent determinants of be-
havior that behavior-change campaigns focused
solely on values, emotions, or knowledge are des-
tined to fail if such change is not facilitated by
an individuals social milieu as well as the sur-
rounding infrastructure.
Humans are driven by external circumstances,
and yet all individuals have a hand in perpetu-
ating or redirecting situational forces. The cur-
rent ecologically destructive trajectory cannot be
reversed without human action to radically trans-
form the anthropogenic and anthropocentric sys-
tems that encourage, support, and reinforce overly
consumptive, wasteful, and polluting lifestyles,
particularly in the industrialized world. At pres-
ent, these systems make truly sustainable living
unappealing and impractical, if not impossible,
for most individuals living in them. Thus, despite
widespread recognition of the dangerous course
that we are on as a species, humanity has not yet
begun the radical transformations that are clearly
needed.
Change is hard. Human beings are reticent
to change their behavior even under the most
compelling of circumstances, and environmental
dangers do not tend to arouse the kind of urgency
that motivates individuals to act. Mass transfor-
mation of unsustainable systems will be even
more difficult than shifting individual behaviors,
for unlike ants and bees, humans are not well
equipped to coordinate behavior for common
benefit. Armed with psychological self-awareness,
however, people can address barriers to change.
We summarize some of these barriers below,
followed by discussions of the critical need for
collective and organizational action, the role of
individuals in creating large-scale change, and
reconnection with nature as the foundation of
true sustainability.
Why changing individual
behavior is hard
To understand the roots of todays environmental
crises, one must first look to the evolutionary
origins of human behavior. Urbanization, indus-
trialization, and technological innovation have
transformed the very foundations of human ex-
istence, creating a vastly different landscape and
lifestyle from those in which the human species
evolved, and to which our brains and bodies are
adapted. Some psychologists argue that urban
industrialized living compromises an individuals
sense of kinship with nonhuman nature (6,7),
thereby opening the door to environmentally de-
structive behavior. Simply put, humans dontprotect
Amel et al., Science 356,275279 (2017) 21 April 2017 1of5
1
Department of Psychology, University of St. Thomas, 2115
Summit Avenue, St. Paul, MN 55105, USA.
2
Department of
Environmental Studies, Macalester College, 1600 Grand
Avenue, St. Paul, MN 55105-1899, USA.
3
Department of
Psychology, Willamette University, 900 State Street, Salem,
OR 97301, USA.
*Corresponding author. Email: elamel@stthomas.edu
PHOTO: GETTY IMAGES/ISTOCKPHOTO
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
what they dont know and value. Indeed, numer-
ous studies have found a significant positive cor-
relation between feeling connected to nature and
ecologically responsible behavior (4)andbetween
significant life experiencesin nature during
childhood and later environmental advocacy (8).
Experiencing the self as separate from nature is
the foundation of humanitys damaged relation-
ship to planetary resources.
The mismatch between humansancient ori-
gins and todays industrialized world leads to an
array of other difficulties in recognizing and re-
sponding to environmentaldegradation.Humans
evolved in a world where dangers were sudden
and obvious, and thus our senses are ill equipped
to detect largely invisible and gradually worsen-
ing ecological problems such as climate change
or species extinction. Without a tangible sensory
signal and attendant emotional jolt, these prob-
lems feel psychologically distant and do little to
move us to action (9).
Also, because systemic problems like these do
not represent an immediate threat to the indi-
vidual, their associated long-term consequences
are less motivating than consequences in the here
and now (10). Many of todays environmentally
damaging behaviors present just such a contin-
gency trap, where personal benefits (or costs) are
much more compelling than far-off and hard-to-
detect ecological costs (or benefits). The salience
of short-term consequences explains why indi-
viduals are unwilling to surrender the convenience
of a personal car or to spend money on energy
efficiency measures that not only save money in
the long run but also help curb greenhouse gas
emissions.
A similar dilemma arises when individuals are
faced with a contradiction between self-interested
behavior and what is ultimately best for the larger
group (11,12).The features that normally curtail
selfishness and encourage cooperation are effec-
tively missing in large-scale environmental com-
mons dilemmas such as global climate change.
Working together to conserve a common-pool re-
source is difficult in the absence of enforceable
limits on who can access the resource, strong so-
cial connections among community members, and
opportunities for face-to-face communication (13).
Even when individuals are willing to forgo
immediate personal benefits in favor of the long-
term greater good, efforts to change are stymied
if a new behavior threatens psychological needs.
Beyond basic physical requirements, human well-
being depends on feeling competent, socially con-
nected, and free to make choices (14,15). Many
behaviors are motivated by a desire to fulfill these
needs, and humans tend to avoid activities and
situations that compromise them. Adopting sus-
tainable behavior that involves learning new ac-
tions (such as composting or a different method
for commuting) can at first be intimidating, making
individuals feel uncertain, incompetent, or fear-
ful of othersdisapproval or rejection.
Humans have a range of other psychological
needs as well, such as a need for safety and se-
curity, and a desire to see the world as a stable
and just place. Dire environmental news creates
a conflict with these deep-seated needs, as it im-
plies that all is not well with the status quo (16)
and, in the extreme case, may prompt unconscious
and deeply uncomfortable fears of death (2). In
response to these existential threats, people may
turn to coping defenses such as denial or distrac-
tion (2), especially if they have little hope that
action will make a difference (17).
The need for social connection is perhaps the
most influential of all, yet individuals greatly un-
derestimate the extent to which their behavior
is subject to social influence (18). Concerns about
social inclusion are undoubtedly rooted in the evo-
lutionary past. For ancestral humans, acceptance
by the group meant access to shared resources
and protection. Modern humans retain a keen
sensitivity to social dynamics; this manifests as
strong emotional reactions to threats of rejection.
Social norms, therefore, constrain human behavior,
as the mere thought of doing something drastically
different from what others are doing (descriptive
norms), or what others appear to approve of (in-
junctive norms), can lead to intense feelings of
discomfort, embarrassment, or shame. For envi-
ronmentally relevant behaviors, these two types
of norms are often at odds: Most people approve
of sustainable behaviors but behave in unsus-
tainable ways. Which norm exerts greater influ-
ence depends on their relative salience in a given
situation (19,20).
Whether particular social norms are relevant
to an individual depends on that personsgroup
affiliations. Individuals identify with ingroups
based on factors including demographic charac-
teristics (e.g., race, gender); social circumstances
(e.g., economic status, geography); and beliefs or
values (e.g., politics, religion). Humans behave ac-
cording to the norms of their affinity groups so
as to fit in, and also to display this social identity
to the world. Conforming to norms promoting
sustainable behavior may actually feel threatening
to individuals whose identity is perceived to be
at odds with being green.
Strong identity affiliations can also erupt into
intergroup conflict, evident in the anger and anti-
pathy between those who embrace the scientific
consensus on climate change and its skeptics (21).
This divide, found in several Western countries,
falls largely along ideological lines, with followers
of conservative parties showing far less concern
for the issue than supporters of liberal parties
(22). Even as climate science data have accumu-
lated and consensus of grave risk has grown in
the scientific community, concern about climate
change has decreased among those with a con-
servative worldview (23).
Thestrikingdifferenceinresponsetoclimate
change stems in part from motivated cognition.
Rather than neutrally receiving information, hu-
man brains privilege that which supports their
preexisting worldview. Given limited mental re-
sources for processing the boundless information
available in the world, evolution favored cognitive
efficiency. New information is processed through
the filters of personal beliefs, first-hand experi-
ences, and social identities. Ideas are dismissed
or assimilated on the basis of a quick but biased
heuristic of whether they line up with what is al-
ready perceived to be true. It is difficult to es-
cape bias, even when exerting conscious mental
effort. Ironically, it appears that those with the
highest science literacy may exhibit more ideology-
based bias than others, because their familiarity
with science makes them better equipped to
find supporting evidence for their preconceived
view (24).
Psychological tools for
individual change
These and other psychological dragons of in-
action(25) explain why humans are failing to
take sufficient action to address environmental
degradation. Social scientists are developing
psychologically informed strategies to overcome
barriers and encourage pro-environmental be-
havior (10,26). Specific tools include framing
information about an issue such as climate
change to emphasize current and local impacts
(27), creating incentives that increase the short-
term rewards of a sustainable action (28), and
encouraging social modeling to reset the per-
ceived social norm around a pro-environmental
behavior.
Devising behavior-change interventions is com-
plex and time-consuming because the effective-
ness of a particular tool varies widely depending
on what, and whose, behavior is at stake (29).
Each individual behavior comes with a unique
set of barriers and benefits, and each person ap-
proaches these with varying levels of motivation.
Despite increased attention from behavioral scien-
tists, few resources exist to guide practitioners
about when and how to apply specific psycholog-
ical tools (28,29). One exception is community-
based social marketing (CBSM) (30), a five-step
community-level approach that matches appro-
priate tools of change to the exact barriers, both
physical and psychological, that inhibit a spe-
cific sustainable action (28).
CBSM has been used to address sustainable
behavior in communities around the world and
remains a promising strategy for individual change.
Yet, given the scale and pace of continued envi-
ronmental destruction , psychologists need to
move beyond targeting individualsprivate-sphere
choices and focus on how to foster collective ac-
tion (Fig. 1).
Amel et al., Science 356,275279 (2017) 21 April 2017 2of5
Psychological research
suggests that humans can
move toward a sustainable
society by creating
conditions that motivate
environmentally responsible
collective action...
ECOSYSTEM EARTH
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
Individuals and collective action
Thepoweroftheindividualtomitigateenviron-
mental harm is severely constrained by physical
and social contexts, such as the industrial infra-
structure for growing and transporting food, gen-
erating energy, and producing goods; the urban
structures built for living, working, and playing;
and the rules and policies of the many groups
and organizations to which people belong. Above
and beyond the ecological damage inflicted by
individualspersonal behaviors is the damage
from the inefficient and wasteful industrial sys-
tems and processes through which individuals
meet their daily needs.
For example, one study estimated that just 90
businesses have generated 63% of the cumulative,
global greenhouse gas emissions (31). Even incre-
mental improvements in systemic processes and
infrastructure will have much broader impacts
than will individual efforts (32,33). Thus, it is
critical that efforts to overcome individualsbar-
riers to change focus not only on motivating them
to behave sustainably in their personal sphere, but
also on inspiring them to participate in collective
efforts to change the larger systems and infra-
structure (34). Recent research in political psy-
chology has begun to provide important insights
for facilitating involvement in such systems-level
change (35).
Unlike changing personal behaviors, transform-
ing systems requires individuals to participate
in public dialogue and activism in both informal
and formal social collectives. If they embrace
change at all, most people gravitate toward pri-
vate, individual behavior and avoid potentially
uncomfortable public advocacy and action (36).
Individual change is already challenging. It takes
even greater courage and perseverance to openly
question the dominant worldview that forms the
bedrock of cultural norms (Fig. 2).
Perceived social risks, such as fear of appear-
ing biased or incompetent, fear of rejection, or
the belief that others disagree about the issue,
inhibit many from speaking out about critical
issues. People tend to underestimate how many
others share their opinion, which hampers willing-
ness to be vocal (37). Emerging evidence sug-
gests, however, that when individuals realize they
are not alone in their beliefs about a contentious
issue, they become willing to speak out. Specifi-
cally, self-censorship about anthropogenic climate
change decreases when people understand just
how many others acknowledge its reality and
are concerned about it (38).
Individual behaviors such as voting, contacting
elected representatives, and supporting issue-focused
organizations are essential to functional democ-
racies. These acts ultimately affect local, national,
and even international policy. Evidence suggests
that political activism about conservation, like
many behaviors, requires the belief that political
action is necessary, influences others, and can
actually change environmental outcomes (36).
Emerging evidence points to several key in-
gredients that must be in place before individuals
enter into more public collective efforts on behalf
of the environment. Alignment with social iden-
tity is critical, and the deeper the identification,
the greater the individuals commitment to the
success of the group. In addition, people only join
efforts if they believe that their individual contri-
butions can make a difference (39). Similarly,
in the case of climate change activism, individ-
uals need not only a sense of urgency about the
issue,butalsoconfidencethatsolutionsarepos-
sible (36).
In addition to grassroots initiatives, efforts
within preexisting social groups can also drive
change. For example, faith communities, hobby
groups, and neighborhoods bring people together
through shared values, rituals, or connection to
place, and can energize larger-scale conservation
actions through these common connections. Place-
based collectives, such as neighborhood associa-
tions, can shift attention away from ideological
differences to focus on tangible community-level
action, such as creating a shared wind farm (40).
Efforts within faith-based communities likely mo-
tivate through alignment with spiritual values, and
canhavewidereachthroughthelargernetworks
of interfaith organizations.
Although psychological research has examined
what motivates people to volunteer and cooper-
ate for social causes, or mobilize around political
campaigns, the results have yet to be applied to
collective efforts for conservation.
Leveraging formal organizations to
influence individuals
Formal organizations can serve as vehicles to
quickly mobilize collective action toward sus-
tainability. Governmental units, educational in-
stitutions, and businesses large and small are
designed to guide the actions of many toward
a coherent purpose. Organizational culture, by
way of norms, values, policy, and leadership,
powerfully influences individual members (41).
Additionally, organizations determine the choice
architecture,or the situational contexts that
guide actions and decisions (33). A greenorga-
nizational culture effectively relieves individuals
from the effortful thinking required to recog-
nize and respond in sustainable ways. For in-
stance, purchasing policiescanprioritizevendors
that meet sustainability criteria, and technol-
ogy policies can set machine defaults to efficien-
cy modes. Individuals no longer have to have
background knowledge, do research, and eval-
uate myriad choices for every behavior relevant
to sustainability.
Theproblemis,aswithindividuals,thefun-
damental assumptions that drive organizations
reflect the worldview of the broader culture. In
todays world, businesses tend to assume a growth
economy based on a take-make-waste model, many
religions elevate the value of humans over other
beings, and schools often fail to prepare gradu-
ates to understand ecology. Thus, the goals, opera-
tions, and resulting organizational behavior run
contrary to ecological realities.
Although organizations are currently ma-
jor contributors to worldwide environmental
degradation, they in fact have the capacity to
move in new, ecologicallysound directions. They
can empower their members to innovate, take risks,
and take the long-term view together (42). First,
however, a catalyst must influence organizational
Amel et al., Science 356,275279 (2017) 21 April 2017 3of5
C
h
a
n
g
e
n
o
r
m
s
s
y
s
t
e
m
s
s
t
o
r
i
e
s
s
y
m
b
o
l
s
R
u
n
f
o
r
p
u
b
l
i
c
o
c
e
c
r
e
a
t
e
l
a
w
s
o
r
g
a
n
i
z
e
v
o
t
e
p
r
o
t
e
s
t
S
h
a
r
e
i
d
e
a
s
c
h
a
n
g
e
p
r
o
c
e
s
s
e
s
&
p
r
o
c
e
d
u
r
e
s
c
r
e
a
t
e
d
u
r
a
b
l
e
p
r
o
d
u
c
t
s
S
h
a
r
e
m
a
t
e
r
i
a
l
g
o
o
d
s
t
e
a
c
h
p
e
r
s
u
a
d
e
s
u
p
p
o
r
t
o
t
h
e
r
s
b
e
a
l
e
a
d
e
r
L
e
a
r
n
d
o
n
a
t
e
m
o
n
e
y
&
t
i
m
e
w
a
l
k
r
e
d
u
c
e
r
e
u
s
e
Person
Private & personal
Smallest impact
Largest impact
Social network
Organizational
Public
Cultural
Fig. 1. An individuals spheres of influence. Individual actions have the greatest effect when they
influence broader systems.
GRAPHIC: ADAPTED BY K. SUTLIFF/SCIENCE
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
direction. In any group or organization, the entity
itself is not the actor. Individualsinformal and
formal leaders, decision-makers, workers, volun-
teers, and membersare the underlying force. So,
the onus is on individuals to initiate and imple-
ment change in these collectives. This is easier
said than done.
Psychologists do not yet know why some are
willing or able to take a bold stand for change in
the same s ituat ion s that drive o the rs to support
the status quo or to simp ly wi th draw ( 43). What
they do know is that resisting the pressure to con-
form,especiallyin thecontext of formal organiza-
tions, requires nothing short of heroic effort (43).
Yet, it is possible to empower ordinary people
to successfully face such challenges. Recogniz-
ing this, a group of influential psychologists
has founded an initiative to educate the public
about negative social influence and provide indi-
viduals with the psyc holog ical t ools t o act w ith
moral courage (44).
Although change can begin anywhere in an
organization, people in leadership roles are ar-
guably best positioned to activate a major shift
toward sustainability (45). Unfortunately, though
well-intentioned, leaders who possess the pre-
vailing modern-industrial worldview may only
make their processes or products less bad(46).
To radically change a groups trajectory, leaders
must think differently; they must internalize an
ecologically grounded worldview and integrate
it into the vision they set for others (47).
Certainly, some leaders have experienced epiph-
anies, recognizing the inconsistency between the
dominant industrial worldview and ecological
systems. The late carpet magnate Ray Anderson
often spoke of the spear in the heartmoment
when he realized his business was endangering
future generations (48). Humanity cannot, how-
ever, depend on spontaneous individual insight
to propel institutions forward; more methodical
approaches are in order. Through mentorship (49)
and applied, inquiry-based educational programs
(50), youth and adults alike can learn to under-
stand the ecological underpinnings of society.
Science-based programs such as The Natural
Step (51) have been designed to support ecolog-
ically consistent organizational
learning. Additional research is
needed to understand how to en-
hance the pace and depth of world-
view change.
Building ecological
understanding through
connection with nature
Thetenacityofthedominantworld-
view in the developed world belies
a more fundamental problem:
Human beings in industrialized
nations are so disconnected from
the natural systems they depend
on that they do not know what
they do not know.
Human behavior can be respon-
sive to local environmental condi-
tions, as demonstrated by the use
of traditional ecological knowl-
edge (TEK) by indigenous cul-
tures around the world (49). TEK,
however, relies heavily on experien-
tial information (49). This suggests
that developing an ecologically
consistent worldview may benefit
from reconnecting with nature so
that humans actually experience
and develop a dynamic understand-
ing of the worlds systems and human-
environment interdependence.
Although worldwide trends to-
ward accelerating urbanization
have generally meant fewer op-
portunities to encounter and build
a connection to nature, urban dwellers need
access to nature in order to rediscover their inter-
dependence with it and deepen their sense of
place. This, in turn, fosters understanding of the
natural environment (50) and inspires efforts
to protect and preserve landscapes and their in-
habitants (4).
Valuable nature experiences do not require
trips to wildnature such as old-growth forests,
but can be found in urban areas as well (52).
Fortunately, new trends in urban design may
help heal the human-nature divide. Recognizing
both conservation and public health benefits,
urban planners and architects are increasingly
incorporating green features such as community
gardens, walking and biking paths, and green
roofs (53) and integrating biophilicdesigns, which
echo natural forms and patterns (i.e., nonhuman
animal and plant), in built environments (54).
Expanded access to urban green space not
only enhances human understanding of natural
systems, it provides critical contact with environ-
ments to which we are best adapted and in which
we can thrive both physically and psychologica lly.
Research affirms that engaging with nature im-
proves both mental and physical well-being (55)
and promotes healthy child development (53,56).
Conclusions
Environmental degradation ultimately stems
from human behavior. Fundamental behavioral
Amel et al., Science 356,275279 (2017) 21 April 2017 4of5
Ecologically grounded worldview Modern-industrial worldview vs.
Earth has plenty of resources Earth’s regenerative capacity
has limits
A linear “take, make, waste”
economy can continue indenitely
Technology
will x our
problems
Eciency
increases prots
Diversity maintains
system integrity
Even small manipulations
of nature have system-
wide consequences
Our systems must be circular;
“waste equals food”
1
2
3
4
Fig. 2. Some of the contrasting assumptions of modern-industrial and ecologically grounded worldviews
depicted in the context of food systems. Similar assumptions underlie transportation, energy generation, water
use, and material consumption. [Adapted from (57)]
Individual change is
already challenging. It
takes even greater courage
and perseverance to openly
question the dominant
worldview that forms the
bedrock of cultural norms.
GRAPHIC: ADAPTED BY K. SUTLIFF/SCIENCE
ECOSYSTEM EARTH
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
changes are thus needed to stop damaging the
natural world and adapt to a permanently al-
tered environment.
Psychological research suggests that humans
can move toward a sustainable society by creat-
ing conditions that motivate environmentally
responsible collective actionconditions that help
people surmount cognitive limits, create new sit-
uational drivers, foster need fulfillment, and sup-
port communities of social change.
Individuals whose actions are informed by a
deeper understanding of how the planet really
works can galvanize collectives to change the la r-
ger systems that drive so much of humanbehav-
ior. To radically alter the way humans think and
live; educate the next generati on; and design
physical, governmental, and cultural systems,
humans must experience and better under-
stand their profound interdependence with the
planet.
Further psychological research needs to elu-
cidate how to accelerate the adoption of eco-
logically grounded worldviews and how to
activate ecologically compatible engagement,
especially leadership, for the collective work
needed to become more sustainable. The future
of humanityand indeed, all life on Earth
depends on it.
REFERENCES
1. C. Roser-Renouf, E. Maibach, A. Leiserowitz, S. Rosenthal,
Global Warmings Six Americas and the Election, 2016
(Yale Program on Climate Change Communication, New Haven,
CT, 2016); http://climatecommunication.yale.edu/
publications/six-americas-2016-election/.
2. J. L. Dickinson, Ecol. Soc. 14,3450 (2009).
3. S. Bamberg, G. Möser, J. Environ. Psychol. 27,1425
(2007).
4. R. Gifford, A. Nilsson, Int. J. Psychol. 49,141157 (2014).
5. L. Steg, J. I. M. De Groot, in The Oxford Handbook of
Environmental and Conservation Psychology, S. D. Clayton,
Ed. (Oxford Univ. Press, 2012), chap. 5, pp. 8192.
6. P. H. Kahn Jr., P. H. Hasbach, Eds., Ecopsychology: Science,
Totems, and the Technological Species (MIT Press, 2012).
7. P. H. Kahn Jr., P. H. Hasbach, Eds., The Rediscovery of the Wild
(MIT Press, 2013).
8. L. Chawla, V. Derr, in The Oxford Handbook of Environmental
and Conservation Psychology, S. D. Clayton, Ed. (Oxford
Univ. Press, 2012), chap. 28, pp. 527555.
9. E. U. Weber, Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Change 7, 125134
(2016).
10. M. van Vugt, V. Griskevicius, P. W. Schultz, Soc. Issues Policy
Rev. 8,132 (2014).
11. G. Hardin, Science 162, 12431248 (1968).
12. R. Osbaldiston, K. M. Sheldon, in Psychology of Sustainable
Development, P. Schmuck, P. W. Schultz, Eds. (Springer,
2002), pp. 3757.
13. T. Dietz, E. Ostrom, P. C. Stern, Science 302,19071912
(2003).
14. L. Tay, E. Diener, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 354365
(2011).
15. R. M. Ryan, E. L. Deci, Am. Psychol. 55,6878 (2000).
16. I. Feygina, J. T. Jost, R. E. Goldsmith, Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
36, 326338 (2010).
17. S. S. Li, J. Environ. Educ. 45, 243257 (2014).
18. R. B. Cialdini, Psychol. Inq. 16, 158161 (2005).
19. R. B. Cialdini, Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion (Harper
Business, revised ed., 2006).
20. N. J. Goldstein, R. B. Cialdini, V. Griskevicius, J. Consum. Res.
35, 472482 (2008).
21. A. M. Bliuc et al., Nat. Clim. Chang. 5, 226229 (2015).
22. Pew Research Center, Global concern about climate change,
broad support for limiting emissions(Pew Research Center,
2015); www.pewglobal.org/files/2015/11/Pew-Research-Center-
Climate-Change-Report-FINAL-November-5-2015.pdf.
23. J. T. Carmichael, R. J. Brulle, J. K. Huxster, Clim. Change 141,
599612 (2017).
24. D. M. Kahan et al., Nat. Clim. Chang. 2, 732735
(2012).
25. R. Gifford, Am. Psychol. 66, 290302 (2011).
26. S. van der Linden, E. Maibach, A. Leiserowitz, Perspect.
Psychol. Sci. 10, 758763 (2015).
27. Center for Research on Environmental Decisions, The
Psychology of Climate Change Communication: A Guide
for Scientists, Journalists, Educators, Political Aides, and the
Interested Public (Center for Research on Environmental
Decisions, New York, 2009); http://guide.cred.columbia.edu/.
28. R. Osbaldiston, J. P. Schott, Environ. Behav. 44, 257299
(2012).
29. P. W. Schultz, Eur. Psychol. 19, 107117 (2014).
30. D. McKenzie-Mohr, N. Lee, P. W. Schultz, P. Kotler, Social
Marketing to Protect the Environment: What Works (Sage,
Thousand Oaks, CA, 2012).
31. R. Heede, Clim. Change 122, 229241 (2014).
32. M. F. Maniates, Glob. Environ. Polit. 1,3152 (2001).
33. C. R. Sunstein, L. A. Reisch, HELR Harvard Environ. Law Rev.
38, 127158 (2013).
34. K. OBrien, Science 350, 11701171 (2015).
35. M. van Zomeren, Polit. Psychol. 37,87114 (2016).
36. C. Roser-Renouf, E. W. Maibach, A. Leiserowitz, X. Zhao,
Clim. Change 125, 163178 (2014).
37. C. J. Glynn, M. E. Huge, in The Spiral of Silence:
New Perspectives on Communication and Public Opinion,
W. Donsbach, C. T. Salmon, Y. Tsfati, Eds. (Routledge, 2014),
pp. 6572.
38. N. Geiger, J. K. Swim, J. Environ. Psychol. 47,7990
(2016).
39. S. Bamberg, J. Rees, S. Seebauer, J. Environ. Psychol. 43,
155165 (2015).
40. J. A. Nevin, Behav. Anal. 33, 189191 (2010).
41. Y. Inoue, P. Alfaro-Barrantes, Bus. Soc. Rev. 120, 137160
(2015).
42. E. Van Velsor, L. Quinn, in Managing Human Resources for
Environmental Sustainability, S. E. Jackson, D. S. Ones,
S. Dilchert, Eds. (Jossey-Bass, Somerset, NJ, 2012), chap. 10.
43. P. Zimbardo, The Lucifer Effect: Understanding How Good
People Turn Evil (Random House, 2007).
44. Z. Franco, P. Zimbardo, The banality of heroism(Greater
Good, University of California, Berkeley, 2006);
http://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/the_banality_of_
heroism/.
45. J. L. Robertson, J. Barling, J. Organ. Behav. 34, 176194
(2013).
46. M. Braungart, W. McDonough, Cradle to Cradle (Random
House, 2009).
47. S. Schein, A New Psychology for Sustainability Leadership: The
Hidden Power of Ecological Worldviews (Greenleaf, Sheffield,
UK, 2015).
48. R. C. Anderson, R. White, Confessions of a Radical Industrialist:
Profits, People, PurposeDoing Business by Respecting the
Earth (Macmillan, 2009).
49. J. Mistry, A. Berardi, Science 352, 12741275 (2016).
50.M.E.Krasny,C.Lundholm,S.Shava,E.Lee,H.Kobori,in
Urbanization, Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services:
Challenges And Opportunities, T. Elmqvist et al., Eds.
(Springer Netherlands, 2013), pp. 629664.
51. B. Natrass, M. Altomare, The Natural Step for Business: Wealth,
Ecology, and the Evolutionary Corporation (New Society,
Gabriola Island, BC, Canada, 2013).
52. S. Clayton et al., Conserv. Lett. 10.1111/conl.12337
(2017).
53. T. Hartig, P. H. Kahn Jr., Science 352, 938940
(2016).
54. S. R. Kellert, J. Heerwagen, M. Mador, Biophilic Design: The
Theory, Science and Practice of Bringing Buildings to Life
(Wiley, 2011).
55. T. Hartig, R. Mitchell, S. de Vries, H. Frumkin, Annu. Rev. Public
Health 35, 207228 (2014).
56. P. H. Kahn, S. R. Kellert, Eds., Children and Nature:
Psychological, Sociocultural, and Evolutionary Investigations
(MIT Press, 2002).
57. B. A. Scott, E. L. Amel, S. M. Koger, C. M. Manning, Psychology
for Sustainability (Routledge, ed. 4, 2016), chap. 3.
10.1126/science.aal1931
Amel et al., Science 356,275279 (2017) 21 April 2017 5of5
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
(6335), 275-279. [doi: 10.1126/science.aal1931]356Science
20, 2017)
Elise Amel, Christie Manning, Britain Scott and Susan Koger (April
toward ecosystem conservation
Beyond the roots of human inaction: Fostering collective effort
Editor's Summary
This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.
Article Tools
http://science.sciencemag.org/content/356/6335/275
article tools:
Visit the online version of this article to access the personalization and
Permissions http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
Obtain information about reproducing this article:
is a registered trademark of AAAS. ScienceAdvancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title
Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright 2016 by the American Association for the
in December, by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York
(print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last weekScience
on April 21, 2017http://science.sciencemag.org/Downloaded from
... Returning to the issue of sustainability around digital technology use in education, it could be strongly argued that a democratic-critical (DC) response would be most effective in tackling associated concerns. In looking at the democratic element, responses to the unsustainable consumption of digital technologies need to be tackled in a collective manner (Amel et al. 2017), as individually led changes, regardless of the sincerity and merits of their intention, are likely to have limited impact as they fail to gain buy-in from the wider collective (Hubbart 2023). Further supporting the need for a democratic element, Thomson et al. (1999) found a strong link between effective communication and buy-in, stating that increasing the effectiveness of communication can significantly increase levels of buy-in. ...
... Our findings indicate that individual social responsibility can indirectly encourage pro-environmental behavior through strengthened environmental responsibility. This underscores the importance of cultivating environmental attitudes to translate social responsibility into sustainable behavior (Amel et al., 2017). • Social Responsibility → PEB via Prosocial Behavior. ...
Article
This study explores how prosocial engagement may enable young people to become committed to environmental protection. Using serial mediation analysis, we examine the data from 530 undergraduate business students in Southeast Europe to depict the psychological pathways connecting prosocial attitudes and behaviors with a propensity for environmental engagement. Statistical analysis is performed using the PROCESS macro Model 6. Although limited by similar socio-economic circumstances across the analyzed region and the cross-sectional research design, our findings suggest that the mediated indirect relationships help to develop attitudes and behaviors responsible toward the environment. Implications of the study include the broader possibilities for education and public policy-making: prosocial engagement could serve as a promising instrument to motivate young adults to accept the concept of sustainable development. In this way, synergies between prosocial engagement and environmental awareness might be used to develop a more sustainable, ecologically aware society. Key words prosociality; environmental engagement; attitudes; behaviors; young adults
... K E Y W O R D S environmental psychology, intervention, pro-environmental behavior, prompts, replication, systematic review, z-curve Simultaneously facing multiple environmental crises arguably represents humanity's greatest challenge of our time (IPCC, 2022;Richardson et al., 2023). Besides political action, individual behavior change toward more pro-environmental behavior (henceforth PEB) is essential to address these crises (Amel et al., 2017;Nielsen et al., 2024), given that environmental crises are caused by human actions (IPCC, 2022). PEB is defined as "behavior that harms the environment as little as possible, or even benefits the environment" (Steg & Vlek, 2009, p. 309). ...
Article
Full-text available
Beyond political action, fostering individuals’ pro-environmental behavior (PEB) is imperative to address environmental crises, such as climate change. Prompts are visual and/or verbal reminders to perform certain PEBs. Meta-analytic results by Osbaldiston and Schott (2012) suggest that prompts can increase PEB. However, in many of the included primary studies in this meta-analysis, prompts were confounded with other interventions (e.g., information campaigns). To study whether and under which conditions prompts have an unconfounded effect on PEB, we conducted a pre-registered systematic review following the PRISMA guidelines (N = 61 studies, k = 114 effect sizes). Additionally, we conducted a z-curve analysis to assess the expected replicability of the observed significant findings and ran meta-analyses to calculate the combined effect of (un)confounded prompt interventions on PEB. We find that prompts are effective in promoting PEB (b = 0.67, p < .001), even when no other interventions are implemented simultaneously (b = 0.66, p < .001). The rate of significant results that could be replicated under the same conditions as estimated by z-curve analysis is high (88% - 95% for unconfounded significant findings). Prompts are particularly effective in promoting resource conservation. The study setting and the PEB level prior to the prompt intervention moderate the effect of prompts on PEB. For example, the effect of prompts was stronger in universities, hotels and public settings compared to work settings, and when initial PEB levels were lower.
... In this case, we could not account for factors other than our billboards which may have affected consumption in our Billboard City disproportionately in either direction It is worth noting that this study focused explicitly on behavioural measures, with a focus on exploring novel methods of measuring animal product consumption beyond self-report. That said, there are plenty of valuable non-consumption outcomes (not meas ured here) which may come from this kind of advertisement, such as perceived norms, support for pro-animal policies, and low potential for backlash (Amel et al., 2017). All of these outcomes might reasonably be seen as valuable from the perspective of an animal advocate. ...
Article
Full-text available
We present a field experiment to evaluate a social marketing campaign encouraging people to try plant-based milk. We ran six anti-dairy billboards for one month in a city in the UK. The billboards featured a photo of a suffering dairy cow, a link to a website with information about dairy cow suffering, and an appeal to try plant-based milk. To estimate the impact of the billboards on plant-based and dairy milk consumption, we triangulated three novel data collection methods. First, we compared changes in regional vs. nationwide sales data from two plant-based milk companies. Second, we evaluated the proportion of dairy-free orders from six cafes in the city where we ran the billboards (‘Billboard City’) before, during, and after the campaign. Third, we compared changes in the proportion of household waste representing plant-based or dairy milk in the Billboard City vs. another UK city with no intervention (‘Control City’). Although descriptively, our results appear to be in line with some positive impact of the billboards, ultimately the study design and data were too limited to support a general claim about the impact of the billboard campaign. There were logistical challenges with each data source, as well as too many extraneous factors for the design to account for adequately. We discuss the challenges of field research, the strengths and weaknesses of each novel data collection method, and present considerations for future research.
... From a historical perspective, many highly urbanised societies may have reduced their interactions and connection with nature Soga & Gaston, 2016). This is purported to be linked with cultural views (particularly those cultures rooted in Judeo-Christian religious origins) of humans as separate from nature (Manfredo et al., 2016;Newman & Dale, 2013), and even when people hold affinity for nature, this does not necessarily result in conservation behaviour (Amel et al., 2017;Gifford, 2011). This is coupled with low levels of ecoliteracy. ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding and shaping human action towards nature conservation is critical to reversing the biodiversity crisis. Psychological science provides tools for understanding individual and collective behaviours, but also for understanding how the behaviour of individuals can drive human–environment systems transitions. As researchers and practitioners spanning distinct disciplines, we draw on our collective knowledge in environmental psychology, systems thinking, economics, and conservation biology, along with experience in practice and government, to consider reasons why people do (or don’t) protect nature. We outline dimensions important to fostering individual conservation behaviour and systems transformation. Such individual dimensions include values, personality traits, and psychological distancing. Broader system influences include cultural, economic, and environmental factors that shape the way people interact with, and care for, nature. Finally, we describe potential tools that may support increasing conservation actions and systems transformation, including strengthening connection with and access to nature, values-based and solutions-focused framing, collective action, and propagating optimism.
Chapter
This chapter explores the role of HTI in sustainable energy, through the lens of the Swedish Energy Agency’s ‘Design for Energy Efficient Everyday Life’ programme. This case study demonstrates the practical application of HTI principles in creating energy-efficient solutions, emphasising user-centric design and technological innovation. Through an analysis of various projects within this programme, the chapter delves into the complex interplay between human behaviour, technological advancements, and sustainability, namely on the perspective of energy efficiency. It assesses the impact of these projects on promoting sustainability, critically examining their methodologies and outcomes in energy conservation and environmental stewardship. The chapter concludes with a discussion about the role of sustainability in shaping HTI based on the findings and analysis from the programme’s outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Social sciences are increasingly recognized as useful for reorienting human action toward environmental conservation. Fully realizing the social sciences’ potential requires applying social science methods to conservation challenges and drawing from and building on human action theories from across the social sciences to better understand how and when actions can realize positive social and environmental priorities. We conducted an in‐depth analysis of a bounded, systematically selected set of conservation science peer‐reviewed articles to investigate the prevalence of social science theories of human action in conservation research and whether these theories represent the richness of the social science literature related to human action. We censused papers published in 2023 in Conservation Biology, Conservation Letters, and Biological Conservation and assessed each paper's geographic scope, social science engagement, whether it investigated human action, and weather it explicitly used human action theories and underlying metatheory (i.e., ways of understanding the world and how one gains knowledge of it). Results across 533 papers showed that 32% of papers incorporated social science and that 64% of these social science papers investigated human action. Twenty‐seven percent of these human action papers used explicit human action theories. The theory of planned behavior was the most used explicit theory (17% of action theory papers). The independent self metatheory was the most prevalent; it underlies the theory of planned behavior and focuses on understanding how personal attributes, such as values, shape intentional individual behavior. The prevalence of a few theories and metatheories in these dominant conservation journals may indicate a limited capacity for conservation research to build on previous research, avoid redundant reinvention, and unmask novel applications of social science theory that could reorient human action toward conservation. Human action theory use in conservation might be broadened by changing attitudes on the importance of human action theories for research; incorporating social theory in conservation education; asking reviewers to comment on theory usage and mandating theory reporting; creating spaces for social scientists and theory scholars; providing social scientists and theorists with decision‐making power in organizations; rewarding theory use; recognizing feedback loops among theory use; and replacing colonial and capitalistic approaches to conservation.
Technical Report
Full-text available
These results come from a nationally representative survey of 1,001 American adults, aged 18 and older. The completion rate was 53 percent. The sample was weighted to correspond with US Census Bureau parameters for the United States. The margin of sampling error is plus or minus 3 percent for the full sample, with 95 percent confidence. The survey was designed by Anthony Leiserowitz of Yale University, and Edward Maibach and Connie Roser-­‐Renouf of George Mason University, and was conducted December 24 through January 3 by Knowledge Networks, using an online research panel of American adults.
Article
Full-text available
Recent scholarship has identified a large and growing divide on how Republicans and Democrats view the issue of climate change. A number of these studies have suggested that this polarization is a product of systematic efforts to spread doubt about the reality of climate change through the media in general and conservative media in particular. However, research to date has largely relied on speculation about such a relationship rather than empirical evidence. We improve on existing research by conducting an empirical analysis of the factors affecting national-level, quarterly shifts in public concern about climate change between January 2001 and December 2014. Our analysis focuses on the potential role played by four factors that should account for changes in levels of concern regarding climate change: (1) media coverage, (2) extreme weather, (3) issuance of major scientific reports, and (4) changes in economic activity and foreign conflict. Some results suggest that partisan media influences beliefs in ways expected by communication scholars who describe “echo chamber” effects and “boomerang” effects. Among other supporting evidence, we find that partisan media not only strengthen views of like-minded audiences but also when Republicans are presented with opposing frames about climate change from liberal media, they appear to reject the messages such that they are less concerned about the issue. Findings also demonstrate that the dissemination of science increases concern about climate change among Democrats but has no influence on Republicans. Finally, extreme weather does not increase concern among Democrats or Republicans. Implications for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Despite decades of awareness about the biodiversity crisis, it remains a wicked problem. Besides preservation and restoration strategies, one approach has focused on increasing public concern about biodiversity issues by emphasizing opportunities for people to experience natural environments. In this essay, we endeavor to complicate the understanding of these experiences of nature (EoN). Because EoN are embedded in social and cultural contexts, transformative or new EoN are emerging in combination with societal changes in work, home, and technology. Policies that acknowledge and accept a diversity of culturally-situated EoN, including negative EoN, could help people reconnect with the complexity and dynamics of biodiversity. A new conceptualization of EoN that encompasses diverse experiences and reflects the sociocultural context could help to stimulate a broader transformation in the relationship between society and nature, one that better integrates the two spheres. Such a transformation is necessary to more effectively address the biodiversity crisis.
Article
Full-text available
Climate change activism has been uncommon in the U.S., but a growing national movement is pressing for a political response. To assess the cognitive and affective precursors of climate activism, we hypothesize and test a two-stage information-processing model based on social cognitive theory. In stage 1, expectations about climate change outcomes and perceived collective efficacy to mitigate the threat are hypothesized to influence affective issue involvement and support for societal mitigation action. In stage 2, beliefs about the effectiveness of political activism, perceived barriers to activist behaviors and opinion leadership are hypothesized to influence intended and actual activism. To test these hypotheses, we fit a structural equation model using nationally representative data. The model explains 52 percent of the variance in a latent variable representing three forms of climate change activism: contacting elected representatives; supporting organizations working on the issue; and attending climate change rallies or meetings. The results suggest that efforts to increase citizen activism should promote specific beliefs about climate change, build perceptions that political activism can be effective, and encourage interpersonal communication on the issue.
Chapter
This handbook is the first to comprehensively study the interdependent fields of environmental and conservation psychology. In doing so, it seeks to map the rapidly growing field of conservation psychology and its relationship to environmental psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology includes basic research on environmental perceptions, attitudes, and values; research on specific environments, such as therapeutic settings, schools, and prisons; environmental impacts on human well-being; and ways to promote a more sustainable relationship between people and the natural environment. This handbook presents an extensive review of current research and is a thorough guide to the state of knowledge about a wide range of topics at the intersection of psychology and the physical environment. Beyond this, it provides a better understanding of the relationship between environmental and conservation psychology, and some sense of the directions in which these interdependent areas of study are heading.
Book
During the last decade, the sustainability position in multinational corporations has grown in influence. Much literature has explored how corporations can play an important role in solving the environmental challenges facing the planet. However, until now, there has been little research on sustainability leadership at the individual level. In this book, Schein explores the deeper psychological motivations of sustainability leaders. He shows how these motivations relate to overall effectiveness and capacity to lead transformational change and he explores the ways in which the complexity of sustainability is driving new approaches to leadership.Drawing on interviews with 75 leaders from over 40 multinational corporations and NGOs, Schein explores how ecological worldviews are developed and expressed in global sustainability practice. By applying key theories from developmental psychology, integral ecology and eco-psychology to sustainability practice, Schein encourages us to think about leadership in a different way. A New Psychology for Sustainability Leadership will be of interest to an interdisciplinary audience of social scientists, educators, corporate executives, and social entrepreneurs. The insights from this book can be usefully integrated into leadership curriculum and development programs to help the next generation of leaders respond to global challenges.
Chapter
Using examples from Asia, Africa, and North America, we demonstrate how restoration and stewardship projects, including those with significant community engagement, provide opportunities for environmental and biodiversity learning in cities. Although research on such programs is in its initial stages, several studies show positive impacts of urban environmental education and related field science inquiry experiences on participant environmental attitudes, awareness of urban nature, science understanding, and self-efficacy, with greater effects correlated with degree of involvement in hands-on, field-based experiences. In addition, programs that actively engage participants in restoration and inquiry reflect social equity, participatory, and environmental principles central to global initiatives in environmental education and sustainability. Such projects also reflect current theories of learning including those focusing on the ways children construct understanding of phenomena they encounter in everyday life (constructivism) and those that describe learning as an outcome of interaction with the socio-cultural and bio-physical environment (social learning). While recognizing the importance of school-based learning, our case examples illustrate the myriad of out-of-school learning arenas connected to projects in which civil society groups, government, and volunteers collaboratively engage in environmental stewardship, such as pond restoration to create dragonfly habitat in Japanese cities, indigenous species restoration at the Edith Stephens Wetland Park in Cape Flats, South Africa, and urban community gardening in vacant lots and other degraded spaces in the USA. More formal restoration projects, such as the daylighting of the Cheonggye-cheon River in Seoul, South Korea, as well as botanic gardens that feature biological and cultural diversity, also integrate nature-based, cultural, historical, and science inquiry learning opportunities. Given that many urban environmental education projects are local in scope, partnerships with global initiatives such as the UN Education for Sustainable Development and the Convention for Biological Diversity Communication, Education and Public Awareness, and with NGOs, governments, and business, are needed to leverage these learning arenas to effect broader regional, national, and even global systemic change.
Article
Conclusion: Indigenous knowledge systems, and the processes for their evolution over time, can support rapid adaptation to complex and urgent crises. Rather than encouraging these knowledge systems to become more “scientific,” we urge a respectful acknowledgement of their distinctiveness and epistemology. We suggest that any effort to solve real-world problems should first engage with those local communities that are most affected, beginning from the perspective of indigenous knowledge and then seeking relevant scientific knowledge—not to validate indigenous knowledge, but to expand the range of options for action. This would make scientific knowledge more acceptable and relevant to the societies that it seeks to support, while critically promoting social justice and establishing self-determination as a key principle of engagement.