ArticlePublisher preview available

Social Epistasis Amplifies the Fitness Costs of Deleterious Mutations, Engendering Rapid Fitness Decline Among Modernized Populations

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Deleterious mutations are typically understood exclusively in terms of their harmful effects on carrier organisms. But there are convincing reasons to think that such adverse effects are not confined to the individual level. We argue that in social species, interorganismal gene-gene interactions, which in previous literatures have been termed social epistasis, allow genomes carrying deleterious mutations to reduce via group-level pleiotropy the fitness of others, including noncarriers. This fitness reduction occurs by way of degradation of group-level processes that optimize the reproductive ecology of a population for intergroup competition through, among other mechanisms, suppression of free-riding. Such damage to group regulatory processes suggests a hidden role for the accumulation of behavior-altering "spiteful" mutations in the dynamics of the demographic transition—these mutations may have contributed to the maladaptive outcomes of this process, such as widespread subreplacement fertility. A structured population model is presented describing aspects of this social epistasis amplification model. This phenomenon is also considered as a potential explanation for the results of Calhoun’s mouse utopia experiments, which provide an opportunity to directly test a major prediction stemming from the model.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
THEORETICAL ARTICLE
Social Epistasis Amplifies the Fitness Costs of Deleterious
Mutations, Engendering Rapid Fitness Decline
Among Modernized Populations
Michael A. Woodley of Menie
1,2
&Matthew A. Sarraf
3
&Radomir N. Pestow
1
&
Heitor B. F. Fernandes
4
Published online: 21 February 2017
#Springer International Publishing 2017
Abstract Deleterious mutations are typically understood ex-
clusively in terms of their harmful effects on carrier organ-
isms. But there are convincing reasons to think that such ad-
verse effects are not confined to the individual level. We argue
that in social species, interorganismal gene-gene interactions,
which in previous literatures have been termed social epista-
sis, allow genomes carrying deleterious mutations to reduce
via group-level pleiotropy the fitness of others, including non-
carriers. This fitness reduction occurs by way of degradation
of group-level processes that optimize the reproductive ecol-
ogy of a population for intergroup competition through,
among other mechanisms, suppression of free-riding. Such
damage to group regulatory processes suggests a hidden role
for the accumulation of behavior-altering "spiteful" mutations
in the dynamics of the demographic transitionthese muta-
tions may have contributed to the maladaptive outcomes of
this process, such as widespread subreplacement fertility. A
structured population model is presented describing aspects of
this social epistasis amplification model. This phenomenon is
also considered as a potential explanation for the results of
Calhouns mouse utopia experiments, which provide an op-
portunity to directly test a major prediction stemming from the
model.
Keywords Mutation accumulation .Mutation load paradox .
Pathological altruism .Spiteful mutations .Structured
population modeling .Social epistasis
Introduction
The Human Mutation Load Paradox
There is considerable interest in the consequences for human
populations of the relaxation of purifying selection against
deleterious, spontaneously occurring mutations (Lynch
2016). As human males age, they bequeath to their sperm
approximately two de novo base-substitution mutations per
year (Kong et al. 2012), and females bequeath around a quar-
ter of this number per year to their eggs (Wong et al. 2016).
Genomic count data indicate that offspring acquire approxi-
mately 40 to 80 de novo mutations per genome per generation
(Rahbari et al. 2016). When rarer spontaneously occurring
mutations are taken into consideration (e.g., insertion-
deletion events and copy number variation), the average off-
springs genome may contain as many as 100 de novo muta-
tions (Lynch 2016). It is believed that a small subset of these
mutations will be actively harmful (i.e., will produce some
pathological change in the offsprings phenotype; Lynch
2016), and some models (Kondrashov and Crow 1993)sug-
gest that this rate of deleterious mutation theoretically requires
substantial reproductive failure (as much as 88% of individ-
uals should fail to reproduce) in order to purify a population,
coupled with significant compensatory reproduction among
the fraction participating in procreation (approximately 16
offspring per woman) in order to stave off mutational
meltdown, i.e., population collapse due to the fitness costs of
accumulating deleterious mutations.
*Michael A. Woodley of Menie
Michael.Woodley@vub.ac.be
1
Technische Universität Chemnitz, Chemnitz, Germany
2
Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies, Vrije Universiteit
Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
3
Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA
4
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
Evolutionary Psychological Science (2017) 3:181191
DOI 10.1007/s40806-017-0084-x
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... Some of these mutations are postulated to cause psychopathology, degrade brain systems for binding moral foundations, or reduce intelligence. The rising frequency of individual pathologies is postulated to cause social pathologies, creating cultural amplifier effects as hypothesized by Dickens and Flynn (2001) and Sarraf et al. (2017). The presence of many "off-types" in the population is predicted to reduce in-group cohesion, and to favor ideological polarization and extremism that are supported by irrational beliefs. ...
Article
Full-text available
This is the review of a book that projects recent and ongoing cultural trends into the future, primarily for the United States, and deals with the future of human civilization more generally.
... spore productivity). A related term used previously is 'group-level pleiotropy,' used for example to refer to alleles that impact fitness of non-carriers through group-level effects (Woodley of Menie et al. 2017). 'Social pleiotropy' has also been used in the context of social animals to indicate when an individual trait impacts "several features of the social organization" (Thierry 2004) or when one locus impacts multiple social traits (Fouks et al. 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Cooperation is widespread among microbes. One mechanism proposed to constrain cheating is antagonistic pleiotropy, wherein mutations that cause defection from cooperation, while potentially under positive selection for this effect, reduce fitness at other traits. The bacterium Myxococcus xanthus engages in pleiotropically connected complex multicellular behaviors, including motility, predation, and starvation-induced fruiting-body development. Sporulation during development is susceptible to cheating. Here we investigate pleiotropic impacts on how cheater spores respond to stressful environmental changes after development, relative to cooperator spores. A cheater with a mutation in the developmental-signaling gene asgB shows antagonistic pleiotropy under both heat and basic-pH stress, while a cheater mutated at csgA shows synergistic pleiotropy under basic-pH stress. Further, in a social form of pleiotropy, cooperator spores formed in mixture with the asgB cheater are less resilient under basic pH than those from pure groups; interaction of cooperators with cheaters reduces the cooperators' physical robustness. Our results indicate that, depending on the mutation, pleiotropy can promote as well as limit cheating alleles. They additionally demonstrate that alleles can pleiotropically alter traits in organisms not carrying those alleles. Synergistic and social pleiotropy may contribute to shaping the evolutionary dynamics of cooperation and cheating in many social systems.
Article
The prediction that reduction of negative selection decreases group-level competitiveness, as reflected in increased individual-focused and diminished group-focused moral foundations, is tested. To measure this hypothesized shift in moral foundations, we conduct a culturomic analysis of the utilization frequencies of items sourced from the moral foundations item pool, tracked among Britannic populations from 1800 to 1999 using Google Ngram Viewer. The resultant higher-order factor, which tracks increasing individualizing values and decreasing binding values, is termed Asabiyyah (capturing social cohesion and collective purpose). Two predictors of this factor are examined: change in the strength of intergroup competition and change in levels of indicators of developmental instability. Both the strength of intergroup competition and levels of developmental instability associate with Asabiyyah. Rising developmental instability mediates the impact of inter-group competition, indicating that reduced between-group competition might have relaxed negative selection against mutations, which might reduce Asabiyyah via their effects on inter-genomic transactions. These results must be interpreted carefully, given the clear real-world evidence that explicit commitment to group-oriented values often features in harmful and maladaptive social and political ideologies of an extreme character.
Article
Full-text available
Participation in social behaviors that enhance group-level fitness may be influenced by mutations that affect patterns of social epistasis in human populations. Mutations that cause individuals to not participate in these behaviors may weaken the ability of members of a group to coordinate and regulate behavior, which may in turn negatively affect fitness. To investigate the possibility that de novo mutations degrade these adaptive social behaviors, we examine the effect of paternal age (as a well-established proxy for de novo mutation load) on one such social behavior, namely religious observance, since religiosity may be a group-level cultural adaptation facilitating enhanced social coordination. Using two large samples (Wisconsin Longitudinal Study and AddHealth), each of a different US birth cohort, paternal age was used to hierarchically predict respondent’s level of church attendance after controlling for multiple covariates. The effect is absent in WLS (β = .007, ns, N = 4560); however, it is present in AddHealth (β = − .046, p < .05, N = 4873) increasing the adjusted model R² by .005. The WLS respondents were (mostly) born in the 1930s, whereas the AddHealth respondents were (mostly) born in the 1970s. This may indicate that social-epistatic regulation of behavior has weakened historically in the USA, which might stem from and enhance the ability for de novo mutations to influence behavior among more recently born cohorts—paralleling the secular rise in the heritability of age at sexual debut after the sexual revolution.
Chapter
We build on the finding of accumulating deleterious mutations with the SEAM (the social epistasis amplification model), which posits that the fitness costs of deleterious mutations are not limited to the organisms that carry them. This is possible in light of the existence of interorganismal genomic interactions, that is, social epistasis, whereby the genome of an organism (or the genomes of organisms) can influence another organism’s (or other organisms’) gene expression and therefore phenotypic traits.
Chapter
We continue the historical theme of Chap. 3. We note that despite the profound enhancement of (components of) human well-being that modernization has produced, there are serious problems associated with the modern condition, which may be quantitatively (but likely not qualitatively) unique in the broader context of human history. Salient among these problems are nihilism and psychopathology, which seem to share phenomenology at the individual level and to be statistically associated at the group level.
Chapter
We critique Steven Pinker’s acclaimed book Enlightenment Now (2018) at length. In his defense of an optimistic view of modernity, Pinker fails to mention a variety of negative trends, such as those indicating declines in important dimensions of human intelligence.
Chapter
Marvin Harris has taken the vagaries of culture and grounded them in ecology. From burning witches, to worshiping animals, to proscribing foods, Harris finds religious and cultural idiosyncrasies to proceed from ecological vagaries. More than this, Harris broaches social structure, demographic constraint, race, death, sex, and fertility, all of which are traced back to some knowable ecological determinant from which they probabilistically derive. Yet, like Keeley, Harris pointedly rejects sociobiological explanations. He believed evolutionary explanations of cultural differences to be impossible, insufficient, and unnecessary. As herein explained, these assumptions stem from a misunderstanding of how rapidly populations can evolve, unfamiliarity with life history theory and related sociobiological explanations that explain intraspecific diversity, and overconfidence in phenotypic plasticity and environmental explanation more generally.
Chapter
Raymond Cattell distinguished crystallized intelligence, akin to stored knowledge, from fluid intelligence, akin to raw reasoning abilities. Likewise, he delineated personality into component parts. Though intelligence and personality each qualify as subdisciplines within psychology, both are subsumed, along with other traits, under the meta-theory of life history evolution. The relationship is profound, though not straightforward. As described in this chapter, both intelligence and personality vary along a life history continuum, such that, as life history slows, population mean intelligence increases, as do personality traits like risk aversion, conscientiousness, anxiety, and agreeableness. Nevertheless, this effect occurs on average, and there is strategic variation occurring, which obscures the relationship between population-mean intelligence, personality, and life history. Once understood, these relationships color and clarify Cattell’s life’s work.
Chapter
With eminently unpropitious timing, Thomas Robert Malthus wrote of resource competition just as humans were bursting the bonds of organic economies. An Essay on the Principle of Population, in warning of the ills consequent to population density and resultant resource competition, may have, however, underappreciated its evolutionary effects. Although the significance of mortality regime has superseded its overall significance, population density, and the resource competition it brings, was the variable around which life history theory was originally constructed. With the coming of density and accompanying competition, life history theory explains how populations change and stratify as they vie to survive and reproduce. As herein argued, the slowing of life history is a consequence of population density that Malthus could not suspect, but might have appreciated.
Preprint
Full-text available
Higher paternal age at offspring conception increases de novo genetic mutations (Kong et al., 2012). Based on evolutionary genetic theory we predicted that the offspring of older fathers would be less likely to survive and reproduce, i.e. have lower fitness. In a sibling control study, we find clear support for negative paternal age effects on offspring survival, mating and reproductive success across four large populations with an aggregate N > 1.3 million in main analyses. Compared to a sibling born when the father was 10 years younger, individuals had 4-13% fewer surviving children in the four populations. Three populations were pre-industrial (1670-1850) Western populations and showed a pattern of paternal age effects across the offspring's lifespan. In 20th-century Sweden, we found no negative paternal age effects on child survival or marriage odds. Effects survived tests for competing explanations, including maternal age and parental loss. To the extent that we succeeded in isolating a mutation-driven effect of paternal age, our results can be understood to show that de novo mutations reduce offspring fitness across populations and time. We can use this understanding to predict the effect of increasingly delayed reproduction on offspring genetic load, mortality and fertility.
Article
Full-text available
Using data from the World Values Survey covering the period from 1981 to 2008, this study examines trends in the strength of religious belief worldwide. At the country level, the most important predictors of current religiosity are high intelligence and a history of communist rule, both of which reduce religious belief. However, religiosity has been rising vigorously in the communist and ex-communist countries during the surveyed period although religion is declining in most countries of non-communist Europe and East Asia. In addition to a history of communist rule, other factors associated with rising religiosity include high religiosity of young people relative to older people, high female relative to male religiosity, and a positive relationship between religiosity and fertility. In addition, religion tends to grow in countries in which less educated people are far more religious than those with higher education. Aside from the rebound of religion in the (ex)communist countries, the religiosity gap between the most advanced and the most backward nations has widened worldwide. The implications of these findings for the future of religion are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Higher paternal age at offspring conception increases de novo genetic mutations. Based on evolutionary genetic theory we predicted older fathers’ children, all else equal, would be less likely to survive and reproduce, i.e. have lower fitness. In sibling control studies, we find support for negative paternal age effects on offspring survival and reproductive success across four large populations with an aggregate N > 1.4 million. Three populations were pre-industrial (1670-1850) Western populations and showed negative paternal age effects on infant survival and offspring reproductive success. In 20th-century Sweden, we found minuscule paternal age effects on survival, but found negative effects on reproductive success. Effects survived tests for key competing explanations, including maternal age and parental loss, but effects varied widely over different plausible model specifications and some competing explanations such as diminishing paternal investment and epigenetic mutations could not be tested. We can use our findings to aid in predicting the effect increasingly older parents in today’s society will have on their children’s survival and reproductive success. To the extent that we succeeded in isolating a mutation-driven effect of paternal age, our results can be understood to show that de novo mutations reduce offspring fitness across populations and time periods.
Article
Full-text available
Worldwide rise of obesity may be partly related to the relaxation of natural selection in the last few generations. Accumulation of mutations affecting metabolism towards excessive fat deposition is suggested to be a result of less purging selection. Using the WHO and UN data for 159 countries there is a significant correlation (r = 0.60, p<0.01) between an index of the relaxed opportunity for selection (Biological State Index) and prevalence of obesity (percentage of individuals with BMI >30kg/m²). This correlation remains significant (r = 0.32., p<0.01) when caloric intake and insufficient physical activity prevalence are kept statistically constant (partial correlation analysis, N = 82). The correlation is still significant when gross domestic product per capita is also kept constant (r = 0.24, p <0.05, N = 81). In the last decades, prevalence of both obesity and underweight has increased in some countries despite no change in caloric intake nor in physical inactivity prevalence. Relaxed selection against genes affecting energy balance and metabolism may contribute to the increase of fatness independent from commonly considered positive energy balance. Diagnoses of individual predispositions to obesity at an early age and individual counselling on diet and behaviour may be appropriate strategies to limit further increases in body mass.
Article
Full-text available
Nothing is presently known about the relationship between individual differences in fertility and life history (LH) speed, as measured by the K-Factor. To examine this relationship, the correlation between LH speed and the number of children was examined in two, large samples (MIDUS II and the Swedish STAGE dataset). Their association was positive and statistically significant in both cross-national samples. The association was robust with respect to statistically controlling for participant age. Nested model comparison of a Model looking only at linear effects with a second Model incorporating a quadratic term did not improve model fit in any instance, suggesting directional selection for slower LH, The heritability of the indicators comprising the K-Factor positively moderated the strength of selection, while K-Factor loading weakly negatively moderated selection strength, suggesting that K-Factor variance, as a multivariate latent construct, is not the primary target of selection. These results are consistent with fertility intentions data indicating positive correlations between slower LH and desired numbers of children. In modern environments, higher mating effort does not appear to result in more offspring, likely because of strategic interference suppressing the fertility of those with fast LH, stemming from influences that may be either endogenous (i.e., contraceptive usage) or exogenous (i.e., the presence of laws, such as alimony) to the individual.
Book
The mouse was first used in immunological research by Paul Ehrlich in 1891 in an extraordinary series of experiments on the maternal transfer of antitoxic immunity. A short 22 years later in 1913 Halsey Bagg acquired a stock of albino mice from a commercial dealer and used them in a series of experiments on learning. Because he was interested in the genetics of intelligence, Halsey Bagg began breeding a pedigreed line of these mice that were subsequently named for him - Bagg Albino. Though Halsey Bagg is not credited with initiating the inbred strains of mice, his stock curiously has played an indisputably important role. Bagg Albinos were progenitors of the present day BALB/c family of sublines - the subject of this book. They were also used as one of the parents in the development of inbred strains A, CBA and C3H, three other very famous strains. Today the BALB/c mouse is among the five most widely used inbred strains in biomedical research and a particular favorite in immunology and infectious disease research. The hallmark of the BALB/c response to so many kinds of infections is susceptibility and sometimes an exaggerated susceptibility, but this paradoxically is not associated with immunodeficiency as BALB/c is an excellent responder to immuni­ zation. These characteristics have made the BALB/c mouse a model for identifying genes that determine susceptibility to infectious and neoplastic diseases. In 1985 the laboratory BALB/c mouse became 72 years old. The current filial generations are somewhere around 350 generations [MURPHY].
Article
The effect of paternal age on offspring attractiveness has recently been investigated. Negative effects are predicted as paternal age is a strong proxy for the numbers of common de novo mutations found in the genomes of offspring. As an indicator of underlying genetic quality or fitness, offspring attractiveness should decrease as paternal age increases, evidencing the fitness-reducing effects of these mutations. Thus far results are mixed, with one study finding the predicted effect, and a second smaller study finding the opposite. Here the effect is investigated using two large and representative datasets (Add Health and NCDS), both of which contain data on physical attractiveness and paternal age. The effect is present in both datasets, even after controlling for maternal age at subject's birth, age of offspring, sex, race, parental and offspring (in the case of Add Health) socio-economic characteristics, parental age at first marriage (in the case of Add Health) and birth order. The apparent robustness of the effect to different operationalizations of attractiveness suggests high generalizability, however the results must be interpreted with caution, as controls for parental levels of attractiveness were indirect only in the present study.
Article
Three large and nationally representative datasets (NCDS, N = 5225, NLSY′79, N = 7598 and Project Talent, N = 76,150) are here examined in order to determine if models incorporating negative quadratic effects of IQ on fertility (which would indicate the presence of stabilizing phenotypic selection) improve model fit, relative to ones that only consider linear effects (which indicate directional phenotypic selection). Also considered were possible interactions among these terms and sex and race. For two datasets (NCDS and NLSY′79) the best fitting models did not include quadratic terms, however significant sex*IQ and race*IQ interactions were found respectively. Only in Project Talent did the inclusion of a quadratic effect (along with IQ*sex and IQ²* sex interactions) yield the best-fitting model. In this instance a small magnitude, significant negative quadratic term was found in addition to a larger magnitude linear term. Post hoc power analysis revealed that power was lacking in the two smaller samples (NCDS and NLSY′79) to detect the quadratic term, however the best fitting and most parsimonious models selected for these datasets did not include the quadratic term. The quadratic terms were furthermore several times smaller in magnitude than the linear terms in all models incorporating both terms. This indicates that stabilizing phenotypic selection is likely only very weakly present in these datasets. The dominance of linear effects across samples therefore suggests that phenotypic selection on IQ in these datasets is principally directional, although the magnitude of selection is relatively small, with IQ explaining at most 1% of the variance in fertility.