Access to this full-text is provided by Canadian Center of Science and Education.
Content available from International Journal of Marketing Studies
This content is subject to copyright.
International Journal of Marketing Studies; Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
ISSN 1918-719X E-ISSN 1918-7203
Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education
155
The Effect of Ambient Scents on Consumer Responses: Consumer
Type and His Accompaniment State as Moderating Variables
Rym Bouzaabia1
1 Institute of High Business Studies of Sousse (IHEC–Sousse), University of Sousse, Tunisia
Correspondence: Rym Bouzaabia, Institute of High Business Studies of Sousse (IHEC–Sousse), University of
Sousse, Tunisia. Tel: 216-20-255-684. E-mail: rymb060@gmail.com
Received: August 24, 2013 Accepted: October 2, 2013 Online Published: January 22, 2014
doi:10.5539/ijms.v6n1p155 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijms.v6n1p155
Abstract
The aim of this research is to study the effect of ambient scents on consumers’ responses and in particular the
moderating effect of consumer type (hedonic vs utilitarian) and his accompaniment state (accompanied vs
single).
An experiment was conducted in a real business context (NIKE store), where 400 customers were surveyed. The
results indicate that a pleasant ambient scent improves evaluation of store environment, products, quality of
services provided, the level of pleasure experienced, the level of stimulation, their intention to revisit the store
and customers’ spending. However, ambient scents have no effect on the real time spent in the store. Finally, in
the presence of an ambient scent, it was found that hedonic customers intend more to revisit the store than
utilitarian ones and that accompanied customers tend to spend more and have more fun shopping than single
customers.
Keywords: store atmospherics, scent, consumer behavior, hedonic consumer, utilitarian consumer, consumer
accompaniment state
1. Introduction
The current economic landscape experiences now a turning point, in which the only survivors are the most
competitive. Indeed, the reasons why the market is becoming more and more difficult to keep up with are many.
Firstly, less and less differentiated products and services make it difficult for retailers to sell their stocks and to
offer themselves a place in the market. Second, the development of e-commerce is not without disadvantages.
Indeed, with all the benefits that e-buying offers to consumers in terms of time, price and delivery time, and
despite the problems of trust towards merchant sites, fewer and fewer consumers visit outlets to make their
purchases. Finally, the act of consumption has undergone deep changes over the last century and the last decade.
The different socio-historical developments have transformed consumption patterns, and contrary to the trend of
material consumption that existed before, we now consume to produce social identity or experience, with an
appeal for the intangible (Rochefort, 1995). In our post-modern society, consumer society changed attitudes
towards leisure: Consumption is cherished and considered an experience-rewarding to consumers looking for
hedonism in their purchases (Caru & Cova, 2003; Filser, 2002; Holbrook & Hirschman, 1982). Thus, hedonism
and the growing maturity of consumers who are becoming increasingly demanding and selective make the task
even more difficult for retailers.
Such threats have forced distributors to adapt their offers to ensure their sustainability and their profitability.
It is in this context that the managerial applications of experiential marketing, especially olfactory marketing,
were a great commercial success in many different economic sectors, in various classes of products such as
cosmetics, hygiene, interior design, automotive, textile, food and catering.
Indeed, retailers realized the potential olfactory elements and other environmental factors offer, although they
still use them mostly intuitively. However, scent marketing is not confined to professionals; academic research is
also interested in understanding its functioning and its effects on consumer responses. Paradoxically, if the
commercial use of olfaction is a great success, academic research in this area is still in its infancy, opening the
path for more empirical research (Maille, 2001).
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
156
Thus, in our research we will examine the effect of the presence vs absence of a nice ambient scent on customer
responses in a ready-to-wear store. We focus in particular the moderating role of customer-type (hedonic vs.
utilitarian) and the consumer accompaniment state (single vs. accompanied).
Then, our paper is structured as follows. We first expose a synthesis of studies of olfactory marketing to justify
the theoretical and practical feasability of the study. Then we will present the methodology and the main results
of a field survey in a real business environment: the NIKE store at the "Champs Elysées" mall in Tunisia, of 400
customers. Finally, we conclude with the theoretical and managerial contributions of this study and propose
future research avenues.
2. Review of the literature
2.1 The Influence of Ambient Scents on Cognitive Reactions
A physiological study of the brain performed with individuals placed in a scented environment states that the
waves recorded by the EEG reveal a cognitive activity caused by the presence of an ambient scent (Lorig &
Roberts, 1990).
Furthermore, another study showed that ambient scent could improve assessments of photographs presented to
the subjects (Kirk-Smith & Booth, 1992).
Some studies have shown that the repetition of a perfume can be a memorizing factor. Can and Ross (1989)
showed that the diffusion of the same scent (versus no scent or a different scent) during learning and memorizing
pictures increases recognition performance and thus memory performance, the same like memorizing words. The
results of Morrin and Ratneshwar (2000) prove the same. These two authors studied the impact of spreading a
pleasant ambient scent on memorizing names of known and unknown brands. Their research indicated that in the
presence of a geranium scent, the recall task is particularly better for unknown brands. These results can be
explained by a greater evaluation time in the presence of a pleasant scent.
Spangengberg et al. (1996) showed that the presence of a pleasant ambient scent allows for a better assessment
of the store, the store environment, goods in general or specific products. Similarly, Chebat and Michon (2003)
highlighted the positive effect of the presence of pleasant scents on the assessment of the global environment and
the products featured in this space. Rotton (1983) was able to highlight the negative impact of an unpleasant
scent on the evaluation of paintings, pictures of people or their descriptions. Finally, it seems that a nice room
smell improves the quality of service encounter (Baron, 1981), but also provides a better assessment of service
quality (Chebat & Michon, 2003; Maille, 2005). Hence, we formulate our research hypotheses H1, H2 and H3.
H1: Diffusion of a pleasant ambient sent positively influences assessment of the overall store environment.
H2: Diffusion of a pleasant ambient sent positively influences assessment of the offered products.
H3: Diffusion of a pleasant ambient sent positively influences assessment of the quality of offered services.
2.2 The Influence of Ambient Scents on Affective Reactions
Unlike taste, there is no universally appreciated scent. Hedonic nature of scents is then fully learned.
Moch & Bonnefoy (1997) highlight in this regard that it is generally considered that the vast majority of
emotional responses to scents is acquired by learning in the family and cultural contexts we have been immersed
in from childhood.
This association between scents and emotions is explained by the fact that scents are processed more directly in
the limbic system than other sensory neurons (Gibbons, 1986). A scent has been described as an emotional sense
as opposed to the so-called "Cognitive" senses like seeing and hearing (Engen, 1982).
Sugano (1992) and Lorig (1992), have established the effect of some scents on measures of physiological
activity. For example, appearance of alpha waves in EEG indicates low arousal. Increases in CNV (Contingent
Negative Variation) and blood circulation showed in turn an increase in arousal. Finally, increases in
micro-vibrations reflect intellectual or muscle tensions.
Knasko (1995) showed that a pleasant scent is associated with a positive mood in the case of a museum visit.
Leenders et al. (1999) showed that the presence of a lemon scent (vs. no scent) positively influences the
emotional state of an individual in a supermarket.
In general, the presence of a pleasant scent seems to improve mood and increase level of enjoyment, whereas an
unpleasant scent seems to deteriorate that emotional state (Ehrlichman & Bastone, 1992).
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
157
Furthermore, the authors find that in the case of a pleasant scent, the effects do not persist over time. Together,
the judgment of pleasantness evolves towards neutrality. However, in the case of an unpleasant scent, the effects
last longer.
Other studies have shown that the diffusion of a pleasant ambient scent may also increase level of customers'
stimulation (Guichard et al., 1998; Leenders et al., 1999). Hence, we formulate our research hypotheses H4 and
H5.
H4: Diffusion of a pleasant ambient sent positively influences level of enjoyment of customers.
H5: Diffusion of a pleasant ambient sent positively influences level of stimulation of customers.
2.3 The Influence of Ambient Scent on Conative Reactions
Knasko (1989) conducted an experiment during several weeks in a jewelry store. The experiment was conducted
in two distinct conditions, one being unscented, and one scented with one of the following scents: a Floral /
Fruity fragrance or a spicy smell. The results indicate that when scent is present, men and women spend more
time in the floral / fruity condition. However, the use of a spicy scent had no effect on men.
Similarly, Gueguen and Petr (2006) showed in their experiments in a restaurant in which they diffused is a lemon
scent, a scent of lavender compared to no scent (control condition), lavender scent but not lemon significantly
increases the real time spent in the restaurant. The authors explained these results by the pleasant nature and the
relaxing effect of lavender.
Other empirical studies conducted confirmed the existence of a significant positive relationship between the
presence of pleasant scents and the real time spent in the store (Bone & Ellen, 1999; Daucé, 2000; Hirsch, 1995;
Knasko, 1995; Leenders et al., 1999).
Hirsh (1995) showed that slot machines players in Las Vegas casinos spend more money as they play in a
scented environment. The same results were noticed in a clothing store (Terrling et al., 1992), a shoe shop, as
well as a Piza restaurant (Gueguen & Petr, 2006). These positive results are encouraging since they were
obtained under real conditions.
Spangenberg et al. (1996) found that subjects in a scented condition express a strong desire to revisit the store.
However, the authors have shown that ambient scents did not significantly increase purchase intentions for
products which consumers already favor. However, Hirsh and Gay (1991) indicate that purchase intentions are
improved when a floral scent is diffused and this, whatever the intensity of the scent and the preferences of
individuals towards such a scent. Finally, Mattila and Wirtz (2001) pointed to the positive effect of a stimulating
versus relaxing scent on intention to revisit. Hence our research hypotheses H6, H7 and H8 are as follows.
H6: The diffusion a pleasant ambient sent positively influences the intention to revisit the store.
H7: The diffusion a pleasant ambient sent positively influences customer spending.
H8: The diffusion a pleasant ambient sent positively influences the real time spent in the store.
2.4 The Moderating Effect of Consumer Type (Hedonic Vs. Utilitarian)
We distinguish two groups of customers, hedonic or utilitarian (Babin et al., 1994; Babin & Attaway, 2000).
• Hedonic customers are more oriented towards the environment's sensory dimensions. They are sensitive to
external environment elements and take pleasure in shopping (Cottet & Vibert, 1999). For this group of
consumers, shopping is recreational and social (Downs, 1961).
• In contrast to hedonic customers, utilitarian experience less pleasure and stimulation to shop. They are less
attracted to shopping malls. These visitors are much less likely to consider environmental factors. They do not
bond with vendors and are more influenced by price than any other factor. They make little or unplanned
impulse purchases, spend a lot less time shopping than hedonic customers and consider shopping as a necessity
rather than pleasure (Cottet & Vibert, 1999).
To our knowledge, no study to date has addressed the moderating role of customer type (utilitarian or hedonic) in
ambient scents effects.
However, we can assume that the effects of ambient scents on emotional states, perceptions and behavior will be
moderated by consumer type (Hedonic / utilitarian). Hedonic customers are more sensory dimension-oriented
seeking sensation or hedonic gratification than utilitarian customers. Thus, the presence of a pleasant ambient
scent could have a greater and more positive impact on hedonic customers' cognitive, affective and conative
responses than utilitarian customers.
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
158
However, utilitarian customers are directed to the environment's cognitive dimension (thoughts) and visit malls
for functional reasons (collect or obtain information about a service or a product without much delay). They will
be less influenced by environment's sensorial elements like its scent, thus:
H9: Consumer type (hedonic vs. utilitarian) moderates the effect of ambient scents on cognitive responses:
- H9.1.: Assessment of the overall store environment
- H9.2.: Assessment of the offered products
- H9.3.: Assessment of the quality of offered services
H10: Consumer type (hedonic vs. utilitarian) moderates the effect of ambient scents on affective responses:
- H10.1.: Pleasure
- H10.2.: Stimulation
H11: Consumer type (hedonic vs. utilitarian) moderates the effect of ambient scents on conative responses:
- H11.1.: The real time spent in the store
- H11.2.: The amounts spent
- H11.3.: The intention to revisit the store
2.5 The Moderating Effect of Consumer Accompaniment State
To our knowledge no study to date has dealt with the moderating role of consumer accompaniment state
(accompanied vs single) in the relationship between ambient scents and customers responses in a retail context.
However, consumer accompaniment state represents an important dimension of the consumption experience.
The few studies in marketing interested in this variable examined the role, history and impact on individual
behavior of visitor accompaniment state, of entertainment venues and more specifically museums.
DeBenetti (2001) claims that accompanied visitors tend to consume more than single ones. These latter often
stop at the bar or restaurant. Time-wise, the author points out those single customers spend less time in a
museum. Hood (1994) explains this difference in behavior in that the museum's annexed services represent
factors for social interactions between visitors. Moreover, the author further explains that single visitors are more
focused on the core of the consumption experience rather than on the experiential dimension of the offer
(McManus, 1994).
In other words, single customers are proved to be the most focused on the functional dimension of the offer (like
the main offered product). Subsequently, accompanied visitors are more oriented and sensitive to the experiential
dimension of the offer, encouraging social interaction among customers.
Therefore, it would be interesting to study this variable in a retail context and put customers in an intimate social
context during their visit to a shopping center, in particular a point of sale, bearing in mind the distinction
between accompanied and single customers.
H12: The consumer accompaniment state (accompanied vs. Single) moderates the effect of ambient scents on
cognitive responses:
- H12.1.: Assessment of the overall store environment
- H12.2.: Assessment of the offered products
- H12.3.: Assessment of the quality of offered services
H13: The consumer accompaniment state moderates the effect of ambient scents on affective responses:
- H13.1.: Pleasure
- H13.2.: Stimulation
H14: The consumer accompaniment moderates the effect of ambient scents on conative responses:
- H14.1.: The real time spent in the store
- H14.2.: The amounts spent
- H14.3.: The intention to revisit the store
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
159
3. Method
3.1 Sample and Data Collection
In order to test our research hypothesis, an experiment, involving a sample of 400 individuals, 50% of which are
accompanied and 50% are single, was conducted in a ready-to-wear "NIKE" store in the "Les Champs Elysées"
shopping center Tunisia (Table 1). To this effect, we administered a face to face questionnaire.
Table 1. Experimental plan
Scented condition Sample
Control condition:
Absence of a pleasant ambient scent
200
Experimental condition:
Presence of a pleasant ambient scent (Ylang Ylang scent)
200
Total 400
The "NIKE" brand was selected because it has a strong marketing department interested in the concept of
sensorial marketing, in particular olfactory marketing, hence their full cooperation. Moreover, none of the items
sold in the "NIKE" store issues any detectable scent and therefore can not be interpreted as particularly scented.
To achieve our aims, we manipulated the condition of ambient scents in the above-mentioned store, all things
being equal for the other atmospheric variables. We insisted that the store manager diffuses the same background
music and keep the same environment set during our experimentation. Similarly, we have controlled the ambient
scent's diffusion intensity.
The scent diffuser has been installed in an unseen corner of the store around the fitting rooms and connected to
the central air conditioning system. Three-second scent sprays every six minutes were set allowing for a perfect
rendition of the scent and a rapid, dry, homogeneous and subtle diffusion providing excellent olfactory comfort.
The choice of the scent for the purpose of our experiment was inspired by the study of Spangenberg et al. (1996).
Consistent with many studies (Daucé, 2000; Roullet, 2004), the choice of the olfactory stimuli in our experiment
is made by carrying out a pre-test on a small sample of the target population, ie customers of the store where the
final experiment will take place. Therefore, for the purposes of this study, we pre-tested 5 floral scents; wood,
citrus, spices and mint to check the hedonic nature of each.
All scents were essential natural oils from a single supplier. As for the subjects of the pretest, a convenience
sample of 50 customers, intercepted near the store entrance. After making sure that the different subjects showed
no scents-related allergies, we invited them to smell for two minutes cotton balls soaked in each olfactory stimuli
of the experiment held in opaque flasks. Finally, subjects were asked to evaluate the stimulating nature, approval
and familiarity of each odor. For this purpose, we used four items of the scale of Daucé (2000) measured on a
semantic differential continuum (attractive / unattractive, pleasant / unpleasant, stimulating / relaxing, motivating
/less motivating), itself inspired by the scale of Fisher (1974), and perceived familiarity measurement scale
(familiar / Not familiar) to assess the presented scents (Crowley, 1993; Spangenberg et al., 1996).
To select the scent that will serve as a stimulus in our experiment, we classified scents according to the means of
approval, stimulation and familiarity (Table 2).
Table 2. Results of the pre-test for "scents"
Scent category Individual scent tested Number of
participants
Approval Stimulation Familiarity
flowers Ylang Ylang 50 0.585 0.680 1.780
Spices Cinnamon 50 0.003 -0.734 4.260
Woods Sandalwood 50 -0.565 -0.386 3.560
Citrus Citrus: (orange; lemon and
grapefruit)
50 0.320 0.624 2.720
Mint Green mint 50 -0.343 -0.183 4.060
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
160
The above results indicate that the Ylang Ylang and citrus scents have the best means of approval, but also
considered as the most stimulating. The retained scent that has the advantage of being neutral in terms of
congruence or incongruence with the products sold in the store and with the cleaners used in the store and did
not prime special memories, as recommended by Spangenber et al. (1996), is the Ylang Ylang scent.
The scent used for the purposes of our experiment was provided to us by the only consulting olfactory
communication agency in Tunisia "Ylang" created in June 2003 and whose partners are:
The European leader in olfactory Marketing "Prolitec" which has developed a technology that
transforms under low-temperature a pure scent concentrate into scent particles below one micron in size which
behave like a gas, completely dry, with constant output to avoid olfactory pollution, deposit and impregnation.
The house creating perfumes and scents, originally-manufactured and customized "Créassence".
3.2 Measurement Scales
In the first step, we conducted an exploratory factor Analysis with a varimax rotation to purify and assess the
dimensionality of constructs (Appendix 1).
In the second step, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis on AMOS 18.0 to test the reliability and validity
of each measurement scale (Appendix 2).
The emotional responses scale was measured with items adapted from Daucé (2000). This scale consisted of
seven items measured by 7-point semantic differential scales.
The assessment of the overall environment of the store was measured with seven items under a 7-point
semantically differentiated scale, adapted from Daucé (2000).
Assessment the products offered by the "NIKE" store was measured by four semantically differentiated scales
with 7 points adapted from Bellizzi et al. (1983).
To measure assessment of the quality of services offered by the "NIKE" store, we used a 7-point semantically
differentiated scale adapted from Taylor (1996).
To measure the intention to revisit the store, we used the scale proposed by Spangenberg et al. (1996). This is a
one-item, and a 7-point semantically differentiated scale. For the real time spent in the store, time records were
made in the store throughout the test period. These surveys may be related or unrelated to questionnaire
administration. To measure the expenditure of store customers, we asked them: How much money did they
spend? Finally, to measure consumer type, we use Babin et al. (1994) measure which has been validated in a real
retail environment by Chebat et al. (2005). This scale consists of seven items and measured by a 5 point Likert
scale ranging from "Strongly agree" to "Strongly disagree."
4. Results
Our study examines the effect of the presence vs. absence of a nice ambient scent on customers' cognitive,
affective and conative responses. Thus, it is necessary to test the effect of our independent variable "ambient
scent" by conducting one-factor analysis of variance. The results are presented in Table 3 as follows:
Table 3. Results of the one-factor analysis of variance
Dependent variables F Sig. Absence Presence
Assessment of the overall
store environment
406,957 ,000 +++ -,713 ,678
Assessment of the offered
products
533,926 ,000 +++ -,755 ,761
Assessment of the quality of
the offered services 324,073 ,000 +++ 3,353 5,707
Pleasure 139,940 ,000 +++ -,527 ,473
Stimulation 229,576 ,000 +++ -,611 ,588
Intention to revisit the store 236,278 ,000 +++ 3,060 5,383
Amounts spent 6,612 ,010 ++ 2,139 2,592
Real time spent 3,840 ,053 NS 24,726 27,223
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
161
The results below indicate clearly that assessment of the overall store environment, assessment of the offered
products, assessment of the quality of services, level of experienced pleasure, level of stimulation, intention to
revisit the store and the amounts spent vary significantly depending on the presence (vs. absence) of a pleasant
ambient scent (Table 4). In fact, customers have better assessments, felt greater pleasure and stimulation,
expressed intention to revisit the store and spent more in a scented environment than in an unscented one (Table
3). Thus, we confirm our hypotheses (H1, H2, H3, H4, H5, H6, H7) and we reject hypothesis (H8). In fact, it
seems that the diffusion of an ambient scent in the store has no effect on real time spent by customers.
- Moderating effects of consumer type (Hedonic vs Utilitarian) and consumer accompaniment state
(Accompanied vs. Single)
The moderating role of our variables (consumer type and consumer accompaniment state) will be tested by an
analysis of variance in which the moderator variable will be inserted as a fixed factor. Moderation is accepted
provided that there is a significant interaction effect of the moderator variable and the independent variable on
the dependent variable (Sharma et al., 1981).
Our continuous variable "consumer type" processed as two classes; Hedonic vs Utilitarian, using a "Two-Step
Cluster Analysis", a method adopted by Drugeon-Lichtlé (1998) and Helm-Guizon (1997).
The table 4 summarizes the results of the interaction between consumer type consumer (hedonic versus
utilitarian), consumer accompaniment state (accompanied vs single) and ambient conditions (presence vs.
absence of ambient scent) and customers reactions. We are now able to confirm or refute our research
hypotheses on the moderating effects of the two variables: consumer type and consumer accompaniment state.
Table 4. Summary of results for the moderating variables "consumer type (hedonic versus utilitarian)" and
"consumer accompaniment state (accompanied vs single)"
Dependent variables
Consumer type x Ambient condition Consumer accompaniment state x
Ambient condition
F Sig F Sig
Assessment of the overall store
environment
2,735 ,099 0,201 ,654
Assessment of the offered
products
,064 ,801 ,045 ,831
Assessment of the quality of
the offered services
1,306 ,254 0,306 ,234
Pleasure ,537 ,464 9,645 ,002
Stimulation ,180 ,672 2,195 ,139
Intention to revisit the store 8,866 ,003 0,240 ,625
Amounts spent ,002 ,968 11,374 ,001
Real time spent No effect (principal) of ambient scent on
real time spent was dtected.
No effect (principal) of ambient scent
on real time spent was dtected.
From the above results, we notice there is no significant interaction between consumer type and ambient
condition on the cognitive, affective and conative responses. Therefore, hypotheses H9, H10, H11.1 and H11.2
are rejected. However, consumer type acts as a moderating variable at the level of the effect of ambient scent on
the intention to revisit the store. To know the meaning of moderation, means of the variable "intention to revisit
the store" were calculated by distinguishing two customer groups as hedonic or utilitarian. Therefore, we can
assume that hedonic customers intended more to revisit the "NIKE" store than utilitarian customers and this is
true whatever the ambient condition in which they are located (C1 and C2), with a peak at C1 (Table 5).
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
162
Table 5. Means of intention to revisit the "NIKE" store according the "consumer type"
Dependent variable Ambient condition Consumer type Mean
Intention to revisit the store Presence of Ylang Ylang scent
(C1)
Utilitarian 10,382
Hedonic 44,065
Absence of Ylang Ylang scent
(C2)
Utilitarian 11,899
Hedonic 37,554
As for consumer accompaniment state, it is found to moderate the relationship between ambient scents, felt
pleasure and amounts spent. Therefore, we confirm hypotheses H13.1 and H14.2 (Table 6). Average level of felt
pleasure, and amounts spent by accompanied consumers are greater than those of single ones, and this whatever
the ambient condition in which they are located (C1 and C2), with a peak at C1 (Table 6 and Table 7).
Table 6. Mean of pleasure according the “consumer accompaniment state”
Dependent variable Ambient condition Consumer accompaniment state Mean
Pleasure Presence of Ylang Ylang scent (C1) Single 0,121
Accompanied 3,456
Absence of Ylang Ylang scent (C2) Single 0,108
Accompanied 1,189
Table 7. Mean of amounts spent according the “consumer accompaniment state”
Dependent variable Ambient condition Consumer accompaniment state Mean
Amounts spent Presence of Ylang Ylang scent
(C1)
Single 1,152
Accompanied 6,367
Absence of Ylang Ylang scent
(C2)
Single 1,065
Accompanied 3,186
5. Discussion
Our study has checked through an experiment conducted in partnership with a ready-to-wear "NIKE" store, the
effect of ambient scents on customers' cognitive (assessment of the overall environment of the store, offered
products and the quality of offered services), affective (pleasure and stimulation) and conative (intention to
revisit the store, real time spent and money spent) responses. We have in particular checked the moderating
effect of consumer type (Hedonic vs Utilitarian) and his accompaniment state (Accompanied vs. Single).
5.1 Limitations
First, the act of conducting our study in a real store makes it difficult to control exogenous variables. Indeed,
consumer behavior can be influenced by uncontrolled factors. For example, other atmospheric variables,
including ambient music, interior design or crowdedness of the store may interact with ambient scents and
trigger different effects on customers (Kotler, 1974; Mattila & Wirtz, 2001).
Then, the external validity of our study and therefore its generalizability is limited because of validity
requirements. The selection of a particular cultural context "Tunisia" and "NIKE" store is a case in point. Indeed,
if local or contact personnel differ from one outlet to another, customers do also; from one neighborhood to
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
163
another, from one region to another, but also from one country to another. Therefore it is important to explore
more experimental sites, and more store types.
Finally, despite the limitations just mentioned, our study is interesting in more than one way. Its contribution lies
in both the theoretical and operational levels.
5.2 The Theoretical and Managerial Contributions
The first contribution of our research was to highlight the importance of developing retail spaces. Indeed, brands
should develop spaces where they can build a special relationship with their customers.
Our research has highlighted the interest of disseminating ambient scents to positively influence customers'
cognitive, affective and conative responses, but also to highlight the need to take into account consumer type
(hedonic vs. utilitarian) and their accompaniment state (accompanied vs single) as moderating variables between
ambient scents and customer responses.
Managerial contributions of this study relate essentially to store atmospherics and in particular olfactory
elements which should be considered by practitioners as a management tool likely to affect their customers'
cognitive, affective and conative reactions.
In our study, we noticed that consumer type and consumer accompaniment state could moderate the sensitivity
of the latter to the presence vs absence of a pleasant ambient scent. Hedonic customers intend more to revisit the
store in the presence of a pleasant ambient scent. Similarly, accompanied customers spend more and express
more pleasure than single customers in the presence of a pleasant ambient scent.
Consequently, managers should consider the importance of the variables: consumer type and his accompaniment
state. They should attach a Particular interest to the experiential dimension of their offer and in Particular
olfactory elements in view of attracting hedonic consumers looking for a pleasant shopping experience. Also,
they should attend to eliminate elements that may be perceived as barriers to social interaction between
companions such as unpleasant atmosphere (unpleasant odor, very loud music...) to lead them to spend more and
have more fun shopping.
Finally, we point out that the NIKE store decided to pursue the experiment in several of its stores in Tunisia.
5.3 Future Research
From the literature and the obtained preliminary results, we can point to several avenues of future research. First,
future research should examine other characteristics of scents (such as scent intensity and its stimulating nature)
other than the approval characteristic. Moreover, future research should consider other environmental factors
(such as music, color, lighting, crowd...) that may interact with the effect of ambient scents on customer
responses.
It would also be interesting to introduce in the general model that we proposed other moderating variables such
as gender, age, optimum stimulation level (OSL), identity of companions etc. (Bitner, 1992; Debenedetti, 2003;
Maille, 2005).
Furthermore, in order to increase external validity of our research, it would be desirable to repeat this research in
a variety of stores to consider generalizing some the obtained results.
Dissemination of scents still poses today implementation difficulties. The ideal diffuser remains still to be
developed and choosing the scent still is not simple. This can be done by setting up conditions and specifications
by which different customers can provide tailored scents.
References
Babin, B. J., & Attaway, J. S. (2000). Atmospheric Affect as a Tool for Creating Value and Gaining Share of
Customer. Journal of Business Research, 49, 91–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(99)00011-9
Babin, B. J., Darden, W. R., & Griffin, M. (1994). Work and/or fun: measuring hedonic and utilitarian Shopping
Value. Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 644–646. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209376
Baron, R. A. (1981). Olfaction and human Social behavior: Effects of a pleasant Scent on attraction and Social
perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7(4), 611–616.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616728174016
Bellizi, J. A., Crowley, A. E., & Hasty, R. W. (1983). The effects of color in store design. Journal of Retailing,
59(1), 23–45.
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
164
Ben Dahmane Mouelhi, N. (2007). The joint influence of ambient factors on customer behavior: The case of
music and ambient odor. Ph.D. Dissertation. University de Caen/Basse–Normandie.
Bitner, M. J. (1992). Servicescapes: the impact of physical surrounding on customers and employees. Journal of
Marketing, 56(2), 57–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1252042
Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1999). Scent in the market place: explaining a fraction of olfaction. Journal of
Retailing, 75(2), 243–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(99)00007-X
Cann, A., & Ross, D. A. (1989). Olfactory stimuli as context cues in human memory. American Journal of
Psychology, 102(1), 91–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423118
Caru, A., & Cova, B. (2003). Approche empirique de l’immersion dans l’expérience de consommation: Les
opérations d’appropriation. Recherche et Applications en Marketing, 17(2), 41–53.
Chebat, J. C., & Michon, R. (2003). Impact of Odors on Shoppers Emotions, Cognition and Spending: A test of
Competitive Causal Theories. Journal of Business Research, 56, 529–539.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(01)00247-8
Chebat, J. C., Eroglu, S. A., & Machleit, K. A. (2005). The interaction of retail density and music tempo: Effects
on shopper responses. Psychology and Marketing, 22(7), 577–589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20074
Cottet, P., & Vibert, F. (1999). La Valorisation Hédonique et/ou utilitaire du Shopping dans le Magasin d’usines.
15ème Congré International de l’Association Française du Marketing. Strasbourg. Mercredi 19 et Jeudi 20
Mai. Tome 1: 93–116.
Crowley, A. E. (1993). The two-dimensional impact of color on shopping. Marketing Letters, 4(1), 59–69.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00994188
Daucé, B. (2000). La diffusion de senteurs d’ambiance au sein d’un lieu commercial: Les premiers résultats
d’une étude menée au sein d’un magasin de prêt à porter. 16ème Congrès International de l’Association
Française du Marketing,Montréal. 18-20 mai, Tome 2: 747–756.
Debenedetti, S. (2001). Experience visiting places of entertainment: the central role of companions. Recherche et
Applications en Marketing, 18(4), 43–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/076737010301800403
Downs, A. (1961). A theory of efficiency. Journal of Retailing, 37, 6–12.
Drugeon-Lichtlé, M. C. (1998). The impact of the color of an advertisement on the front felt emotions
announcement, attitudes and beliefs about the brand. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Paris–Dauphine.
Ehrlichman, H., & Bastone, L. (1992). The use of odour in the study of emotion. In S. van Toller & G. H. Dodd
(Eds.), Fragrance: The psychology and biology of perfume (pp. 143–159). Elsevier Applied Science.
Engen, T. (1982). The perception of odors. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Fisher, J. (1974). Situation specific variables as determinants of perceived environmental esthetic quality and
perceived crowdedness. Journal of research in personality, 8, 177–188.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(74)90019-1
Guéguen, N., & Petr, C. (2006). Odors and consumer behavior in a restaurant. International Journal of
Hospitality Management, 25, 335–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2005.04.007
Guichard, N., Lehu, J. M., & Vanheems, R. (1998). Marketing du cinquième sens :L’aromachologie au service
de la stratégie marketing. Décisions Marketing, 14, 7–17.
Helm-Guizon, A. (1997). Mental imagery and effects of persuasive communication: application to a work of art
included in an advertisement. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Paris–Dauphine.
Hirsch, A. R. (1995). Effects of ambient odors on slot-machine usage in Las Vegas casino. Psychology and
Marketing, 12(7), 585–594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.4220120703
Hirsch, A. R., Gay, S. E. (1991). Effect of ambient olfactory stimuli on the evaluation of a common consumer
product. Chemical Senses, 16, 115–136.
Holbrook, M. B., & Hirschman, E. C. (1982). The experiential aspects of consumption: Consumer Fantasies,
Feelings and Fun. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 132–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208906
Hood. (1994). L’ interaction sociale au musée, facteur d’attraction des visiteurs occasionnels. Publics et Musées,
5, 45–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/pumus.1994.1036
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
165
Kirk-Smith, M. D., & Booth, D. A. (1992). Effects of Natural and Synthetic Odorants on Mood and Perception
of other People. Chemical Senses, 17(6), 835–849.
Knasko, S. C. (1989). Ambient odor and Shopping behavior. Chemical Senses, 14(5), 719.
Knasko, S. C. (1995). Pleasant odors and Congruency: Effects on Approach Behavior. Chemical Senses, 18(5),
479–487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/20.5.479
Kotler, P. (1974). Atmospherics as a Marketing Tool. Journal of Retailing, 49(4), 48–64.
Leenders, M., Smidts, A., & Langeveld, M. (1999). Effects of Ambient Scent in Supermarkets: A Field
Experiment. 28th International Conference EMAC. Berlin.
Lorig, T. S., & Roberts, M. (1990). Odor and cognitive alteration of the contingent negative variation. Chemical
Senses, 15(5), 537–545. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/15.5.537
Maille, V. (2001). L’influence des stimuli olfactifs sur le comportement du consommateur: un état des
recherches. Recherche et Applications en Marketing, 16(2), 51–74.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/076737010101600204
Maille, V. (2005). Perfume outlets of a public service: is it sufficient that it smells good? Days Trend Marketing,
January, Paris.
Mattila, A. S., & Wirtz, J. (2001). Congruency of Scent and Music as a Driver of In-Store Evaluations and
Behavior. Journal of Retailing, 77(2), 273–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00042-2
McManus, P. M. (1994). Le contexte social, un des déterminants du comportement d'apprentissage dans les
musées. Publics et Musées, 5, 59–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/pumus.1994.1037
Moch, A., & Bonnefoy, B. (1997). Odeurs et environnement urbain: Le métro parisien. Psychologie Française,
42(2), 175–182.
Morrin, M., & Ratneshwar, S. (2000). The impact of ambient Scent on evaluation, attention, and memory for
familiar and unfamiliar brands. Journal of Business Research, 49(2), 157–165.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(99)00006-5
Rochefort, R. (1995). Society of consumer. Paris.
Rotton, J. (1983). Affective and cognitive consequences of malodorous pollution. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 4(2), 171–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp0402_5
Roullet, B. (2004). The influence of color in marketing: Towards a neuropsychology of consumer. Thèse de
Doctorat, Université de Rennes I, Décembre, 628 pages.
Sharma. S., Durand, R. M., & Gur Arie, O. (1981). Identification and analysis of moderator variables. Journal of
Marketing Research, 18, 291–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3150970
Spangenberg, E. C., Crowley, A. E., & Handerson, P. W. (1996). Improving the Store Environment: Do
olfactory cues affect evaluations and behaviors? Journal of Marketing, 60(2), 67–80.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1251931
Sugano, H. (1992). Psychological Studies of fragrances. In S. V. Toller & G. Dodd (Eds.), Fragrance: The
psychology and biology of perfume. Elsevier Science Publishers LTD.
Taylor, S. (1996). The role of effective states and locus of attribution in evaluations of service. Journal of
Consumer Research, 13(3), 216–225.
Terrling, A., Niscdor, R., & Koster, E. P. (1992). The effect of Ambient odors on Shopping. Chemical Senses,
17(6), 886.
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
166
Appendix 1. Exploratory factor analysis results
Measurement scale 1: The emotionals responses
KMO= 0.893;
Rounded Khi square= 2154.283; Ddl=15
Significance of Bartlett= 0.000
Cronbach Alpha= 0. 928
Contribution to postmen training
F1
% Explained Variance = 75.976%
Sad / Joyful .892
Unhappy/Happy .899
Pessimistic / Optimistic .860
Restless/Relaxed .888
Upset/ Calm .854
Impatient/ Clear mind .835
Measurement scale 2: Assessment of the overall store environment
KMO= 0.932;
Rounded Khi square= 2312.978; Ddl=15
Significance of Bartlett= 0.000.
Cronbach Alpha= 0. 935
Contribution to postmen training
F1
% Explained Variance = 80.534%
Unattractive / Attractive .893
Depressing / Joyful .879
Boring / Stimulating .882
Bad / Good .912
Less motivating / Motivating .906
Unpleasant / Pleasant .912
Measurement scale 3: Assessment of the offered products
KMO= 0.712;
Rounded Khi square= 456.754; Ddl=15
Significance of Bartlett= 0.000.
Cronbach Alpha= 0. 821
Contribution to postmen training
F1
% Explained Variance = 56.993%
Style (modern / outdated) .860
choice (suitable / unsuitable) .836
quality (good / bad) .852
price (low / high) .639
Measurement scale 4: Consumer type
KMO= 0.775; Rounded Khi-square=1941.486;
Ddl=15
Significance of Bartlett= 0.000.
Contribution to postmen training
F1: Hédoniste
% Explained Variance= 61.836%
Cronbach Alpha= 0. 933
F2 : Utilitariste
% Explained Variance=
23.580%
Cronbach Alpha= 0. 851
This shopping experience was really felt like an
escape.
.916 -.240
In comparison to other activities that I have done, the
time spent shopping was really nice.
.920 -.217
When shopping, I felt the sensation of adventure. .906 -.126
I loved being surrounded by new products. .848 -.070
I could not buy what I really needed. -.107 .936
I made the purchases I wanted during my shopping
experience.
-.216 .916
www.ccsenet.org/ijms International Journal of Marketing Studies Vol. 6, No. 1; 2014
167
Appendix 2. Confirmatory factor analysis results
Construct Item Contribution
factorielle
St. Loading
factor
(st. error)
Squared
multiple
correlation
SMC
Composite
reliability
(rhô de
Jöreskog)
ρ (A)>0.7
Convergent
validity
ρvc>0.5
Discriminant
validity
ρvc>r2
Emotionals responses: X2/df= 1.124; CFI= 1.000; NFI= 0.996; GFI= 0.992; RMR= 0.037; RMSEA= 0.018
Pleasure
Cronbach’
=0.933
Sad / Joyful 0.918 0.842
0.923
0.800
yes
Unhappy/Happy 0.883 0.780
Pessimistic / Optimistic 0.920 0.847
Stimulation
Cronbach’
=0.909
Restless/Relaxed 0.848 0.718
0.909
0.770
yes
Upset/ Calm 0.857 0.734
Impatient/ Clear mind 0.927 0.860
Assessment of the overall store environment: X2/df= 4.018; CFI= 0.988; NFI= 0.984; GFI= 0.968; RMR=0.048; RMSEA= 0.087
Assessment
of the
overall
store
environme
nt
Sad / Joyful 0.864 0.747 0.951 0.765
Unhappy/Happy 0.843 0.711
Pessimistic / Optimistic 0.850 0.723
Restless/Relaxed 0.899 0.808
Upset/ Calm 0.893 0.798
Impatient/ Clear mind 0.900 0.810
Assessment of the offered products: X2/df= 2.688; CFI= 0.996; NFI= 0.994; GFI= 0.997; RMR= 0.030; RMSEA= 0.065
Assessment
of the
offered
products
Style (modern / outdated) 0.794 0.631 0.818 0.535
choice (suitable / unsuitable) 0.722 0.522
quality (good / bad) 0.821 0.675
price (low / high). 0.561 0.468
Consumer type: X2/df= 2.876; CFI= 0.978; NFI= 0.987; GFI= 0.978; RMR=0.072; RMSEA= 0.069
Hedonic
This shopping experience was really felt like an
escape.
0.952 0.907 0.934 0.782 yes
In comparison to other activities that I have
done, the time spent shopping was really nice.
0.953 0.908
When shopping, I felt the sensation of
adventure.
0.867 0.751
I loved being surrounded by new products. 0.750 0.562
Utilitarian I could not buy what I really needed. 0.775 0.601 0.883 0.793 yes
I made the purchases I wanted during my
shopping experience.
0.993 0.986
Copyrights
Copyright for this article is retained by the author(s), with first publication rights granted to the journal.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.