ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Developing strong knowledge about mathematics is important for success academically, economically, and in life, but many children fail to become proficient in math. Research on the developmental relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge of math provides insights into the development of knowledge about math. First, competency in math requires children to develop conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility. Second, conceptual and procedural knowledge often develop in a bidirectional, iterative fashion, with improvements in one type of knowledge-supporting improvements in the other, as well as procedural flexibility. Third, learning techniques such as comparing, explaining, and exploring promote more than one type of knowledge about math, indicating that each is an important learning process. Researchers need to develop and validate measurement tools, devise more comprehensive theories of math development, and build more bridges between research and educational practice.
!
Developing Mathematics Knowledge
Bethany Rittle-Johnson
Vanderbilt University
Child Development Perspectives, 2017
doi:10.1111/cdep.12229!
Published Version available at:
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdep.12229/abstract
!
Key words: conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, flexibility
Abstract
Developing strong knowledge about mathematics is important for success academically,
economically, and in life, but more than one children fail to become proficient in math. Research
on the developmental relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge of math provides
insights into the development of knowledge about math. First, competency in math requires
children to develop conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility.
Second, conceptual and procedural knowledge often develop in a bidirectional, iterative fashion,
with improvements in one type of knowledge supporting improvements in the other, as well as
procedural flexibility. Third, learning techniques such as comparing, explaining, and exploring
promote more than one type of knowledge about math, indicating that each is an important
learning process. Researchers need to develop and validate measurement tools, devise more
comprehensive theories of math development, and bridge more between research and educational
practice.
!
Proficiency in mathematics is critical to success academically, economically, and in life.
Greater success in math is related to entering and completing college, earning more in adulthood,
and making more optimal decisions concerning health (1, 2). Knowledge of math begins to
develop at a young age, and this early knowledge matters: Knowledge of math at or before
school entry predicts outcomes in math and reading across primary and secondary school (3).
More than one children struggle to learn math. For example, only 40% of fourth-grade
and 33% of eighth-grade students in the United States performed at or above proficiency in math
on the 2015 National Assessment of Educational Progress, and proficiency rates were even lower
for African-American and Hispanic children and for children from low-income homes (4). More
than one students do not master challenging math content.
Thus, it is critical to understand how children develop knowledge about math and how
educators can support this process more effectively. For example, when children practice solving
math problems, does this enhance their understanding of the underlying concepts? Under what
circumstances do abstract math concepts help children invent or implement correct procedures?
How do knowledge of math concepts and procedures contribute to flexible problem solving?
These questions tap a central research topic—the developmental relations between conceptual
and procedural knowledge of math—which is the focus of this article.
Developmental Relations Between Types of Knowledge
Conceptual knowledge refers to knowledge of concepts, which are abstract and general
principles such as cardinality and numeric magnitude (5, 6). Conceptual knowledge can be
explicit or implicit, meaning some conceptual knowledge cannot be put into words. Procedural
knowledge is often defined as knowledge of procedures—what steps or actions to take to
!
accomplish a goal (5, 6). This knowledge often develops through problem-solving practice, and
thus is tied to particular types of problems. Both types of knowledge promote procedural
flexibility, which is knowing more than one procedures and applying them adaptively to a range
of situations (7). For example, mathematicians know and use more procedures than novices,
appreciate efficient and elegant solutions to problems, and identify the most appropriate
procedure for a given problem based on different factors (e.g., characteristics of problems; 8).
Table 1 provides examples that represent each type of knowledge.
Historically, researchers have debated whether conceptual knowledge develops first or
procedural knowledge develops first (see 7, 9 for reviews). According to a concepts-first view,
children initially acquire conceptual knowledge by learning from adults or by innate constraints.
Then, they derive and build procedural knowledge from their conceptual knowledge through
repeated practice solving related problems. According to a procedures-first view, children
initially learn procedures by imitating adults, and then gradually derive conceptual knowledge
from implementing the procedures, abstracting the structure and principles of the problems.
More recently, I proposed an iterative view in which the causal relations are bidirectional, with
increases in conceptual knowledge leading to subsequent increases in procedural knowledge and
vice versa (6). For example, in one study (6), prior conceptual knowledge of decimals predicted
gains in procedural knowledge after a brief problem-solving intervention, which in turn predicted
subsequent gains in conceptual knowledge.
The iterative view is now the most well-accepted perspective among researchers (10, 11).
First, this view accommodates gradual improvements in each type of knowledge over time. Each
type of knowledge is multifaceted, and if knowledge is measured using continuous rather than
categorical measures, one type of knowledge is not well developed before the other emerges,
!
arguing against a strict view that puts concepts or procedures first. Second, an iterative view
accommodates evidence that supports concepts-first and procedures-first views, as initial
knowledge can be conceptual or procedural, depending on environmental input and relevant
prior knowledge. For example, even if children are born with a basic ability to track and
discriminate between numerical magnitudes (12), conceptual knowledge of numerical magnitude
develops in concert with experience counting and learning the counting procedure. Third, an
iterative view recognizes the role each type of knowledge can play in developing the other.
Conceptual knowledge can help with constructing, selecting, and appropriately executing
problem-solving procedures, and practice implementing procedures may help students develop
and deepen their understanding of concepts, especially if the practice is designed to make
underlying concepts more apparent (5).
Evidence also supports an iterative view. Numerous longitudinal studies indicate
predictive, bidirectional relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge. For example,
in one study, elementary school children’s knowledge of fractions was assessed in the winter of
fourth grade and the spring of fifth grade (13). Procedural knowledge in fourth grade predicted
conceptual knowledge in fifth grade after controlling for prior conceptual knowledge and other
factors; similarly, conceptual knowledge in fourth grade predicted procedural knowledge in fifth
grade.
Similar bidirectional relations across grade levels have been found in elementary school
children’s knowledge of whole number concepts and procedures (14). Over shorter time frames,
bidirectional relations have been found in preschoolers learning about counting (e.g., 15),
elementary school children learning addition and subtraction (e.g., 5) and about decimals (6, 16),
and middle school students learning about solving equations (17; see Figure 1).
!
Causal evidence for bidirectional relations comes from studies that experimentally
manipulate at least one type of knowledge and then measure both types of knowledge. For
example, in one study (18), elementary school children were given a brief lesson on a procedure
for solving problems of mathematical equivalence (e.g., 6 + 3 + 4 = 6 + __) or the concept of
mathematical equivalence, or were given no lesson. Children who received either lesson gained
greater conceptual knowledge and greater procedural knowledge than children who received no
lesson, indicating that a lesson on a procedure led to improvements in conceptual knowledge and
a lesson on a concept led to improvements in procedural knowledge.
Furthermore, studies on carefully constructed practice problems (5) suggest that
improving procedural knowledge can support improvements in conceptual knowledge. Practicing
nontraditional arithmetic problems such as __ = 3 + 5 improved second- and third-grade students’
procedural knowledge as well as their conceptual knowledge of the equal sign relative to
traditional practice formats such as 3 + 5 = ___ or no practice (e.g., 19). Overall, both
longitudinal and experimental studies indicate that procedural knowledge improves conceptual
knowledge, and vice versa, suggesting that the relations between the two types of knowledge are
bidirectional.
An iterative view further predicts that the bidirectional relations between conceptual and
procedural knowledge persist, with increases in one supporting increases in the other in an
iterative feedback loop (6). In addition, iterating between lessons on concepts and procedures on
decimals supported greater procedural knowledge and equivalent conceptual knowledge than
presenting concept lessons before lessons on procedure (16). These studies suggest that relations
between the two types of knowledge are bidirectional and iterative over time.
!
This does not mean that relations between the two types of knowledge are always
symmetrical. In a recent study, the relations were symmetrical—the strength of the relationship
from prior conceptual knowledge to later procedural knowledge was the same as it was from
prior procedural knowledge to later conceptual knowledge (17). However, in other studies,
conceptual knowledge or conceptual instruction influenced procedural knowledge more strongly
than vice versa (13, 18). Furthermore, brief procedural instruction or practice-solving problems
does not always support growth in conceptual knowledge (5, 20). How much gains in procedural
knowledge support gains in conceptual knowledge is influenced by the nature of the procedural
instruction or practice (e.g., 19). Crafting procedural lessons to encourage children to notice
underlying concepts can promote a stronger link from improved procedural knowledge to gains
in conceptual knowledge (5).
Relations to Procedural Flexibility
Although it has received much less attention than conceptual and procedural knowledge,
evidence on the development of procedural flexibility has emerged recently. The development of
procedural flexibility is related to children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge (21). For
example, greater procedural flexibility for multidigit arithmetic is related to greater conceptual
and procedural knowledge of arithmetic (22) [AU: than?]. Furthermore, middle school students’
prior conceptual and procedural knowledge for solving equations each uniquely predicted their
procedural flexibility at the end of a classroom unit on solving equations (see Figure 1; 17).
Summary
!
Proficiency in math requires developing conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge,
and procedural flexibility. Evidence from a variety of math domains indicates that the
development of conceptual and procedural knowledge is often bidirectional and iterative, with
one type of knowledge supporting gains in the other. Greater conceptual and procedural
knowledge is also related to greater procedural flexibility, and evidence suggests that conceptual
and procedural knowledge support the development of procedural flexibility.
Learning Techniques for Improving Mathematics Knowledge
Given the importance of developing conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and
procedural flexibility, we need to understand how learning techniques improve these types of
knowledge. Three powerful activities—comparing, self-explaining, and exploring before
instruction—can promote both conceptual and procedural knowledge, and one (comparing) also
improves procedural flexibility. This experimental research also helps validate instructional
methods for promoting knowledge of math.
Comparing
Comparing is a ubiquitous cognitive process, and comparing alternative ways to solve
problems can promote learning in math. In five studies, students looked at pairs of examples
illustrating two correct procedures for solving the same problem and were prompted to compare
them, or they studied the examples individually and were prompted to reflect on them (23). For
students who knew one of the solution procedures at pretest, comparing procedures supported
greater conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility. For novices,
who did not know one of the solution procedures at pretest, comparing improved procedural
!
flexibility, but not conceptual or procedural knowledge. Comparing can improve all three types
of knowledge in part because comparing examples side by side promotes perceptual learning of
the structure of problems within the domain (24).
In addition, comparing incorrect procedures to correct ones can also aid conceptual and
procedural knowledge (25). For example, fourth- and fifth-grade students gained greater
conceptual and procedural knowledge when they compared examples of correct and incorrect
solution procedures rather than comparing only correct procedures (26). Another promising form
of comparison is when students compare easily confusable problem types, which helps learners
distinguish the two problem types and improves procedural knowledge (27).
Self-Explaining
Generating explanations to make sense of new information (i.e., self-explanation) is
another common and powerful learning process (28, 29). Furthermore, prompting students to
explain new information, such as examples of solutions to math problems, helps promote
learning in math. For example, prompting primary school children to explain why solutions to
problems of math equivalence were correct or incorrect supported greater conceptual and
procedural knowledge than having them solve problems without self-explanation prompts (30).
Self-explanation aids conceptual knowledge by integrating knowledge, as explanations often link
new information or link new information with prior knowledge (31). In addition, self-explaining
facilitates conceptual and procedural knowledge by guiding attention to structural features
instead of to surface features of the content to be learned, helping students notice key structural
features of exemplars and use procedures less frequently tied to particular surface features of the
exemplars (20, 30).
!
Our recent meta-analysis of 26 experimental studies on prompted self-explanation with a
wide range of ages (age 4 to 22) learning math confirmed that self-explanation prompts promote
greater procedural knowledge, especially procedural transfer, as well as greater conceptual
knowledge when knowledge was assessed immediately after the intervention (32). The effect
was stronger if support for high-quality explanation was provided, such as partial explanations to
complete. Without support, children and adults sometimes have difficulty generating useful
explanations when prompted. Training on self-explanation and structured self-explanation
responses, such as selecting an explanation from a list, supported learners effectively. Overall,
prompting children to generate explanations when learning math promotes conceptual and
procedural knowledge, especially when explanations are supported.
Exploring Before Instruction
Children are intrinsically driven to explore, and exploration can help children discover
and pay attention to important information (28). At the same time, children often fail to discover
important information on their own and benefit from direct instruction (33). A productive
combination is to offer opportunities for children to explore problems before instruction (34). For
example, primary school children solved unfamiliar math problems and received a lesson on
equivalence, and the order of problem solving and the lesson was manipulated (35, 36).
Compared to children who solved the problems after the lesson, children who solved the
unfamiliar problems before the lesson gained more conceptual knowledge or procedural
knowledge. Similarly, middle school students who explored problems and invented their own
formulas for calculating density before receiving instruction on density gained deeper conceptual
and procedural knowledge of the topic than students who had the lessons first (34). Exploring
!
problems followed by instruction fits with the recommendation from researchers of math
education that students have opportunities to struggle—to figure out something that is not
immediately apparent—before direct instruction (37).
Summary
Comparing, self-explaining, and exploring before instruction are learning techniques that
can improve conceptual and procedural knowledge of math. Comparing solution procedures also
improves procedural flexibility. Research confirms the causal role of each type of knowledge in
math development and validates techniques educators can use to promote such development.
Certainly, more than one other learning techniques promote math development. These
include studying worked-out examples of solution procedures (38), and discussing math ideas
with peers (39). These activities promote active thinking about math concepts and procedures,
not simply memorizing terms and solution procedures as dictated by adults. More than one
children in U.S. math classrooms spend much of their time implementing procedures
demonstrated by their teachers rather than reflecting actively on concepts and procedures (40).
Concluding Remarks
Overall, research on developmental psychology has helped illuminate how children learn
math. Competency in math requires that children develop conceptual knowledge, procedural
knowledge, and procedural flexibility. These three types of knowledge often develop
bidirectionally and iteratively, with improvements in one type of knowledge supporting
improvements in the other types. Furthermore, comparing, self-explaining, and exploring before
!
instruction promote conceptual and procedural knowledge of math, and comparing also promotes
procedural flexibility.
Despite a growing number of studies on the psychology of math development, current
research has its limits. First, researchers have not developed standardized approaches to assess
the different types of knowledge with proven validity, reliability, and objectivity (10). Rather,
they typically develop their own study-specific measures, often without evidence of convergent
or divergent validity. Some topics, such as conceptual knowledge of cardinality and numeric
magnitude, are receiving increased attention, but we lack consensus on the most effective way to
measure each construct (41). As a field, we need to invest more resources in measurement
development and validation. In Table 1, I have provided examples of types of items that lend
themselves to standardized administration and scoring. Evidence for bidirectional relations may
be driven, in part, by impure measures that each tap a mixture of types of knowledge rather than
by true bidirectional relations in the underlying constructs. Only one study has provided evidence
for bidirectional relations after establishing the divergent validity of the measures (17).
Second, we need a more comprehensive, integrative theory of how the different types of
knowledge develop and interact. Such a theory should consider how age and individual
differences affect relations between the three types of knowledge and the effectiveness of
different learning techniques. It should also identify when developmental relations and learning
process differ for different math topics, as well as the impact of affective factors such as math
anxiety (42).
Finally, we need to invest more effort in bridging research and practice (43). Instead of
trying to apply our research to practice, we need to do research that is inherently relevant to and
driven by the needs of practice. We should incorporate research topics and methods that consider
!
current problems of practice (e.g., what math educators identify as their most pressing concerns).
We also need to conduct research within educational settings to ensure the method is feasible
outside the lab and the findings generalize to those settings. For example, we have capitalized on
the common educational practice of partner work. We randomly assigned pairs of students to
different conditions within classrooms, having students work on our materials with a partner
during their math class on content relevant for that course (23). Such research is often most
successful when conducted by interdisciplinary research teams that include psychologists, math
education researchers, mathematicians, and math teachers. Collaboration like this can often lead
to publishing findings in journals for practitioners (e.g., Teaching Children Mathematics), which
require a different approach to writing than journals for researchers. Interdisciplinary work also
facilitates translating research-based findings into curriculum and professional-development
materials for teachers (44). Translating psychological principles and findings into useable
practices is not straightforward. For example, psychological research often focuses on isolating
particular processes and components of knowledge, and rarely speaks to how to combine and
integrate different processes and components to address broad learning goals, but this is
necessary in practice (45). Overall, bridging research and practice benefits both, and will help
advance our understanding of how children learn math and how we can promote this learning
more effectively.
!
Author’s Note
Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Peabody
College, Vanderbilt University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Bethany Rittle-Johnson,
Department of Psychology and Human Development, 230 Appleton Place, Peabody #552,
Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37203; e-mail: bethany.rittle-johnson@vanderbilt.edu.
!
References
1. Reyna, V. F., Nelson, W. L., Han, P. K., & Dieckmann, N. F. (2009). How numeracy
influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 135,
943-973. doi:10.1037/a0017327
2. Ritchie, S. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Enduring links from childhood mathematics and reading
achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychological Science, 24, 1301-1308.
doi:10.1177/0956797612466268
3. Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., . . .
Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43,
1428-1446. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
4. National Assessment of Educational Progress. (2015). 2015 mathematics results. Retrieved
10/1/2016 from http://www.nationsreportcard.gov/reading_math_2015/!-!
mathematics?grade=4
5. Canobi, K. H. (2009). Concept-procedure interactions in children's addition and subtraction.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 131-149. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.008
6. Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual
understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 93, 346-362. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.93.2.346
7. Baroody, A. J. (2003). The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration
of conceptual and procedural knowledge. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The
development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 1-34).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
!
8. Star, J. R., & Newton, K. J. (2009). The nature and development of expert's strategy flexibility
for solving equations. ZDM Mathematics Education, 41, 557-567. doi:10.1007/s11858-
009-0185-5
9. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Siegler, R. S. (1998). The relation between conceptual and procedural
knowledge in learning mathematics: A review. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The development of
mathematical skills (pp. 75-110). London, UK: Psychology Press.
10. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and procedural
knowledge of mathematics. In R. C. Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford handbook of
numerical cognition (pp. 1118-1134). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
11. Rittle-Johnson, B., Schneider, M., & Star, J. R. (2015). Not a one-way street: Bidirectional
relations between procedural and conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Educational
Psychology Review, 27, 587-597. doi:10.1007/s10648-015-9302-x
12. Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense in human infants. Developmental
Science, 8, 88-101. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x
13. Hecht, S. A., & Vagi, K. J. (2010). Sources of group and individual differences in emerging
fraction skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 843-859. doi:10.1037/a0019824
14. Cowan, R., Donlan, C., Shepherd, D.-L., Cole-Fletcher, R., Saxton, M., & Hurry, J. (2011).
Basic calculation proficiency and mathematics achievement in elementary school
children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 786-803. doi:10.1037/a0024556
15. Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children's counting and concept of number. New York, NY: Springer-
Verlag.
!
16. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Koedinger, K. R. (2009). Iterating between lessons concepts and
procedures can improve mathematics knowledge. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 79, 483-500. doi:10.1348/000709908X398106
17. Schneider, M., Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). Relations among conceptual
knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility in two samples differing in
prior knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1525-1538. doi:10.1037/a0024997
18. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Alibali, M. W. (1999). Conceptual and procedural knowledge of
mathematics: Does one lead to the other? Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 175-
189. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.91.1.175
19. McNeil, N. M., Fyfe, E. R., & Dunwiddie, A. E. (2014). Arithmetic practice can be modified
to promote understanding of mathematical equivalence. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 107, 423-436. doi:10.1037/a0037687
20. Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation and direct
instruction. Child Development, 77, 1-15. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00852.x
21. Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 36, 404-411. Retrieved [AU: Please add date retrieved] from
http://www.jstor.org/stable/30034943
22. Blöte, A. W., Van der Burg, E., & Klein, A. S. (2001). Students' flexibility in solving two-
digit addition and subtraction problems: Instruction effects. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93, 627-638. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.93.3.627
23. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). The power of comparison in learning and instruction:
Learning outcomes supported by different types of comparisons. In J. P. Mestre & B. H.
!
Ross (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Cognition in education (Vol. 55, pp.
199-222). Waltham, MA: Elsevier.
24. Day, S. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (2012). The import of knowledge export: Connecting findings
and theories of transfer of learning. Educational Psychologist, 47, 153-176.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2012.696438
25. Booth, J. L., Lange, K. E., Koedinger, K. R., & Newton, K. J. (2013). Using example
problems to improve student learning in algebra: Differentiating between correct and
incorrect examples. Learning and Instruction, 25, 24-34.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.11.002
26. Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to
support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22, 206-214.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001
27. Ziegler, E., & Stern, E. (2016). Consistent advantages of contrasted comparisons: Algebra
learning under direct instruction. Learning and Instruction, 41, 41-51.
doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.09.006
28. Legare, C. H. (2014). The contributions of explanation and exploraton to scientific reasoning.
Child Development Perspectives, 8, 101-106. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12070
29. Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. TRENDS in Cognitive
Science, 10, 464-470. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.004
30. McEldoon, K. L., Durkin, K. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2013). Is self-explanation worth the
time? A comparison to additional practice. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83,
615-632. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02083.x
!
31. Chi, M. T. H. (2000). Self-explaining: The dual processes of generating inference and
repairing mental models Advances in Instructional Psychology: Educational Design and
Cognitive Science (Vol. 5., pp. 161-238). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
32. Rittle-Johnson, B., Loehr, A. M., & Durkin, K. (2017). Promoting self-explanation to
improve mathematics learning: A meta-analysis and instructional design principles. ZDM
Mathematics Education. (online first, so no volume or page numbers yet)
doi.10.1007/s11858-017-0834-z
33. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during
instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery,
problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41,
75-86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
34. Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., Chin, D. B., & Oppezzo, M. (2011). Practicing versus
inventing with contrasting cases: The effects of telling first on learning and transfer.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 759-775. doi:10.1037/a0025140
35. Loehr, A. M., Fyfe, E. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2014). Wait for it… delaying instruction
improves mathematics problem solving: A classroom study. The Journal of Problem
Solving, 7, 36-49. doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1166
36. DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). Exploring mathematics problems prepares
children to learn from instruction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 552-
568. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
37. Hiebert, J., & Grouws, D. (2007). The effects of classroom mathematics teaching on students’
learning. Second Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, 1, 371-
!
404. [AU: Is this a journal?] [AU: Please provide doi.]It is a book chapter - see below for
citation
Hiebert, J., & Grouws, D. (2007). The effects of classroom mathematics teaching on students’
learning. In F. K. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching
and learning (pp. 371-404). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
38. Renkl, A. (2011). Instruction based on examples. In R. E. Mayer & P. A. Alexander (Eds.),
Handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 272-295). New York, NY:
Routledge.
39. Webb, N. M., Franke, M. L., Ing, M., Wong, J., Fernandez, C. H., Shin, N., & Turrou, A. C.
(2014). Engaging with others’ mathematical ideas: Interrelationships among student
participation, teachers’ instructional practices, and learning. International Journal of
Educational Research, 63, 79-93. doi:10.1016/j.ijer.2013.02.001
40. Hiebert, J., Gallimore, R., Garnier, H., Givvin, K. B., Hollingsworth, H., Jacobs, J.,…Stigler,
J. W. (2003). Teaching mathematics in seven countries: Results from the TIMSS 1999
video study (NCES 2003-013). Retrieved 10/1/2016 from http://nces.ed.gov/timss
41. Price, G. R., Palmer, D., Battista, C., & Ansari, D. (2012). Nonsymbolic numerical
magnitude comparison: Reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome
measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. Acta Psychologica,
140, 50-57. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.008
42. Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety,
working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of
Cognition and Development, 14, 187-202. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.664593
!
43. Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2016). Toward an educational psychology of mathematics
education. In L. Corno & E. Anderman (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd
ed., pp. 257-268). New York, NY: Routledge.
44. Star, J. R., Pollack, C., Durkin, K., Rittle-Johnson, B., Lynch, K., Newton, K., & Gogolen, C.
(2015). Learning from comparison in algebra. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 40,
41-54. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.05.005
45. Davenport, J., Kao, Y. S., & Schneider, S. A. (2013). Integrating cognitive science principles
to redesign a middle school math curriculum. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I.
Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual conference of the cognitive science
society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
!
Table 1
Sample Tasks and Items Used to Assess Each Type of Mathematics Knowledge
*Note: Designating problems as being in a familiar format requires some knowledge of students’
instructional history, but can often be inferred because either the researcher intentionally exposed
students to the problem type before the assessment or it is reasonable to assume from content
standards for mathematics in the particular country or state.
Sample!task!type!
Sample!Item!
Conceptual+Knowledge++
a.!Evaluate!examples!of!concept!
a.!Decide!whether!the!number!sentence!3!=!3!
makes!sense!
b.!Translate!quantities!between!
representational!systems!
b.!Place!symbolic!numbers!on!number!lines!
c.!Compare!quantities!
c.!Indicate!which!symbolic!integer!or!fraction!
is!larger!
d.!Generate!or!select!definitions!of!
concepts!
d.!Define!the!equal!sign!
Procedural+Knowledge+
a.!Solve!problems!in!a!familiar!
format*!
a.!8/10!+!6/10!=!__!
b.!Solve!problems!with!a!new!
surface!or!problem!feature!
b.!2!½!+!¼!=!__!
Procedural+Flexibility+
a.!!Generate!multiple!methods!
a.!Solve!this!equation!in!two!different!ways:!
4(x!+!2)!=!12!!
b.!Evaluate!nonconventional!
methods!
b.!Do!you!think!this!way!of!starting!this!
problem!is!(a)!a!very!good!way;!(b)!OK!to!do,!
but!not!a!very!good!way;!(c)!not!OK!to!do?!!
!
Figure 1. Regression paths of the best-fitting structural equation model of the relations
among conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility in Study 1 (17).
Conceptual+
Knowledge)
Time%1
Procedural*
Knowledge)
Time%1
Conceptual+
Knowledge)
Time%2
Procedural*
Knowledge)
Time%2
Procedural*
Flexibility)
Time%2
!
.46
.34
.30
.28
.26
.42
.80
.87
.89
... Some studies divide knowledge into four dimensions: factual, procedural, conceptual, and metacognitive knowledge (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001). On the other hand, several other studies categorised mathematical knowledge into two dimensions: conceptual and procedural knowledge (Hallett et al., 2012;Rittle-Johnson, 2017;Schneider & Stern, 2010;Star & Stylianides, 2013). In this study, we focus on two forms of knowledge: conceptual and procedural. ...
... Conceptual knowledge is the ability to integrate pieces of information to generate a full knowledge that underpins both explicit and implicit mathematical structures, whereas procedural knowledge is the understanding of how procedures function (Rittle-Johnson, 2017). Some scholars consider that gain of procedural knowledge must be preceded by gain of conceptual knowledge (Byrnes & Wasik, 1991), but most researchers agree that these should be intertwined (Kilpatrick et al., 2001). ...
... Rittle-Johnson (2017) contends that conceptual and procedural knowledge have an iterative bidirectional relationship, with advances in one enabling improvements in the other and procedural flexibility (Hallett et al., 2012). In Rittle-Johnson (2017), Canobi argues that conceptual knowledge contributes to the development, selection, and implementation of proper problem-solving procedures. Converse and applying procedures can help students develop and deepen their understanding of concepts, especially if it is designed to make the underlying concepts clearer. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the body of knowledge in mathematics education research, fractions are one of the researchers' concerns. The reason is because fractions are very difficult for students to understand. This study explores elementary school students' knowledge and obstacles in dealing with the multiplication of fractions. This study employs descriptive quantitative and qualitative approaches. Data were collected using the fractional knowledge test administered to 56 fifth-grade students and cognitive semi-structured interviews with six students depending on their test scores. The results of this study show that students’ knowledge of fractions is restricted, with challenges interpreting context-based problems and the usage of “of” terms. Another finding shows that students’ procedural knowledge is more dominant than conceptual knowledge. To develop students’ knowledge of fractions, the portion of context-based learning must be an emphasis. The importance of developing research-based textbooks based on a suitable learning trajectory is highlighted.
... Regarding conceptual knowledge, previous reviews (Crooks & Alibali, 2014;Prather & Alibali, 2009;Rittle-Johnson, 2017) agreed on the difficulty of measuring it because of the lack of a consensual definition and standardized tasks. These reviews emphasize the need to define each concept individually and to develop standardized tasks that would simultaneously measure different conceptual knowledge and make cross-study comparisons possible. ...
... Moreover, it appeared that both indicators reflecting conceptual and procedural knowledge accounted for the explained variance, apparently with neither type being more important than the other. This result is consistent with previous works on conceptual and procedural knowledge (see Rittle-Johnson, 2017;Rittle-Johnson & Schneider, 2015) stating that both types of knowledge are moderately related. Following these studies, any increase in one should be correlated with increased performance in the other. ...
... This result was later interpreted as being an element in favor of the distinction between two different types of conceptual knowledge: inversion with addition and subtraction and inversion with multiplication and division (Robinson, 2017;Robinson et al., 2018). This interpretation is in line with our findings and is consistent with existing works on the relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge (Rittle-Johnson, 2017;Rittle-Johnson & Schneider, 2015;Rittle-Johnson et al., 2001;Schneider et al., 2011). ...
Article
Cognitive arithmetic classically distinguishes procedural and conceptual knowledge as two determinants of the acquisition of flexible expertise. Whereas procedural knowledge relates to algorithmic routines, conceptual knowledge is defined as the knowledge of core principles, referred to as fundamental structures of arithmetic. To date, there is no consensus regarding their number, list, or even their definition, partly because they are difficult to measure. Recent findings suggest that among the most complex of these principles, some might not be “fundamental structures” but rather may articulate several components of conceptual knowledge, each specific to the arithmetic operation involved. Here, we argue that most of the arithmetic principles similarly may rather articulate several core concepts specific to the operation involved. Data were collected during a national mathematics contest based on an arithmetic game involving a large sample of 9- to 11-year-old students (N = 11,243; 53.1% boys) over several weeks. The purpose of the game was to solve complex arithmetic problems using five numbers and the four operations. A principal component analysis (PCA) was performed. The results show that both conceptual and procedural knowledge were used by children. Moreover, the PCA sorted conceptual and procedural knowledge together, with dimensions being defined by the operation rather than by the concept. This implies that “fundamental structures” rather regroup different concepts that are learned separately. This opens the way to reconsider the very nature of conceptual knowledge and has direct pedagogical implications.
... This study builds on prior research that identified and examined multiple strategies for solving constant change problems Brown et al., 2018;Riggs et al., 2015;2017). In these previous studies, the most common strategy used by undergraduates was the summation strategy. ...
... Research on mathematical learning holds that accurately implementing a correct strategy reflects procedural knowledge, whereas understanding why certain strategies work is a form of conceptual knowledge (Crooks & Alibali, 2014). Both forms of knowledge are critical for success in mathematics (Canobi, 2009;Rittle-Johnson, 2017), and there has been extensive debate regarding the proper order in which instruction should focus on these two forms of knowledge (see, e.g., Rittle-Johnson et al., 2015). Some research suggests that providing students with relevant conceptual knowledge prior to introducing problem-solving procedures leads to deeper learning (Rittle-Johnson & Alibali, 1999). ...
Article
Why do people shift their strategies for solving problems? Past work has focused on the roles of contextual and individual factors in explaining whether people adopt new strategies when they are exposed to them. In this study, we examined a factor not considered in prior work: people's evaluations of the strategies themselves. We presented undergraduate participants from a moderately selective university (N = 252; 64.8% women, 65.6% White, 67.6% who had taken calculus) with two strategies for solving algebraic word problems and asked them to rate these strategies and their own strategy on a variety of dimensions. Participants' ratings loaded onto two factors, which we label quality and difficulty. Participants' initial evaluations of the quality of the strategies were associated with whether they used the strategies at posttest, and this effect held even when controlling for individual and contextual factors. However, people's evaluations of the difficulty of the strategies were not consistently associated with their later adoption of those strategies. We also examined individual and contextual predictors of strategy ratings and strategy adoption. Participants' need for cognition and their spatial visualization ability were associated with their strategy evaluations, and the framing of the story problems was associated with their strategy adoption. The findings highlight that strategy adoption depends on multiple interacting factors, and that to understand strategy change, it is critical to examine how people evaluate strategies.
... This model could contribute to the improvements of student achievement in terms of both types of knowledge (conceptual and procedural) and, in particular, the relationship between the two types of mathematical proficiency, known as the iterative views of Rittle-Johnson (2017). The two types of knowledge (conceptual and procedural understanding) can be seen as bidirectional, iterative views, which means that improvements in one type of knowledge lead to improvements in the other (Nordlander, 2021;Rittle-Johnson, 2017). Both conceptual and procedural understanding lead to high achievement in calculus and interact with each other (Hoyles & Forman, 1995;Rittle-Johnson, 2017). ...
... The two types of knowledge (conceptual and procedural understanding) can be seen as bidirectional, iterative views, which means that improvements in one type of knowledge lead to improvements in the other (Nordlander, 2021;Rittle-Johnson, 2017). Both conceptual and procedural understanding lead to high achievement in calculus and interact with each other (Hoyles & Forman, 1995;Rittle-Johnson, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
This article presents an instructional technology-based cycle model intended to support and facilitate the teaching and learning of mathematics, particularly calculus. The study used quantitative methods with quasi-experimental research that uses non-randomised assignments of the study group that are categorised into experimental and control groups including 36 and 30 students in the control and experimental groups, respectively, at a university in Ethiopia. A pre-test was administered to the experimental and control groups before the intervention (statistical control over the groups) to identify students' abilities in the two groups. Based on nine steps, Vygotsky's theory of learning model was implemented in the classroom using GeoGebra software. The findings suggest that the GeoGebra classroom oriented approach to learning differential calculus, using the cycle model, had a positive effect on students' conceptual understanding and a very positive effect on their procedural understanding. The article recommends that using the cycle model of instruction in the teaching and learning of calculus can bring important benefits to different stages of schooling in the Ethiopian context and beyond.
... The first one is knowledge in carrying out a number of mathematical problems using skills, algorithms, methods, and procedures, whereas the other one is knowledge in having a 'sense' of the mathematical concepts and skills (e.g. Canobi, 2009;Hiebert & Lefevre, 1986;Hurrell, 2021;Long, 2005;Miller & Hudson, 2007;Rittle-Johnson, 2017;Skemp, 2020). Interestingly, those two distinct types of knowledge are technically termed differently across the literature. ...
Article
Full-text available
[English]: Some students might have the proper knowledge to use mathematical procedures where relevant, but do they actually have a solid understanding of “why or how” those procedures work? Such an incomplete understanding of mathematics concepts can be a stumbling block in students’ success in mathematics. This paper aims to propose and elaborate a framework for developing proceptual knowledge combining both procedural and conceptual knowledge on differentiation that are constructed on existing mathematics learning theories on how we understand mathematics, besides my personal reflections from the independent learning on differentiation. The theoretical and practical perspectives proposed in this article share insight with anyone in developing a more meaningful mathematics-independent learning experience, especially on topics with complex mathematical formulas or procedures, such as differentiation. [Bahasa]: Sebagian siswa mungkin memiliki pengetahuan yang tepat dalam menggunakan prosedur matematika secara relevan, namun apakah mereka sungguh memiliki pemahaman yang utuh tentang "mengapa atau bagaimana" prosedur matematika tersebut diperoleh? Pemahaman yang tidak utuh tersebut berpotensi menjadi penghalang kesuksesan siswa dalam memahami konsep matematika. Artikel ini mengusulkan kerangka kerja untuk mengembangkan proceptual knowledge (pengetahuan proseptual) pada materi turunan, yakni kombinasi pengembangan pengetahuan prosedural dan konseptual matematika yang dibangun di atas teori-teori pembelajaran matematika yang ada, serta hasil refleksi pribadi penulis dari proses belajar mandiri tentang konsep rumus diferensial. Perspektif teoritis dan praktis yang diusulkan dalam artikel ini dapat menjadi panduan bagi siapa saja untuk mengembangkan pengalaman belajar matematika yang lebih bermakna, khususnya pada topik dengan rumus dan prosedur matematis yang kompleks seperti pada turunan.
Article
Prior research has shown that game‐based learning tools, such as DragonBox 12+, support algebraic understanding and that students' in‐game progress positively predicts their later performance. Using data from 253 seventh‐graders (12–13 years old) who played DragonBox as a part of technology intervention, we examined (a) the relations between students' progress within DragonBox and their algebraic knowledge and general mathematics achievement, (b) the moderating effects of students' prior performance on these relations and (c) the potential factors associated with students' in‐game progress. Among students with higher prior algebraic knowledge, higher in‐game progress was related to higher algebraic knowledge after the intervention. Higher in‐game progress was also associated with higher end‐of‐year mathematics achievement, and this association was stronger among students with lower prior mathematics achievement. Students' demographic characteristics, prior knowledge and prior achievement did not significantly predict in‐game progress beyond the number of intervention sessions students completed. These findings advance research on how, for whom and in what contexts game‐based interventions, such as DragonBox, support mathematical learning and have implications for practice using game‐based technologies to supplement instruction. Practitioner notes What is already known about this topic DragonBox 12+ may support students' understanding of algebra but the findings are mixed. Students who solve more problems within math games tend to show higher performance after gameplay. Students' engagement with mathematics is often related to their prior math performance. What this paper adds For students with higher prior algebraic knowledge, solving more problems in DragonBox 12+ is related to higher algebraic performance after gameplay. Students who make more in‐game progress also have higher mathematics achievement, especially for students with lower prior achievement. Students who spend more time playing DragonBox 12+ make more in‐game progress; their demographic, prior knowledge and prior achievement are not related to in‐game progress. Implications for practice and/or policy DragonBox 12+ can be beneficial as a supplement to algebra instruction for students with some understanding of algebra. DragonBox 12+ can engage students with mathematics across achievement levels. Dedicating time and encouraging students to play DragonBox 12+ may help them make more in‐game progress, and in turn, support math learning. What is already known about this topic DragonBox 12+ may support students' understanding of algebra but the findings are mixed. Students who solve more problems within math games tend to show higher performance after gameplay. Students' engagement with mathematics is often related to their prior math performance. What this paper adds For students with higher prior algebraic knowledge, solving more problems in DragonBox 12+ is related to higher algebraic performance after gameplay. Students who make more in‐game progress also have higher mathematics achievement, especially for students with lower prior achievement. Students who spend more time playing DragonBox 12+ make more in‐game progress; their demographic, prior knowledge and prior achievement are not related to in‐game progress. Implications for practice and/or policy DragonBox 12+ can be beneficial as a supplement to algebra instruction for students with some understanding of algebra. DragonBox 12+ can engage students with mathematics across achievement levels. Dedicating time and encouraging students to play DragonBox 12+ may help them make more in‐game progress, and in turn, support math learning.
Article
This study defined Task Knowledge and Learning-Process Knowledge based on current concepts regarding instructors' knowledge. It examined whether instructors' acquisition of each knowledge type changes their desirability judgments on educational methods involving students' activities or teachers' guidance. In a pretest-posttest design experiment, university students were presented with a complex mathematical problem and ten questionnaire items; half of these pertained to classroom situations involving students' activities and half teachers' guidance. Participants in the Task Knowledge condition (N=147) were provided with the solution steps and correct answer for the problem. Those in the Learning-Process Knowledge condition (N=136) were provided with examples of fifth-grade students' incorrect answers and the appearance rates of each answer pattern. Both groups assessed the problem difficulty level for fifth-grade public elementary school students with average academic ability and rated the educational desirability for the ten questionnaire items. Results indicated that participants in both conditions evaluated the problem difficulty level to be higher in the posttest than in the pretest. However, in the two conditions, their desirability judgments changed differently. The Task Knowledge condition participants scored the teachers' guidance items higher in the posttest than the pretest, whereas the Learning-Process Knowledge condition participants scored the students' activities items higher.
Article
This study examined repeating and growing pattern knowledge and their associations with procedural and conceptual arithmetic knowledge in a sample of U.S. children (N = 185; Mage = 79.5 months; 55% female; 88% White) and adults (N = 93; Mage = 19.5 years; 62% female; 66% White) from 2019 to 2020. Three key findings emerged: (1) repeating pattern tasks were easier than growing pattern tasks, (2) repeating pattern knowledge robustly predicted procedural calculation skills over and above growing pattern knowledge and covariates, and (3) growing pattern knowledge modestly predicted procedural and conceptual math outcomes over and above repeating pattern knowledge and covariates. We expand existing theoretical models to incorporate these specific links and discuss implications for supporting math knowledge.
Article
The current study assessed whether adding worked examples with self-explanation prompts focused on making connections between mathematical principles, procedures, and concepts of rational numbers to a curriculum focused on invented strategies improves pre-algebra students’ fraction number line acuity, rational number concepts and procedures. Finally, the study assessed whether individual differences in prior knowledge of rational numbers moderate the effect of the textbooks. The experimental textbooks resulted in significantly greater improvements overall in fraction number line estimation skills than control textbooks. Further, example-supplemented textbooks were more effective than control textbooks at improving understanding of rational number concepts for those with low prior conceptual knowledge. Both textbooks were equally effective at improving procedural skills.
Article
Full-text available
Promoting self-explanation (i.e., generating explanations for oneself in an attempt to make sense of new information) is a recommended study strategy and instructional practice. A meta-analysis of the literature on prompting self-explanation to improve mathematics learning confirmed that prompted self-explanation leads to a small to moderate improvement in procedural knowledge, conceptual knowledge and procedural transfer when assessed immediately after the intervention. However, evidence that self-explanation reliably promotes learning within a classroom context or retention of knowledge over a delay is much more limited. Moderator analyses indicated that the effect on immediate outcomes was stronger if scaffolding of high-quality explanation was provided but did not vary based on whether time on task was controlled across conditions. Based on the research literature, we propose instructional recommendations for mathematics educators: (a) scaffold high-quality explanations via training on self-explanation or structuring the self-explanation responses, (b) design explanation prompts so they do not sacrifice attention to other important content, (c) prompt learners to explain correct information, and (d) prompt learners to explain why common misconceptions are incorrect. We conclude with issues for future research, such as the need for additional research on effective use of self-explanation in classroom contexts and the merits of self-explanation relative to alternative instructional techniques.
Chapter
Full-text available
When children practise solving problems, does this also enhance their understanding of the underlying concepts? Under what circumstances do abstract concepts help children invent or implement correct procedures? These questions tap a central research topic in the fields of cognitive development and educational psychology: the relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge. Delineating how these two types ofknowledge interact is fundamental to understanding how knowledge development occurs. lt is also central to improving instruction. Our goals of the current paper were: (1) discuss prominent definitions and measures of each type of knowledge, (2) review recent research on the developmental relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge for learning mathematics, (3) highlight promising research on potential methods for improving both types of knowledge, and (4) discuss problematic issues and future directions.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined relations between children's conceptual understanding of mathematical equivalence and their procedures for solving equivalence problems (e.g., 3 + 4 + 5 = 3 + 9). Students in 4th and 5th grades completed assessments of their conceptual and procedural knowledge of equivalence, both before and after a brief lesson. The instruction focused either on the concept of equivalence or on a correct procedure for solving equivalence problems. Conceptual instruction led to increased conceptual understanding and to generation and transfer of a correct procedure. Procedural instruction led to increased conceptual understanding and to adoption, but only limited transfer, of the instructed procedure. These findings highlight the causal relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge and suggest that conceptual knowledge may have a greater influence on procedural knowledge than the reverse. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The authors propose that conceptual and procedural knowledge develop in an iterative fashion and that improved problem representation is 1 mechanism underlying the relations between them. Two experiments were conducted with 5th- and 6th-grade students learning about decimal fractions. In Experiment 1, children's initial conceptual knowledge predicted gains in procedural knowledge, and gains in procedural knowledge predicted improvements in conceptual knowledge. Correct problem representations mediated the relation between initial conceptual knowledge and improved procedural knowledge. In Experiment 2, amount of support for correct problem representation was experimentally manipulated, and the manipulations led to gains in procedural knowledge. Thus, conceptual and procedural knowledge develop iteratively, and improved problem representation is 1 mechanism in this process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Contrasted comparisons are an effective means of concept learning. Many studies have investigated the effects of comparisons in self-learning settings. However, because direct instruction remains a widespread instructional method, we adapted self-learning materials from a previous experiment that demonstrated the benefits of a contrasted introduction of algebraic addition and multiplication for direct instruction at the blackboard. Ninety-eight sixth-graders were randomly assigned to two groups: in the contrast group, addition and multiplication were simultaneously introduced and compared at the blackboard, whereas in the sequential group, addition was taught for two days followed by two days of multiplication training. The contrast learners clearly outperformed the sequential learners in differentiating superficially similar algebraic concepts on three follow-up measures. Furthermore, similar to our previous results, the benefits of contrasted comparison only appeared with delay. Our results extend the applicability of contrasted comparison to the direct instruction method.
Article
This study examined relations between children's conceptual understanding of mathematical equivalence and their procedures for solving equivalence problems (e.g., 3 + 4 + 5 = 3 + -). Students in 4th and 5th grades completed assessments of their conceptual and procedural knowledge of equivalence, both before and after a brief lesson. The instruction focused either on the concept of equivalence or on a correct procedure for solving equivalence problems. Conceptual instruction led to increased conceptual understanding and to generation and transfer of a correct procedure. Procedural instruction led to increased conceptual understanding and to adoption, but only limited transfer, of the instructed procedure. These findings highlight the causal relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge and suggest that conceptual knowledge may have a greater influence on procedural knowledge than the reverse.