ArticlePDF Available

Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior and Adolescent Sexual Orientation: A Longitudinal Population-Based Study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Lesbian and gay individuals have been reported to show more interest in other-sex, and/or less interest in same-sex, toys, playmates, and activities in childhood than heterosexual counterparts. Yet, most of the relevant evidence comes from retrospective studies or from prospective studies of clinically referred, extremely gender nonconforming children. In addition, findings are mixed regarding the relation between childhood gender-typed behavior and the later sexual orientation spectrum from exclusively heterosexual to exclusively lesbian/gay. The current study drew a sample (2,428 girls and 2,169 boys) from a population-based longitudinal study, and found that the levels of gender-typed behavior at ages 3.5 and 4.75 years, although less so at age 2.5 years, significantly and consistently predicted adolescents' sexual orientation at age 15 years, both when sexual orientation was conceptualized as 2 groups or as a spectrum. In addition, within-individual change in gender-typed behavior during the preschool years significantly related to adolescent sexual orientation, especially in boys. These results suggest that the factors contributing to the link between childhood gender-typed behavior and sexual orientation emerge during early development. Some of those factors are likely to be nonsocial, because nonheterosexual individuals appear to diverge from gender norms regardless of social encouragement to conform to gender roles. (PsycINFO Database Record)
Content may be subject to copyright.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 1
!
Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior and Adolescent Sexual Orientation:
A Longitudinal Population-Based Study
Gu Li, Karson T. F. Kung, and Melissa Hines
University of Cambridge
Author Note
Gu Li, Karson T. F. Kung, and Melissa Hines, Department of Psychology, University
of Cambridge.
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives
for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers,
computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers,
managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome
Trust (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for
ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and Gu Li will serve as guarantor for
the contents of this paper. This specific study was supported by a Cambridge International
Scholarship awarded to Gu Li and by research funding from the Department of Psychology,
University of Cambridge.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gu Li, Department of
Psychology, University of Cambridge, Free School Lane, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK. E-mail:
gl369@cam.ac.uk
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 2
!
This manuscript was accepted for publication in Developmental Psychology on November 17,
2016. This is the accepted author manuscript. APA’s final edited version of this article is
available online on February 20, 2017 at http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000281
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 3
!
Abstract
Lesbian and gay individuals have been reported to show more interest in other-sex, and/or
less interest in same-sex, toys, playmates, and activities in childhood than heterosexual
counterparts. Yet, most of the relevant evidence comes from retrospective studies or from
prospective studies of clinically-referred, extremely gender nonconforming children. In
addition, findings are mixed regarding the relationship between childhood gender-typed
behavior and the later sexual orientation spectrum from exclusively heterosexual to
exclusively lesbian/gay. The current study drew a sample (2,428 girls and 2,169 boys) from a
population-based longitudinal study, and found that the levels of gender-typed behavior at
ages 3.50 and 4.75 years, although less so at age 2.50 years, significantly and consistently
predicted adolescents’ sexual orientation at age 15 years, both when sexual orientation was
conceptualized as two groups or as a spectrum. In addition, within-individual change in
gender-typed behavior during the preschool years significantly related to adolescent sexual
orientation, especially in boys. These results suggest that the factors contributing to the link
between childhood gender-typed behavior and sexual orientation emerge during early
development. Some of those factors are likely to be nonsocial, because nonheterosexual
individuals appear to diverge from gender norms regardless of social encouragement to
conform to gender roles.
Keywords: gender nonconformity, sexual orientation, gender-typed behavior, LGB,
ALSPAC
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 4
!
Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior and Adolescent Sexual Orientation:
A Longitudinal Population-Based Study
Childhood gender-typed behavior is perhaps the most extensively studied early
behavioral predictor of sexual orientation. Gender-typed behavior, or gender role behavior,
refers to behaviors, attitudes, and personality traits that differ on average for females and
males (Hines, 2004). Among children, gender-typed behavior can be observed in preferences
for male-typical or female-typical toys (e.g., toy trucks versus dolls), playmates (e.g., boys
versus girls), and play activities (e.g., rough-and-tumble play versus playing house)
(Golomok & Rust, 1993a, 1993b; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Martin, Eisenbud, & Rose,
1995). Childhood gender-typed behavior shows a large sex difference and has been
considered as an important component of human gender development (Hines, 2010, 2015).
Another domain of gender development that differs substantially between males and
females is sexual orientation (Hines, 2010, 2011). Sexual orientation directs a person’s
sexuality to men, to women, to both, or to neither; it can be measured using multiple
indicators such as romantic and sexual attraction, sexual behavior, sexual identity, and
physiological sexual arousal (Bailey et al., 2016; Savin-Williams, 2006). Sexual orientation is
manifested in adolescence by ages 12–17 years (Calzo, Masyn, Austin, Jun, & Corliss, 2016;
Russell & Fish, 2016), and may be seen in children as young as 10 years of age (McClintock
& Herdt, 1996). During adolescence, the most observable indicators of sexual orientation are
sexual attraction and perhaps some sexual activities such as kissing on the mouth (Calzo,
Antonucci, Mays, & Cochran, 2011; D’Augelli, Grossman, Starks, & Sinclair, 2010; Li &
Hines, 2016; McClintock & Herdt, 1996; Rosario et al., 1996; Savin-Williams & Diamond,
2000). Theoretical perspectives and empirical research suggest that individuals with different
sexual orientations may show different childhood gender-typed behaviors.
Theoretical Perspectives
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 5
!
Both genetic and hormonal theories suggest a link between childhood gender-typed
behavior and sexual orientation. Genes are suggested to influence childhood gender-typed
behavior (e.g., Iervolino, Hines, Golombok, Rust, & Plomin, 2005; Knafo, Iervolino, &
Plomin, 2005) and (male) sexual orientation (e.g., Sanders et al., 2015; see also Bailey et al.,
2016, for a review), and it is possible that some common genes contribute to both of these
domains. Twin studies suggest that additive genetic factors account for a modest to
substantial amount of the covariance between recalled childhood gender-typed behavior and
sexual orientation in women (Alanko et al., 2010; Bailey, Dunne, & Martin, 2000; Burri,
Cherkas, Spector, & Rahman, 2011) and in men (Alanko et al., 2010; Bailey et al., 2000).
Early androgen exposure may also contribute to the development of both childhood
gender-typed behavior and sexual orientation (Hines, 2011). Substantial evidence suggests
that androgen exposure during prenatal and neonatal periods contributes to enduring sex
differences in the mammalian brain and behavior (Arnold, 2009; Hines et al., 2016;
McCarthy & Arnold, 2011; Morris, Jordan, & Breedlove, 2004). In nonhuman animals,
numerous experiments have demonstrated that administration of androgens during early
development masculinizes and/or defeminizes later sexual behaviors, as well as other
behaviors that differ on average for males and females (Arnold, 2009; Hines, 2004, 2011). In
humans, the strongest empirical support for influences of early androgens on gender
development comes from research involving females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia
(CAH), a recessive autosomal condition that results in increased exposure to testosterone and
other androgens, beginning before birth (New, 1998). A number of studies have reported that,
compared to unaffected relatives and to matched controls, females with CAH show increased
male-typical behavior in childhood, as well as increased nonheterosexual fantasy and
behavior in adulthood (Hines, 2011). There is also some evidence that normal variability in
androgen exposure relates to childhood gender-typed behavior in typically-developing
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 6
!
children (Hines et al., 2002; Lamminmäki et al., 2012; Pasterski et al., 2015). However, little
is known about the influences of early androgen exposure on sexual orientation in typically-
developing individuals, perhaps due to the difficulty in measuring early androgen
concentrations reliably and accurately in a sufficiently large sample, and following the
sample into adolescence (e.g., Hines et al., 2002; Hines, Constantinescu, & Spencer, 2015).
Research on Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior and Sexual Orientation
The majority of past studies comparing childhood gender-typed behavior in
individuals with different sexual orientations are retrospective. These studies, more than 60 in
total, consistently reported that lesbian women and gay men recalled significantly more
gender nonconforming behavior than heterosexual counterparts (reviewed in Bailey &
Zucker, 1995; Zucker, 2008). Bailey and Zucker estimated that the magnitude of the
difference was large, with an overall Cohen’s d of 1.3 for men and of 1.0 for women; by
Cohen’s (1988) recommendation, d values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 represent small, medium, and
large effects, respectively.
Despite these consistent findings, retrospective studies can be criticized as susceptible
to memory bias (e.g., Ross, 1980; Gottschalk, 2003). In response to this potential concern,
Rieger, Linsenmeier, Gygax, and Bailey (2008) collected lesbian/gay and heterosexual
adults’ childhood home videos and had their childhood gender-typed behavior evaluated by
independent raters who watched the videos and coded the gender-typicality of the behaviors
shown. They observed a large and significant overall difference (ds = 1.0 for men and 1.2 for
women) in the rated childhood gender-typed behavior between the two sexual orientation
groups, suggesting that the difference is more than a product of memory bias.
Another line of evidence supporting the link between childhood gender-typed
behavior and later sexual orientation comes from clinically referred children, many of whom
demonstrate extreme cross-gender behavior that partially or fully meets the diagnostic criteria
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 7
!
for gender dysphoria/gender identity disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2013). Across 11 such studies,
over 30% of extremely gender nonconforming girls and over 60% of extremely gender
nonconforming boys reported some same- or both-sex sexual fantasy or behavior in
adolescence or adulthood (Drummond, Bradley, Peterson-Badali, & Zucker, 2008; Green,
1987; Singh, 2012; Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008; also see Zucker & Bradley, 1995 for a
summary of another 6 studies). These percentages exceed similar figures for same- or both-
sex sexual fantasy or behavior in the general population (estimated as 3% of women and men;
summarized from Figure 1 in Bailey et al., 2016), especially for men. Even if individuals in
the general population who are mostly heterosexual are included, the prevalence rates of
nonheterosexual women and men are 13% and 7% respectively, still lower than the 30% and
60% figures for extremely gender nonconforming girls and boys who would become
nonheterosexual. However, because these findings are based on clinical samples, it is
unknown how well they apply to the general population.
To date, only one prospective study has analyzed the relation between childhood
gender-typed behavior and later sexual orientation in a general-population sample (Steensma,
van der Ende, Verhulst, & Cohen-Kettenis, 2013). This study included a sample of 473 girls
and 406 boys. Among these participants, 41 girls and 10 boys were classified as gender
nonconforming in childhood based on a single administration of a parent-reported measure
when the participants were 4 to 11 years old. When followed up in adulthood, gender
nonconforming girls were up to 11 times as likely to report nonheterosexuality as all girls,
and gender nonconforming boys were up to 13 times as likely to report nonheterosexuality as
all boys. Taken together, converging evidence from retrospective and prospective research
suggests that, on average, nonheterosexual men and women are more gender nonconforming
during childhood compared to their same-sex heterosexual peers.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 8
!
Despite much theoretical and empirical work, several gaps remain. First, most prior
studies linking childhood gender-typed behavior to sexual orientation were either
retrospective or based on clinical samples, and both of these types of studies have limitations.
Further, the reliability of the results from the one prior longitudinal population-based study
might be limited by the use of a 2-item measure of childhood gender nonconformity, which
showed weak internal consistency (a = .41), and by the small number of gender
nonconforming children, especially boys (Steensma et al., 2013). Therefore, more
prospective research, using a large sample from the general population and more reliable
measures of childhood gender-typed behavior and sexual orientation, is needed.
Second, the age at which any behavioral differences between lesbian/gay and
heterosexual individuals emerge is unknown. Rieger et al. (2008) estimated, in their study of
home videos, that the difference appeared to manifest at around age 3 years, and that it
appeared to become increasingly pronounced from early to late childhood. The possible
developmental increase in the predictive power of gender nonconforming behavior resembles
the development of gender-typed behavior more generally (Golombok et al., 2008;
Golombok & Hines, 2002; Hines, 2015; Maccoby, 1988; Wong & Hines, 2015). The early
emergence of gender-typed behavior suggests that the factors affecting gender-typed
behavior and sexual orientation may come into play before the preschool years. As a
consequence, it would be useful to examine the age trend in a longitudinal population-based
study.
Third, few studies have evaluated relations between childhood gender-typed behavior
and nonexclusive attractions (e.g., mostly heterosexual, bisexual, and mostly lesbian/gay),
although nonexclusively attracted individuals outnumber exclusively lesbian/gay individuals
and comprise the majority of sexual minorities (Diamond, Bonner, & Dickenson, 2015;
Savin-Williams, 2014, 2016; Savin-Williams & Vrangalova, 2013). In other words, while
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 9
!
there is a consistent mean difference between lesbian/gay and heterosexual individuals, it is
not clear if there is a monotonic increase in childhood gender nonconforming behavior across
the sexual orientation spectrum from exclusively heterosexual to exclusively lesbian/gay.
Previous research has yielded mixed findings. While some studies have reported a significant
linear relation between childhood gender-typed behavior and later position on the sexual
orientation spectrum (Alanko et al., 2010; Burri et al., 2011; Dunne, Bailey, Kirk, & Martin,
2000; Roberts, Rosario, Corliss, Koenen, & Austin, 2012), others have not (Cardoso, 2009;
Steensma et al., 2013). Notably, the studies that reported a significant linear association had
more participants (Ns > 3,000) than the other studies (Ns < 900), resulting in larger samples
of each sexual orientation group and perhaps a more accurate estimate of the relation. In
addition, most of these studies were retrospective, and so could be affected by biased recall.
The Current Study
The current study addresses these gaps in knowledge using a large sample from a
prospective cohort study in England. This study assessed children’s gender-typed behavior
using a standardized measure, at three ages during the preschool period. These three time
points allowed investigation of the age at which any difference related to sexual orientation
might emerge. Repeated measurement also allowed us to investigate for the first time if
within-individual change in gender-typed behavior over time during the preschool years
predicted sexual orientation. Sexual orientation was primarily self-reported in reference to
relative sexual attraction to same- and other-sex peers at age 15 years. In addition, same- and
other-sex sexual activities were assessed as secondary indicators of adolescent sexual
orientation at age 15 years.
Three questions were addressed using these data. First, do lesbian/gay adolescents
show different levels of gender-typed behavior in childhood than heterosexual counterparts,
and at what age does this difference emerge? Second, is there a monotonic relation between
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 10
!
the levels of childhood gender-typed behavior and later positions on the sexual orientation
spectrum? Third, how does change in childhood gender-typed behavior over the preschool
years relate to adolescent sexual orientation?
Method
Participants
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a prospective
study that recruited pregnant women who were scheduled to give birth between April 1, 1991
and December 31, 1992 in a geographically defined area in Avon, Southwest England. The
eligible child cohort, including 1-year live births but excluding one random individual per
twin pair and all triplets and quadruplets according to ALSPAC regulations, consists of 7,065
girls and 7,433 boys. About 80% of the ALSPAC families lived in owner occupied
accommodation, 91% had a car, 79% were married couples, and 2% were non-White.
Compared to the 1991 UK Census data, the ALSPAC sample is slightly more affluent and
less likely to be non-White. For additional information about the ALSPAC cohort, see Boyd
et al. (2013). The study website contains details of all the data, which are available through a
fully searchable data dictionary: http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-
dictionary/. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law
Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees.
The current study analyzed ALSPAC data using four time points, when the children
were 2.50, 3.50, 4.75, and 15 years of age. The participation rate at each time point was 74%,
71%, 65%, and 36%, respectively. Children who had at least one valid assessment of
childhood gender-typed behavior at the first three time points and a valid response of sexual
orientation at the fourth time point were included in this study, N = 4,597 (2,428 girls and
2,169 boys), representing 32% of the eligible ALSPAC child cohort. Girls were more likely
to be included in the current study (34%) compared to boys (29%), χ2(1, n = 14498) = 44.75
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 11
!
(with Yates’s correction of continuity), p < .001, and so were White than non-White families
(37% vs. 30%, respectively), χ2(1, n = 11923) = 15.25 (with Yates’s correction of continuity),
p < .001.
Measures
Childhood gender-typed behavior. Caregivers (usually the child’s mother)
evaluated the child’s gender-typed behavior using the Preschool Activities Inventory (PSAI;
Golombok & Rust, 1993a, 1993b) at three time points, when the child was 2.50, 3.50, and
4.75 years of age. The PSAI is standardized for use with children two to six years of age
(Golombok & Rust, 1993b), and it shows a large sex difference in children as young as two
years old (Golombok et al., 2008; Golombok & Rust, 1993a, 1993b; Wong & Hines, 2015). It
consists of 12 female-typical and 12 male-typical items measuring children’s preferences for
toys (7 items; e.g., tea set [female-typical]), activities (11 items; e.g., playing house [female-
typical]), and characteristics (6 items; e.g., enjoys rough-and-tumble play [male-typical]).
Caregivers rate the child’s behavior in the past month on a 5-point Likert scale,
ranging from 1 = never to 5 = very often. The total score for female-typical items is
subtracted from the total score for male-typical items to form a composite score. These
composite scores are then standardized using the same equation for girls and boys, with a
standardization target of M = 40 and SD = 10 for girls and M = 60 and SD = 10 for boys
(Golombok & Rust, 1993a, 1993b). This standardizing procedure aims to map childhood
gender-typed behavior on a pseudo-T scale with M = 50 and SD = 10 (Golombok & Rust,
1993a). Thus, larger standardized scores indicate more male-typical behavior and/or less
female-typical behavior for both girls and boys. The PSAI demonstrated good internal
consistency in the current sample, αs = .82, .88, and .91, in the 2.50-, 3.50-, and 4.75-year
data collections, respectively. Pearson’s correlations between PSAI scores at 2.50 years and
3.50 years, 2.50 years and 4.75 years, and 3.50 years and 4.75 years were .64, .53, and .70 for
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 12
!
girls, respectively, and .64, .53, and .68 for boys, respectively, all ps < .001, demonstrating
high stability for gender-typed behavior across these years. These correlations are similar to
those reported previously for children in the ALSPAC sample across a similar age range
(Golombok et al., 2008).
Adolescent sexual orientation. At age 15 years, adolescents reported their sexual
orientation privately, on a computer, a procedure that can enhance self-disclosure of personal
information (Turner et al., 1998). As in Austin et al. (2009), Remafedi, Resnick, Blum, and
Harris (1992), and Saewyc, Skay, Bearinger, Blum, and Resnick, (1998), participants were
asked to report their sexual orientation on a 5-point scale similar to the Kinsey scale (Kinsey,
Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948): 1 = 100% heterosexual, 2 = mostly heterosexual but also
attracted to the same sex, 3 = bisexual (equally attracted to both sexes), 4 = mostly
lesbian/gay but also attracted to the other sex, 5 = 100% lesbian/gay, 6 = not sexually
attracted to either sex, 7 = not sure. Participants who selected 6 (n = 13) or 7 (n = 69) were
removed from analyses involving sexual orientation, because (a) there are no clear
predictions regarding differences in childhood gender-typed behavior between adolescents
with an asexual or questioning orientation and those with a heterosexual orientation and (b)
the majority of asexual and unsure adolescents are likely to self-identify as heterosexual in
late adolescence or adulthood (Ott, Corliss, Wypij, Rosario, & Austin, 2011; Savin-Williams
& Joyner, 2014a), so that it does not allow reliable comparisons of asexual or unsure
adolescents to heterosexual adolescents. This 5-point measure of sexual orientation has
shown similarly good stability (i.e., test-retest reliability) in adolescents and in young adults
(Ott et al., 2011), expected associations with the sex of sexual partners among adolescents
(Saewyc et al., 1998), and relatively low nonresponse rate among adolescents compared to
sexual orientation defined on other components (e.g., sexual fantasy; Saewyc et al., 2004).
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 13
!
Thus, age 15 years appears to be a good age to start assessing self-reported sexual orientation
in a longitudinal study.
Considering recent debates about the reliability of measuring adolescent sexual
orientation (Katz-Wise, Calzo, Li, & Pollitt, 2015; Li, Katz-Wise, & Calzo, 2014; Savin-
Williams & Joyner, 2014a, 2014b), data on sexual behavior were also analyzed as a
reliability check. Sexual behavior was assessed using the Adolescent Sexual Activities Index
(ASAI; Hansen, Paskett, & Carter, 1999). Fourteen sexual activities were presented in the
order from low (e.g., “hugging”) to high (e.g., “having sexual intercourse”) intensity (Tables
5 & 6). Adolescents reported whether or not they had had each of the experiences in the past
year. Two stopping rules were used: If participants (1) answered “no” or skipped inquiries
about “cuddling,” “laying down together,” or “being undressed with private parts showing,”
or (2) skipped inquiries about “touching or fondling another young person’s private parts” or
“private parts being touched or fondled,” they did not progress to the rest of the questions.
The ASAI showed good internal consistency in this study, α = .93.
In addition to occurrence, adolescents reported the sex(es) of the person(s) with whom
they engaged in each sexual activity. For the purpose of the current study, adolescents who
reported exclusive other-sex contacts in a given sexual activity received a score of 0 on that
activity, and those who reported any same-sex contacts (including same-sex and both-sex
contacts) in a given sexual activity received a score of 1 on that activity. Comparisons were
not made between adolescents who had any same-sex (or other-sex) sexual contacts and those
who had no sexual contacts, because these two groups may not reflect differences in sexual
orientation, but may rather reflect differences in other factors such as the availability of
sexual partners. Also, because few adolescents had exclusive same-sex sexual contacts, they
were not distinguished from those with both-sex sexual contacts in the comparisons of
childhood gender-typed behavior.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 14
!
Missing Data
At the scale level, the four key variables had 2–8% missingness due to item
nonresponse (Table 1). These missing data were handled by full information maximum
likelihood in Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2015) in the latent growth modeling and by
multiple imputation in Amelia II 1.7.3 (Honaker, King, & Blackwell, 2012) followed by
Zelig 4.2-1 (Choirat, Honaker, Imai, King, & Lau, 2015) to pool the imputed data in the
ordinary least squares regression.
Results
Were There Differences in the Levels of Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior Between
Lesbian/Gay and Heterosexual Adolescents?
Sample size, mean, standard deviation, and range of each study variable are reported
in Table 1. To fully use the longitudinal assessments of childhood gender-typed behavior,
latent growth modeling was performed (Little, 2013). Latent intercept was separated from
latent slope, with the former estimating the mean level of childhood gender-typed behavior at
a chosen time point, and the latter estimating the rate of within-individual change from the
first to the last assessment. Previous research has found that children who were gender
nonconforming at age 3.50 years became increasingly so later in childhood (Golombok et al.,
2008); consequently, lesbian/gay individuals may exhibit more gender nonconforming
behavior than heterosexual counterparts, because they were gender nonconforming to start
with, or because they became more gender nonconforming over time. Distinguishing latent
intercept from latent slope allowed the investigation of relations between these individual
differences in the development of gender-typed behavior and adolescent sexual orientation.
Latent growth models were fitted separately by sex, with the standardized PSAI
scores at ages 2.50, 3.50, and 4.75 years used as indicators. Factor loadings on the latent
intercept were all fixed at 1; those on the latent slope were fixed by the time intervals in years
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 15
!
between assessments. The latent intercept was alternately set to the three ages, so that the
mean level of gender-typed behavior was estimated for each age (Biesanz, Deeb-Sossa,
Papadakis, Bollen, & Curran, 2004). All other parameters were freely estimated. Table 2
shows that latent growth models fit well with the developmental trajectories of preschool
gender-typed behavior in girls and boys. All estimates presented in Table 2 were significantly
different from 0 at p < .001, two-tailed. The estimates of latent intercepts and slope indicated
that from ages 2.50 to 4.75 years, girls and boys on average increasingly conformed to the
behavioral norm of their own gender. The reproduced values of latent intercepts and slope for
each child were then predicted by adolescent sexual orientation and sexual activities in
ordinary least squares regression. Using the same set of latent intercepts and slope, rather
than estimating them in individual conditioned latent growth models (i.e., controlling for
sexual orientation or sexual activities while estimating latent growth factors), ensures high
accuracy in the estimates of latent intercepts and slope, especially when the sample size is
small (e.g., comparing adolescents who had oral sex with any same-sex partners to those with
exclusively other-sex partners in Tables 5 & 6).
Ordinary least squares regression demonstrated differences in the levels of childhood
gender-typed behavior between lesbian/gay and heterosexual adolescents (Tables 3 & 4).
Bonferroni corrections were applied to control for the inflated family-wise error rate due to
multiple comparisons. Starting at age 3.50 years in girls and 2.50 years in boys, pre-
lesbian/gay children exhibited significantly higher levels of gender nonconforming behavior
than same-sex pre-heterosexual peers. By Cohen’s (1988) benchmarks, the differences were
large from 3.50 years of age, ds > 0.8 (Tables 3 & 4).
To illustrate the size of the difference, histograms of the levels of gender-typed
behavior at ages 2.50, 3.50, and 4.75 years were drawn as a function of the sexual orientation
group (lesbian/gay or heterosexual) at age 15, for girls and boys (Figure 1). According to the
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 16
!
frequency distributions, 6%, 6%, and 19% of lesbian girls scored above 56.45 on the PSAI
(equating to the 95th percentile in the target standardized distribution with an M of 40 and an
SD of 10) at ages 2.50, 3.50, and 4.75 years (and thus appeared to be extremely gender
nonconforming), respectively, while only 0.8%, 0.5%, and 1% of heterosexual girls did so
respectively. Similarly, 0%, 4%, and 13% of gay boys scored below 43.55 on the PSAI
(equating to the 5th percentile in the target standardized distribution with an M of 60 and an
SD of 10) at ages 2.50, 3.50, and 4.75 years (and thus appeared to be extremely gender
nonconforming), respectively, while the corresponding percentages for heterosexual boys
were 0.4%, 0.2%, and 0.5%, respectively.
Parallel analyses revealed a significant group difference in the levels of childhood
gender-typed behavior, starting at age 2.50 years, by the sex(es) of partner(s) with whom
adolescents had sexual contacts (Tables 5 & 6). Overall, 15-year-old adolescents who
reported certain types of sexual contacts with any same-sex partners showed higher levels of
gender nonconformity than same-sex peers who reported corresponding sexual contacts with
other-sex partners only. The effect sizes of the differences varied, with ranges of ds of [0.01,
1.04] in girls (median d = 0.24) and [-1.54, 0.03] in boys (median d = -0.36). The most
consistently predicted sexual activities by the levels of childhood gender-typed behavior were
ones of an intermediate to high level of intensity, such as kissing and being kissed on the
mouth, laying down together, touching partners under clothes, touching or fondling partners’
private parts, private parts being touched or fondled, and having oral sex (Tables 5 & 6). A
larger number of significant differences were observed in boys than in girls (28 versus 18;
Tables 5 & 6). Moreover, the levels of gender-typed behavior at older ages seemed to relate
to adolescents’ sexual behavior more strongly than the levels of gender-typed behavior at age
2.50 years, especially in boys (Tables 6).
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 17
!
Was There a Monotonic Relation Between the Levels of Childhood Gender-Typed
Behavior and the Sexual Orientation Spectrum?
From self-identified heterosexual to bisexual to lesbian/gay, adolescents demonstrated
a monotonic increase in the levels of gender nonconforming behavior at ages 3.50 and 4.75
years, although not at age 2.50 years, after Bonferroni corrections (Tables 3 & 4). Further
division of sexual orientation into five categories saw similar monotonic associations with the
levels of gender-typed behavior at older ages, after Bonferroni corrections (Tables 3 & 4).
These associations were all linear (Tables 3 & 4).
What Was the Relation Between Change in Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior Over
Time and Adolescent Sexual Orientation?
Ordinary least squares regression demonstrated that change in gender-typed behavior
during preschool years also related significantly to adolescent sexual orientation. Specifically,
heterosexual adolescents increasingly conformed to their own gender norms from ages 2.50
to 4.75 years, whereas nonheterosexual adolescents became gender conforming at a slower
rate than heterosexual counterparts or became more gender nonconforming over the
preschool years (Tables 3 & 4). The significant differences in the latent slope between
heterosexual and lesbian/gay adolescents approximated a large size in girls (d = 0.72) and in
boys (d = -1.09). When sexual orientation was coded as a spectrum (either as three or five
groups) from heterosexual to lesbian/gay, the latent slope linearly increased in girls and
linearly decreased in boys, suggesting that compared to heterosexual adolescents, bisexuals
became gender conforming at a lower rate, and lesbian/gay individuals became gender
conforming at an even lower rate, or became more gender nonconforming before age 5 years.
After controlling for average levels of gender-typed behavior, change in gender-typed
behavior continued to significantly predict adolescent sexual orientation in boys in the same
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 18
!
manner as before, but did not predict adolescent sexual orientation in girls, regardless of how
adolescent sexual orientation was coded (Table S3).
Results for sexual activities suggested a similar picture: Adolescent girls who had
same-sex sexual contacts rarely differed significantly in change in childhood gender-typed
behavior during preschool years from girls who had exclusively other-sex sexual contacts
(Table 5). In contrast, adolescent boys who had same-sex contacts in some activities became
gender conforming at a lower rate or became more gender nonconforming during preschool
years than boys who had exclusively other-sex contacts in those activities (Table 6).
Discussion
This study examined the association between childhood gender-typed behavior and
adolescent sexual orientation, in a large sample from a longitudinal, population-based study.
The findings suggest that self-identified lesbian/gay adolescents are more likely than
heterosexual counterparts to have shown high levels of gender nonconforming behavior in
childhood. This significant and large group difference was seen before age 5 years.
Compared to heterosexual adolescent girls, lesbian girls were 12–19 times as likely to display
extreme levels of gender nonconforming behavior at ages 3.50 and 4.75 years; compared to
heterosexual adolescent boys, gay boys were 20–26 times as likely to display extreme levels
of gender nonconforming behavior at ages 3.50 and 4.75 years. Similarly, adolescents who
reported any same-sex sexual contacts (especially activities of an intermediate to high level
of intensity) also reported significantly higher levels of gender nonconforming behavior,
starting at age 2.50 years but more strongly and consistently at older ages (especially in boys),
than adolescents who had exclusively other-sex sexual contacts. In addition, the levels of
childhood behavioral gender nonconformity increased monotonically across the sexual
orientation spectrum from exclusively heterosexual to exclusively lesbian/gay, for both girls
and boys. Finally, we explored whether change in gender-typed behavior across the pre-
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 19
!
school years related significantly to adolescent sexual orientation, and to sexual activities,
and we found this to be the case, particularly in boys.
The findings in this study converge with those of previous studies to support a
difference in the levels of childhood gender nonconformity between groups of self-identified
lesbian/gay and heterosexual individuals. Further, the size of this group difference is
uniformly large: In retrospective studies, Cohen’s ds of 1.0 or larger have been observed (e.g.,
Bailey & Zucker, 1995; Rieger et al., 2008), exceeding the value of 0.8 that is considered to
be a large effect (Cohen, 1988). While retrospective studies are criticized as subject to self-
recall bias, the only existing longitudinal study also reported a large group difference (odds
ratios equaled 6.6 for women and 13.7 for men, corresponding to ds of 1.0 and 1.4; see Card,
2010, p. 119, for the equation that converts odds ratio to d; Steensma et al., 2013). Our d
values of 0.9 for girls and 1.2 for boys in a longitudinal, population-based sample, when
gender-typed behavior was measured at age 4.75 years, are similar to Steensma et al.’s
findings. The prospective design of the current study and of Steensma et al.’s suggests that
the connection is not merely a product of self-recall bias due to lesbian and gay people’s
internalized societal stereotypes (Ross, 1980; Gottschalk, 2003). It should also be noted that,
among potential childhood behavioral predictors of sexual orientation (e.g., familial factors
such as the parent-child relationship), gender-typed behavior appears to be the strongest one,
especially when predicting dichotomous differences between heterosexual and lesbian/gay
individuals (e.g., Bell, Weinberg, & Hammersmith, 1981; also reviewed in Bailey et al.,
2016).
Comparisons between two sexual activity groups yielded, with occasional exceptions,
smaller magnitudes of differences in childhood gender-typed behavior than comparisons
between two sexual orientation groups, according to Cohen’s ds (Tables 4 & 5). This may
have occurred because adolescent sexual activity is not a perfect indicator of adolescent
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 20
!
sexual orientation. For example, some self-identified nonheterosexual adults recall having
other-sex sexual contacts during adolescence (Herdt & Boxer, 1996; Rosario et al., 1996;
Savin-Williams, 1998), and some self-identified heterosexual adults recall having same-sex
sexual contacts during adolescence (Knight & Hope, 2012; Morgan, 2012; Morgan &
Thompson, 2011).
The present findings may have implications for understanding the factors that
influence individual variability in sexual orientation. We found that the differences in levels
of gender-typed behavior among sexual orientation groups emerged early, at as young as 2.50
to 3.50 years. This finding resembles the results of Rieger et al.’s (2008) study, which
reported that lesbian/gay individuals were more gender nonconforming than heterosexual
individuals starting from around age 3 years. In the current study, however, this difference
was statistically nonsignificant or smaller at age 2.50 years than it was at age 3.50 or 4.75
years. This smaller effect at age 2.50 years may reflect reduced sensitivity of the measure of
gender-typed play at the youngest age, given that sex differences in childhood gender-typed
play become more pronounced (and thus more observable) across childhood (Hines, 2015;
Wong & Hines, 2015). Nevertheless, the early emerging differences in gender-typed behavior
among sexual orientation groups suggest that any common factors affecting both traits are
likely to be present early in development.
The current study found that not only levels of gender nonconformity, but also change
in gender-typed behavior across the preschool years related significantly to later sexual
orientation, especially in boys. When heterosexual individuals, who comprised the majority
of participants, increasingly conformed to respective gender norms, nonheterosexual
individuals appeared to conform less, or became more nonconforming, over time. The link
between change in gender-typed behavior over time and adolescent sexual orientation is
unlikely to be caused by social factors, because there is widespread social encouragement to
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 21
!
conform to gender roles (Eagly, Wood, & Diekman, 2000; Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990),
and childhood gender nonconforming behavior can be associated with victimization
experiences (Katz-Wise & Hyde, 2012; Roberts, Rosario, Slopen, Calzo, & Austin, 2013),
which should discourage pre-nonheterosexual children from diverging from gender norms.
Nevertheless, how developmental change in childhood gender-typed behavior relates to later
sexual orientation merits further investigation, because evidence from this study is less
supportive of such a link in girls than in boys, and because we only assessed gender-typed
behavior across a short period of time.
Finally, the current study found that childhood gender nonconformity was
monotonically associated with same-sex sexual orientation within each sex. This finding is
consistent with predictions based on evidence that gendered traits may be correlated because
they share a common, monotonic influence of early androgen exposure (Hines, 2011). For
example, the extent to which a female with CAH prefers male-typical over female-typical
childhood activities, and female over male sexual partners both appear to be monotonically
influenced by the degree of prenatal androgen exposure due to CAH (Frisén et al., 2009;
Meyer-Bahlburg, Dolezal, Baker, & New, 2008). Similarly, in typically-developing girls and
boys, there is evidence that childhood gender-typed behavior is monotonically related to early
androgen concentrations (Hines et al., 2002; Lamminmäki et al., 2012; Pasterski et al., 2015),
although little is known about the relationship between early androgen levels and sexual
orientation in typically-developing individuals.
Results of the current study should be interpreted with some limitations in mind. First,
while the majority of prior studies relating childhood gender nonconformity to later sexual
orientation measured sexual orientation in adulthood (with the exception of Rieger & Savin-
Williams, 2012), the current study measured sexual orientation in adolescence. Because the
number of people who identify as nonheterosexual, especially as lesbian/gay, increases from
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 22
!
adolescence to adulthood (Austin et al., 2009; Remafedi et al., 1992), it is possible that
assessments of our cohort at later ages would produce somewhat different results. In addition,
because our study focused on adolescents, some of our participants were at earlier pubertal
stages than others. Nevertheless, in the current study the significance levels for the relations
between childhood gender-typed behavior and adolescent sexual orientation remained largely
unchanged after controlling for pubertal development statuses (Tables S4 & S5). Second,
ongoing debates question whether adolescents accurately report their sexual orientation
(Katz-Wise et al., 2015; Li et al., 2014; Savin-Williams & Joyner, 2014a, 2014b). Although
our measurement of sexual orientation at age 15 years appeared to be sufficiently reliable to
detect similar relations to childhood gender nonconformity to those seen in prior studies,
future research might usefully study this cohort at older ages. Future studies also might
usefully explore the connection between childhood gender-typed behavior and sexual
orientation measured using methods other than self-reports, such as pupil dilation and genital
arousal (e.g., Rieger, Savin-Williams, Chivers, & Bailey, 2015).
Conclusion
By age 2.50 to 3.50 years, children’s gender-typed behavior significantly predicts
future sexual orientation. Children who exhibit more gender nonconformity in regard to toys,
playmates, and activities are more likely to later report more same-sex and/or less other-sex
sexual attraction and behavior. The current results converge with other lines of retrospective
and prospective research to suggest that childhood gender nonconforming behavior is a
consistent early predictor of future nonheterosexual orientations. This observed relation may
be partly driven by other factors, such as early androgen exposure or common genes, that
affect behavior early in development.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 23
!
References
Alanko, K., Santtila, P., Harlaar, N., Witting, K., Varjonen, M., Jern, P., … Sandnabba, N. K.
(2010). Common genetic effects of gender atypical behavior in childhood and sexual
orientation in adulthood: A study of Finnish twins. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39,
81–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9457-3
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Arlington, VA: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.
Arnold, A. P. (2009). The organizational–activational hypothesis as the foundation for a
unified theory of sexual differentiation of all mammalian tissues. Hormones and
Behavior, 55, 570–578. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.011
Austin, S. B., Ziyadeh, N. J., Corliss, H. L., Rosario, M., Wypij, D., Haines, J., … Field, A. E.
(2009). Sexual orientation disparities in purging and binge eating from early to late
adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 238–245.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.02.001
Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Genetic and environmental influences
on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin sample. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 524–536. http://doi.org/10.1037//0022-
3514.78.3.524
Bailey, J. M., Vasey, P. L., Diamond, L. M., Breedlove, S. M., Vilain, E., & Epprecht, M.
(2016). Sexual orientation, controversy, and science. Psychological Science in the
Public Interest, 17, 45–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100616637616
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 24
!
Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation:
A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 31, 43–55.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.1.43
Bell, A. P., Weinberg, M. S., & Hammersmith S. K. (1981). Sexual preference: Its
development in men and women. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Biesanz, J. C., Deeb-Sossa, N., Papadakis, A. A., Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2004). The
role of coding time in estimating and interpreting growth curve models. Psychological
Methods, 9, 30–52. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.1.30
Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., … Davey Smith,
G. (2013). Cohort profile: The “children of the 90s”—the index offspring of the Avon
Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology,
42, 111–127. http://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys064
Burri, A., Cherkas, L., Spector, T., & Rahman, Q. (2011). Genetic and environmental
influences on female sexual orientation, childhood gender typicality and adult gender
identity. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e21982. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021982
Calzo, J. P., Antonucci, T. C., Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2011). Retrospective recall of
sexual orientation identity development among gay, lesbian, and bisexual adults.
Developmental Psychology, 47, 1658–1673. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0025508
Calzo, J. P., Masyn, K. E., Austin, S. B., Jun, H.-J., & Corliss, H. L. (2016). Developmental
latent patterns of identification as mostly heterosexual versus lesbian, gay, or bisexual.
Journal of Research on Adolescence. Online first publication.
http://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12266
Card, N. A. (2010). Applied meta-analysis for social science research. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 25
!
Cardoso, F. L. (2009). Recalled sex-typed behavior in childhood and sports’ preferences in
adulthood of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men from Brazil, Turkey, and
Thailand. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 726–736.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9312-6
Choirat, C., Honaker, J., Imai, K., King, G., & Lau, O. (2015). Zelig: Everyone’s statistical
software (Version 4.2-1) [software]. Retrieved from http://zeligproject.org/
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
D’Augelli, A. R., Grossman, A. H., Starks, M. T., & Sinclair, K. O. (2010). Factors
associated with parents’ knowledge of gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths’ sexual
orientation. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 6, 178–198.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15504281003705410
Diamond, L. M., Bonner, S. B., & Dickenson, J. (2015). The development of sexuality. In R.
M. Learner (Ed.) & M. E. Lamb (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and
developmental science: Vol. 3. Socioemotional processes (7th ed., Ch. 21). New York,
NY: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy321
Dorn, L. D., & Biro, F. M. (2011). Puberty and its measurement: A decade in review. Journal
of Research on Adolescence, 21, 180–195. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-
7795.2010.00722.x
Drummond, K. D., Bradley, S. J., Peterson-Badali, M., & Zucker, K. J. (2008). A follow-up
study of girls with gender identity disorder. Developmental Psychology, 44, 34–45.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.34
Dunne, M. P., Bailey, J. M., Kirk, K. M., & Martin, N. G. (2000). The subtlety of sex-
atypicality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 549–565.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 26
!
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and
similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The
developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy
effects, and parents' socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46,
183–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x
Frisén, L., Nordenström, A., Falhammar, H., Filipsson, H., Holmdahl, G., Janson, P. O., …
Nordenskjöld, A. (2009). Gender role behavior, sexuality, and psychosocial
adaptation in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to CYP21A2 deficiency.
The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 94, 3432–3439.
http://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2009-0636
Golombok, S., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in social behavior. In P. K. Smith & C. H.
Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp. 117–136).
Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1993a). The measurement of gender role behaviour in pre-school
children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 805–811.
Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1993b). The Pre-School Activities Inventory: A standardized
assessment of gender role in children. Psychological Assessment, 5, 131–136.
Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T., Golding, J., & Hines, M. (2008).
Developmental trajectories of sex-typed behavior in boys and girls: A longitudinal
general population study of children aged 2.5–8 years. Child Development, 79, 1583–
1593.
Gottschalk, L. (2003). Same-sex sexuality and childhood gender non-conformity: A spurious
connection. Journal of Gender Studies, 12, 35–50.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0958923032000067808
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 27
!
Green, R. (1987). The “sissy boy syndrome” and the development of homosexuality. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hansen, W. B., Paskett, E. D., & Carter, L. J. (1999). The Adolescent Sexual Activity Index
(ASAI): A standardized strategy for measuring interpersonal heterosexual behaviors
among youth. Health Education Research, 14, 485–490.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/14.4.485
Hedges, L. V. (1982). Estimation of effect size from a series of independent experiments.
Psychological Bulletin, 92, 490–499. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-
2909.92.2.490
Herdt, G. H., & Boxer, A. (1996). Children of horizons: How gay and lesbian teens are
leading a new way out of the closet. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 14, 448–456. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005
Hines, M. (2011). Prenatal endocrine influences on sexual orientation and on sexually
differentiated childhood behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 32, 170–182.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.02.006
Hines, M. (2015). Gendered development. In R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.) & M. E. Lamb (Vol.
Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 3.
Socioemotional processes (7th ed., Ch. 20). New York, NY: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy320
Hines, M., Constantinescu, M., & Spencer, D. (2015). Early androgen exposure and human
gender development. Biology of Sex Differences, 6(3), 1–10.
http://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-015-0022-1
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 28
!
Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K. J., Golding, J., & Avon Longitudinal Study
of Parents and Children Study Team. (2002). Testosterone during pregnancy and
gender role behavior of preschool children: A longitudinal, population study. Child
Development, 73, 1678–1687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00498
Hines, M., Spencer, D., Kung, K. T., Browne, W. V., Constantinescu, M., & Noorderhaven,
R. M. (2016). The early postnatal period, mini-puberty, provides a window on the role
of testosterone in human neurobehavioural development. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 38, 69–73. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.02.008
Honaker, J., King, G., & Blackwell, M. (2012). Amelia II: A program for missing data
(Version 1.7.3) [software]. Retrieved from http://gking.harvard.edu/amelia/
Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Vol.
Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social
development (4th ed., pp. 387–467). New York, NY: Wiley.
Iervolino, A. C., Hines, M., Golombok, S. E., Rust, J., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic and
environmental influences on sex-typed behavior during the preschool years. Child
Development, 76, 826–840. http://www.jstor.org/stable/3696731
Katz-Wise, S. L., Calzo, J. P., Li, G., & Pollitt, A. (2015). Same data, different perspectives:
What is at stake? Response to Savin-Williams and Joyner (2014a) [Letter to the
Editor]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 15–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-
014-0434-8
Katz-Wise, S. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2012). Victimization experiences of lesbian, gay, and
bisexual individuals: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sex Research, 49, 142–167.
http://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2011.637247
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male.
Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 29
!
Knafo, A., Iervolino, A. C., & Plomin, R. (2005). Masculine girls and feminine boys: Genetic
and environmental contributions to atypical gender development in early
childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 400–412.
http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.400
Knight, L. F. M., & Hope, D. A. (2012). Correlates of same-sex attractions and behaviors
among self-identified heterosexual university students. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
41, 1199–1208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9927-5
Lamminmäki, A., Hines, M., Kuiri-Hänninen, T., Kilpeläinen, L., Dunkel, L., & Sankilampi,
U. (2012). Testosterone measured in infancy predicts subsequent sex-typed behavior
in boys and in girls. Hormones and behavior, 61, 611–616.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.013
Li, G., & Hines, M. (2016). In search of emerging same-sex sexuality: Romantic attractions
at age 13 years. Archives of Sexual Behavior. Online first publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0726-2
Li, G., Katz-Wise, S. L., & Calzo, J. P. (2014). The unjustified doubt of add health studies on
the health disparities of non-heterosexual adolescents: Comment on Savin-Williams
and Joyner (2014) [Letter to the Editor]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 1023–1026.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0313-3
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: The Guilford
Press.
Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.6.755
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA.:
Stanford University Press.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 30
!
Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys.
Child Development, 66, 1453–1471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1995.tb00945.x
McCarthy, M. M., & Arnold, A. P. (2011). Reframing sexual differentiation of the brain.
Nature Neuroscience, 14, 677–683. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2834
McClintock, M. K., & Herdt, G. (1996). Rethinking puberty: The development of sexual
attraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 178–183.
http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512422
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., & New, M. I. (2008). Sexual orientation
in women with classical or non-classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia as a function
of degree of prenatal androgen excess. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 85–99.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9265-1
Morgan, E. M. (2012). Not always a straight path: College students’ narratives of
heterosexual orientation development. Sex Roles, 66, 79–93.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0068-4
Morgan, E. M., & Thompson, E. M. (2011). Processes of sexual orientation questioning
among heterosexual women. The Journal of Sex Research, 48, 16–28.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490903370594
Morris, J. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2004). Sexual differentiation of the
vertebrate nervous system. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1034–1039.
http://doi.org/10.1038/nn1325
Morris, N. M., & Udry, J. R. (1980). Validation of a self-administered instrument to assess
stages of adolescent development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, 271–280.
http://doi.org/10.1007/BF02088471
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 31
!
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles,
CA: Authors.
New, M. I. (1998). Diagnosis and management of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Annual
Review of Medicine, 49, 311–328.
Ott, M. Q., Corliss, H. L., Wypij, D., Rosario, M., & Austin, S. B. (2011). Stability and
change in self-reported sexual orientation identity in young people: Application of
mobility metrics. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 519–532.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-9691-3
Pasterski, V., Acerini, C. L., Dunger, D. B., Ong, K. K., Hughes, I. A., Thankamony, A., &
Hines, M. (2015). Postnatal penile growth concurrent with mini-puberty predicts later
sex-typed play behavior: Evidence for neurobehavioral effects of the postnatal
androgen surge in typically developing boys. Hormones and Behavior, 69, 98–105.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.01.002
Remafedi, G., Resnick, M., Blum, R., & Harris, L. (1992). Demography of sexual orientation
in adolescents. Pediatrics, 89, 714–721.
Rieger, G., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2012). Gender nonconformity, sexual orientation, and
psychological well-being. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 611–621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9738-0
Rieger, G., Linsenmeier, J. A. W., Gygax, L., & Bailey, J. M. (2008). Sexual orientation and
childhood gender nonconformity: Evidence from home videos. Developmental
Psychology, 44, 46–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.46
Rieger, G., Savin-Williams, R. C., Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2015). Sexual arousal and
masculinity-femininity of women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Online first publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000077
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 32
!
Roberts, A. L., Rosario, M., Corliss, H. L., Koenen, K. C., & Austin, S. B. (2012). Childhood
gender nonconformity: A risk indicator for childhood abuse and posttraumatic stress
in youth. Pediatrics, 129, 410–417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1804
Roberts, A. L., Rosario, M., Slopen, N., Calzo, J. P., & Austin, S. B. (2013). Childhood
gender nonconformity, bullying victimization, and depressive symptoms across
adolescence and early adulthood: An 11-year longitudinal study. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 143–152.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.006
Rosario, M., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Hunter, J., Exner, T. M., Gwadz, M., & Keller, A. M.
(1996). The psychosexual development of urban lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths.
Journal of Sex Research, 33, 113–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499609551823
Ross, M. W. (1980). Retrospective distortion in homosexual research. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 9, 523–531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01542156
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York, NY: Wiley
& Sons.
Russell, S. T., & Fish, J. N. (2016). Mental health in lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender
(LGBT) youth. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12, 465–487. .
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093153
Saewyc, E. M., Bauer, G. R., Skay, C. L., Bearinger, L. H., Resnick, M. D., Reis, E., &
Murphy, A. (2004). Measuring sexual orientation in adolescent health surveys:
Evaluation of eight school-based surveys. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35, 345.e1
345.e15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.06.002
Saewyc, E. M., Skay, C. L., Bearinger, L. H., Blum, R. W., & Resnick, M. D. (1998). Sexual
orientation, sexual behaviors, and pregnancy among American Indian
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 33
!
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 23, 238–247.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(98)00016-0
Sanders, A. R., Martin, E. R., Beecham, G. W., Guo, S., Dawood, K., Rieger, G., ... & Duan,
J. (2015). Genome-wide scan demonstrates significant linkage for male sexual
orientation. Psychological medicine, 45, 1379–1388.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002451
Savin-Williams, R. C. (1998). “...And then I became gay”: Young men’s stories. London, UK:
Routledge.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it matter? Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 15, 40–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-
7214.2006.00403.x
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2014). An exploratory study of the categorical versus spectrum nature
of sexual orientation. The Journal of Sex Research, 51, 446-453.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2013.871691
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2016). Sexual orientation: Categories or continuum? Commentary on
Bailey et al. (2016). Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17, 37-44.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100616637618
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual orientation trajectories among
sexual-minority youths: Gender comparisons. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 607–
627. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1002058505138
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Joyner, K. (2014a). The dubious assessment of gay, lesbian, and
bisexual adolescents of Add Health. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 413–422.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0219-5
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 34
!
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Joyner, K. (2014b). The politicization of gay youth health:
Response to Li, Katz-Wise, and Calzo (2014) [Letter to the Editor]. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 43, 1027–1030. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0359-2
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Vrangalova, Z. (2013). Mostly heterosexual as a distinct sexual
orientation group: A systematic review of the empirical evidence. Developmental
Review, 33, 58–88. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2013.01.001
Singh, D. (2012). A follow-up study of boys with gender identity disorder. University of
Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. Retrieved from
https://tspace.library.utoronto.ca/bitstream/1807/34926/1/Singh_Devita_201211_PhD
_Thesis.pdf
Steensma, T. D., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2013). Gender
variance in childhood and sexual orientation in adulthood: A prospective study.
Journal of Sexual Medicine, 10, 2723–2733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-
6109.2012.02701.x
Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at adolescence. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Turner, C. F., Ku, L., Rogers, S. M., Lindberg, L. D., Pleck, J. H., & Sonenstein, F. L.
(1998). Adolescent sexual behavior, drug use, and violence: Increased reporting with
computer survey technology. Science, 280, 867–873.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5365.867
Wallien, M. S. C., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2008). Psychosexual outcome of gender-
dysphoric children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 47, 1413–1423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e31818956b9
Wong, W. I., & Hines, M. (2015). Preferences for pink and blue: The development of color
preferences as a distinct gender-typed behavior in toddlers. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 44, 1243–1254. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0489-1
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 35
!
Zucker, K. J. (2008). Reflections on the relation between sex-typed behavior in childhood
and sexual orientation in adulthood. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Mental Health, 12(1–
2), 29–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J529v12n01_04
Zucker, K. J., & Bradley, S. J. (1995). Gender identity disorder and psychosexual problems
in children and adolescents. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 36
!
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics of Key Variables by Sex
Girls
Boys
n
M
SD
Range
n
M
SD
Range
Sexual orientation at age 15 yearsa
2,428
1.17
0.48
1–5
2,169
1.13
0.48
1–5
Gender-typed behavior at age 2.50 yearsb
2,241
40.99
8.46
8.65–71.35
2,010
59.87
8.24
27.35–92.25
Gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 yearsb
2,360
37.08
9.31
4.25–72.45
2,125
61.54
8.69
20.75–95.55
Gender-typed behavior at age 4.75 yearsb
2,228
35.28
9.51
4.25–90.05
2,026
63.42
8.78
16.35–93.55
a1 = 100% heterosexual; 5 = 100% lesbian/gay.
bLarger scores represent higher levels of male-typical behavior and/or lower levels of female-typical behavior.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 37
!
Table 2
Latent Linear Growth Models of Childhood Gender-Typed Behavior by Sex With Intercepts Fixed at Different Ages
Intercept
Slope
Intercept-slope
Intercept fixed at
Mean
Variance
Mean
Variance
Covariance
Girls
Model fit: c2(1) = 113.63, p < .001; CFI = .956; TLI = .867; RMSEA [90% CI] = .22 [.18, .25]
2.50 years
40.55 (0.18)
55.19 (2.61)
-2.42 (0.09)
11.53 (1.02)
-5.93 (1.12)
3.50 years
38.12 (0.16)
54.86 (1.91)
-2.42 (0.09)
11.54 (1.02)
5.60 (0.81)
4.75 years
35.09 (0.20)
86.89 (3.89)
-2.42 (0.09)
11.54 (1.02)
20.02 (1.70)
Boys
Model fit: c2(1) = 0.90, p = .342; CFI > .999; TLI > .999; RMSEA [90% CI] = .00 [.00, .06]
2.50 years
59.83 (0.18)
52.39 (8.78)
1.61 (0.08)
8.78 (0.93)
-6.31 (1.07)
3.50 years
61.43 (0.16)
48.55 (1.77)
1.61 (0.08)
8.78 (0.93)
2.47 (0.73)
4.75 years
63.44 (0.19)
68.45 (3.34)
1.61 (0.08)
8.78 (0.93)
13.45 (1.47)
Note. Each row represents an individual latent growth model, but the model fit remained the same within each sex regardless how the intercept
was fixed. All estimates in this table were significantly different from 0 at p < .001, two-tailed. Values outside and inside the parentheses
represent unstandardized estimates and standard errors. CFI = comparative fit index. TLI = Tucker–Lewis index. RMSEA = root mean square
error of approximation. CI = confidence interval.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 38
!
Table 3
Latent Growth Factors of Gender-Typed Behavior During Preschool Years by Sexual Orientation at Age 15 Years in Girls
Sexual orientation
n
Intercept at age 2.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 3.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 4.75
yearsa
Slopeb
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
2-groupc
Heterosexuald
2,366
40.47
6.55
38.03
6.96
34.99
8.82
-2.44
2.81
Lesbiane
16
44.11
8.03
43.70
8.87
43.18
11.69
-0.41
3.78
df
0.56
0.81
0.93
0.72
3-groupg
Heterosexuald
2,366
40.47
6.55
38.03
6.96
34.99
8.82
-2.44
2.81
Bisexual
46
43.34
7.07
40.90
8.51
37.84
11.32
-2.45
3.14
Lesbiane
16
44.11
8.03
43.70
8.87
43.18
11.69
-0.41
3.78
5-grouph
100% heterosexual
2,094
40.27
6.56
37.83
6.95
34.78
8.78
-2.44
2.80
Mostly heterosexual
272
42.04
6.24
39.61
6.84
36.57
8.95
-2.43
2.90
Bisexual
46
43.34
7.07
40.90
8.51
37.84
11.32
-2.45
3.14
Mostly lesbian
12
42.68
8.22
41.30
7.48
39.58
8.59
-1.37
3.40
100% lesbian
4
48.42
6.43
50.89
9.81
53.97
14.34
2.47
3.79
aLarger scores represent higher levels of male-typical behavior and/or lower levels of female-typical behavior.
bAbove zero: larger scores represent a larger increase in male-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in female-typical behavior; below zero:
smaller scores represent a larger increase in female-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in male-typical behavior.
cLinear regression with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/4 = .013, two-tailed) indicated a significant difference between heterosexual and lesbian
girls in the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 years, B = 5.66, SE = 1.75, p < .001, and at age 4.75 years, B = 8.19, SE = 2.22, p
< .001, and in the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = 2.02, SE = 0.71, p = .004.
dComprising 100% heterosexual and mostly heterosexual.
eComprising mostly lesbian and 100% lesbian.
fCohen’s d was pooled following Rubin’s (1987) rule. Similarly to Bailey and Zucker (1995), Hedges’s (1982; Formula 4) unbiasing correction
was applied.
gOrthogonal polynomial contrasts with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/8 = .006, two-tailed) indicated a significant linear effect of sexual
orientation groups in girls on the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 years, B = 4.01, SE = 1.24, p < .001, and at age 4.75 years, B =
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 39
!
5.79, SE = 1.58, p < .001, and on the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = 1.43, SE = 0.50, p = .004. No other significant
linear effects or any quadratic effects were detected, ps > .027 (see Table S1 for more information).
hOrthogonal polynomial contrasts with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/16 = .003, two-tailed) indicated a significant linear effect of sexual
orientation groups in girls on the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 years, B = 8.79, SE = 2.30, p = .001, and at age 4.75 years, B =
13.08, SE = 2.92, p < .001, and on the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = 3.44, SE = 0.93, p < .001. No other significant
linear effects or any quadratic, cubic, or quartic effects were detected, ps > .004 (see Table S1 for more information).
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 40
!
Table 4
Latent Growth Factors of Gender-Typed Behavior During Preschool Years by Sexual Orientation at Age 15 Years in Boys
Sexual orientation
n
Intercept at age 2.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 3.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 4.75
yearsa
Slopeb
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
2-groupc
Heterosexuald
2,117
59.90
6.42
61.56
6.47
63.63
7.53
1.66
2.24
Gaye
24
56.45
6.05
55.66
6.82
54.67
9.21
-0.79
3.04
df
-0.54
-0.91
-1.19
-1.09
3-groupg
Heterosexuald
2,117
59.90
6.42
61.56
6.47
63.63
7.53
1.66
2.24
Bisexual
28
57.34
8.70
57.12
9.32
56.84
11.56
-0.22
3.40
Gaye
24
56.45
6.05
55.66
6.82
54.67
9.21
-0.79
3.04
5-grouph
100% heterosexual
1,981
60.01
6.41
61.69
6.47
63.79
7.53
1.68
2.22
Mostly heterosexual
136
58.32
6.37
59.64
6.12
61.28
7.16
1.31
2.51
Bisexual
28
57.34
8.70
57.12
9.32
56.84
11.56
-0.22
3.40
Mostly gay
13
55.99
5.90
55.35
6.85
54.54
9.79
-0.65
3.47
100% gay
11
56.99
6.47
56.03
7.10
54.83
8.95
-0.96
2.60
aLarger scores represent higher levels of male-typical behavior and/or lower levels of female-typical behavior.
bAbove zero: larger scores represent a larger increase in male-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in female-typical behavior; below zero:
smaller scores represent a larger increase in female-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in male-typical behavior.
cLinear regression with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/4 = .013, two-tailed) indicated a significant difference between heterosexual and gay boys
in the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 2.50 years, B = -3.45, SE = 1.32, p = .009, at age 3.50 years, B = -5.90, SE = 1.33, p < .001, and
at age 4.75 years, B = 8.96, SE = 1.55, p < .001, and in the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = -2.45, SE = 0.46, p < .001.
dComprising 100% heterosexual and mostly heterosexual.
eComprising mostly gay and 100% gay.
fCohen’s d was pooled following Rubin’s (1987) rule. Similarly to Bailey and Zucker (1995), Hedges’s (1982; Formula 4) unbiasing correction
was applied.
gOrthogonal polynomial contrasts with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/8 = .006, two-tailed) indicated a significant linear effect of sexual
orientation groups in boys on the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 years, B = -4.17, SE = 0.95, p < .001, and at age 4.75 years, B =
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 41
!
-6.33, SE = 1.11, p < .001, and on the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = -1.73, SE = 0.33, p < .001. No other significant
linear effects or any quadratic effects were detected, ps > .009 (see Table S1 for more information).
hOrthogonal polynomial contrasts with Bonferroni correction (α = .05/16 = .003, two-tailed) indicated a significant linear effect of sexual
orientation groups in boys on the intercepts of gender-typed behavior at age 3.50 years, B = -4.94, SE = 1.38, p < .001, and at age 4.75 years, B =
-7.80, SE = 1.61, p < .001, and on the slope of gender-typed behavior during preschool years, B = -2.29, SE = 0.48, p < .001. No other significant
linear effects or any quadratic, cubic, or quartic effects were detected, ps > .053 (see Table S1 for more information).
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 42
!
Table 5
Latent Growth Factors of Gender-Typed Behavior During Preschool Years by Sexual Activities at Age 15 Years in Girls Who Had Sexual
Contacts
Sexual activity
n
Intercept at age 2.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 3.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 4.75
yearsa
Slopeb
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Hug
Exclusive other-sex contacts
173
40.26
6.95
37.74
6.89
34.58
8.16
-2.52
2.68
Any same-sex contacts
2,231
40.55
6.55
38.13
7.04
35.10
8.98
-2.42
2.85
d
0.04
0.06
0.06
0.04
Hold hands
Exclusive other-sex contacts
864
40.39
6.54
37.75
6.86
34.45
8.58
-2.64c
2.73
Any same-sex contacts
1,209
40.47
6.55
38.19
7.02
35.33
9.00
-2.28
2.90
d
0.01
0.06
0.09
0.13
Spend time alone
Exclusive other-sex contacts
476
40.13
6.63
37.55
6.99
34.33
8.76
-2.58
2.79
Any same-sex contacts
1,775
40.56
6.56
38.17
6.99
35.18
8.87
-2.39
2.82
d
0.07
0.09
0.10
0.06
Kiss on the mouth
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,483
40.15c
6.55
37.70c
6.87
34.64c
8.63
-2.45
2.79
Any same-sex contacts
366
41.80
6.72
39.55
7.43
36.74
9.68
-2.25
3.02
d
0.25
0.27
0.24
0.07
Be kissed on the mouth
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,486
40.34c
6.57
37.93c
6.95
34.92c
8.77
-2.41
2.81
Any same-sex contacts
266
41.49
6.78
39.31
7.32
36.59
9.40
-2.17
3.00
d
0.17
0.20
0.19
0.08
Cuddle
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,363
40.35
6.58
37.85
6.95
34.73
8.79
-2.50
2.84
Any same-sex contacts
565
40.76
6.40
38.44
7.06
35.54
9.20
-2.32
2.90
d
0.06
0.08
0.09
0.06
Lay down together
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 43
!
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,159
40.23c
6.58
37.83c
6.89
34.83
8.63
-2.40
2.77
Any same-sex contacts
365
41.32
6.28
38.89
6.83
35.85
8.83
-2.43
2.82
d
0.17
0.15
0.12
-0.01
Be touched under clothes
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,191
40.50
6.55
38.06
6.87
35.01
8.61
-2.44
2.77
Any same-sex contacts
50
42.07
8.05
39.82
9.15
37.00
11.89
-2.25
3.48
d
0.24
0.25
0.23
0.07
Touch under clothes
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,018
40.46
6.61
38.02c
6.88
34.97c
8.54
-2.44
2.73
Any same-sex contacts
49
42.51
8.03
41.03
9.26
39.18
12.25
-1.48
3.66
d
0.31
0.43
0.48
0.35
Be undressed with private parts
showing
Exclusive other-sex contacts
742
40.47c
6.56
38.02
6.87
34.96
8.56
-2.45
2.73
Any same-sex contacts
59
42.99
6.58
39.98
7.85
36.22
10.72
-3.01
3.28
d
0.39
0.28
0.14
-0.20
Touch or fondle private parts
Exclusive other-sex contacts
700
40.54c
6.62
38.09c
6.94
35.02
8.66
-2.46
2.77
Any same-sex contacts
26
44.06
7.15
41.93
9.07
39.25
12.47
-2.14
3.50
d
0.53
0.55
0.48
0.11
Private parts be touched or
fondled
Exclusive other-sex contacts
710
40.53c
6.54
38.04c
6.85
34.93
8.58
-2.49
2.76
Any same-sex contacts
25
44.93
7.44
42.41
9.05
39.25
12.14
-2.52
3.38
d
0.67
0.63
0.50
-0.01
Have oral sex
Exclusive other-sex contacts
570
40.65c
6.48
38.10c
6.78
34.91c
8.55
-2.55
2.80
Any same-sex contacts
11
47.40
8.70
45.18
11.80
42.40
16.49
-2.23
4.34
d
1.04
1.03
0.86
0.11
Have sexual intercourse
Exclusive other-sex contacts
483
40.56
6.57
38.06
6.79
34.93
8.43
-2.50
2.75
Any same-sex contacts
6
43.39
8.90
40.73
8.70
37.40
8.48
-2.66
0.45
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 44
!
d
0.43
0.39
0.29
-0.07
Note. Girls who did not engage in a given sexual activity or did not indicate the sex(es) of the partner(s) were excluded from the analysis.
Cohen’s d was pooled following Rubin’s (1987) rule. Similarly to Bailey and Zucker (1995), Hedges’s (1982; Formula 4) unbiasing correction
was applied.
aLarger scores represent higher levels of male-typical behavior and/or lower levels of female-typical behavior.
bAbove zero: larger scores represent a larger increase in male-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in female-typical behavior; below zero:
smaller scores represent a larger increase in female-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in male-typical behavior.
cValue significantly differs from that in the row immediately below, after Bonferroni correction (α = .05/4 = .013, two-tailed). See Table S2 for
more information.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 45
!
Table 6
Latent Growth Factors of Gender-Typed Behavior During Preschool Years by Sexual Activities at Age 15 Years in Boys Who Had Sexual
Contacts
Sexual activity
n
Intercept at age 2.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 3.50
yearsa
Intercept at age 4.75
yearsa
Slopeb
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Hug
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,121
59.85
6.67
61.45
6.77
63.45
7.89
1.60
2.29
Any same-sex contacts
949
59.85
6.27
61.51
6.37
63.60
7.57
1.66
2.32
d
0.00
0.01
0.02
0.03
Hold hands
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,531
60.10
6.54
61.74
6.59
63.78
7.67
1.64
2.27
Any same-sex contacts
161
59.36
6.42
60.79
6.83
62.57
8.51
1.43
2.60
d
-0.11
-0.14
-0.16
-0.09
Spend time alone
Exclusive other-sex contacts
920
60.40c
6.67
62.00c
6.77
64.00c
7.79
1.60
2.17
Any same-sex contacts
927
59.41
6.34
61.02
6.48
63.02
7.76
1.60
2.39
d
-0.15
-0.15
-0.13
0.00
Kiss on the mouth
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,448
60.29c
6.50
61.95c
6.53
64.01c
7.58
1.65
2.24
Any same-sex contacts
69
58.21
5.81
59.35
6.32
60.78
8.62
1.14
3.07
d
-0.32
-0.40
-0.42
-0.22
Be kissed on the mouth
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,361
60.37c
6.46
62.02c
6.50
64.07c
7.53
1.64
2.22
Any same-sex contacts
65
58.62
5.95
59.77
6.76
61.22
9.35
1.16
3.21
d
-0.27
-0.34
-0.37
-0.21
Cuddle
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,466
60.11
6.49
61.72
6.58
63.73c
7.69
1.61
2.27
Any same-sex contacts
104
59.18
6.26
60.23
6.65
61.55
8.67
1.06
2.96
d
-0.14
-0.22
-0.28
-0.24
Lay down together
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 46
!
Exclusive other-sex contacts
1,048
60.55
6.54
62.17c
6.54
64.20c
7.48
1.62
2.17
Any same-sex contacts
82
58.95
6.30
60.11
7.14
61.56
9.60
1.16
3.10
d
-0.24
-0.31
-0.34
-0.21
Be touched under clothes
Exclusive other-sex contacts
843
60.69c
6.61
62.35c
6.65
64.42c
7.60
1.65c
2.14
Any same-sex contacts
34
57.40
4.74
57.74
5.47
58.16
8.79
0.33
3.67
d
-0.50
-0.70
-0.82
-0.60
Touch under clothes
Exclusive other-sex contacts
849
60.58c
6.59
62.24c
6.61
64.33c
7.54
1.67c
2.13
Any same-sex contacts
34
57.41
4.59
57.73
5.76
58.14
9.51
0.32
3.87
d
-0.49
-0.69
-0.81
-0.61
Be undressed with private parts
showing
Exclusive other-sex contacts
544
60.84
6.76
62.52
6.80
64.62c
7.75
1.68c
2.15
Any same-sex contacts
38
59.43
5.75
60.02
7.18
60.75
10.51
0.59
3.56
d
-0.21
-0.37
-0.49
-0.48
Touch or fondle private parts
Exclusive other-sex contacts
513
61.01c
6.78
62.73
6.89
64.87c
7.90
1.72c
2.16
Any same-sex contacts
26
57.41
4.88
56.79
5.39
56.01
8.49
-0.62
3.60
d
-0.54
-0.87
-1.12
-1.04
Private parts be touched or
fondled
Exclusive other-sex contacts
498
61.07
6.72
62.78c
6.81
64.91c
7.78
1.71c
2.13
Any same-sex contacts
26
59.11
6.64
57.91
7.23
56.42
9.94
-1.19
3.59
d
-0.29
-0.70
-1.05
-1.26
Have oral sex
Exclusive other-sex contacts
413
61.30c
6.75
62.94c
6.79
64.99c
7.70
1.64c
2.11
Any same-sex contacts
22
57.34
4.57
56.58
5.47
55.63
8.88
-0.76
3.66
d
-0.60
-0.95
-1.21
-1.09
Have sexual intercourse
Exclusive other-sex contacts
322
61.19
6.82
62.78c
6.93
64.76c
7.94
1.59c
2.17
Any same-sex contacts
8
60.07
4.66
58.17
7.14
55.81
12.33
-1.89
4.78
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 47
!
d
-0.17
-0.66
-1.11
-1.54
Note. Boys who did not engage in a given sexual activity or did not indicate the sex(es) of the partner(s) were excluded from the analysis.
Cohen’s d was pooled following Rubin’s (1987) rule. Similarly to Bailey and Zucker (1995), Hedges’s (1982; Formula 4) unbiasing correction
was applied.
aLarger scores represent higher levels of male-typical behavior and/or lower levels of female-typical behavior.
bAbove zero: larger scores represent a larger increase in male-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in female-typical behavior; below zero:
smaller scores represent a larger increase in female-typical behavior and/or a larger decrease in male-typical behavior.
cValue significantly differs from that in the row immediately below, after Bonferroni correction (α = .05/4 = .013, two-tailed). See Table S2 for
more information.
Running head: GENDER-TYPED BEHAVIOR AND SEXUAL ORIENTATION 48
!
Figure 1. Frequency distributions of the levels of gender-typed behavior at preschool years by 2-group sexual orientation at age 15 years.
Within-group percentages are presented. Levels of gender-typed behavior were estimated using the Preschool Activity Inventory (PSAI;
Golombok & Rust, 1993a, 1993b). Larger PSAI intercepts indicate more male-typical behavior and/or less female-typical behavior.
... Conversely, CSAB in males involves an interest in female-typical toys, a preference for girls as playmates, an interest in female-typical pretend play, and a disinterest or aversion to male-typical play. CSAB is considered an early psychodevelopmental correlate of same-sex sexual orientation, which can be expressed as early as 2.5 years of age (Li et al., 2017). ...
... For example, raters of childhood home movies who were unaware of the participants' sexual orientation assessed the behavior of cisgender androphilic males and cisgender gynephilic females as more sex-atypical than that of cisgender gynephilic males and cisgender androphilic females, respectively (Rieger et al., 2008;Watts et al., 2018). Furthermore, using Euro-American samples, communitybased prospective studies undertaken in the last decade have clearly linked CSAB in male and female children with an elevated probability of androphilia and gynephilia later in life, respectively (Li et al., 2017;Steensma, van der Ende, et al., 2013;Xu et al., 2019). ...
... In addition to the co-occurrence of CSAB with female gynephilia and male androphilia, there is evidence showing a positive correlation between CSAB and degree of same-sex attraction in both males and females. Two community-based prospective studies have reported that expression of CSAB was associated with increased probability of ambiphilia in adolescence and adulthood for both sexes (Li et al., 2017;Xu et al., 2019; but see Steensma, van der Ende, et al., 2013). Likewise, Rieger et al. (2020) examined the relation between recalled CSAB and sexual attraction in a large sample of European/North American cisgender ambiphilic participants and found that as degrees of same-sex sexual attraction increased (i.e., along the Kinsey scale; Kinsey et al., 1948), the levels of recalled CSAB also increased. ...
Article
Same-sex sexual attraction in both males and females has been associated with childhood sex-atypical behavior. Gynephilic females recall behaving in a manner that is less female-typical and more male-typical compared to cisgender androphilic females, whereas androphilic males recall behaving in a manner that is less male-typical and more female-typical compared to cisgender gynephilic males. In addition, male and female ambiphilic individuals exhibit intermediate levels of childhood sex-atypicality. In this study, we examined recalled childhood sex-typed behavior among Iranian cisgender gynephilic males (n = 236), cisgender ambiphilic males (n = 51), cisgender androphilic males (n = 191), transgender androphilic males (n = 60), cisgender androphilic females (n = 243), cisgender ambiphilic females (n = 96), cisgender gynephilic females (n = 32), and transgender gynephilic females (n = 122). Both cisgender androphilic males and cisgender gynephilic females recalled elevated childhood sex-atypicality, scoring intermediate between cisgender gynephilic males and cisgender androphilic females. Male and female ambiphilic participants scored intermediate between their other- and same-sex attracted cisgender counterparts. Transgender androphilic males exhibited hyper-feminized childhood behavior, scoring even more female-typical than cisgender androphilic females. Transgender gynephilic females recalled behaving as male-typical as cisgender gynephilic males. Consistent with previous research, our findings from Iran - a non-Western, Middle Eastern culture - provide cross-cultural support for the universality of childhood sex-atypicality as a normative developmental precursor of monosexual or ambisexual same-sex attraction in both males and females who are cisgender and transgender.
... Figure obtained from Abé et al. (2021b) The picture that emerges from these studies is that in some brain areas homosexual males tend to be more female-typical, and homosexual females tend to be more maletypical (Abé et al., 2021b). This is known as a cross-sex shift, which is also found in various behavioral traits (Allen & Robson, 2020;Bailey et al., 2016;Li et al., 2017;Xu et al., 2017). This cross-sex-shift was confirmed in the largest brain imaging-genetics study to date, which included brain scans of 18,645 individuals (Abé et al., 2021b) (Fig. 3). ...
... Non-heterosexual individuals repeatedly displayed structural and functional brain features that are more similar to that of their opposite sex heterosexual counterpart (cross-sex shift), predominantly in visuo-sensory brain areas (Abé et al., 2021b). If the observed brain differences and their underlying mechanisms are linked to prior manifestations is unclear, but, considering the cross-sex shifts observed in brain structure, function, and behavioral traits (Allen & Robson, 2020;Bailey et al., 2016;Li et al., 2017;Xu et al., 2017), it is reasonable to entertain the idea that a cross-sex shift in sexual prior content may be present. While a complete sexual prior 'switch' could well explain the observation that homosexual individuals are attracted to people of same sex, in individuals attracted to both sexes, a sexual prior 'shift' may result in sexual priors containing both male and female featural information. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sexuality is an integral part of human nature, yet we know little about its origins and underlying mechanisms. Understanding how the brain processes sexual stimuli is crucial for advancing our knowledge of the origins of sexuality and its variations. Only if a stimulus was internally evaluated as ‘attractive’ the information can progress to evoke sexual desire or arousal. Importantly, such evaluation processes require an internal reference against which external information is compared. These processes, however, remain largely unexplored, and hence we still do not understand why we are attracted to what we are attracted to. By synthesizing the existing literature and integrating existing models, this narrative review proposes a novel neuroscientific framework on sexuality, addressing key theoretical gaps. As a result, the concept of sexual priors in the brain is introduced. Within this framework, sexual priors refer to mental ‘images’ representing a collection of internally stored information of what we regard as sexually attractive. Such mental representations act as reference in early internal evaluation processes of sexual stimuli. It is suggested that sexual priors are (partly) innate and sex-specific, contributing to heterosexuality. Evolutionary aspects that could explain the development of these priors are discussed. Furthermore, the framework provides possible explanations for variations in sexual orientation through potential cross-sex shifts in sexual prior content. The refinement of sexual prior content over time may explain the diversity of sexual interests and attraction patterns among humans. Moreover, the concept can be applied to a variety of psychiatric conditions related to atypical and maladaptive sexual behaviors. A proposition is put forward regarding brain areas and networks that may be involved in the formation of sexual priors, serving as potential targets for future research. In summary, by integrating sexual priors into models of sexual stimuli processing, this article fills theoretical gaps while inspiring further research. This work aims to enhance understanding of sexuality, which ultimately could increase social awareness, foster tolerance, and promote psychological well-being.
... [1][2][3] This finding is robustly found in both retrospective and prospective designs. [1][2][3][4] Longitudinal research has also found that girls displayed less gender nonconformity from ages 2 to 5 years, but the change rates in gender nonconformity from ages 2 to 5 years among bisexual and lesbian girls were lower than that in heterosexual girls, and bisexual and gay boys became more gender nonconforming from ages 2 to 5 years compared with heterosexual boys. 3,4 Childhood gender nonconformity may have important implications for sexual orientation disparities in psychopathology. ...
... [1][2][3][4] Longitudinal research has also found that girls displayed less gender nonconformity from ages 2 to 5 years, but the change rates in gender nonconformity from ages 2 to 5 years among bisexual and lesbian girls were lower than that in heterosexual girls, and bisexual and gay boys became more gender nonconforming from ages 2 to 5 years compared with heterosexual boys. 3,4 Childhood gender nonconformity may have important implications for sexual orientation disparities in psychopathology. Elevated childhood gender nonconformity displayed by ...
Article
Purpose: This study tested whether sexual orientation disparities in depressive symptoms are partially explained by recalled childhood gender nonconformity and whether the proportion of this association explained by childhood gender nonconformity is moderated by recalled parental attitudes toward childhood gender nonconformity. Methods: A convenience sample of young adults was recruited from two Chinese online survey platforms (272 heterosexual males, 272 bisexual males, 272 gay males, 272 heterosexual females, 272 bisexual females, and 272 lesbian females). Both mediation and moderated mediation models were conducted. Results: For both sexes, bisexual and gay/lesbian individuals reported significantly higher levels of depressive symptoms than heterosexual individuals, with total effects (standardized path coefficients) ranging from 0.25 to 0.38, all ps < 0.01. These sexual orientation disparities in depressive symptoms were partially explained by childhood gender nonconformity, with indirect effects ranging from 0.08 to 0.17, all ps < 0.001. The effect of childhood gender nonconformity on depressive symptoms was significantly moderated by parental attitudes. The mediating effect of childhood gender nonconformity on sexual orientation disparities in depressive symptoms was strongest at the more negative levels (one standard deviation [SD] above the mean) of parental attitudes and weakest at more tolerant levels (one SD below the mean) of parental attitudes. Conclusions: Childhood gender nonconformity may be a partial contributor to sexual orientation disparities in depressive symptoms and this indirect effect may be moderated by parental attitudes toward childhood gender nonconformity, with the indirect effect decreasing when parental attitudes move from negative toward more tolerant levels.
... Some evidence suggests that the stereotype that gender atypicality is associated with non-heterosexuality may not be entirely unfounded. Homosexual men tend to be more feminine and homosexual women more masculine than their heterosexual counterparts, specifically, gender nonconformity in childhood behaviors (Bailey & Zucker, 1995;Li et al., 2017;Rieger et al., 2008;Xu et al., 2019) and childhood photographs (Watts et al., 2018) predicts non-heterosexuality in adulthood. Some research has even indicated that the gender typicality of certain facial features (e.g., the nose or forehead shape) is associated with homosexuality (Skorska et al., 2015), and can be used by others to infer sexual orientation (González-Álvarez, 2017;González-Álvarez & Sos-Peña, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
People use cues of facial gender typicality when making sexual orientation judgements, where gender typical faces (masculine men and feminine women) are more likely to be judged as heterosexual and gender atypical faces (feminine men and masculine women) are judged as non-heterosexual. Individual differences in the belief of associated stereotypes have been shown to influence how these stereotypes are used to make social judgments of others; therefore, across two studies, we tested whether the strength of beliefs in gender stereotypes impacted how facial gender typicality cues were used when making sexual orientation judgements. In both Study 1 and 2 (n = 283 and 219, respectively), participants made sexual orientation judgements of 80 faces (40 male, 40 female) that varied in gender typicality and completed a measure of belief in gender stereotypes. In Study 2, participants also completed a sexual prejudice measure. In line with predictions, both studies found that the strength in belief of gender stereotypes significantly moderated the use of facial gender typicality cues when making sexual orientation judgements. Participants with a greater belief in gender stereotypes were more likely to judge a face as heterosexual as gender typicality increased. In Study 2, the association between sexual prejudice and use of gender typicality cues was fully mediated by beliefs in gender role stereotypes. These results highlight the importance of considering individual differences of the perceiver and how they can interact with cues from a target, particularly when making sexual orientation judgements.
... At 15 years, children indicated their attraction to others on a scale ranging from 100% heterosexual to 100% homosexual. Lesbians and gay adolescents had been rated earlier by their caregivers as having greater gender atypicality, with a larger effect size for boys (Li et al., 2017;Xu et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper integrates evidence across a range of research programs to make the case that gender nonconformity and sexual orientation are interconnected in a way that reflects a degree of reality. One body of research concerns the accurate judgments of sexual orientation of adult targets based on nonverbal gendered cues, focusing on targets’ spontaneous presentation of the self that includes cues from body shape, movement, the face, and the voice. A second body of research examines the perceived gender atypicality of child targets who later come out as lesbian, gay, or bisexual (LGB) in adulthood. This research points to growing evidence concerning the reality of gender atypicality for members of the LGB community on the aggregate. The scope of this reality, as well as implications for the LGB community, are discussed.
... When examining developmental correlates of sexual orientation, the importance of childhood sex-typed behavior cannot be overstated. Studies conducted across different cultures using various methodologies have consistently found, in both males and females, a large association between degree of childhood sex-atypicality and degree of same-sex attraction irrespective of gender identity/presentation (Bailey & Zucker, 1995;Bartlett & Vasey, 2006;Cardoso, 2009;Gómez Jiménez et al., 2020;Li et al., 2017;Rieger et al., 2008Rieger et al., , 2020Roshan et al., 2019;Semenyna et al., 2017;Whitam & Mathy, 1986, 1991Xu & Rahman, 2024;Xu et al., 2019). In light of this, a regression analysis indicated that more male-typical childhood behavior predicted higher MF-Occ or more maletypical occupational preferences (Prediction 5). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research suggests that both same-sex attraction and the personality trait “openness” are associated with sex-atypical preferences and behaviors. Here, we examined the links between adulthood occupational preferences, childhood play behavior, and openness among Iranian cisgender gynephilic males (n = 228), cisgender ambiphilic males (n = 48), cisgender androphilic males (n = 178), transgender androphilic males (n = 58), cisgender androphilic females (n = 226), cisgender ambiphilic females (n = 94), cisgender gynephilic females (n = 31), and transgender gynephilic females (n = 121) from Iran. Cisgender and transgender same-sex attracted males and females exhibited sex-atypical occupational preferences with the latter group showing even more sex-atypicality than the former. The personality trait openness did not differ between cisgender groups. Transgender androphilic males had a significantly higher mean score for openness compared to cisgender androphilic females and transgender gynephilic females, whereas transgender gynephilic females had a significantly lower mean score compared to cisgender androphilic males. In both males and females, childhood sex-atypicality, same-sex attraction, and openness were associated with sex-atypical occupational preferences. Our findings from Iran provides cross-cultural support for interconnectedness of childhood and adulthood sex-atypicality, openness, and same-sex attraction in males and females who are cisgender and transgender.
... Also, in population-based samples associations between gender-typical behavior in childhood and sexual orientation are found. For example, in one population-based study, children with gender nonconformity regarding play behavior at ages 3-5, were more likely to report a bisexual or gay sexual orientation later in life (Li et al., 2017). The association between gender variance and homosexual orientation in adulthood was reported to be less strong in general population samples compared to groups of individuals clinically referred for gender dysphoria (Steensma et al., 2013b). ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is an important period for the development of gender identity. We studied the development of gender non-contentedness, i.e., unhappiness with being the gender aligned with one’s sex, from early adolescence to young adulthood, and its association with self-concept, behavioral and emotional problems, and adult sexual orientation. Participants were 2772 adolescents (53% male) from the Tracking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey population and clinical cohort. Data from six waves were included (ages 11–26). Gender non-contentedness was assessed with the item “I wish to be of the opposite sex” from the Youth and Adult Self-Report at all six waves. Behavioral and emotional problems were measured by total scores of these scales at all six waves. Self-concept was assessed at age 11 using the Global Self-Worth and Physical Appearance subscales of the Self-Perception Profile for Children. Sexual orientation was assessed at age 22 by self-report. In early adolescence, 11% of participants reported gender non-contentedness. The prevalence decreased with age and was 4% at the last follow-up (around age 26). Three developmental trajectories of gender non-contentedness were identified: no gender non-contentedness (78%), decreasing gender non-contentedness (19%), and increasing gender non-contentedness (2%). Individuals with an increasing gender non-contentedness more often were female and both an increasing and decreasing trajectory were associated with a lower global self-worth, more behavioral and emotional problems, and a non-heterosexual sexual orientation. Gender non-contentedness, while being relatively common during early adolescence, in general decreases with age and appears to be associated with a poorer self-concept and mental health throughout development.
Article
Full-text available
Biological mechanisms proposed to play a role in the development of sexual orientation in men include hormonal, genetic, and immunological factors. The posited roles of these factors are not mutually exclusive; instead, they may be at play to different degrees in different individuals. Direct measurement of these influences is challenging; thus, researchers rely on putative markers. We collected data on five well-established markers in a sample of gay and heterosexual men. We then (1) compared the levels of those markers in gay and straight men, (2) identified latent profiles based on those markers, and (3) compared the proportions of gay and straight men within the profiles. Gay men reported less gender conformity in childhood, a higher proportion of older brothers, were more right-handed, had more non-heterosexual relatives, and had more feminized digit ratios. Of the six identified profiles, the most numerous, containing a significantly higher proportion of straight men, had masculine digit ratios, masculine behavior in childhood, and was the most right-handed. Proportions of gay and straight men did not differ in the profile with the most feminine digit ratio, the profile associated with the highest proportion of older brothers, and the profile associated with left-handedness. Two remaining profiles, associated with familiality, and the most feminine childhood gender behaviors, consisted predominantly of gay men. The study suggests that further investigations of differences within sexual orientation categories are warranted.
Article
Full-text available
Ongoing political controversies around the world exemplify a long-standing and widespread preoccupation with the acceptability of homosexuality. Nonheterosexual people have seen dramatic surges both in their rights and in positive public opinion in many Western countries. In contrast, in much of Africa, the Middle East, the Caribbean, Oceania, and parts of Asia, homosexual behavior remains illegal and severely punishable, with some countries retaining the death penalty for it. Political controversies about sexual orientation have often overlapped with scientific controversies. That is, participants on both sides of the sociopolitical debates have tended to believe that scientific findings—and scientific truths—about sexual orientation matter a great deal in making political decisions. The most contentious scientific issues have concerned the causes of sexual orientation—that is, why are some people heterosexual, others bisexual, and others homosexual? The actual relevance of these issues to social, political, and ethical decisions is often poorly justified, however.
Article
Full-text available
Sex-typed behavior in childhood is significantly related to sexual orientation in adulthood. In addition, same-sex attractions in early adolescence are more non-exclusive than in adulthood and can differ from later same-sex orientations. However, little research has focused on romantic attractions as they emerge during early adolescence. Drawing a sample from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (197 girls, 204 boys), the current study examined whether same-sex romantic attractions at age 13 years were exclusive, and whether they were predicted by sex-typed behavior at age 3.5 years. No young adolescents in this sample reported exclusive same-sex attractions, and increased same-sex attractions were not significantly related to reduced other-sex sexualities. Childhood sex-typed behavior did not significantly predict early same-sex attractions, suggesting that early same-sex attractions differ from later same-sex orientations. The current study highlights the importance of studying the development of sexuality beginning prior to adulthood. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0726-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
Scant research exists on the development of mostly heterosexual identity, the largest sexual orientation minority subgroup. We used longitudinal latent class analysis to characterize the patterns of identification with lesbian, gay, bisexual (LGB), or mostly heterosexual identities from ages 12 to 23 in 13,859 youth (57% female) in a U.S. national cohort. Three classes emerged: completely heterosexual (88.2%), mostly heterosexual (9.5%), and LGB (2.4%). LGB class youth generally identified with sexual minority identities by ages 12–17. In contrast, mostly heterosexual class youth identified with sexual minority identities gradually, with steady increases in endorsement starting at the age of 14. Developmental implications of these differential patterns are discussed.
Article
Bailey et al. (2016) have provided an excellent, state-of-the-art overview that is a major contribution to our understanding of sexual orientation. However, whereas Bailey and his coauthors have examined the physiological, behavioral, and self-report data of sexual orientation and see categories, I see a sexual and romantic continuum. After noting several objections concerning the limitations of the review and methodological shortcomings characteristic of sexual-orientation research in general, I present evidence from research investigating in-between sexualities to support an alternative, continuum-based perspective regarding the nature of sexual orientation for both women and men. A continuum conceptualization has potential implications for investigating the prevalence of nonheterosexuals, sexual-orientation differences in gender nonconformity, causes of sexual orientation, and political issues.
Article
Experimental research in non-human mammals indicates that testosterone exposure during early periods of rapid brain development has enduring influences on brain and behaviour. These influences are exerted when testosterone is higher in developing males than females, and the affected characteristics are those that differ by sex. Testosterone is higher in males than in females from about weeks 8 to 24 of human gestation and then again during early infancy, and both of these periods are times of rapid brain development. Substantial evidence suggests that testosterone prenatally influences human neurobehavioral development. Emerging evidence suggests that the early postnatal period is important too. This early postnatal period could provide a window for studying testosterone interacting with experience to shape human gender development.
Article
Today's lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT) youth come out at younger ages, and public support for LGBT issues has dramatically increased, so why do LGBT youth continue to be at high risk for compromised mental health? We provide an overview of the contemporary context for LGBT youth, followed by a review of current science on LGBT youth mental health. Research in the past decade has identified risk and protective factors for mental health, which point to promising directions for prevention, intervention, and treatment. Legal and policy successes have set the stage for advances in programs and practices that may foster LGBT youth mental health. Implications for clinical care are discussed, and important areas for new research and practice are identified. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Clinical Psychology Volume 12 is March 28, 2016. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.