ArticlePDF Available

'The Three Good Things' – The effects of gratitude practice on wellbeing: A randomised controlled trial

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: There are mixed findings concerning the effectiveness of gratitude interventions. This study investigated a commonly used gratitude-based intervention (‘count-your-blessings’) in promoting physical and psychological wellbeing in a UK sample. Methods: A randomised controlled trial to examine the efficacy of a three-week gratitude intervention in promoting psychological and physical health. 108 healthy participants (aged 18–36) were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 conditions (gratitude or no-assigned activity). Those in the gratitude condition kept daily gratitude journals for 21 days. Participants completed self-report measures related to gratefulness, affect, prosociality, physical and subjective wellbeing. These measures were collected at two time-points (pre- and post-intervention), three weeks apart. Two separate analyses were conducted for participants who completed the intervention (Completer analysis) and including those who dropped out (Intention-to-treat analysis [ITT]). Findings: Completer analysis revealed that the gratitude condition resulted in heightened feelings of gratefulness. Those in the grateful condition showed greater increases in state gratitude and positive affect, relative to the control condition, who reported a reduction in wellbeing. Overall, the ITT analysis yielded essentially the same findings as the completer analysis. Discussion: Counting one’s blessings can improve wellbeing and could be incorporated in existing psychotherapies. This brief and simple intervention warrants further investigation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
10 Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017
Research Paper
‘The Three Good Things’ – The effects of
gratitude practice on wellbeing:
A randomised controlled trial
Siew Tim Lai & Ronan E. O’Carroll
Background: There are mixed ndings concerning the effectiveness of gratitude interventions. This study
investigated a commonly used gratitude-based intervention (‘count-your-blessings’) in promoting physical
and psychological wellbeing in a UK sample.
Methods: A randomised controlled trial to examine the efcacy of a three-week gratitude intervention in
promoting psychological and physical health. 108 healthy participants (aged 18–36) were randomly assigned
to 1 of 2 conditions (gratitude or no-assigned activity). Those in the gratitude condition kept daily gratitude
journals for 21 days. Participants completed self-report measures related to gratefulness, affect, prosociality,
physical and subjective wellbeing. These measures were collected at two time-points (pre- and post-intervention),
three weeks apart. Two separate analyses were conducted for participants who completed the intervention
(Completer analysis) and including those who dropped out (Intention-to-treat analysis [ITT]).
Findings: Completer analysis revealed that the gratitude condition resulted in heightened feelings of
gratefulness. Those in the grateful condition showed greater increases in state gratitude and positive affect,
relative to the control condition, who reported a reduction in wellbeing. Overall, the ITT analysis yielded
essentially the same ndings as the completer analysis.
Discussion: Counting one’s blessings can improve wellbeing and could be incorporated in existing
psychotherapies. This brief and simple intervention warrants further investigation.
Keywords: Gratitude; positive affect; wellbeing; health; positive psychology interventions.
Introduction
Studies that aimed to promote wellbeing have
shown that individuals with positive mental
health hold numerous physical, social and intel-
lectual benets compared to their unhappy
counterparts (Fredrickson, 2001). This
increased emphasis on positive phenomena
has led the United Nation General Assembly
to review the approaches to systematic meas-
urements of happiness at the individual and
national level (Layard, 2013). Although there
are various ways to increase wellbeing, the culti-
vation of gratitude is thought to be the quin-
tessential positive personality trait, allowing
individuals to lead a ourishing life (Wood et
al., 2009).
Conceptualisation of gratitude
Gratitude is considered as both trait
and state. State gratitude is the feeling
of awe, gratefulness and appreciation for
outcomes received (McCullough et al.,
2002). Emmons (2007) characterizes these
experiences as acute, intense and co-occur-
ring with joyful feelings. Trait gratitude is
described as an individual’s predisposition
to experience this state in life (McCullough
et al., 2002).
Theoretically, being thankful in life is
expected to be strongly associated to well-
being. This is explained in the schematic
hypothesis (Wood et al., 2010), which claims
that grateful individuals are more likely to
Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017 11
‘The Three Good Things’ – The effects of gratitude practice on wellbeing: A randomised controlled trial
notice the positivity in life, and this inu-
ences how they interpret their life events
(Wood et al., 2010). Watkins (2004) elabo-
rated that in times of adversity, gratitude
shifts one’s mindset of deprivation to appre-
ciation; this particular interpretative lens
enhances subjective wellbeing. Drawn from
Fredrickson’s (2001) broaden-and-build theory,
positive emotions like gratitude broadens
through thought-action repertoires which
builds a person’s psychosocial and spiritual
resources. The broadening hypothesis is
compatible with aforementioned accounts
of the mechanisms linking gratitude to well-
being. As Fredrickson (2001) suggested, grat-
itude facilitates ‘meaning-nding’ in ordinary
events and within the adversity itself. This
may fuel psychological resilience, leading
to appreciable increases in emotional well-
being over time (Kashdan & Rottenberg,
2010). Although emerging perspectives
complementing this notion have highlighted
the inter-intrapersonal benets of gratitude
(McCullough et al., 2001), the effects of
gratitude on human health identied thus
far are likely just the tip of the proverbial
iceberg.
Empirical research on gratitude and
wellbeing
Emmons and McCullough (2003) employed
a self-guided journal method to determine
the effect of gratitude on wellbeing among
college students. Their ndings revealed
that those who recorded grateful thoughts
had positive results (i.e. increased in posi-
tive emotions, altruistic behaviour and fewer
physical complaints). Grateful contempla-
tion reduces excessive worrying and levels
of depression over time (Wood et al., 2007).
Whilst other research demonstrated grateful
individuals reported less post-traumatic
symptoms when compared to their less
grateful counterparts following a stressful
life-event (Wood et al., 2010).
A meta-analysis has shown that the impact
of gratitude practice on wellbeing and its
efcacy is moderated by various factors
(Wood et al., 2010). For instance, previous
control groups used in gratitude interven-
tion involved downward social comparison,
writing about earliest memories, and listing
hassles. The diverse control conditions may
not generate equal expectancy effects as the
gratitude condition. This makes it problem-
atic to infer the effectiveness of the interven-
tion. Moreover, some studies did not explore
whether gratitude was successfully cultivated
(Sin et al., 2009) leaving doubts to its posi-
tive changes.
To explore these issues, we replicated
Emmons and McCullough’s (2003) study.
We included validated scales (e.g. the Grati-
tude Adjectives Checklist [GAC] and Grati-
tude Dispositional Scales [GQ-6]) to track
the changes in gratitude. A control group
without any assigned activity was used to
minimise expectancy effects, allowing us to
gauge the true effect of the intervention.
We utilised both completer and intention-
to-treat analysis (ITT). Given that the inter-
vention will not be effective for everyone,
we also explored the relationship between
effort and time taken to perform the inter-
vention and outcome.
Predictions
We predict that a grateful contemplation
would result in higher levels of state and
trait gratitude, positive affect and wellbeing.
Participants in the gratitude condition would
score higher on the perception of inter-
personal quality and report fewer physical
complaints compared to participants in the
control condition.
Methods
Design
A three-week experimental randomised
controlled trial (RCT) was employed to
address the research aim. The independent
variable comprised of two experimental
conditions: (1) Gratitude (participants
assigned with gratitude-inducing activity)
and (2) Control (no assigned task). Both
groups were measured at two time points,
12 Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017
Siew Tim Lai & Ronan E. O’Carroll
three weeks apart: (a) Pre-test (prior to inter-
vention); (b) Post-test (immediately after
intervention).
Participants
Eligible participants were students from a
university in the UK, age range 18–21, able to
read and write English; recruited via online
advertisement and the face-to-face approach.
In an effort to minimise unintended vari-
ation in the procedure, both approaches
utilised standard instructions that partici-
pants read themselves.
The study was advertised as ‘A Study on
Human Emotions’ to mask the trial hypoth-
eses. The face-to-face method contains a
set of questionnaires and consent form in
a sealed envelope which were distributed
randomly in the campus. Those who agreed
to participate then signed the consent form
and returned the required documents to
the investigator. For the online recruitment,
interested candidates commenced the study
via a web-link, completion of baseline meas-
ures was considered as informed consent.
A total 108 participants (62 females, 46
males) were recruited and randomised into
either the gratitude (N=51) or the control
(N=57) condition. The average age was 22
years (SD=2.85), with over 65 per cent unem-
ployed university graduates. Almost half
of the sample was White (53.7 per cent),
and the rest was Asia/Pacic Islander (41.7
per cent). The nal analysis included 36
in the gratitude condition and 45 in the
control group, 27 participants (gratitude=15,
control=12) dropped out from the study.
Measures
At baseline participants were asked to provide
demographic information, including age,
sex, education, employment status, and
ethnicity. Participants completed additional
measures relating to gratitude, affect, well-
being, health complaints and perceived
quality of interpersonal relationship at base-
line and 21 days after intervention. All meas-
ures were administered online.
State and dispositional gratitude (GAC
and GQ-6) (McCullough et al., 2002). The
Grateful Adjectives Checklist consists of
three adjectives (appreciative, thankful, and
grateful) to examine participant’s feelings of
state gratefulness on a 5-point Likert scale
(1=not at all, 5=extremely). Scores range
from 3 to 15, higher scores indicates higher
levels of state gratitude.
The Gratitude Dispositional Scales
assesses an individual’s disposition to expe-
rience this state. It has six descriptive items
that is scored on a 7-point Likert scale
(1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree).
Scores range from 6 to 42.
Positive and negative affect scale
(PANAS) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1998).
The PANAS includes 20 affective states rated
on a 5-point Likert scale (1=very slightly,
5=extremely). Scores range from 10 to 50
and higher scores indicate higher levels of
positive or negative emotions experienced.
Warwick-Edinburgh mental wellbeing
scale (WEMWBS) (Tennant el al., 2007). The
WEMWBS is a 14-item questionnaire that
measures mental wellbeing on a 5-point
Likert scale (1=none of the time, 5=all of
the time). Scores vary from 14 to 70, higher
scores indicates higher levels of mental well-
being.
Physical symptoms checklist (Emmons &
McCullough, 2003). A list of 13 physical symp-
toms (e.g. runny/congested nose, headache
and etc.) requires participants to indicate
whether they had experienced these symp-
toms for the past two weeks. Participant’s
perceived health status was obtained by
summing up the symptoms experienced.
Quality of relationship with signicant
others (Martinez-Marti et al., 2010). Four
items rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1=not
at all, 4=a great deal) was used to assess
the participant’s quality of relationship with
close others.
An exploratory effort and motivation form
was given to participants at post-intervention.
The form included questions developed by
Odou et al. (2013) to assess participant’s
Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017 13
‘The Three Good Things’ – The effects of gratitude practice on wellbeing: A randomised controlled trial
effort (e.g. on average how many minutes
did it take you to complete your activity?)
and motivation (e.g. how motivated did you
feel about doing the activity?) on a 5-point
Likert scale (1=not at all, 5=extremely).
To measure adherence for the assigned
activity, participants were asked to answer a
question on a 7-point Likert scale (1=never,
7=daily). The question was ‘We understand
that at some point there are circumstances that
cause you to be unable to carry out the assigned
activity as intended. We appreciate your honesty to
indicate throughout these 21 days, how often you
think you actually did the assigned activity.’
Procedure
Participants who provided consent and
completed baseline measures were assigned
an identication number that can only be
identied by the researcher. They were then
randomised to the experimental condi-
tions using a computer software package
(Urbaniak & Plous, 2013). After randomi-
sation, specic instructions were given to
participants according to their respective
groups. Participants in both conditions were
contacted by mail before, during and after
21 days of the intervention to complete their
online measures.
The gratitude condition received an
instruction to cultivate gratefulness for
21 days. The information to perform the
gratitude-inducing activity incorporated
instructions from both studies (Emmons
& McCullough, 2003; Martinez-Marti et al.,
2010). The instructions were:
There are many things in our lives, both large
and small, that we might consider as a form of
blessing. It could even be those who help us to reach
our goals, or just make our lives easier with small
details. If we try to put ourselves in their shoes,
appreciate their efforts, and notice the voluntary
nature of their acts, we have a good reason to feel
grateful. Please think of today and write down
three things in your life that you are grateful for.’
They were required to record their
daily grateful thoughts in a journal that was
provided. To encourage adherence they
were invited to submit their journal on an
appointed date after post-test. Participants in
the control condition were not assigned any
activity, but to complete the same measures
given to the gratitude condition.
Analytic plan
Descriptive statistics were generated for
the total sample. Missing cases (<5%) were
addressed with means substitution. Two-way
mixed ANCOVA was used to test for change
over time among participants who completed
the intervention versus control. Effects were
calculated for time and time × group interac-
tions. Covariates were ethnicity and gender
because randomisation revealed unequal
distributions of these variables in the exper-
imental conditions. Pearson’s correlation was
used to explore the relationship between indi-
vidual’s engagement (e.g. effort and motiva-
tion) and the effects of gratitude practice.
Two analyses were conducted – Completer
analysis (participants who completed the
intervention) and Intention-to-treat (ITT)
analysis (included participants who dropped
out). The ITT was carried out using the
last observation carried forward (LOCF)
method. For all hypotheses testing, it is
expected that participants in the gratitude
condition would have better outcomes than
the control condition.
Results
We only present the main ndings for
completer analysis (Table 1) because all
measures yielded essentially the same results
as the ITT analysis.
Dispositional and state gratitude
The main effect of group on disposi-
tional gratitude scores was signicant [F(1,
77)=4.892, p=.030, partial η2=.060]. The
condition × time interaction for disposi-
tional gratitude was also signicant [F(1, 77)
=4.84, p=.044, partial η2 =.052]. Dispositional
gratitude decreased in the control condition
relative to the gratitude intervention (see
Table 1).
14 Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017
Siew Tim Lai & Ronan E. O’Carroll
The condition × time interaction for
state gratitude was signicant [F(1, 77)=8.48,
p=.005, partial η2=.099]. State gratitude
increased in the gratitude intervention (see
Figure 1). Thus, those who practiced grat-
itude had higher levels of state and trait
gratitude relative to the control condition.
Positive affect
The condition × time interaction for positive
affect was signicant [F(1, 77)=4.90, p=.030,
partial η2 =.060] – see gure 2.
Wellbeing
The completer analysis for the condition ×
time interaction for well-being measure was
of borderline signicance (p=.053); however,
the results for the ITT analysis was signicant
[F(1, 104)=4.73, p=.032, partial η2=.043].
There was greater drop in wellbeing in the
control condition (see Table 1).
Physical health
Table 1 showed no difference was observed
for reported physical illness (p=.055) in both
experimental conditions.
Perceived quality of interpersonal
relationships
For participants in the gratitude condition,
the perception of relationship quality with
others was not signicantly different (p=.871)
than the control condition (see table 1).
Individual differences in practicing
gratitude
Motivation and effort were positively corre-
lated with increase in state gratitude, moti-
vation (r=.333, p=.047) and effort (r=.414,
p=.012). Participants with greater motivation
were more likely to adhere to the activity
(r=.321, p=.046) and have higher well-being
scores (r=.331, p=.049). Motivation and
effort, however, was not signicantly related
to dispositional gratitude and both the affec-
tivity scores.
Figure 1: Change in state gratitude from pre- to post-intervention for the gratitude
group condition versus the control group condition.
Pre- Post-
Estimated marginal means of state gratitude
Intervention periods
Means of gratitude at state levels
.00
2.50
5.00
7.50
10.00
12.50
15.00
Conditions
Control
Gratitude
Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017 15
‘The Three Good Things’ – The effects of gratitude practice on wellbeing: A randomised controlled trial
Table 1: Completer analysis on the effects of gratitude intervention in
the experimental and control group (
N
=81).
Figure 2: Change in positive affect from pre- to post-intervention for the gratitude
group condition versus the control group condition.
Measures Pre-test
mean (SD)
Post-test
mean (SD)
Time Group × time
interactions
GRAT CTRL GRAT CTRL F-ratio p F-ratio p
WEMWBS 46.17
(10.00)
45.69
(9.38)
45.50
(10.81)
39.47
(13.87)
5.110 .027* 3.87 .053
PANAS
PA
31.38
(8.31)
30.87
(7.13)
33.47
(8.23)
28.69
(8.22)
1.987 .163 4.895 .030*
NA 22.00
(9.05)
21.93
(7.65)
20.83
(6.62)
23.38
(8.93)
.147 .703 1.753 .189
GAC 9.83
(3.87)
9.69
(2.75)
11.67
(2.55)
9.13
(3.62)
.416 .521 8.476 .005**
GQ-6 33.44
(5.99)
31.82
(6.18)
33.36
(5.80)
28.16
(8.86)
7.884 .006** 4.184 .044*
Perceived
quality of
relationship
14.39
(4.44)
13.36
(3.15)
13.61
(2.50)
12.78
(3.40)
.496 .483 .871 .354
Physical
symptoms
4.46
(2.98)
5.07
(3.20)
3.77
(3.01)
4.34
(3.11)
5.563 .021* .055 .816
* p < .05;
** p < .01
Intervention periods
Pre- Post-
Conditions
Control
Gratitude
Estimated marginal means of positive affect
Means of positive affect
10.00
.00
20.00
30.00
40.00
50.00
16 Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017
Siew Tim Lai & Ronan E. O’Carroll
Discussion
One approach to boost an individual’s
contentment is the deliberate practice of
counting one’s blessings. To date, grateful
induction to alter one’s dispositional grati-
tude has not been investigated in a system-
atic manner (Emmons, 2007). Future work
can explore whether continuous practice
of gratitude-oriented activities increases a
person’s disposition toward gratitude.
Some may express gratefulness only
under certain circumstances (Watkins,
2004). Through journaling one’s grateful
thoughts, we can determine whether the
immediate feelings of gratefulness can be
cultivated. Our results suggest that keeping
a gratitude journal increases state gratitude;
this is congruent with ndings from previous
studies (Emmons & McCullough, 2003;
Martínez-Martí et al., 2010).
Another main nding emerged from
this study is that participants who practiced
gratitude had more positive affect (PA) and
less negative affect (NA) compared to the
control condition. Gratitude may operate
through savouring – a conscious focus on
one’s fortunate moments that could have
been otherwise (Bryant, 2003). Our gratitude
intervention may have successfully increased
PA through the accessibility and retrieva-
bility of pleasant memories (Watkins, 2004),
contributing to one’s subjective wellbeing.
Consistent with Emmons and McCullough’s
(2003) study, the gratitude intervention
did not lead to signicant reduction in NA.
Nevertheless, the mean scores for NA were
in the expected direction. The attrition rate
(over 30 per cent) may have led to insuf-
cient power to detect signicant differences.
We found that those who keep a grati-
tude journal had relatively stable wellbeing
scores. Fredrickson’s (2001) broaden-and-
build model suggests that gratitude harnesses
behavioural and cognitive repertoires by
undoing’ the adverse effects of negative
emotions. Grateful responses to life – as
Fredrickson (2001) claimed, build enduring
coping resources to stabilise mood when
stressful life-events arise. Several studies (e.g.
Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004) support this
interpretation.
Somewhat contrary to our expectations,
gratitude appeared to worsen the perception
of relationship quality. Gratitude prototypi-
cally, stems from the recognition of received
aid is evaluated as costly and altruistic (Wood
et al., 2007). It is possible that the expression
of gratitude causes momentary experiences of
indebtedness’ – an obligated feeling to return
the giver the valuable act (Emmons, 2007).
Indebtedness is associated with feelings of
shame and guilt (Emmons, 2007) which may
complicate or reduce the quality of interper-
sonal relationships. Both ‘thankfulness’ and
indebtedness’ are embedded in reciprocity,
but the subtle differences in appraising the
benefactor’s intentions (benevolent vs. ambig-
uous) determines the recipient’s emotional
responses (Tsang, 2006). This suggests that
received benets should be considered when
future research examines its interaction with
interpersonal consequences.
Regarding physical wellbeing, there were
no differences between conditions. Though
the main effect of time revealed signicant
changes across conditions, both groups
reported having experienced less physical
symptoms.
The current study sought to examine how
a person’s motivation and effort inuence
the success of the intervention. The ndings
showed participants who put greater motiva-
tion and effort into the gratitude-inducing
activity had increased feelings of grateful-
ness. This is consistent with previous studies
(Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006), which
suggested that when the mood-enhancing
activity ‘ts’ the person’s interest and values
and is performed with appropriate effort,
the goal of developing a grateful thinking is
achievable.
Changes in wellbeing, however, were not
signicantly correlated with effort. Volitional
activities like the gratitude-inducing exercise
require commitment and a certain amount
of effort to initiate, engage, and maintain
Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017 17
‘The Three Good Things’ – The effects of gratitude practice on wellbeing: A randomised controlled trial
the activity (Sin et al., 2009). As Sheldon et
al. (2006) stated, if a person yearns for happi-
ness but does not exert any effort to practice
the mood-enhancing strategy, they may not
experience signicant increase in wellbeing.
While motivation and effort are important
variables for continued adherence to the
activity (Lyubomirsky et al., 2011), only moti-
vation was positively correlated with adher-
ence in this study.
Limitations
First, the randomisation was not entirely
successful as some differences between
groups were evident in pre-intervention.
This was dealt with by including these vari-
ables as covariates in the analyses. Second,
reduction in sample size due to attrition
resulted in reduced power to detect signi-
cant changes; however our ITT results were
very similar. Last but not least, the lack of
longer-term follow-up means that we cannot
comment on the sustainability of interven-
tion effects.
Future directions
Although the current ndings have advanced
our understanding of how gratitude affects
an individual’s wellbeing, many questions
remained unanswered. Pre-existing traits
(e.g. trait gratitude or affective traits) may
affect an individual’s ability to benet from
the gratitude intervention (Emmons, 2007).
Can we expect gratitude induction to work
better in improving health outcomes of
less or more grateful individuals? Further
research is required to explore the trait-
moderator link.
Conclusion
Our results suggest that the count-your-
blessings approach may be an effective tech-
nique to increase individuals’ predisposition
to experience gratitude. A mindfulness
appreciation helps individuals to focus on
benets (Emmons, 2007). Future studies are
required to test the effectiveness of gratitude
interventions against active control condi-
tions and other psychological interventions.
This study shows that a simple count-your-
blessings intervention can improve psycho-
logical wellbeing and could potentially be
included alongside existing psychological
interventions.
The Authors
Siew Tim Lai, and Ronan E. O’Carroll,
University of Stirling, Scotland, UK
Correspondence
Lai Siew Tim
Department of Psychology,
University of Stirling, Stirling,
Email: lai.siewtim@gmail.com
18 Health Psychology Update, Volume 26, Issue 1, Spring 2017
Siew Tim Lai & Ronan E. O’Carroll
References
Bryant, F.B. (2003). Savouring beliefs inventory (SBI):
A scale for measuring beliefs about savouring.
Journal of Mental Health, 12(2), 175–196.
doi: .1080/0963823031000103489
Emmons, R.A. (2007). Thanks! How the new science
of gratitude can make you happier. New York:
Houghton Mifin Company.
Emmons, R.A. & McCullough, M.E. (2003). Counting
blessings versus burdens: An experimental
investigation of gratitude and subjective well-
being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84(2), 311–389. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.84.2.3.77
Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive
emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-
and-build theory of positive emotions. American
Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Layard, R. (2013). First world happiness report launched
at the United Nations. Retrieved from http://www.
earth.columbia.edu/articles/view/2960
Martínez-Martí, M.L., Avia, M.D. & Hernández-
Lloreda, M.J. (2010). The effects of counting
blessings on subjective well-being: A gratitude
intervention in a Spanish sample. The Spanish
Journal of Psychology, 13(2), 886–896.
McCullough, M.E., Emmons, R.A. & Tsang, J.A.
(2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual
and empirical topography. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 82, 112–127.
Odou, N. & Vella-Brodrick, D.A. (2013). The efcacy
of positive psychology interventions to increase
well-being and the role of mental imagery
ability. Social Indicators Research, 110, 111–129.
doi: 10.1007/s11205-011-9919-1
Sheldon, K.M. & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to
increase and sustain positive emotion: The effects
of expressing gratitude and visualising best
possible selves. The Journal of Positive Psychology,
1(2), 73–82. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510676
Sin, N.L., Della Porta, M.D. & Lyubomirsky, S.
(2009). Tailoring positive psychology inter-
ventions to treat depressed individuals. In S.
I. Donaldson, M. Csikszentmihalyi, & J. Naka-
mura (Eds.), Applied positive psychology: Improving
everyday life, schools, work, health, and society. New
York: Routledge.
Tennant, R., Hiller, L., Fishwick, R., Platt, S.,
Joseph, S., Weich, S., et al. (2007). The
Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale
(WEMWBS): Development and UK validation.
Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5(1), 63–75.
doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-63.
Tugade, M.M. & Fredrickson, B.L. (2004). Resillient
individuals use positive emotions to bounce back
from negative emotional experiences. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333.
Urbaniak, G.C. & Plous, S. (2013). Research Rand-
omizer (Version 4.0) [Computer software].
Retrieved on June 22, 2013, from http://www.
randomizer.org/
Watkins, P.C. (2004). Gratitude and subjective well-
being. In R.A. Emmons & M.E. McCullough
(Eds.), The Psychology of Gratitude (pp.167–192).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A. & Tellegen, A. (1988). Devel-
opment and validation of brief measures of
positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
1063–1070.
Wood, A.M., Froh, J.J. & Geraghty, A.W.A. (2010).
Gratitude and well-being: A review and theo-
retical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 1-16.
doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
Wood, A.M., Joseph, S. & Linley, P.A. (2007). Coping
style as a psychological resource of grateful
people. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
26, 1108–1125.
... Awareness of the aspects of daily life that one can be grateful for can foster resilience and positive well-being in an individual. Researchers have developed and tested many ways we can increase our state levels of gratitude, such as a daily gratitude diary (Emmons & McCullough, 2003), writing a gratitude letter (Adair et al., 2020b;Kini et al., 2016;Seligman et al., 2005;Sheldon & Lyubomirsky, 2006), and listing grateful events (Emmons & McCullough, 2003;Froh et al., 2008;Watkins et al., 2003), including the 'Three Good Things' exercise (Guo et al., 2020;Laguna et al., 2021;Lai, 2017;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017;Rippstein-Leuenberger et al., 2017;Seligman et al., 2005;Sexton & Adair, 2019). The Three Good things exercise is a classic gratitude intervention where participants are asked to write down three things that they are grateful for in their lives daily, either for a week, or up to eight weeks. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has significantly disrupted the daily lives of college students, resulting in elevated levels of stress, anxiety, and isolation. Research suggests positive psychology interventions aimed at practicing gratitude, offer potential benefits in reducing these common mental health problems. However, there is a limited understanding of how or why these interventions work nor what function gratitude plays in the lives of young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. As such, the purpose of the paper was to explore the sources and targets of gratitude of college students during the COVID-10 lockdown in Ireland. This study aimed analyse the content of the ‘Three Good Things’ intervention as reported by young adults during the COVID-19 lockdowns in Ireland. A total of 109 college students participated in a 7-day online ‘Three Good Things’ intervention, where they were prompted to reflect on and document three positive experiences each day. Participants were asked to elaborate on how these experiences made them feel and to highlight their role in facilitating these positive experiences. Thematic analysis was used to analyze the 2,200 submitted responses. The findings revealed three overarching themes relating to participants’ expressions of gratitude during the COVID-19 lockdown: (1) cultivating positive social interactions, (2) prioritizing meaningful self-care, and (3) fostering hope for a more normal life post-pandemic. By delving into the lived experiences of college students, this study sheds light on the elements central to their expressions of gratitude during the COVID-19 pandemic. The findings underscore the importance of social connections, self-care practices, and hopeful prospects as sources of gratitude among students.
... The subsequent self-administration tool, very simple and easy to use which to a minimal degree disrupts daily functioning is the practice of gratitude [52]. This consists in keeping a gratitude journal where the participants for three weeks every day write down the things for which they are grateful and why [53]. Gratitude has been conceptualized as an emotion, attitude, habit, personality trait, strategy of coping, or life orientation focused on thinking about positive aspects of a difficult situation and appreciating the positive aspects of the world [54]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction and objective: The aim of the study was to review the current state of knowledge regarding the treatment of emotional consequences of infertility using psychosocial interventions and their effectiveness. Review methods: The review was based on data obtained from scientific articles published in the Pub Med, Science Direct, the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Embase, Scopus, Ovid MEDLINE, Ovid EMbase, and The Cochrane Library between 1997-2022. Brief description of the state of knowledge: Assistance offered to persons afflicted by the problem of infertility may take various forms, depending not only on the needs of those interested and the stage of treatment, but also on the capabilities of the staff (type and level of education). Commonly available, well-developed and researched methods of therapy are most frequently applied, often expanded by such elements as education concerning fertility and life style, and information about diagnosis and treatment. Modified methods of psychological intervention specially adjusted to persons struggling with infertility are also used. The indicators of the effectiveness of these methods are: reduction of stress, better coping with difficulties, decrease in the rates of psychological disorders, as well as an improvement of sperm parameters in men, and an increase in pregnancy and live birth rates. Summary: The most frequently recommended methods of psychosocial intervention are these based on cognitive behavioural therapy and variants of stress reduction techniques. It is recommended that patients with infertility are provided with psychological care throughout the treatment period, and that the medical staff is equipped with standardized methods of assessing their mental state.
... SPPIs consist of one or more activities that aim to enhance only one component of well-being (Hendriks et al., 2020), most often hedonic well-being. For example, gratitude interventions (e.g., Cunha et al., 2019;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017), the best possible selves intervention (see Loveday et al., 2018 for a review) and savouring interventions (e.g. Quoidbach et al., 2010;Smith & Hanni, 2019). ...
Chapter
Harmony is recognized as fundamental to being and functioning well in philosophical traditions and empirical research globally and in Africa. The aim of this study was to explore and describe harmony as a quality of happiness in South Africa (N = 585) and Ghana (N = 420). Using a qualitative descriptive research design, participants’ responses to an open-ended question from the Eudaimonic-Hedonic Happiness Investigation (EHHI, Delle Fave et al., Soc Indic Res 100:185–207, 2011) on what happiness meant to them were coded according to the formalized EHHI coding manual. Responses that were assigned any of the following codes were considered: codes from the “harmony/balance” category in the “psychological definitions” life domain; and codes from any other life domain containing the words “harmony”, “balance”, or “peace”. This resulted in 222 verbatim responses from South Africa and 80 from Ghana that were analyzed using content analysis to get a sense of the experiential texture of harmony as a quality of happiness. Findings showed that happiness was often expressed as harmony and balance within and between intrapersonal, interpersonal, transcendental, and universal levels of functioning, with wholeness, interconnectedness, and synergy implied. These findings, resonating with philosophical reflections on harmony from Africa and elsewhere, suggest that harmony as a quality of happiness is essentially holistic and contextually embedded and that context-sensitive interdisciplinary approaches to theory building and intervention development pertaining to harmony are needed locally and globally.
... SPPIs consist of one or more activities that aim to enhance only one component of well-being (Hendriks et al., 2020), most often hedonic well-being. For example, gratitude interventions (e.g., Cunha et al., 2019;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017), the best possible selves intervention (see Loveday et al., 2018 for a review) and savouring interventions (e.g. Quoidbach et al., 2010;Smith & Hanni, 2019). ...
Chapter
Positive mental health, and the validity of its assessment instruments, are largely unexplored in the Ghanaian context. This study examined the factor structure of the Twi version of the Mental Health Continuum-Short Form and explored the prevalence of positive mental health in a sample of rural Ghanaian adults (N = 444). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modelling (ESEM) model fit the data better than competing models (confirmatory factor analysis [CFA], bifactor CFA, and ESEM models). We found a high omega reliability coefficient for the general positive mental health factor (ω = .97) and marginal reliability scores for the emotional (ω = .51) and social well-being (ω = .57) subscales, but a low reliability score for the psychological well-being subscale (ω = .41). Findings support the existence of a general mental health factor, and confirm the underlying three-dimensional structure of mental health, but suggest that caution should be applied when interpreting subscale scores, especially for the psychological well-being subscale. Based on Keyes’s criteria for the categorical diagnosis of the presence of positive mental health, 25.5% of the sample were flourishing, with 74.5% functioning at suboptimal levels (31.1% languishing, 41.4% with moderate mental health) and may benefit from contextually relevant positive psychological interventions, which may also buffer against psychopathology.
... SPPIs consist of one or more activities that aim to enhance only one component of well-being (Hendriks et al., 2020), most often hedonic well-being. For example, gratitude interventions (e.g., Cunha et al., 2019;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017), the best possible selves intervention (see Loveday et al., 2018 for a review) and savouring interventions (e.g. Quoidbach et al., 2010;Smith & Hanni, 2019). ...
Chapter
Psychology is concerned with human behaviour, therefore all psychologies are contextually-embedded and culturally informed. A movement towards globalising psychology would invariably diminish the localised socio-cultural situatedness of psychology, and instead seek to advance a dominant Euro-American centred psychology even in regions where such applications do not fit. The emergence of strong voices, and theoretically grounded and empirically supported positions from the global South in general and sub-Saharan Africa in particular, in studies of well-being allows for the opportunity to explore and describe an Africa(n) centred positive psychology. Acknowledging the limitations of cross-cultural psychological approaches, which have encouraged the uncritical transportation of Euro-American centred concepts and values, in this chapter we utilise assumptions from critical, cultural and African psychology to present our initial thoughts about a culturally embedded, socially relevant and responsive, and context respecting Africa(n) centred positive psychology. This challenge warrants consideration of early contributions to the study of well-being, its current data-driven positivist tendency, as well as African worldviews grounded in interdependence, collectivism, relatedness, harmony with nature, and spirituality. For an Africa(n) centred positive psychology, it is also essential to consider questions of epistemology, ways of knowing about the world and the human condition, context respecting knowledge, and theory building. Drawing on current scholarly evidence in sub-Saharan Africa, which emphasises relationality and societal values and norms shaping experiences of well-being, we propose future directions and discuss implications for empirical research and theory building within positive psychology which seeks to centre Africa and African experiences.
... SPPIs consist of one or more activities that aim to enhance only one component of well-being (Hendriks et al., 2020), most often hedonic well-being. For example, gratitude interventions (e.g., Cunha et al., 2019;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017), the best possible selves intervention (see Loveday et al., 2018 for a review) and savouring interventions (e.g. Quoidbach et al., 2010;Smith & Hanni, 2019). ...
Chapter
Child marriage has been identified as a violation of human rights and an obstacle to promoting the development goals concerning gender, health and education. All these impacts undermine the development of the girl child. Despite the potential for negative outcomes, the presence of intrinsic and extrinsic resources can buffer the adverse effects (e.g., psychological, physical and economic impact) of early marriage. This study employed a qualitative exploratory, descriptive design to explore and describe protective resources utilised by married girls in the Northern region of Ghana to cope with the challenges in their marriage and to promote positive outcomes. Using semi-structured interviews, data was collected from 21 married girls who were aged between 12 and 19 years. Findings, from a thematic analysis of data, showed that intrinsic resources that promoted positive outcomes included possession of resilience attitudes, the use of help-seeking and active coping, and in some instances avoidance coping for problems they perceived as unsolvable. Extrinsic resources included interpersonal support networks, however, participants reported limited access to community and NGO support, which were also identified as protective resources. Policy makers and clinicians should consider a social justice approach in evaluating and recommending protective resources to girls in early marriages when working to promote their well-being. In so doing, attention should be placed on making external support systems accessible to married girls.
... The Three Good Things journal showed increased happiness and decreased depressive symptoms within one month that persisted and increased at three-month and six-month follow-ups . Several research teams have successfully replicated these positive results (e.g., Gander et al., 2013;Lai & O'Carroll, 2017;Mongrain & Anselmo-Matthews, 2012;Rippstein-Leuenberger et al., 2017;Sexton & Adair, 2019). Notably, the Three Good Things journal differs from many gratitude interventions, which can become repetitive and subsequently less effective, in that it identifies specific, unique events each day as a fresh source of positivity. ...
Article
Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotion, this article suggests that efforts by social workers to find joy in the profession may help them broaden their scope of attention, cognition, and action; build physical, intellectual, and social resources for future use; and increase resilience. This article explains Fredrickson's broaden-and-build theory as a basis for exploring and seeking positivity and outlines several practical strategies that social workers or their leaders can adopt to promote joy and other positive emotions. The strategies can be implemented at individual or group levels in classrooms or agencies. Finally, this article considers how these strategies may contribute to social worker self-care, self-awareness, and ongoing growth and development.
... Interestingly, Watkins et al. (2021) highlighted gratitude to be the positive emotion that was most experienced during the pandemic, compared with happiness, hope, relief, and joy, highlighting a critical role for gratitude-and opportunities for promoting itduring a time of great suffering. Importantly, gratitude interventions, such as the three good things exercise (Lai, 2017), have been shown to increase levels of gratitude, providing opportunities to promote sustainable wellbeing (Bohlmeijer et al., 2021), and facilitate PTG during times of trauma. ...
Article
Full-text available
COVID-19 presented a major societal challenge including threat to life, bereavement, self-isolation, loss of income and significant psychological distress. Yet, it is possible that such suffering may also lead to post-traumatic growth (PTG) and subsequent wellbeing. The current study aimed to investigate the contributors to PTG and whether PTG mediated their relationship with wellbeing, measured using the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale. In a cross-sectional sample of 136 participants (mean age = 30.52; SD = 13.80), a hierarchical regression and mediation analysis was conducted, focusing on physical activity, gratitude, tragic optimism, social support, and nature connection, guided by our recently published ‘GENIAL’ framework (Mead, Fisher, & Kemp, 2021). The regression analysis highlighted that our variables predicted up to 18% of the variance in PTG, whilst controlling for age, gender and subjective social status, with gratitude and nature connection being key predictors – indicating the importance of these factors over and above previously reported contributors to PTG, such as social support. Our findings provide new evidence on the drivers of PTG and raise important questions concerning the relationship between the related constructs of PTG and wellbeing. Limitations and suggestions for future research are discussed.
Article
Nurse leader compassion fatigue contributes to role dissatisfaction and leader turnover, staff turnover, low morale and engagement, safety, and satisfaction concerns. Both staff and leader turnover lead to costs for recruitment, onboarding, orientation, and staffing. Fifteen nurse leaders participated in a daily gratitude exercise, Three Good Things, over an 8-week period. Three Good Things was found to be statistically significant in increasing compassion satisfaction of this population. A practice of gratitude fosters compassion satisfaction which is a protective factor against compassion fatigue, and it is beneficial in increasing role satisfaction and decreasing turnover in this population.
Chapter
Research on positive psychology interventions (PPIs) are expanding, also in non-Western contexts. This study examined literature on PPIs in African countries through a scoping review. Databases were searched for studies implemented between 2006 and 2019. The bibliographic search used a broad and inclusive definition of PPIs and yielded 23 studies for analysis. The results indicated that the majority of studies were implemented in group settings among adults in South Africa, using quantitative research designs. However, research elsewhere on the continent is starting to emerge. There is a need for more research among youth, older persons and clinical populations. Future studies should also focus on cultural adaptation of existing PPIs, and take cultural practices and traditions into account.
Article
Full-text available
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Article
This document reports on the findings of a survey conducted by the Division of Health Psychology Practitioners and Research Sub–committees.