ChapterPDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Although different determinants, including genetics, temperament, and a variety of social-contextual influences, play roles in young people’s development, the role of parents is paramount to healthy psychosocial adjustment. When children’s psychological needs are satisfied, children report more well-being, engage in activities with more interest and spontaneity (intrinsic motivation), more easily accept guidelines for important behaviors (internalization), display more openness in social relationships, and are more resilient when faced with adversity and distress. This chapter will focus on how parental supports or thwarts for children’s basic psychological needs either promote or diminish the children’s mental health, social adjustment, and psychological growth.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Soenens, B. Deci, E. L., & Vansteenkiste, M. (in press). How parents contribute to children’s
psychological health: The critical role of psychological need support. In L. Wehmeyer, T. D.
Little, S. J. Lopez, K. A. Shogren, & R. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook on the development of self-
determination. New York: Springer.
How Parents Contribute to Children’s Psychological Health:
The Critical Role of Psychological Need Support
Bart Soenens Edward L. Deci Maarten Vansteenkiste
Ghent University University of Rochester Ghent University
Anyone observing young children in a playground will easily notice remarkable
differences among them. Some of the children explore the playground with curiosity and have
a great time; others are more withdrawn and feel uncomfortable with other children around.
At home some children may accept parental rules or negotiate constructively with the parents;
others may feel forced to comply with parental rules or even react defiantly against them.
Later, in adolescence, some youngsters willingly share their thoughts and feelings with
parents; others disclose much less and may even be secretive. How can these differences
among the children’s and adolescents emotional, social, and behavioral adjustments be
explained? Although different determinants, including genetics, temperament, and a variety of
social-contextual influences, play roles in young people’s development, this chapter will focus
on the role of parents. Specifically, we address how parental supports or thwarts for children’s
basic psychological needs either promote or diminish the children’s mental health, social
adjustment, and psychological growth.
Basic Psychological Needs and Children’s Psychosocial Adjustment
Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000) argues
that children’s psychosocial adjustment depends to a substantial degree on satisfactions of
three basic psychological needs, namely, the needs for autonomy, competence, and
2
relatedness (see also Chapter 4, this volume). Satisfaction of the need for autonomy manifests
in experiences of volition, psychological freedom, authenticity, and ownership of one’s
behaviors and choices. When the need for competence is satisfied, children feel efficacious
and able to deal with optimally challenging tasks. The need for relatedness is satisfied when
children feel appreciated by and closely connected to important people. In SDT,
psychological need satisfactions are considered essential and universal nutrients for healthy
psychological development (Deci & Ryan, 2000). When children’s psychological needs are
satisfied , they report more well-being, engage activities with interest and spontaneity
(intrinsic motivation), more easily accept guidelines for important behaviors (internalization),
display more openness in social relationships, and are more resilient when faced with
adversity and distress (Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013).
While much research on psychological need satisfaction involved university students
and adults (e.g., Chen, Vansteenkiste, et al., 2015), recent research has also demonstrated the
importance of the psychological needs for children’s and adolescents’ adjustment. For
example, Veronneau, Koestner, and Abela, 2005) found among 3rd and 7th graders that
satisfaction of each of the three needs was related to positive affect. Satisfaction of the need
for competence in particular predicted decreases in depressive symptoms across a 6-week
interval. Luyckx, Vansteenkiste, Goossens, and Duriez (2009) found psychological need
satisfaction to be critical for adolescents’ thorough exploration of identity options and
stronger commitments to identity choices.
Recent work has also focused on people’s dark sides resulting from psychological
need frustration (e.g., Bartholomew et al., 2011). When social-contextual factors are thwarting
of children’s needs, the needs are likely to be frustrated, leaving the children feeling
controlled (autonomy frustration), inferior (competence frustration), and lonely (relatedness
frustration). In SDT need frustration is not equated with an absence of need satisfaction.
3
Rather, frustration ensues when the psychological needs are actively undermined rather than
merely unsatisfied. Because frustration results from intruding on the children’s sense of self, it
is a serious threat that renders the children vulnerable to ill-being and psychopathology (Ryan,
Deci, & Vansteenkiste, 2015). Research increasingly supports the notion that psychological
need frustration is particularly predictive of maladaptive developmental outcomes. It has been
shown, for instance, that need frustration is related to physiological indicators of stress
(Bartholomew et al., 2011), interpersonal problems (Costa, Ntoumanis, & Bartholomew,
2015), and eating-disorder symptoms (Boone et al., 2014).
The Nurturing Role of Parents in Children’s Development
Given the pivotal role of the basic psychological needs in children’s and adolescents’
well-being and adjustment, a key developmental question is how socialization figures, and
parents in particular, affect psychological need satisfaction and psychological need
frustration. SDT argues that parents, in interaction with other key individuals (i.e., the
children’s teachers and peers), play a crucial role in the nurturing versus thwarting of
children’s psychological needs. Paralleling the distinctions among the three needs, differences
in parents’ style of interacting with children are conceptualized with three concepts (Grolnick,
Deci, & Ryan, 1997; Joussemet, Landry, & Koestner, 2008): (a) relatedness supports or
involvement (e.g., respect and warmth), (b) competence supports or structure (e.g., offering
clear expectations, adequate help, and non-critical feedback), and (c) autonomy support (e.g.,
acknowledging the children’s perspective, providing choice, and encouraging exploration).
Each of these contextual, need-supportive concepts has a need-thwarting dark side just
as each of the need satisfactions has a need-frustration dark side. For instance, relatedness
thwarts are characterized by parental behaviors that are cold, neglectful, and rejecting;
competence thwarts are demeaning and chaotic; and autonomy thwarts include pressuring
demands and coercion. Importantly, being low in need supports does not necessarily mean
4
that parents will be actively and intrusively thwarting of children’s needs (Skinner, Snyder, &
Johnson, 2005), and similarly, being low in need thwarting does not necessarily mean that
parents will be actively and happily supportive of children’s needs. However, when parents
are actively need supportive, it has been shown that they will foster experiences of need
satisfaction (and subsequent well-being and positive adjustment), and when parents are
actively need thwarting it has been shown to bring about experiences of need frustration (and
subsequent ill-being and maladjustment).
We do note that there is not a simple one-to-one association between one of the
parental need-supportive dimensions and satisfaction of the children’s corresponding need
(Grolnick et al., 1997), or between a parental need-thwarting dimension and frustration of the
children’s corresponding need. Each of the dimensions of need-supportive parenting is to
some extent relevant to satisfaction to each of the three needs. For example, when parents take
their children’s perspective in a conversation, the children are likely to feel some relatedness
satisfaction and also some indication of parental trust in the children’s capabilities. In this
regard, the graphical representation in Figure 1 is a simplification of reality, for there could be
an arrow from each support to each need satisfaction, and from each thwart to each need
frustration.
In the remainder of this chapter we focus on the three dimensions of need-supportive
parenting for our primary goal is the facilitation of greater self-determination. [Those
interested in further discussion of need-thwarting parenting are referred to Assor, Kanat-
Maymon, and Roth, (2014); Grolnick, (2003); and Soenens and Vansteenkiste (2010)]. In
discussing need-supportive parenting we describe the basic attitude underlying each
dimension as well as their more specific manifestations (Vansteenkiste & Soenens, 2015). We
also provide a selective discussion of research relevant to each dimension.
Relatedness Support
5
The basic attitude behind relatedness-support is characterized by love, care and a
genuine desire to support the child (Vansteenkiste & Soenens, 2015). Parents supporting their
child’s need for relatedness deeply care about the child’s well-being and enjoy being in the
child’s company (Deci & Ryan, 2014). These parents engage in warm and sensitive
interactions with the child, interactions that build a child’s sense of attachment security
(Bowlby, 1988). As a consequence, a child feels protected and learns to trust and rely on the
parent when experiencing distress. This supportive orientation can be contrasted with a cold
parental orientation, where parents are largely unavailable, unresponsive to a child’s requests
for support, or even rejecting.
A basic requirement for all need-supportive parenting is parental presence and
involvement. Parents who support children’s need for relatedness spend a sufficient amount of
time in the presence of their children and get at least minimally involved in the children’s
activities (e.g., Grolnick, Kurowski, & Gurland, 1999). However, parental involvement and
investment of time is not a sufficient condition for children to feel deeply connected to their
parents. Research shows that there is no straightforward association between the amount of
time parents spend with their children and children’s well-being (Milkie, Nomaguchi, &
Denny, 2015) nor between parental involvement in the child’s activities and the children’s
motivation for and performance in these activities (e.g., homework: Pomerantz, Moorman, &
Litwack, 2007).
For children to really benefit from their parents’ involvement and presence, the quality
of parents’ involvement needs to be sufficiently high. In this regard, it is important for parents
to be mentally present, to be alert to the children’s feelings, and to proactively consider the
impact of situations on the children’s feelings. For instance, parents can try to anticipate how
the children will respond to an episode of separation (e.g., leaving the children with a
babysitter) or to a potentially painful situation (e.g., a doctor’s visit). By announcing what will
6
happen, these situations may become less emotionally unpredictable and overwhelming and
parents can proactively help children to regulate their emotions. When a child actually
experiences emotional distress or physical pain, parents high on relatedness need-support
react in a responsive fashion. They offer comfort and they are available for help. In doing so,
they provide a safe haven for the children to turn to when feeling upset (Bowlby, 1988).
In addition to being involved, alert, and responsive, parents high on relatedness need-
support are warm and affectionate. This warmth can be expressed emotionally, through
friendly, humorous, and positive interactions with the child as well as physically (e.g.,
through hugs, kisses, or an embrace). A final element of parental relatedness need-support is
engagement in joint activities. Parents can engage in enjoyable and interesting activities with
their children one-on-one (e.g., father and son playing basketball or playing a board game) or
with the family as a whole (e.g., making a trip, going to a music festival, travelling together).
While activities with an individual child can strengthen the parent-child bond, activities with
the family can build a sense of cohesion and collective identity in the family.
There is a longstanding tradition of research, some of which is rooted in attachment
theory, demonstrating the importance of parents’ relational need support for children’s
development. Relatedness need support has been shown to predict a plethora of adaptive
outcomes, including secure attachment representations (van Ijzendoorn, 1995), self-worth
(Brummelman et al., 2015), social competence (Barber, Stolz, & Olsen, 2005), and social
skills contributing to social competence such as adequate emotion regulation (Davidov &
Grusec, 2006) and empathy (Soenens, Duriez, Vansteenkiste, & Goossens, 2007). In contrast,
cold and rejecting parenting has been found to predict a host of developmental problems,
including internalizing and externalizing behaviors (Putnick et al., 2015).
Structure
7
Parents who provide structure assist their children in building a sense of competence.
Their basic attitude involves a focus on the development of their children’s skills and
emerging abilities. They are process-oriented, meaning that they are interested in discovering
the children’s talents and in providing support to nurture these talents (Reeve, 2006;
Vansteenkiste & Soenens, 2015). They are aware that children learn through trial and error
and that wide individual differences exist in the timing and rhythm of children’s development
of capacities. Parents who provide structure take into account these individual differences and
try to provide a level of support and help that is attuned to the child’s developmental level and
possibilities. Parental structure can be contrasted with chaos, which is characteristic of parents
who do not match their level and type of involvement to the child’s abilities. They fail to
provide clear guidelines for adequate behavior and they provide vague or even confusing
guidelines. They give unwanted help and irrelevant information, and, at times, they can even
become explicitly critical of the children’s behavior and achievements.
The components of structure can be organized according to the timing with which they
are typically used; that is, before, during, or after competence-relevant activities (Reeve,
2006). Two important elements are particularly relevant prior to children’s engagement in an
activity. When a child is about to start an activity, parents high on structure provide clear
guidelines, they communicate limits about which behaviors are allowed and which are not.
They also discuss consequences that might follow if rules are not followed, but they do that in
an informative rather than controlling fashion. Further, they provide the necessary help for the
child to set goals and, if needed, also offer a step-by-step script so children know how to
achieve the introduced goals. Parents who provide structure also attend to the kinds of
activities their children engage in. Specifically, they try to stimulate activities and create
conditions that are optimally challenging to the children. Activities that slightly exceed the
children’s developmental level but are still within reach (i.e., activities in the children’s zones
8
of proximal development) stimulate the children to learn new skills (Vygotsky, 1978). For
parents to create these optimally challenging conditions, they need to be aware of the
children’s abilities and present the activities in ways that are not overwhelming to the
children. It is also important for parents to openly convey their trust in the children’s abilities
to do well and master new skills.
Parents can also provide structure during the children’s engagement with activities.
They can do so by monitoring the children’s progress in a process-oriented fashion. When
parents and children agreed upon a rule, parents high on structure are consistent in following
up on this rule. They signal to the children in non-intrusive but consequent ways when
agreements are not respected. Further, parents high on structure provide adequate help during
children’s engagement in the tasks. They are available in case the children ask for help. When
their help is solicited, parents give advice or they break down the task into smaller units to
make the task more feasible to the children. There is a thin line between appropriate and
inappropriate helpthat is, information and instructionwith inappropriate help defined as
help that is unwanted or that is excessive, in which case the parents are essentially taking over
the task, thereby precluding a possible learning opportunity for the children. Yet, parents may
also provide too little help such that children feel like they are left helpless. The provision of
help in a way that really contributes to the children’s competence again requires an accurate
parental assessment of the children’s abilities and need for assistance.
Both during and after the children’s engagement in a task or activity, parents can
provide structure by giving informational feedback. Ideally, this feedback is process-oriented
and focused on the children’s efforts and strategies (e.g., “You seem to have found a good
way of studying this course”) rather than on the person as a whole (e.g., “You are so smart”)
(Kamins & Dweck, 1999). Even when children did not do well at a task, parents can be
supportive. To encourage self-reflection, prior to the parents giving their own take on the
9
situation, they may invite the children to reflect on what happened, and perhaps whether they
see different ways they might try the task next time. This will allow the feedback interchanges
to be learning experiences and allow the children to feel a sense of ownership. That is, when
children are able to identify their own strengths and weaknesses, they are likely to develop a
stronger willingness to improve their skills and to build a sense of mastery and a feeling of
control over their own development. During the interchange, the parents may need to provide
some informational feedback by pointing out things that did go well that the children did not
notice. They may also formulate suggestions and hints in specific and constructive ways.
In sum, there is more to structure than rule-setting and the communication of
expectations. Clear expectations and rules are a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for
children to develop a sense of competence (Mouratidis, Vansteenkiste, Michou, & Lens,
2013). Children are more likely to feel competent when parents also provide adequate help,
give process-oriented feedback, and assist the children in reflecting upon their learning
process. Further, structure is relevant not only to activities that involve learning (e.g.,
homework) and play (e.g., games) but also to rule-compatible behavior. Also when teaching
children to behave well (according to moral, conventional, or prudential standards), parents
can provide structure by communicating clear guidelines, by giving advice about how to
respond in challenging situations, and by giving constructive feedback on the children’s
behavior.
Compared to research on supports for relatedness and autonomy, there is less research
on parental structure, although a relevant study on teachers providing structure to adolescents
did predict more student behavioral engagement (Jang, Reeve, & Deci, 2010). Further, studies
are beginning to show that parental structure is related to important motivational and
developmental outcomes in different life domains, including academic competence,
engagement, and performance (e.g., Farkas & Grolnick, 2010; Grolnick, Raftery-Helmer,
10
Flamm, Marbell, & Cardemil, 2015), feelings of competence during unsupervised time (e.g.,
activities with friends in the absence of parents; Grolnick et al., 2014), and engagement and
positive experiences during parent-child conversations about sensitive topics such as sexuality
(Mauras, Grolnick, & Friendly, 2013). In contrast, parental chaos has been found to relate to
problem behaviors such as substance use and delinquency (Skinner et al., 2005).
Autonomy-Support
Parental autonomy-support is the parenting concept most unique to SDT. Autonomy-
supportive parents tend to focus on their children’s perspectives. Rather than prioritizing their
personal agenda, they are interested in the children’s point of view (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, &
Leone, 1994; Vansteenkiste & Soenens, 2015). Also, they unconditionally accept the children
as they are (Rogers, 1961). As a result, children experience a sense of volition and feel able to
be who they want to be. Autonomy-supportive parents are confident that children are
naturally inclined to grow and develop in a positive direction (Landry et al., 2008), so they do
not feel a constant need to intervene in the children’s development. Instead, they are patient,
they respect the children’s pace of development, and they display a sincere curiosity for what
happens in the children’s lives. Autonomy support can be contrasted with a more controlling
approach, where parents impose their own frame of reference and subsequently evaluate or
even judge the children in light of their capacity to meet expectations and standards that
matter primarily to the parent (Grolnick, 2003).
A first important feature of autonomy-support is parental fostering of task enjoyment.
As much as possible, autonomy-supportive parents try to emphasize the intrinsic value of
activities, they capitalize on children’s interest or they add fun elements to promote the
children’s enjoyment of activities (Reeve, 2009). Even seemingly uninteresting activities,
such as brushing teeth and cleaning up, can be made more fun by making a game out of it, by
telling stories, or by appealing to children’s fantasies. This appeal to the children’s inner
11
motivational resources can be contrasted with an approach relying on external contingencies,
such as rewards and threats of punishment such as removal of privileges.
Further, autonomy-supportive parents allow input and encourage dialogue. They leave
room for negotiation, offer choices, and encourage initiative (Soenens et al., 2007). Such a
participative approach allows children to explore possibilities and different roles and to have a
say in important decisions. Of course, parents cannot always allow their children to make
decisions freely. Sometimes they introduce rules that set limits to the children’s behavior. But
even in these instances parents can be autonomy-supportive by providing a meaningful
rationale. Rather than simply imposing a rule or giving a parent-centered reason for following
a rule, they given explanations that are relevant to the children. Doing so helps children accept
and internalize the personal importance of the rule (Deci et al., 1994).
Autonomy-supportive parents are attuned to the children’s rhythms and pace of
development. When a child gets stuck on a task (e.g., homework), they help patiently and
leave room for the child to come up with a solution rather than taking over the learning
process. This requires that parents trust the child’s natural capacity to develop skills (Landry
et al., 2008). Parental support for autonomy also entails an open attitude towards children’s
negative emotions, oppositional behaviors, and diverging opinions. Rather than minimizing
negative emotions, suppressing undesirable behavior, or invalidating different opinions,
autonomy-supportive parents show an active interest in these “deviant” feelings, behaviors,
and opinions. Rather than perceiving those as irritating, they curiously explore their meaning
or role to fully understand the children’s perspectives. For instance, even when children defy
parental rules, autonomy-supportive parents pay attention to children’s reasons for doing so
and to the feelings that elicited reactance. Having heard the children’s opinions, they
acknowledge the children’s perspective and perhaps flexibly adjust the rule or, if the rule
cannot be changed, explain why the rule is meaningful.
12
Finally, autonomy-supportive parents rely on inviting rather than coercing or
pressuring language. They say things such as “You can try to …”, “I suggest that you.. “, and
“I propose that you …” instead of “You have to …”, “You must …”, and “I expect you to
…”. Pressuring language can be quite overt and explicit but also more subtle. Psychologically
controlling parents (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2010) or parents relying on conditional regard
(Assor et al., 2014) in particular tend to pressure children in insidious ways by expressing
disappointment non-verbally or by appealing to feelings of shame and guilt.
Autonomy-supportive parenting has been found to predict need satisfaction and high-
quality motivation in different domains of life, including school (Grolnick et al., 1991), sports
(Gagné, Ryan, & Bargmann, 2003), and friendships (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2005). When
children perceive their parents as autonomy-supportive, they engage in activities with a sense
of volition and because they want to rather than because they have to. Autonomy support is
also related to high-quality motivation in the context of adherence to parental rules. Children
of autonomy-supportive parents display deeper internalization of parental rules
(Vansteenkiste, Soenens, Van Petegem, & Duriez, 2014). They follow these rules because
they accept and understand the rules rather than because they feel compelled to do so.
Relatedly, autonomy-support fosters open and honest communication in parent-child
relationships (Bureau & Mageau, 2014; Wuyts, Vansteenkiste, Soenens, & Van Petegem,
2015). Possibly because of these beneficial effects of parental autonomy support on children’s
need satisfaction and motivation, autonomy-support is related to adjustment in specific
domains of life and to children’s and adolescents’ overall well-being (Joussemet, Koestner,
Lekes, & Landry, 2005). Parental autonomy-support also contributes to key developmental
skills, such as adequate emotion regulation (Brenning, Soenens, Van Petegem, &
Vansteenkiste, 2015), cognitive self-regulation (Bindman, Pomerantz, & Roisman, 2015), and
altruism and moral development (Roth, 2008).
13
In contrast, controlling parenting has been shown to predict need frustration (Mabbe,
Soenens, Vansteenkiste, & Van Leeuwen, in press), secrecy in parent-child relationships
(Tilton-Weaver, Kerr, Pakalniskeine, Tokic, Salihovic, & Stattin, 2010), maladaptive
motivational orientations such as amotivation (Garn & Jolly, 2015) and oppositional defiance
(Vansteenkiste et al., 2014), and developmental problems such as internalizing distress
(Soenens, Luyckx, Vansteenkiste, Duriez, & Goossens, 2008) and externalizing behaviors
(Joussemet, Vitaro, et al., 2008).
About the Interplay between the Three Dimensions of Parental Need Support
To fully understand the role of parents in children’s satisfaction of the three basic
psychological needs, it is important to consider the interplay of the three dimensions of
parental need support. Of particular relevance is the interplay between structure and
autonomy-support. Some developmental scholars tend to confuse autonomy-support with
parental permissiveness, leniency, and an absence of rules (Baumrind, 2012). However,
autonomy-support can be (and ideally is) combined with structure, in which case parents
provide clear guidelines for behavior and at the same time respect the children’s perspectives
(e.g., by providing a rationale and leaving room for the children’s voices). Autonomy-
supportive parents are more likely to provide structure in a way that fosters competence and
autonomy because their communication of expectations and their provision of assistance is
better attuned to the child’s abilities, preferences and interests. In line with this reasoning,
Sher-Censor, Assor, and Oppenheim (2015) showed that maternal communication of
expectations for behavior (as a feature of structure) was related negatively to adolescents’
externalizing problems only when mothers at the same time scored high on perspective taking
(as a feature of autonomy-support). The combination of structure and autonomy-support
probably helped adolescents to understand the importance of the expectations and to
experience more self-endorsement while behaving in accordance with them.
14
While the combination of structure and autonomy-support gives rise to a harmonious
experience of satisfaction of several needs, other parental behaviors give rise to a conflicting
relationship between different needs. A case in point is conditional regard, a parenting
practice characteristic of parents who provide more love and affection than usual when the
child meets parental expectations and who withdraw their affection and appreciation when the
child fails to meet standards (Assor, Roth, & Deci, 2004). While this parental practice may
yield at least momentary and superficial satisfaction of the need for relatedness, it is a
controlling practice undermining children’s feelings of autonomy and competence. Research
even shows that the detrimental effects of conditional regard are more pronounced when it is
combined with parental warmth (Kanat-Maymon & Assor, 2010). This combination of
conditional regard and warmth may create a loyalty conflict, where children strongly feel that
they need to choose between having a close bond with their parent and preserving a sense of
autonomy. Such internal conflicts ultimately give rise to feelings of resentment towards
parents and to emotional costs in children (Assor et al., 2004, 2014).
The Role of Cultural, Developmental, and Individual Differences
The SDT-based argument that need-supportive parenting appeals to basic and
fundamental needs that universally foster children’s growth is a strong statement that may
lead one to wonder whether in this perspective on parenting there is room for contextual and
individual differences in effects of need-supportive parenting.
An important notion in SDT speaking to this issue is the notion of functional
significance (Deci & Ryan, 1987). This notion refers to differences in the way people appraise
and interpret events. Most things that happen to people can be interpreted in different ways by
different people. For instance, a reward to one child for doing homework may have an
informational value indicating that he or she did a good job, but another child who gets the
reward may interpret it as a control to get him or her to do more homework (Deci, Koestner,
15
& Ryan, 1999). Depending on factors such as age, culture, and personality, different children
may interpret such practices differently.
For example, Pomerantz and Eaton (2000) showed that with increasing age elementary
school children were more likely to view parental involvement in homework as signaling
incompetence and as a threat to their autonomy. As regards culture, several studies have
shown that children and adolescents living in collectivist societies have more benign
interpretations of potentially autonomy-suppressing parenting practices than children from
individualist societies (Miller, Chakravarthy, & Das, 2008; Rudy, Carlo, Lambert, & Awong,
2014). Finally, to capture personality-based differences in the way social events are appraised,
SDT distinguishes between autonomous and controlled causality orientations (Deci & Ryan,
1985), although as a general orientation this typically emerges clearly only in later
adolescence. Research shows that individuals high on the autonomous orientation are inclined
to see the informational value of interpersonal (e.g., parental) behaviors, whereas individuals
high on the controlled orientation tend to more easily experience interpersonal behaviors as
pressuring and intrusive. In fact, a study by Hagger, & Chatzisarantis (2011) showed that
individuals who were high in autonomy interpreted rewards as informational and those high in
controlled orientation interpreted them as controlling.
The fact that there are contextual and individual differences in children’s appraisal and
perception of parental behavior does not contradict SDT’s claims about the universal
importance of the psychological needs. The universality claim in SDT deals with individuals’
experiences of need satisfaction and need frustration. While children may differ in the way
they interpret potentially autonomy-supportive practices, subjectively felt autonomy is said to
be beneficial for all children. Indeed, SDT argues that children’s perceptions of parental
behavior in terms of need support or need thwarting ultimately affect the children’s
developmental outcomes. When parental practices are experienced as supportive of the three
16
psychological needs, they will foster well-being and adjustment. In contrast, when practices
are experienced as a threat to these needs, they will undermine development and increase the
risk for ill-being. Consistent with these claims, evidence shows that subjectively experienced
need-supportive and need-thwarting parenting are related to outcomes similarly across
developmental periods (Joussemet, Landry, & Koestner, 2008), across cultures (Ahmad,
Vansteenkiste, & Soenens, 2013; Chirkov & Ryan, 2001), and irrespective of children’s
personality (Mabbe et al., in press).
We also note that there are limits to the degree to which parental behavior can be
interpreted in various ways (Soenens, Vansteenkiste, & Van Petegem, 2015). Although
children may differ somewhat in the way they perceive parental practices, there are real and
important mean-level differences between parental practices in terms of how need-supportive
and motivating they are. For instance, meta-analyses have shown that while rewards generally
undermine intrinsic motivation (Deci et al., 1999), the provision of choice typically enhances
it (Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008). Thus, while children may vary in the degree to which
they perceive rewards as controlling, they are unlikely to perceive the provision of choice as
more controlling than the provision of rewards. In line with the notion that certain practices
are generally more need-supportive than others, Chen, Soenens, et al. (2015) showed that
while Chinese adolescents had a more benign interpretation of parental guilt-induction than
Belgian adolescents, both Chinese and Belgian adolescents perceived guilt-induction as more
controlling and need-thwarting than parental autonomy-support. Thus, autonomy-supportive
practices were perceived to be generally more favorable to adolescents’ development across
cultures.
Clearly, SDT highlights children’s agency in the socialization process (Reeve, 2013;
Soenens et al., 2015). Rather than being passive recipients of environmental influences,
children give meaning to parental behaviors and actively develop perceptions and
17
representations of their parents. In addition, children also differ in the way they cope with
need-thwarting parental behaviors (Skinner & Edge, 2002). While some children respond to
controlling parental behavior constructively (e.g., by negotiating and by trying to create a
compromise between the parents goals and their own), other children respond defiantly or in
other ways that may contribute to their own need frustration such as simply complying
passively. Although these responses appear to be quite different, in both cases children
experience frustration of their need for autonomy because they do not stay true to their
personal goals and preferences. Future research on these coping responses may reveal why
some children are more resilient to need-thwarting parenting than others and why need-
thwarting parenting is related to different developmental problems in different children. For
instance, while passive compliance may give rise to internalizing difficulties, oppositional
defiance may render children more vulnerable to externalizing problems.
Conclusion
Children have a natural tendency to develop towards higher levels of psychosocial
maturity as they grow older (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Parents can contribute to this
psychological-growth process by supporting children’s needs for relatedness (e.g., by being
warm and responsive), competence (e.g., by providing clear guidelines and by giving positive
feedback and help), and autonomy (e.g., by recognizing the child’s perspective and by
encouraging initiative). When parents thwart these very same needs, they risk forestalling
children’s development or even increasing vulnerability to psychopathology. Various factors
(including age, cultural background, and personality) affect the degree to which potentially
need-supportive parental behaviors are actually experienced by children as need-supportive.
Regardless, the subjective experience of parental need support is universally related to better
psychosocial adjustment, resilience, and well-being.
18
References
Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Roth, G. (2014). Parental conditional regard: Psychological
costs and antecedents. In N. Weinstein (Ed.), Human motivation and interpersonal
relationships (pp. 215-237). Springer Netherlands.
Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional costs of parents’ conditional regard:
A self-determination theory analysis. Journal of Psychology, 72, 47-88.
Barber, B. K., Stolz, H. E., & Olsen, J. A. (2005). Parental support, psychological control, and
behavioral control: Assessing relevance across time, culture, and method. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 70, 1-137.
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., Bosch, J. A., & Thogerson-Ntoumani, C.
(2011). Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: The role of interpersonal
control and psychological need thwarting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37,
1459-1473.
Baumrind, D. (2012). Differentiating between confrontive and coercive kinds of parental
power-assertive disciplinary practices. Human Development, 55, 35-51.
Bindman, S. W., Pomerantz, E. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). Do children’s executive
functions account for associations between early autonomy-supportive parenting and
achievement through high school? Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 756-770.
Boone, L., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Van der kaap-Deeder, J., & Verstuyf, J. (2014).
Self critical perfectionism and binge eating symptoms: A longitudinal test of the
intervening role of psychological need frustration. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61,
363-373.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base, Clinical applications of attachment theory. London:
Routledge.
19
Brenning, K., Soenens, B., Van Petegem, S., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2015). Perceived maternal
autonomy support and early adolescent emotion regulation: A longitudinal study. Social
Development, 24, 561-578.
Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Nelemans, S. A., De Castro, B. O., Overbeek, G., &
Bushman, B. J. (2015). Origins of narcissism in children. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 112, 3659-3662.
Bureau, J. S., & Mageau, G. A. (2014). Parental autonomy support and honesty: The
mediating role of identification with the honesty value and perceived costs and benefits of
honesty. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 225-236.
Chen, B., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Van Petegem, S., & Beyers, W. (2015). Where do
the cultural differences in dynamics of controlling parenting lie? Adolescents as active
agents in the perception of and coping with parental behavior. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Chen, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Beyers, W., Boone, L., Deci, E. L., et. al (2015). Basic psychological
need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motivation and Emotion,
39, 216-236.
Chirkov, V. I., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Parent and teacher autonomy-support in Russian and
U.S. adolescents: Common effects on well-being and academic motivation. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 618-635.
Costa, S., Ntoumanis, N., & Bartholomew, K. J. (2015). Predicting the brighter and darker
sides of interpersonal relationships: Does psychological need thwarting matter? Motivation
and Emotion, 39, 11-24.
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to
distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development, 77, 44-58.
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The
self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119-142.
20
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments
examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological bulletin,
125, 627-668.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The General Causality Orientations Scale: Self-
determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109-134.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and the “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Autonomy and need satisfaction in close relationships:
Relationships Motivation Theory. In N. Weinstein (Ed.), Human motivation and
interpersonal relationships (pp. 53-73). Springer Netherlands.
Farkas, M. S., & Grolnick,W. S. (2010). Examining the components and concomitants of
parental structure in the academic domain. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 266-279.
Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in
the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 372-
390.
Garn, A. C., & Jolly, J. L. (2015). A model of parental achievement-oriented psychological
control in academically gifted students. High Ability Studies, 26, 105-116.
Grolnick, W. S. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How well-meant parenting
backfires. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The
self-determination theory perspective. In J. E. Grusec, & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting
and children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135-
161). New York: Wiley.
Grolnick, W. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). Issues and challenges in studying parental
control: Toward a new conceptualization. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 165-170.
21
Grolnick, W. S., RafteryHelmer, J. N., Flamm, E. S., Marbell, K. N., & Cardemil, E. V.
(2015). Parental provision of academic structure and the transition to middle school.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25, 668-684.
Grolnick, W. S., Raftery-Helmer, J. N., Marbell, K. N., Flamm, E. S., Cardemil, E. V., &
Sanchez, M. (2014). Parental provision of structure: Implementation and correlates in three
domains. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 60, 355-384.
Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources for school achievement:
Motivational mediators of children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 83, 508-517.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2011). Causality orientations moderate the
undermining effect of rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 47, 485-489.
Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in learning activities: It's not
autonomy support or structure, but autonomy support and structure. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 102, 588-600.
Joussemet, M., Koestner, R., Lekes, N., & Landry, R. (2005). A longitudinal study of the
relationship of maternal autonomy support to children’s adjustment and achievement in
school. Journal of Personality, 73, 1215-1235.
Joussemet, M., Landry, R., & Koestner, R. (2008). A self-determination theory perspective on
parenting, Canadian Psychology, 49, 194-200.
Joussemet, M., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Côté, S., Nagin, D. S., Zoccolillo, M., & Tremblay,
R. E. (2008). Controlling parenting and physical aggression during elementary school.
Child Development, 79, 411-425.
22
Kamins, M. L., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Person versus process praise and criticism:
implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology, 35, 835-
848.
Kanat-Maymon, M., & Assor, A. (2010). Perceived maternal control and responsiveness to
distress as predictors of young adults' empathic responses. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 36, 33-46.
Landry, R., Whipple, N., Mageau, G., Joussemet, M., & Koestner, R. (2008). Trust in
organismic development, autonomy-support, and adaptation among mothers and their
children. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 173-188.
Luyckx, K., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., & Duriez, B. (2009). Basic need satisfaction
and identity exploration and commitment: Bridging self-determination theory and process-
oriented identity research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 276-288.
Mabbe, E., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Van Leeuwen, K. (in druk). Do personality
traits moderate relations between psychologically controlling parenting and problem
behavior in adolescents? Journal of Personality.
Mauras, C. P., Grolnick, W. S., & Friendly, R. W. (2013). Time for “The Talk” … Now
what? Autonomy support and structure in mother-daughter conversations about sex.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 33, 458-481.
Milkie, M. A., Nomaguchi, K. M., & Denny, K. E. (2015). Does the amount of time mothers
spend with children or adolescents matter? Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 355-372.
Miller, J. G., Das, R., & Chakravarthy, S. (2011). Culture and the role of choice in agency.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 46-61.
Mouratidis, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Michou, A., & Lens, W. (2013). Perceived structure and
achievement goals as predictors of students' self-regulated learning and affect and the
23
mediating role of competence need satisfaction. Learning and Individual Differences, 23,
179-186.
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation
and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134,
270-300.
Pomerantz, E. M., & Eaton, M. M. (2000). Developmental differences in children’s
conceptions of parental control: “They love me, but they make me feel incompetent”.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 140-167.
Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D. (2007). The how, whom, and why of
parents’ involvement in children’s academic lives: More is not always better. Review of
Educational Research, 77, 373-410.
Putnick, D. L., Bornstein, M. H., Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T.,
... & Alampay, L. P. (2014). Perceived mother and father acceptancerejection predict
four unique aspects of child adjustment across nine countries. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 56, 923-932.
Reeve, J. (2006). Extrinsic rewards and inner motivations. In C. Weinstein, & T. L. Good
(Eds.), Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues
(pp. 645-664). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how
they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44, 159-175.
Reeve, J. (2013). How students create motivationally supportive learning environments for
themselves: The concept of agentic engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105,
579-595.
Rogers, C. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
24
Roth, G. (2008). Perceived parental conditional regard and autonomy support as predictors of
young adults’ self- versus other-oriented prosocial tendencies. Journal of Personality, 76,
513-533.
Rudy, D., Carlo, G., Lambert, M. C., & Awong, T. (2014). Undergraduates’ perceptions of
parental relationship-oriented guilt induction versus harsh psychological control: Does
cultural group status moderate their associations with self-esteem? Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 45, 905-920.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78.
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2015). Autonomy and autonomy disturbances
in self-development and psychopathology: Research on motivation, attachment, and
clinical process. To appear in D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (3rd Ed.
Vol ). New York: Wiley.
Sher-Censor, E., Assor, A., & Oppenheim, D. (2015). The interplay between observed
maternal perspective taking and clear expectations: Links with male adolescents’
externalizing and internalizing problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 930-
936.
Skinner, E. A., & Edge, K. (2002). Parenting, motivation, and the development of children's
coping. In L. J. Crockett (Ed.), Agency, motivation, and the life course: The Nebraska
symposium on motivation, Vol. 48 (pp. 77-143). Lincoln, NE: University Of Nebraska
Press.
Skinner, E. Johnson, S., & Snyder, T. (2005). Six dimensions of parenting: A motivational
model. Parenting-Science and Practices, 5, 175-235.
25
Soenens, B., Duriez, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Goossens, L. (2007). The intergenerational
transmission of empathy-related responding in adolescence: The role of maternal support.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1-13.
Soenens, B., Luyckx, K., Vansteenkiste, Duriez, B., & Goossens, L. (2008). Clarifying the
link between perceived parental psychological control and adolescents’ depressive
feelings: A test of reciprocal versus unidirectional models of influence. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 54, 411-444.
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents and outcomes of self-determination in
three life domains: The role of parents’ and teachers’ autonomy support. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 34, 589-604.
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the concept of parental
psychological control: Proposing new insights on the basis of self-determination theory.
Developmental Review, 30, 74-99.
Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Beyers, W., & Ryan, R.
M. (2007). Conceptualizing parental autonomy support: Adolescent perceptions of
promotion of independence versus promotion of volitional functioning. Developmental
Psychology, 43, 633-646.
Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Van Petegem, S. (2015). Let us not throw out the baby
with the bathwater: Applying the principle of universalism without uniformity to
autonomy-supportive and controlling parenting. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 44-49.
Tilton-Weaver, L., Kerr, M., Pakalniskeine, V., Tokic, A., Salihovic, S., & Stattin, H. (2010).
Open up or close down: How do parental reactions affect youth information management?
Journal of Adolescence, 33, 333-346.
26
Van IJzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and
infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment
Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387-403.
Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2013). On psychological growth and vulnerability: Basic
psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. Journal of
Psychotherapy Integration, 23, 263-280.
Vansteenkiste, M. & Soenens, B. (2015). Vitamines voor groei: Ontwikkeling voeden vanuit
de zelf-determinatie theorie [Vitamins for psychological growth: A self-determination
theory perspective on support for children’s development]. Acco: Leuven, Belgium.
Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Van Petegem, S., & Duriez, B. (2014). Longitudinal
associations between adolescent perceived degree and style of prohibition and adolescent
internalization and defiance. Developmental Psychology, 50, 229-236.
Veronneau, M.-H., Koestner, R., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2005). Intrinsic need satisfaction and
well-being in children and adolescents: An application of the self-determination theory.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 280-292.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wuyts, D., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., & Van Petegem, S. (2015). The role of observed
maternal autonomy support, reciprocity, and psychological need satisfaction in adolescent
disclosure. Manuscript submitted for publication.
27
Figure 1
Conceptual Model of the Associations among Parental Support for Children’s Needs, Needs Experiences, and Developmental Outcomes
Relatedness
Supports
+ Involvement, warmth,
responsiveness
- Cold, aloof, indifference,
rejection
+ Structure, clear and
positive communications
- Chaotic communication,
unpredictability, criticism
+ Empathy, allowing choice,
encouraging initiative
Adaptive developmental,
motivational, and social
outcomes
Maladaptive developmental,
motivational, and social
outcomes
- Controllingness,
domination, intrusiveness
Relatedness
Thwarts
Competence
Supports
Competence
Thwarts
Autonomy
Supports
Autonomy
Thwarts
... Parental psychological control includes behaviors such as intrusiveness, criticism, and manipulation (Barber et al., 2012;Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2010). Parental emotional support (hereafter called support) includes warmth, affection, companionship, and intimacy (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985;Soenens et al., 2017). According to the Self-Determination Theory (SDT), parental psychological control actively thwarts children's psychological functioning, whereas parental support actively promotes children's psychological functioning . ...
... Accordingly, parental psychological control can be understood as a risk factor, with more psychological control hindering children's psychological functioning, whereas a lack of psychological control is not necessarily fostering better functioning. Parental support can be understood as a promotive factor, with more support promoting better psychological functioning, whereas a lack of support is not necessarily hindering children's functioning (Farrington et al., 2016;Soenens et al., 2017;Stouthamer-Loeber et al., 2002). Hence, we examined whether changes (in relation to an individual's average) in parental psychological control and/or support predicted within-family changes in adolescents' psychological functioning. ...
... (i.e., sensory processing sensitivity) than all others. Finally, a substantive number of adolescents responded in opposite way from what is expected from universal parenting theories (Rohner et al., 2005;Soenens et al., 2017), whom did not fit in hypothesized responsivity patterns. ...
Article
Full-text available
According to environmental sensitivity models, children vary in responsivity to parenting. However, different models propose different patterns, with responsivity to primarily: (1) adverse parenting (adverse sensitive); or (2) supportive parenting (vantage sensitive); or (3) to both (differentially susceptible). This preregistered study tested whether these three responsivity patterns coexist. We used intensive longitudinal data of Dutch adolescents (N = 256, Mage = 14.8, 72% female) who bi-weekly reported on adverse and supportive parenting and their psychological functioning (tmean = 17.7, tmax = 26). Dynamic Structural Equation Models (DSEM) indeed revealed differential parenting effects. As hypothesized, we found that all three responsivity patterns coexisted in our sample: 5% were adverse sensitive, 3% vantage sensitive, and 26% differentially susceptible. No adolescent appeared unsusceptible, however. Instead, we labeled 28% as unperceptive, because they did not perceive any changes in parenting and scored lower on trait environmental sensitivity than others. Furthermore, unexpected patterns emerged, with 37% responding contrary to parenting theories (e.g., decreased psychological functioning after more parental support). Sensitivity analyses with concurrent effects and parent-reported parenting were performed. Overall, findings indicate that theorized responsivity-to-parenting patterns might coexist in the population, and that there are other, previously undetected patterns that go beyond environmental sensitivity models.
... Parental autonomy support is defined by two main components: (a) the provision of choice and allowance of independent decision-making, and (b) acknowledgment and interest in the adolescent's perspective (Soenens et al., 2017). To capture both components, adolescents rated two items that were adapted from a 4-item daily autonomy support scale (van der Kaap-Deeder et al., 2017). ...
... Parental warmth includes the: (a) provision of affection, and (b) parental care and responsiveness (Soenens et al., 2017), which was rated by adolescents with two items. The items were adapted from a Dutch daily diary study (Keijsers et al., 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Who influences whom in the parent-adolescent relationship? We propose that the answer to this longstanding question is specific to the family. To test whether the direction of effects indeed varies across families, we applied an idiographic (family-specific) approach. Dutch adolescents (N=159, Mage=13.31, 62% female) reported on perceived parenting and their affective well-being for 100 consecutive days. Pre-registered analysis revealed that some families showed a reciprocal day-to-day effect between parenting and adolescent affective well-being (11.4%-54.7%), whereas others showed a parent-driven effect (8.2%-43.4%), an adolescent-driven effect (10.1%-27.0%), or no effects at all (15.7%-60.1%). Also within a family, direction of effects varied across parenting dimensions. Adolescents with higher trait levels of environmental sensitivity and neuroticism seemed more strongly influenced by parenting. Thus, our study suggests that daily parent-adolescent dynamics are family specific. This stresses the need to move towards an idiographic parenting science to truly understand the dynamics within unique families.
... Parental influences on child mental health are well established in the literature, such that previous research finds a strong association between elements of parent-child relationships and psychopathology (Barger et al., 2019;Rothenberg et al., 2020). Healthier relational contexts characterized by more parental involvement, support, mutual regard, and lower conflict have consistently been associated with better psychological states at various stages of development (Soenens et al., 2017). Higher quality parent-child relationships also influence the development of social skills and prosocial behaviors, often stemming from a key factor -empathy (Barger et al., 2019;Daniel et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
A key facet of socioemotional development is the ability to practice prosocial behavior. Prior research has emphasized the role parenting has in developing this skill from early childhood to adolescence. However, the continuity of this relationship during the complex developmental period of emerging adulthood is understudied. The current study examined the indirect effects of mothers’ and fathers’ relationship quality on internalizing and externalizing problems via empathy for emerging adult women and men. Participants (N = 469) were college-attending emerging adults (aged 18–25) who reported on their current parent-child relationship quality, empathy, and psychopathology. Partially consistent with hypotheses, a direct effect between paternal parent-child relationship quality and empathy in women and men was found; however, a significant direct effect between maternal parent-child relationship quality and empathy occurred only in women. There was a significant direct effect between empathy and internalizing/externalizing problems in women and men. For women, maternal and pater- nal relationship quality had indirect effects on internalizing and externalizing problems via empathy. However, an indirect effect in men was demonstrated only for paternal relationship quality on externalizing problems. Implications include how fostering healthy parent-child relationships might impact display of prosocial behaviors in adulthood, which is essential for developing and maintaining healthy relationships throughout the lifespan.
... The concept of sensitivity is somewhat similar to SDT's concept of relatedness support, which indeed refers to parental warmth, involvement, and responsiveness to distress (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997;Joussemet, Landry, & Koestner, 2008;Soenens, Deci, & Vansteenkiste, 2017). However, according to SDT, there is more to relatedness support than sensitivity in moments of distress (Ryan, Brown, & Creswell, 2007). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Self-Determination Theory (SDT) is a broad theory of psychological growth and wellness that has revolutionized how we think about human motivation and the driving forces behind personality development. SDT focuses on people’s basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness and how social environments that support these needs foster more volition, vitality, and full functioning. SDT has supplied the basis for new and more effective practices in parenting, education, business, sport, healthcare, and other areas of life, fostering higher-quality motivation, engagement, and satisfaction. Drawing on over four decades of evidence-based research and application, The Oxford Handbook of Self-Determination Theory delivers a truly integrative volume by the top researchers and experts within the field of SDT. Edited by SDT co-founder Richard M. Ryan, this Handbook not only provides the theory’s historical and scientific underpinnings but also draws together the latest research and insights, covering topics from the social and biological underpinnings of motivation and wellness to practical applications in all aspects of life. This volume will be an invaluable resource for both researchers and practitioners, as well as any student of human nature, with practical research and guidance.
... Autonomia refere-se à capacidade de o indivíduo tomar decisões sobre a sua própria vida, de forma independente, estabelecendo as suas normas de conduta (Soenens et al., 2017). Para os autores, a construção da autonomia é um ponto-chave no processo de desenvolvimento, que se inicia desde cedo, mas assume-se como uma tarefa com especial relevância no período da adolescência. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Abstract Introduction: Suicidal in teenagers has increased. Although uncommon, the use of sodium nitrate has also increased, stimulated by online information of unpainful death and easy acquisition. It is an oxidant that converts hemoglobin into methemoglobin, causing asphyxia, vasodilation and hypotension, with high mortality when consumed in high doses. There is effective therapy, if early initiated. Clinical case: This is a case of sodium nitrate voluntary ingestion, bought online as a fertilizer, in a previously healthy 17-year-old boy. The boy called 911 due to vomiting and lower limb paresthesia. At the arrival of the rescuers, the boy was dizzy, reactive, hypotensive, tachycardic, hypoxemic and cyanotic. During transportation, he entered cardiorespiratory arrest and basic life support was initiated. In emergency room he was intubated and ventilated effectively (PaO2 293 mmol/L), maintaining exuberant cyanotic extremities. The initial rhythm was electrical activity without pulse, followed by ventricular fibrillation, with approach following the algorithms. The Antivenom Information Center was contacted and methylene blue was started. He did not recover spontaneous circulation after 35 minutes of resuscitation. His sister was evaluated by the Hospital Organization for Child and Youth Risk Management and started having pedopsychiatric appointments, due to suicidal ideation.
... Autonomia refere-se à capacidade de o indivíduo tomar decisões sobre a sua própria vida, de forma independente, estabelecendo as suas normas de conduta (Soenens et al., 2017). Para os autores, a construção da autonomia é um ponto-chave no processo de desenvolvimento, que se inicia desde cedo, mas assume-se como uma tarefa com especial relevância no período da adolescência. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Abstract Background: SARS-CoV-2 pandemic has led to socioeconomic and family changes. During lockdown, due to increased intrafamily conflict and lack of contact with other protective adults, there was an increased risk of child physical abuse. Concomitantly, with school closures, there was a decrease in the risk of physical aggression at school. Objective: To compare pediatric emergency admissions due to physical abuse at the beginning of the school year before and during SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. Methods: Retrospective and descriptive study. Children observed in the emergency department due to physical abuse between September and November of four consecutive years (2018 to 2021) were included. Results: Included 169 emergency admissions (102 pre-pandemic vs. 67 during the pandemic). In 33% (n=56) there was a previous history of physical abuse. School aggression was the main form of aggression (46% vs. 40%), followed by family aggression (30% vs. 31%) and delinquency (23,5% vs. 28% during). The episodes were considered moderate to severe in 17% before the pandemic versus 27% during the pandemic. During the pandemic, there was a greater referral to the Hospital Center for Children and Teenagers at Risk (p=0.011), a greater number of children/teenagers referred to an institution (p=0.013) and to the Public Ministry (p=0.023). Conclusions: There was a decrease in emergency admissions due to physical abuse during the pandemic. There was a decrease in school aggression and an increase in cases of delinquency. There was no increase in intrafamilial physical abuse. There is greater awareness of health care professionals of these cases, with more referral.
... Employees' levels of motivation for their professional activities influence both their performance and their overall well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Since individual's learning and inquisitive attitudes toward their jobs are strongly ingrained in their curiosity for their jobs (Olafsen et al., 2018;Soenens et al., 2017). SDT has the potential to be a useful instrument for investigating the origins, processes, and antecedents of work curiosity that enhance employee creativity. ...
... Employees' levels of motivation for their professional activities influence both their performance and their overall well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Since individual's learning and inquisitive attitudes toward their jobs are strongly ingrained in their curiosity for their jobs (Olafsen et al., 2018;Soenens et al., 2017). SDT has the potential to be a useful instrument for investigating the origins, processes, and antecedents of work curiosity that enhance employee creativity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Curiosity motivates human inquisitiveness, teaches us new things, and encourages us to be open to new forms of creativity. The probing nature of a curious mind is often believed to enhance an individual's ability to generate new ideas and enhance creativity. Based on Self-determination theory, we examine the effect of work curiosity on employee creativity. Further, we studied the mediating role of linking ideas between work curiosity and employee creativity. In this study, mindfulness acts as a moderating variable between work curiosity and employee creativity. To examine the direct effect and the mediation purpose was used model 4 and for moderation we used model 1 from Process Hayes. The results shows that there is positive association between work curiosity and employee creativity and linking ideas mediates the relation. Mindfulness moderates the relation between work curiosity and employee creativity. Future research implications are discussed.
Chapter
Self-Determination Theory (SDT) is a broad theory of psychological growth and wellness that has revolutionized how we think about human motivation and the driving forces behind personality development. SDT focuses on people’s basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness and how social environments that support these needs foster more volition, vitality, and full functioning. SDT has supplied the basis for new and more effective practices in parenting, education, business, sport, healthcare, and other areas of life, fostering higher-quality motivation, engagement, and satisfaction. Drawing on over four decades of evidence-based research and application, The Oxford Handbook of Self-Determination Theory delivers a truly integrative volume by the top researchers and experts within the field of SDT. Edited by SDT co-founder Richard M. Ryan, this Handbook not only provides the theory’s historical and scientific underpinnings but also draws together the latest research and insights, covering topics from the social and biological underpinnings of motivation and wellness to practical applications in all aspects of life. This volume will be an invaluable resource for both researchers and practitioners, as well as any student of human nature, with practical research and guidance.
Chapter
Motivation science has advanced tremendously in the past decade. However, it is now clear that future progress is going to be stalled by the extent of disagreement among motivation scientists to some basic, yet controversial, questions. To help move motivation science toward greater coherence, the editors recruited prominent scholars to debate their contrasting perspectives. Such debate is not only interesting, but it also makes future research, discoveries, collaborations, and applications more fruitful. Because many excellent handbooks on motivation exist, the editors wanted to try something different—be provocative. They wanted to provoke creative ideas among the authors and readers. To achieve that end, they asked 10 thought-provoking questions that define contemporary motivation science’s most important, controversial, and provocative ideas. The questions deal with the nature of motivation, cultural differences in motivational processes, evidence-based strategies to enhance motivation, unresolved controversies, predictions of the future, and more. This volume features 67 individual author responses to these questions. Multiple authors shared their current thinking and insights to the same controversial question. This volume provides readers with a rare opportunity to see how different theorists and researchers recognize, evaluate, and prescribe solutions to the same motivation problem. By sharing current thinking and providing innovative insights into the important questions and controversies in the study of motivation, this volume informs readers about cutting-edge theory and research in motivation that they can use to generate fresh and effective applications and interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Although research increasingly addresses the role of parenting in fostering adolescent disclosure, most research relied on self-report measures of parenting and did not address the role of autonomy support. In the present observational study (conducted in Belgium), with 62 mother-adolescent dyads (mean age mothers = 44 years; mean age adolescents = 14 years; 77% of adolescents female), we rated mothers' provision of autonomy support during a 10-minute conversation about friendships. We found that observed maternal autonomy support was related positively to adolescents' degree of and volitional reasons for disclosure about friends. These associations were mediated by observed non-verbal reciprocity during the conversation and by adolescent satisfaction of their needs for autonomy and relatedness. Mothers' autonomy-support and mother-adolescent reciprocity also predicted mothers' own psychological need satisfaction and conversation pleasure. The relevance of the findings for adolescent autonomy and disclosure are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined parents' provision of structure, defined as the organization of the environment to facilitate competence, and the degree to which it supports versus controls children's autonomy, in the domains of homework and studying, unsupervised time, and responsibilities in a diverse sample of sixth-grade children and their parents. Four components of structure and four components of autonomy support were combined into composites that were independent. Parents provided the most structure and least autonomy support in the unsupervised domain. Structure was associated with several competence outcomes in the unsupervised domain, whereas relations between autonomy support and outcomes were more prevalent in the other domains. Results suggest the importance of differentiating structure and the way it is implemented and considering the meaning of structure within different domains.
Article
Full-text available
There is ongoing debate about the universal or culture-specific role of controlling parenting in children’s and adolescents’ development. This study addressed the possibility of cultural variability in how controlling parenting practices are perceived and dealt with. Specifically, we examined Belgian ('N' = 341) and Chinese ('N' = 316) adolescents’ perceptions of and reactions towards a vignette depicting parental guilt-induction, relative to generally controlling and autonomy supportive vignettes. Whereas Belgian adolescents perceived guilt-induction to be as controlling as generally controlling parental behavior, Chinese adolescents’ perception of guilt-induction as controlling was more moderate. Belgian and Chinese adolescents also showed some similarities and differences in their responses to the feelings of need frustration following from the controlling practices, with compulsive compliance for instance being more common in Chinese adolescents. Discussion focuses on cross-cultural similarities and differences in dynamics of controlling parenting.
Chapter
Self-determination theory (SDT) maintains that the adequate support and satisfaction of individuals' psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness promotes the gradual unfolding of individuals' integrative tendencies, as manifested through intrinsic motivation, internalization, identity development, and integrative emotion regulation. At the same time, the thwarting of these same psychological needs and the resultant need frustration is presumed to evoke or amplify a variety of psychopathologies, many of which involve autonomy disturbances. We begin by defining what autonomy involves and how socializing agents, particularly parents, can provide a nurturing (i.e., need-supportive) environment, and we review research within the SDT literature that has shed light on various integrative tendencies and how caregivers facilitate them. In the second part of this chapter, we detail how many forms of psychopathology involve autonomy disturbances and are associated with a history of psychological need thwarting. We especially focus on internally controlling regulation in internalizing disorders; impairments of internalization in conduct disorders and antisocial behavior; and fragmented self-functioning in borderline and dissociative disorders. The role of autonomy support as an ameliorative factor in treatment settings is then discussed among other translational issues. Finally we highlight some implications of recognizing the important role of basic psychological needs for both growth-related and pathology-related processes. Keywords: autonomy; attachment; internalization; parenting; psychological needs; self-determination theory
Book
De ontwikkeling van kinderen en jongeren staat in onze snel evoluerende maatschappij onder druk. Het aanbod aan ontwikkelingsmogelijkheden is dan wel enorm groot, het is voor kinderen niet vanzelfsprekend om een stevig verankerd intern kompas op te bouwen dat hun persoonlijke interesses en overtuigingen weerspiegelt. Ook ouders worden op de proef gesteld om hun kinderen te helpen bij de vorming van een intern kompas en hun ontwikkeling te ondersteunen. In dit handboek wordt, op basis van de Zelf-Determinatie Theorie, betoogd dat de psychologische behoeftes aan autonomie, relationele verbondenheid ,en competentie de cruciale vitamines zijn voor groei. Door in deze vitamines te voorzien voeden ouders, leerkrachten en hulpverleners de ontwikkeling van kinderen en jongeren, terwijl het ondermijnen van deze behoeftes hun groei vertraagt of zelfs verzet en psychopathologie in de hand werkt. Er wordt in het boek toegelicht hoe kinderen die deze psychologische vitamines krijgen nieuwsgierig en geboeid door het leven gaan, eerder luisteren en meewerken uit vrije wil dan omdat het moet, en er beter in slagen om een gefundeerd intern kompas op te bouwen, dat hen richting geeft bij het maken van identiteitskeuzes. Het handboek sluit af met een uitgebreid deel over de manier waarop ouders, maar ook hulpverleners, een motiverende rol kunnen spelen in de opvoeding en hoe ze in deze broodnodige vitamines van groei voor hun kroost kunnen voorzien. [Self-Determination Theory is a highly influential theory on motivation and development. This book provides an accessible and up-to-date overview of key principles of Self-Determination Theory and explores its relevance for key outcomes of child development, including internalization of parental rules, identity formation, and well-being. Self-Determination Theory is discussed and compared with classic and contemporaneous theories in developmental psychology. On the basis of Self-Determination Theory, it is argued that three basic psychological needs represent crucial ‘vitamins’ for human psychological growth: autonomy, competence and relatedness. By providing these vitamins, parents, teachers and community workers actively nourish the development of children and teenagers. If these needs are thwarted, however, vulnerability to psychopathology might ensue. Citing many research results and discussing many case studies, the authors map out the developmental stages and motivational problems of children in several key domains life. Research increasingly demonstrates that children who receive vitamins for growth are more curious and fascinated by things that happen in their life. They listen to their parents because they want to rather than because they have to.]