ArticlePDF Available

Putting attachment in its place: Disciplinary and cultural contexts

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This paper examines the reception of John Bowlby’s and Mary Ainsworth’s ethological theory of attachment among anthropologists and cultural psychologists. First, it shows that from Margaret Mead’s criticisms in the mid 1950s to the present, many of them have challenged the main tenets of attachment theory but attachment theorists ignored those challenges. Second, it argues that we need to understand the different disciplinary goals of psychology and anthropology after WWII in order to illuminate the lack of attention to children’s cultural context in attachment research. The privileging within psychology of laboratory data over field observations supported the rise of attachment research focused on the strange situation procedure and contributed to the neglect of ethnographic data about children in their socio-cultural milieu. Recognizing the importance of studying children in context, however, recent studies by anthropologists and developmental psychologists sensitive to the power of culture have deepened the challenge to attachment theory.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pedp20
Download by: [University of Toronto Libraries] Date: 06 April 2017, At: 12:58
European Journal of Developmental Psychology
ISSN: 1740-5629 (Print) 1740-5610 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pedp20
Putting attachment in its place: Disciplinary and
cultural contexts
Marga Vicedo
To cite this article: Marga Vicedo (2017): Putting attachment in its place: Disciplinary and cultural
contexts, European Journal of Developmental Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2017.1289838
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1289838
Published online: 14 Feb 2017.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 40
View related articles
View Crossmark data
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1289838
Putting attachment in its place: Disciplinary and
cultural contexts
Marga Vicedo
Institute for the History and Philosophy of Science and Technology, University of Toronto, Toronto,
Canada
ABSTRACT
This paper examines the reception of John Bowlby’s and Mary Ainsworth’s
ethological theory of attachment among anthropologists and cultural
psychologists. First, it shows that from Margaret Mead’s criticisms in the mid 1950s
to the present, many of them have challenged the main tenets of attachment theory
but attachment theorists ignored those challenges. Second, it argues that we need
to understand the dierent disciplinary goals of psychology and anthropology
after WWII in order to illuminate the lack of attention to children’s cultural context
in attachment research. The privileging within psychology of laboratory data
over eld observations supported the rise of attachment research focused on
the strange situation procedure and contributed to the neglect of ethnographic
data about children in their socio-cultural milieu. Recognizing the importance
of studying children in context, however, recent studies by anthropologists and
developmental psychologists sensitive to the power of culture have deepened
the challenge to attachment theory.
ARTICLE HISTORY Received 24 September 2016; Accepted 28 January 2017
KEYWORDS Attachment theory; strange situation; anthropology; ethnographic studies of childrearing;
cultural context of child development
Introduction
In 1951 British psychiatrist John Bowlby published a widely inuential report,
Maternal Care and Mental Health, for the World Health Organization. He asserted
that psychiatrists and psychologists had determined that maternal care and love
is essential for the proper emotional development of infants (Bowlby, 1951). In
the following years, Bowlby turned to animal studies to develop his ethological
attachment theory. According to this theory, evolution has designed a system
of attachment between mother and child; without that secure attachment, chil-
dren grow up emotionally scarred (Bowlby, 1969). Since American/Canadian
© 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
CONTACT Marga Vicedo marga.vicedo@utoronto.ca
2 M. VICEDO
psychologist Mary Ainsworth presented empirical work to support Bowlby’s
views, today they are considered the co-founders of the ethological attachment
theory, which has become an inuential paradigm in child-socialization.
Yet, numerous critics have pointed out substantial aws in attachment theory.
My historical examination of the development and reception of Bowlby’s and
Ainsworth’s views from the 1950s to the 1980s showed that they ignored the
criticisms (Vicedo, 2013). In this book, I focused on Bowlby’s appeal to animal
research in order to support his theory.
Here, I want to show how many anthropologists and cultural psychologists
have also challenged attachment theory from its inception. First, I present
Margaret Mead’s views. Mead noted that her own studies in non-Western soci-
eties revealed the existence of a greater diversity of behavior than Bowlby’s
theory acknowledged. However, Bowlby and Ainsworth ignored the call to
recognize the diversity of child rearing patterns uncovered by ethnographic
studies. Second, I analyze how the strange situation, a laboratory procedure
to evaluate attachment patterns developed by Ainsworth, contributed to the
success of attachment theory. I argue that the rise of attachment theory, despite
the glaring omission of data about a variety of cultures, was enabled in part by
the focus of psychologists on laboratory procedures. This methodological pref-
erence contributed to the study of children in isolation from their socio-cultural
contexts. Nevertheless, from the 1980s on, ethnographic work about mothers,
children, and families in their socio-cultural contexts has deepened the chal-
lenges to central tenets of the ethological theory of attachment behavior and
to the primacy of the laboratory over eldwork in studies of human behavior
and emotions.
Mead: Variability in the eld
Margaret Mead was among the rst anthropologists who paid serious attention
to childhood in her work about Samoan society and her photographic study
of children in Balinese culture (Bateson & Mead, 1942). She became one of the
leaders of the Culture and Personality movement, which aimed to study the
eects of childrearing on individual personality and larger social structures.
With the rise of interest in children’s emotional development after WWII, Mead
became a prominent participant in interdisciplinary discussions about this topic.
Mead expressed her concern about growing trends in child development
research around mid-century. Commenting on Bowlby’s (1951) WHO report and
other studies that also emphasized the detrimental eects of maternal sepa-
ration, she deplored the inated statements about the role of a single factor in
child rearing and the ‘exaggerated and poorly supported claims of the impor-
tance of the mother as a single gure in the infant’s life’ (Mead, 1954, p. 476).
Contrary to Bowlby and other researchers who focused almost exclusively on
the role of mothers, Mead maintained that ‘the character formation of the child
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 3
represents the child’s total environmental situation as it is responded to and
introcepted by that child in terms of its constitution and individual life history’
(Mead, 1954, p. 474). In her opinion, many authors overstated the damaging
eects on children of separation – even for brief times – from their mothers.
Mead pointed out that there was no anthropological evidence to support ‘the
value of such an accentuation of the tie between mother and child. On the con-
trary, cross-cultural studies suggest that adjustment is most facilitated if the
child is cared for by many warm, friendly people’ (Mead, 1954, p. 477, my empha-
sis). Bowlby, however, continued to focus on the tie between mother and child.
Ignoring Mead’s criticisms, Bowlby tellingly entitled his rst presentation
of his ethological theory of attachment, ‘The nature of the child’s tie to his
mother. In this classic paper, Bowlby did not review data from dierent cul-
tures. Nevertheless, he postulated a biological basis for the bond between a
mother and her child and argued that a secure bond is necessary for a child’s
adequate psychological development (Bowlby, 1958). However, many scholars
criticized the data Bowlby was relying upon, and in 1962 the WHO published
a reassessment of its 1951 report (WHO, 1962). Although this publication is
rarely cited, in contrast to the 1951 WHO report this volume included articles
by major researchers who criticized the conclusions Bowlby drew from existing
studies about maternal separation. Among many methodological aws that they
criticized were his extrapolations from studies about maternal deprivation in
extreme circumstances to the everyday relations between mothers and children
in regular households. Bowlby declined an opportunity to reply to his critics.
But he supervised the reply oered by Ainsworth in this volume (see Vicedo,
2013 for further details).
Mead penned one of papers in that 1962 volume, and again pointed out the
existence of dierent types of families and child rearing practices. In her view,
Bowlby wrongly assumed that childcare could ‘not be safely distributed among
several gures, and that all separations from mother were ‘necessarily harmful
in character, emotionally damaging, if not completely lethal’ (Mead, 1962, p. 55).
Mead claimed that studies of other cultures, including her own work in Samoa,
showed that children who were cared for by dierent people grew up to be
well-adjusted individuals. She concluded that by giving the mores of his own
culture the status of universal behavior and positing a biological underpinning,
Bowlby had committed the sin of ‘reication’ (Mead, 1962, p. 58).
Mead and other anthropologists in this period emphasized the need to con-
duct further cross-cultural and interdisciplinary studies before making any asser-
tions about the universality of behavior and emotions. Those studies showed
an enormous diversity of practices and beliefs about childrearing in diverse
societies (Mead, 1954, 1955; Whiting & Child, 1953; on culture and personality
studies, see Levine, 2001).
In contrast, Bowlby appealed to studies of animal behavior to support his
views. Building upon his 1958 paper, in his inuential 1969 book Attachment he
4 M. VICEDO
claimed that the attachment between mother and child is an adaptation forged
by natural selection in the ancestral environment of human evolution. Further,
deviations from the mother-child patterns already set in that environment
would lead to child pathologies and imperil human survival. Again, although
he postulated a theory of child development of universal character, Bowlby did
not review the existing ethnographic literature on children. He mentioned Mead
only once in a footnote, just to state briey she had misunderstood him (Bowlby,
1969, p. 303). However, he oered no explanation or rebuttal of her arguments.
Bowlby’s reliance on animal studies (the work of Konrad Lorenz, Niko
Tinbergen, and Harry Harlow) and his claim that attachment was an adaptation
became central to the success of his ethological theory of attachment. First, his
book came out during a period of unprecedented interest in animal behavior
during the late 1960s and 1970s; as evidence suce it to note that Lorenz and
Tinbergen won the Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicine in 1973. Second,
by appealing to the higher authority of biology Bowlby’s views on attachment
benetted from a halo eect. However, I have argued elsewhere that Bowlby
selectively referred to those studies supporting his positions while ignoring
other studies that did not support his views (Vicedo, 2013; for a critical rebuttal
of attachment theorists’ appeal to evolution and animal studies, see Vicedo,
2017a). Yet, as important as Bowlby’s appeal to biology was, it is doubtful that
his theory would have become widespread were it not for the development of a
laboratory tool that allowed attachment researchers to operationalize the study
of attachment relations. American/Canadian psychologist Mary Ainsworth car-
ried out the work that according to attachment scholars provided the empirical
support for Bowlby’s theory.
Ainsworth: Uniformity in the laboratory
During 1954–1955, Ainsworth studied infant care practices among the Ganda
people of central Africa. For a period of 7months, she conducted home visits
to about 20 Ugandan households to observe the interaction between mothers
and 28 infants. After moving to Johns Hopkins University in the United States
in 1955, Ainsworth conducted another observational study of 26 White, mid-
dle-class mother-infant pairs in Baltimore.
Neither of these studies included in-depth ethnographic information about
the socio-economic or cultural context of the families and about their goals and
beliefs about childrearing. Ainsworth’s aim was not to understand childrear-
ing practices in their socio-cultural context. Consonant with the psychological
tradition in which she was trained and the universal aspirations of attachment
theory, she aimed to discover universal patterns of behavior and their causes.
In 1967 Ainsworth published Infancy in Uganda. To assess attachment, she
focused on three aspects of an infant’s behavior: the use of mother as a secure
base for exploration; the infant’s distress during brief separations from mother;
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 5
and the infant’s fear when encountering strangers. She classied the infants she
had observed in three groups: secure-attached, insecure-attached, and non-at-
tached. Ainsworth identied several factors as signicant for the development of
infant-mother attachment, but she concluded that the determinant ones were
the conduct and the feelings of the mother, mainly her sensitivity in responding
to her baby’s signals (Ainsworth, 1967, p. 400).
In the Baltimore home study, Ainsworth could not observe the attachment
behaviors she had identied in her Ugandan study. But Ainsworth did not see
those negative results as evidence for the diversity of children’s behaviors in
dierent cultures. Rather than interpreting those observations as a challenge to
the universality of attachment responses, she devised a procedure to try to elicit
the behaviors she had observed in Uganda: the Strange Situation Procedure
(SSP). This 20-min laboratory experiment comprised 7 episodes of 3min each.
Twenty-three of the infants from the Baltimore study, ranging in age from 9 to
24months, were tested to see how they reacted when their mothers left them in
an unfamiliar environment alone, with a stranger, and also how they responded
upon being reunited with their mothers.
The alleged power of the SSP grew quickly. In a short period, researchers went
from presenting it as a tool used to elicit children’s behaviors to accepting it as
a reliable test used to categorize infants. They moved from justifying its use as
a hypothesis-generating procedure to claiming that it was a method capable of
establishing causal relations. In 1970 Ainsworth and Bell (1970, p. 52) put forward
the SSP as a tool ‘to illustrate’ children’s attachment and exploratory behav-
iors in a ‘laboratory microcosm. But in 1978 Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall
(1978) presented the SSP as a reliable test for categorizing not only behaviors,
but also children and their mothers. Depending mainly on the infant’s behav-
ior upon the mother’s return, the infants were classied as securely attached,
anxious-avoidant, and anxious-resistant (1978, p. xi). Since the authors claimed
that maternal sensitivity is the main factor shaping the attachment, the cate-
gorization of children automatically entailed a categorization of the mothers
as well. As they put it:
… the ndings here reported suggest that one can establish a fair basis for predict-
ing strange-situation behavior from home behavior and, perhaps more important,
that one can assess certain general aspects of the infant’s characteristic relation-
ship with his mother from his behavior in the strange situation. (Ainsworth et al.,
1978, p. 135)
Thus, according to these attachment theorists, a twenty-minute laboratory
procedure suced to establish causal relations between a mother’s care and
her child’s personality. Furthermore, Ainsworth et al. argued that their ndings
were normative and supported Bowlby’s theory of attachment (Ainsworth et
al., 1978, p. 95).
In my view, the SSP procedure played a crucial part in the advancement of
Bowlby’s theory among American researchers because it tted well within the
6 M. VICEDO
dominant vision of science that psychologists defended. Psychology is a large
eld that embraces many dierent approaches. But, as historians of science have
documented (Herman, 1995; Solovey, 2013), after WWII a large number of psy-
chologists and social scientists increasingly turned towards a model of science
that aimed to emulate the natural sciences in their reliance on experiments,
predictions, and universal generalizations. Developmental psychologists criti-
cal of triumphalist narratives of their eld have also noted this scientistic turn.
Moving away from its interdisciplinary beginnings, child development studies
focused increasingly on psychology and on one conception of that discipline
– as an experimental laboratory endeavor seeking universal generalizations
and principles along the lines of the physical and biological sciences, as Kessel
(2009, p. 830) noted.
In addition, attachment theory supported a particular vision of children that
gained currency after WWII. Mayes and Lassonde (2014, p. 6) have identied
several dening features in the American postwar developmental paradigm,
including ‘a declining faith in children’s innate resiliency and preprogrammed
constitutional strength in favor of a view of children as fragile and highly sen-
sitive to their environments; a scheme of children’s development as stage-like
with consequential outcomes, and the stress on ‘the inuence of parental care.
For Bowlby and Ainsworth, a secure attachment between mother and child has
to develop early on in order for emotional development to proceed in the right
way. They also saw children as possessing no resilience to overcome the lack of
sensitive parenting. Thus, their views about infants and about the signicance
or parental care tted well within the postwar developmental paradigm.
However, towards the end of the 1970s, several prominent child psycholo-
gists criticized the vision of science and of children supported by attachment
theory as atomistic and reductionistic. In direct reference to SSP studies, Urie
Bronfenbrenner (1977, p. 513) minced no words: ‘Much of contemporary devel-
opmental psychology is the science of the strange behavior of children in
strange situations with strange adults for the briefest possible periods of time.
In ‘The American child and other cultural inventions William Kessen criticized the
universalistic aspirations of contemporary developmental psychology by calling
attention to the dierent conceptions of childhood and of science existing in
dierent times and places. Kessen found especially problematic the belief in a
‘free standing’ and ‘self-contained’ child with its instincts, traits, and attachments
as essential components of its nature. Not only was it empirically incorrect, but
that vision also enabled psychologists to take the child as the unit of study rather
than considering the child as part of a more complex system of relations and
network of inuences. Thus, the conception of the ‘isolable’ child had important
and troubling methodological consequences: ‘Basically, we have observed those
parts of development that the child could readily transport to our laboratories
or to our testing sites’ (Kessen, 1979, p. 819). In sum, critical voices pointed out
that the reliance on laboratory experiments led developmental psychologists
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 7
to a distorted vision of children because it ignored the role of context in child
development.
The need to study children in context became a recurrent theme in discus-
sions about the state of developmental psychology. In the 1979 special issue
of American Psychologist that the APA dedicated to ‘Psychology and Children’
Brofenbrenner pointed to the ‘missing data’ on contexts of child reading in
studies of human development, and called for explorations of how ecologi-
cal contexts aect the course of psychological growth (Bronfenbrenner, 1979,
p. 844). Michael Cole and his collaborators at the Laboratory of Comparative
Human Cognition at UC-San Diego criticized developmental research that did
not acknowledge what anthropologist Franz Boas had stated as early as 1911:
‘The existence of a mind absolutely independent of conditions of life is unthink-
able.’ They argued that taking this insight seriously ‘would entail recognizing
that laboratory tasks cannot stand alone as a privileged means for discovering
psychological principles.’ And they concluded: ‘Cross-cultural psychology has
forced upon us the recognition that interplay between laboratory and obser-
vational research is a necessity if we are to understand how experience aects
our minds’ (LCHC, 1979, pp. 829, 832). Schwartz wrote that developmental psy-
chology had ‘largely missed the opportunity to consider the child in the cultural
milieu, which is the sine qua non of the developmental completion of a human
nature’ (1981, pp. 11, 12). Super and Harkness (1986) also criticized the focus
on the universal and decontextualized child and put forward the concept of
the ‘developmental niche to include an individual’s experience of culture and
environmental inuences.
Despite the fact that these and other researchers from anthropology and
cultural psychology continued to call for a better integration of ethnographic
insights obtained in the eld with the knowledge gained via experimental and
observational studies in the lab, some researchers remained pessimistic. Gustav
Jahoda lamented that, despite the rise of cross-cultural developmental studies,
developmental psychology remained ‘unduly parochial’ because major areas
were not taking cultural diversity seriously (Jahoda, 1986, p. 417). Still, he saw
the cross-cultural study of emotional development as a promising and exciting
eld. The cross-cultural study of the emotions would indeed blossom in the
late 1980s. But this rich work on diverse communities and time periods did not
become integrated with attachment research.
Most attachment researchers continued to focus on using the SSP proce-
dure to study children in the lab, despite the challenges mentioned above to
this approach as well as the mounting critiques to the procedure itself. In the
mid-eighties, several researchers disputed the SSP’s validity and reliability in
the Baltimore study (Kagan, 1984; Lamb et al., 1984). In addition, other scholars
have pointed out that the SSP embeds Western cultural assumptions and thus
cannot be used to study children in non-Western cultures (Levine & Miller, 1990;
Meehan & Hawks, 2013; Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyke, & Morelli, 2000; see also
8 M. VICEDO
Vicedo, 2013 for a critique of the view that the SSP results provide evidence for
Bowlby’s views).
Yet, the SSP’s ability to operationalize research on attachment facilitated an
explosion of work. To put it plainly, administering a 20-min test in the connes
of a laboratory is a much easier, faster, and cheaper method to obtain fast results
and publications than conducting a eld ethnographic study away from your
own home and oce. A comparison of citations of Ainsworth’s books, Patterns
of Attachment and Infancy in Uganda illustrates the prominence of the SSP, and
shows how the laboratory instrument achieved far greater prominence than her
observational study. Patterns of Attachment (Figure 1(a)) has been cited between
ten and fteen times more often than Infancy in Uganda (Figure 1(b)).
Figure 1.(a) Publications citing Patterns of Attachment (1978) (from Scopus), (b) Publications
citing Infancy in Uganda (1967) (from Scopus).
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 9
It is worth pointing out the irony in attachment researchers’ reliance on the
SSP procedure while ignoring observational studies of children in dierent con-
texts. Bowlby and Ainsworth emphasized their ethological approach in all their
writings. However, ethologists Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen had made
observation in natural environments the rallying call of their new approach
to study behavior in an objective manner. As a corollary, ethologists sharply
criticized the behaviorists’ analysis of animal behavior in the laboratory. For
them, the articial conditions of the laboratory distorted the natural behavior
of animals. Furthermore, for them, experiments were always secondary to eld
observation. One had to rst observe animals in their ecological context. Only
after having a thorough knowledge of an animal’s behavioral patterns in his
natural habitat should an ethologist turn to laboratory or experimental studies.
Indeed, Tinbergen privately expressed his shock upon discovering that Bowlby
had not carried out observational studies of children in order to develop his
views about child development (Vicedo, 2017b).
Contrary to the ethological approach in which context is integral to under-
standing behavior, the bulk of attachment research has relied on the SSP, a
cheap, quick, and simple laboratory procedure, has focused on testing a single
variable (security), and has appealed to a single explanatory factor (maternal
sensitivity). Mead’s fears that child development research often isolated one
single factor and that factor was usually the mother have remained pertinent
in the eld of attachment.
In recent decades, however, anthropologists have challenged the universal-
istic claims of attachment theory again on the basis of extensive research on
diverse cultural settings.
Anthropology re-dux: Infants and mothers in context
A full account of ethnographic studies of childhood is beyond the scope of this
paper. But a brief overview of some recent work about mothers and children
reveals how the diversity uncovered calls into question basic tenets of attach-
ment theory.
In an inuential study, Death Without Weeping, Nancy Scheper-Hughes
showed how the socio-economic context inuences maternal attitudes and
emotions. She followed three generations of mothers living in the slums of
Brazil where the average woman had 9.5 pregnancies, 8 births, and 3.5 infant
deaths. Aware that their babies may not survive, the mothers did not imme-
diately ‘personalize’ their infants, did not name them right after birth, and did
not mourn their deaths as is common in Western culture. Scheper-Hughes con-
cluded that mother love is not a uniform, universal, and naturally monolithic
aect. Challenging attachment theory, she called for recognizing that mother
love ‘represents a matrix of images, meanings, sentiments, and practices that
10 M. VICEDO
are everywhere socially and culturally produced’ (Scheper-Hughes, 1992, p. 341;
see also Scheper-Hughes, 2014).
Other anthropologists demonstrated that socio-cultural contexts inuence
parents’ goals in childrearing and, consequently, their practices, including their
emotional involvement with their infants. Levine et al. (1994) found that Kenyan
Gusii mothers, who bear an average of 10 children, focus rst on their survival
and then on teaching them compliance, contrary to mothers in the U.S. who do
not aim for compliance in childrearing. In her studies of Samoan family relations,
Mageo (2013) found that socialization is not oriented to develop feelings of secu-
rity, but to encourage separation. Since mothers have diverse goals depending
on their socio-economic conditions, cultural traditions, and social class, they also
assess children’s behavior dierently. For example, Harwood, Miller, and Irizarry
(1995) discovered that U.S. mothers in the Anglo and Puerto Rican communities
of Connecticut have dierent conceptions of what a ‘good child’ is, and these
aected their assessment of their children’s behavior in a simulated strange situ-
ation. Some mothers disapproved of ‘secure’ behavior and approved of behavior
that would be classied as ‘insecure.Weisner (2005) also reported that some U.S.
mothers were happy to see their child behave in an ‘independent’ way, though
that behavior corresponded to the ‘avoidant’ category in the attachment clas-
sication system. Thus, even in middle-class US groups, parents may not desire
the behavior that attachment scholars consider both natural and ideal.
Cross-cultural studies have also made clear that, contrary to attachment the-
ory’s focus on the mother-infant dyad, childrearing is often a cooperative enter-
prise. As Mead pointed out in her criticisms of Bowlby’s views in the mid-fties,
distributed care is quite common. In many societies, mothers are not the only
ones taking care of children: siblings, fathers, aunts, grandparents, and other
members of the community are involved in dierent ways. Developmental psy-
chology has been slow in acknowledging this fact and its signicance. Weisner
and Gallimore (1977) pointed out that the 1969 Handbook of Socialization Theory
and Research included practically no one but parents as caretakers in its 1182
pages. However, their review of anthropological studies showed the importance
of children as caretakers of their siblings. More recent ethnographic work has
conrmed that children and other family and community members are an intrin-
sic part of caretaking networks in many societies. Among the Beng people of
West Africa, for example, a mother returns to work in the elds when her child
is between 2 and 4months old. During most of the day, diverse sitters carry the
baby on their back from 5 to 20min at a time. The comings and goings of chil-
dren among caretakers are not perceived as a risk; on the contrary, they convey
a feeling of safety to children (Gottlieb, 2004). For other studies of societies with
distributive care (see Keller & Chaudhary, 2017; Meehan & Hawks, 2013; Morelli
et al., 2017; Rogo, 2003; Seymor, 2013; Weisner, 2014).
Finally, contrary to what attachment theory considers normative, parents
in many communities try to avoid attaching to their infants right away. For
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 11
example, in The afterlife is where we come from: The culture of infancy in West
Africa, anthropologist Alma Gottlieb reports that some Beng mothers intention-
ally break the gaze of their infants to their own eyes in order to decrease their
children’s emotional attachment to them (Gottlieb, 2004, p. 161). Anthropologist
David Lancy reviewed over 200 ethnographic and archaeological records from
a variety of geographical areas and historical periods and found that in many
societies infants are not given status as persons at birth and parents resist attach-
ing to them. He concluded that these ndings contradict the universal claims
of attachment theorists (Lancy, 2015). In his view:
‘Attachment theory’ and its descendants have created a narrative of infants ‘at risk’
of emotional maladjustment. In this survey of sources from cultural anthropology,
history, and archeology this perceived risk is absent … . The survey also reveals
that an alternate narrative identies attachment rather than attachment failure as
the risk. A strong, emotional bond is seen as impeding a process whereby infants
are pragmatically sorted into categories of wanted versus unwanted, timely ver-
sus untimely, legitimate versus illegitimate, strong ghters versus sickly ghosts,
and innocent versus demonized, leading to sustained nurturance or extinction.
(Lancy, 2014, p. 89)
Clearly, if mothers have a biological instinct for attachment, as Bowlby claimed,
this innate impulse has been overridden by cultural beliefs and practices in
these communities that delay forming emotional bonds with infants until their
status is evaluated.
Furthermore, all these studies show that infants can thrive in a number of
dierent environments and within diverse systems of child-care. As two more
recent books also make clear (Gottlieb & DeLoache, 2016; Levine & Levine, 2016),
children across the world are raised in many dierent ways. In many societies,
multiple care is a widespread and normal practice.
In sum, ethnographic studies of child-rearing beliefs, goals, and practices
in dierent societies have challenged the universalistic claims of attachment
theory and its focus on the mother-infant dyad. In addition, regardless of the
accuracy of any particular study or the validity of any specic insight, the anthro-
pological work has made clear that context matters when studying child-rear-
ing practices, child development, and emotions in human societies. To explain
behavior and aects, researchers must understand the meanings they acquire
within a particular culture. As both anthropological and historical works have
shown, child rearing practices depend on conceptions of the self, personhood,
and the social good that vary among dierent societies and that have changed
over time (Koops, 2003; Morelli et al., 2017).
Conclusion
In this paper I have examined some powerful criticisms that anthropologists
and cultural psychologists leveled against attachment theory from its inception
to the present. From Mead to current researchers cross-cultural investigations
12 M. VICEDO
have demonstrated the existence of various visions of children and develop-
ment. As Levine and Norman have put it: ‘there is a wider range of pathways to
normal emotional development than has been imagined in attachment theory’
(2008, p. 139). Anthropologists have revealed the variability and diversity in
conceptions of childhood and practices of childcare by studying children in
context. On the basis of that work, many researchers have criticized the failure
of attachment theorists to take culture seriously (Gottlieb, 2014; Keller & Bard,
2017; Lancy, 2014; Levine, 2014; Morelli et al., 2017; Otto & Keller, 2014; Quinn
& Mageo, 2013; Weisner, 2014).
By showing the need to study children in context, cross-cultural studies
have also challenged psychologists’ reliance on laboratory experiments as the
primary methodological tool for uncovering universal behaviors and emotions.
In the case of attachment theory, I have argued that the alleged uniformity of
attachment behaviors was not discovered in the eld but constructed in the
laboratory in problematic ways. Based on studies of isolated children, attach-
ment theorists postulated the existence of universal patterns of attachment.
But children do not grow up in isolation. Thus, the evidence collected in the
lab always needs to be assessed in relation to the observations collected in
eld studies.
If children’s development needs to be understood in context, so does the
development of a scientic theory. Here, I have argued that the rise of attach-
ment studies owes much to the disciplinary goals of psychological research.
Riding upon the coattails of the natural sciences, psychology after WWII aimed
to develop universal laws of human behavior that could be tested in the labo-
ratory. In that context, Ainsworth’s strange situation procedure became instru-
mental in the growth of attachment theory, despite the numerous criticisms of
its application and its interpretation. Anthropologists of childhood, in contrast,
emphasized the need for ethnographic observational studies of childrearing
practices in order to understand the diversity of human experiences.
For several decades scholars have noted that dierent visions of science
created a rift between psychology and anthropology (Edgerton, 1974; Price-
Williams, 1974; Super & Harkness, 1986). Tensions between the two elds con-
tinue to exist in the area of child development as a consequence of their dierent
epistemological and methodological commitments. As long as mainstream psy-
chology remains committed to a model focused on ‘revealing universal laws
about human experience, personality, social life, and subjectivity’ (Herman,
1995, p. 12), it is hard to see how it can accept the claim that ‘there is no one
human psychology and no one pathway of development’ (Keller, 2007, p. 270).
In the recent past, however, many psychologists have also recognized the need
to study children in their socio-cultural milieu, which requires careful analy-
sis of social networks (Lewis, 2005) and cultural contexts (Cole, 1996). Others
have emphasized the need for a new framework of ‘developmental contextu-
alism’ to understand the complex and changing relations aecting children’s
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 13
development across the lifespan (Lamb, 2005, p. 110). More and more psycholo-
gists now believe that human development is a cultural process’ (Rogo, 2003,
p. 3).
Yet, neither the extensive anthropological literature nor the critical psycho-
logical literature have been integrated within attachment research. Thus, the
challenges reviewed here have not been answered.
But we must end on an optimistic note. Recently, there has been a growing
concern that many claims about behavior have been based mostly on data
obtained about people from WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich,
and Democratic) societies (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Historians have
also been accumulating evidence that cultural practices of childrearing and
the meaning of emotions have changed through time (Mintz, 2009; Stearns,
2003). If cultural psychologists and anthropologists are right that ‘culture and
psyche make each other up’ (Shweder & Sullivan, 1993, p. 498), the joint eorts
of psychologists, anthropologists, and historians will be necessary to illumi-
nate the co-evolution of cultural contexts and children’s development. As Mead
stressed half a century ago, understanding childhood requires interdisciplinary,
cross-cultural studies of children in context.
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the author.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore,
MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated
by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49–67.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bateson, G., & Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character: A photographic analysis. New York:
New York Academy of Sciences.
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. International Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 39, 350–373.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development.
American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). Contexts of child rearing. Problems and prospects. American
Psychologist, 34, 844–850.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Edgerton, R. B. (1974). Cross-cultural psychology and psychological anthropology: One
paradigm or two? Reviews in Anthropology, 1, 52–65.
14 M. VICEDO
Gottlieb, A. (2004). The afterlife is where we come from: The culture of infancy in West Africa.
Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Gottlieb, A. (2014). Is it time to detach from attachment theory? Perspectives from the
West African rain forest. In H. Otto & H. Keller (Eds.), Dierent faces of attachment:
Cultural variations on a universal human need (pp. 187–214). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Gottlieb, A., & DeLoache, J. (2016). A world of babies: Imagined childcare guides for eight
societies (Updated ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). Culture and attachment: Perceptions of
the child in context. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Henrich, J., Heine, H. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. The
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.
Herman, E. (1995). The romance of American psychology: Political culture in the age of
experts. Berkeley: The University of California Press.
Jahoda, G. (1986). A cross-cultural perspective on developmental psychology.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 417–437.
Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Keller, H., & Chaudhary, N. (2017). Is the mother essential for attachment? Models of care
in dierent cultures. In H. Keller & K. A. Bard (Eds.), Contextualizing attachment: The
cultural nature of attachment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Keller, H. & Bard, K. A. (Eds.). (2017). Contextualizing attachment: The cultural nature of
attachment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kessel, F. S. (2009). Research on child development: Historical perspectives. In R. A.
Shweder, et al. (Eds.), The child: An encyclopedic companion (pp. 828–833). Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago Press.
Kessen, W. (1979). The American child and other cultural inventions. American Psychologist,
34, 815–820.
Koops, W. (2003). Imaging childhood. In W. Koops & M. Zuckerman (Eds.), Beyond
the century of the child. Cultural history and developmental psychology (pp. 1–18).
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. (1979). Cross-cultural psychology’s
challenges to our ideas of children and development. American Psychologist, 34,
827–833.
Lamb, M. E. (2005). Attachments, social networks, and developmental contexts. Human
Development, 48, 108–112.
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., Charnov, E. L., & Estes, D. (1984). Security of
infantile attachment as assessed in the “strange situation”: Its study and biological
interpretation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 127–147.
Lancy, D. F. (2014). “Babies aren’t persons”: A survey of delayed personhood. In H. Otto &
H. Keller (Eds.), Dierent faces of attachment: Cultural variations on a universal human
need (pp. 66–109). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lancy, D. F. (2015). The Anthropology of childhood. Cherubs, chattel, changelings (2nd ed.).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Levine, R. A. (2001). Culture and personality studies, 1918–1960: Myth and history. Journal
of Personality, 69, 803–818.
Levine, R. A. (2014). Attachment theory as cultural ideology. In H. Otto & H. Keller (Eds.),
Dierent faces of attachment: Cultural variations on a universal human need (pp. 50–65).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 15
Levine, R. A., Dixon, S., Levine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., & Brazelton, T.
B. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press.
Levine, R. A., & Levine, S. (2016). Do parents matter? Why Japanese babies sleep soundly,
Mexican siblings don’t ght, and American families should just relax. New York, NY:
PublicAairs.
Levine, R. A., & Miller, P. M. (1990). Commentary. Human Development, 33, 73–80.
Levine, R. A., & Norman, K. (2008). Attachment in anthropological perspective. In R. A.
Levine & R. S. New (Eds.), Anthropology and child development: A cross-cultural reader
(pp. 127–142). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell.
Lewis, M. (2005). The child and its family: The social network model. Human Development,
48, 8–27.
Mageo, J. M. (2013). Toward a cultural psychodynamics of attachment: Samoa and
US comparison. In N. Quinn & J. Mageo (Eds.), Attachment reconsidered: Cultural
perspectives on a Western theory (pp. 191–214). New York, NY: Palgrave.
Mayes, L. C., & Lassonde, S. (2014). Introduction. In L. C. Mayes & S. Lassonde (Eds.), A girl’s
childhood: Psychological development, social change, and the Yale child study center (pp.
1–11). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Mead, M. (1954). Some theoretical considerations on the problem of mother-child
separation. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 24, 471–483.
Mead, M. (1955). Theoretical settings-1954. In M. Mead & M. Wolfenstein (Eds.), Childhood
in contemporary cultures (pp. 3–20). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Mead, M. (1962). A cultural anthropologist’s approach to maternal deprivation.
Deprivation of maternal care: A reassessment of its eects (pp. 45–62). Geneva: World
Health Organization.
Meehan, C. L., & Hawks, S. (2013). Cooperative breeding and attachment among the Aka
foragers. In N. Quinn & J. Mageo (Eds.), Attachment reconsidered: Cultural perspectives
on a Western theory (pp. 85–113). New York, NY: Palgrave.
Mintz, S. (2009). Huck’s raft: A history of American childhood. Ann Arbor: University of
Michigan.
Morelli, G. A., Chaudhary, N., Gottlieb, A., Keller, H., Murray, M., Quinn, N., Rosabal-Coto,
M., Scheidecker, G., Takada, A., & Vicedo, M. (2017). Taking culture seriously: Developing
a pluralistic theory of attachments. In H. Keller & K. A. Bard (Eds.), Contextualizing
attachment: The cultural nature of attachment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Otto, H., & Keller, H. (Eds.). (2014). Dierent faces of attachment: Cultural variations on a
universal human need. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Price-Williams, D. (1974). Psychological experiment and anthropology: The problem of
categories. Ethos, 2, 95–114.
Quinn, N., & Mageo, J. M. (Eds.). (2013). Attachment reconsidered: Cultural perspectives on
a western theory. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Rogo, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Miyke, K., & Morelli, G. (2000). Attachment and culture:
Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, 55, 1093–1104.
Scheper-Hughes, N. (1992). Death without weeping: The violence of everyday life in Brazil.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Scheper-Hughes, N. (2014). Family life as bricolage – reections on intimacy and
attachments in Death without weeping. In H. Otto & H. Keller (Eds.), Dierent faces of
attachment: Cultural variations on a universal human need (pp. 230–260). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Schwartz, T. (1981). The acquisition of culture. Ethos, 9, 4–17.
16 M. VICEDO
Seymor, S. C. (2013). “It takes a village to raise a child”: Attachment theory and multiple
child care in Alor, Indonesia, and in North India. In N. Quinn & J. M. Mageo (Eds.),
Attachment reconsidered: Cultural perspectives on a Western theory (pp. 115–139). New
York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. A. (1993). Cultural psychology: Who needs it? Annual Review
of Psychology, 44, 497–523.
Solovey, M. (2013). Shaky foundations: The politics-patronage-social science nexus in Cold
War America. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Stearns, P. N. (2003). Anxious parents: A history of modern childrearing in America. New
York: New York University Press.
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at
the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
9, 545–569.
Vicedo, M. (2013). The nature and nurture of love. From imprinting to attachment in Cold
War America. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Vicedo, M. (2017a). The strange situation of the ethological theory of attachment in
historical perspective. In H. Keller & K. A. Bard (Eds.), Contextualizing attachment: The
cultural nature of attachment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vicedo, M. (2017b). Ethopathology and civilization diseases: Niko and Elisabeth Tinbergen
on autism. Canadian Bulletin of Medical History.
Weisner, T. S. (2005). Attachment as a cultural and ecological problem with pluralistic
solutions. Human Development, 48, 89–94.
Weisner, T. S. (2014). The socialization of trust: Plural caregiving and diverse pathways
in human development across cultures. In H. Otto & H. Keller (Eds.), Dierent faces of
attachment: Cultural variations on a universal human need (pp. 263–277). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Weisner, T. S., & Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child and sibling caretaking.
Current Anthropology, 18, 169–190.
Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality: A cross-cultural study.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
World Health Organization. (1962). Deprivation of maternal care: A reassessment of its
eects. Geneva: Author.
... In addition, marriage, couple, and family therapy research on perinatal mental health has historically sampled White middle class couples and excluded the experiences of marginalized couples (Goyal et al., 2023;Tseng et al., 2021;Tseng et al., 2023). The benefits of this research include: (1) initiating new discourse on how ecotherapy can be incorporated in the field of marriage, couple, and family therapy, especially when working with marginalized expecting couples; (2) offering an expanded understanding and application of attachment theory, which has been criticized for perpetuating Eurocentric, neoliberal and patriarchal notions of family and community (Carr & Batlle, 2015;Choate & Tortorelli, 2022;Cleary, 1999;Keller, 2018;Keller et al., 2018;Neckoway et al., 2007;Rothbaum et al., 2000;Sear, 2016;Sosteric & Ratkovic, 2022;Vicedo, 2017aVicedo, , 2017b, by interweaving a psychotherapeutic exploration of intergenerational, couple, parent-fetal, and ecological connection narratives; and (3) understanding how the field of marriage, couple, and family therapy can better serve marginalized expecting couples by utilizing ecotherapy as they navigate bias and discrimination within the U.S. maternal healthcare system. participants. ...
... Its focus on early attachment makes it a well-suited lens for interventions supporting the life transition of pregnancy and becoming a parent. Attachment theory has been criticized for focusing too narrowly on the infant-caregiver bond, which can give rise to motherblame narratives; and for imposing Western, neoliberal values on human development based in individuality and nuclear, patriarchal family structures (Carr & Batlle, 2015;Choate & Tortorelli, 2022;Cleary, 1999;Keller, 2018;Keller et al., 2018;Neckoway et al., 2007;Rothbaum et al., 2000;Sear, 2016;Sosteric & Ratkovic, 2022;Vicedo, 2017aVicedo, , 2017b. For this reason, a systemic, whole family and community approach that broadens attachment-based intervention for pregnancy to encompass polyadic, intergenerational, and all parental bonds richens the potential for support; enhancing cultural sensitivity; and deconstructing the dominant culture norms and values within attachment theory that oppress marginalized pregnant people, their partners, and their families (Neckoway et al., 2007;Paley & Hajal, 2021Pérez C. et al., 2017;Sear, 2016). ...
... The theory's assertion of universality; and the cross-cultural appropriateness of parental sensitivity and responsiveness constructs (Ainsworth et al., 1974) have been long debated (Keller, 2018;Keller et al., 2018;Mesman et al., 2016;Mesman et al., 2018;G. Morelli, 2015; G. A. Morelli et al., 2017;Vicedo, 2017aVicedo, , 2017b. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The experience of pregnancy within the U.S. healthcare system is life threatening for pregnant people in the margins. Minority populations, including Black and Native American people, people of color, cultural and ethnic minorities, LGBTQ+ people, migrants and refugees, and the socioeconomically marginalized all face serious disparities in pregnancy, childbirth and postnatal health care outcomes. This study sought to understand how ecotherapy for marginalized expecting couples can support attachment repair, foster family resilience to mitigate the stresses of marginalized pregnancy and create a protective benefit for navigating maternal health care. A literature review provides critical synthesis of three areas of discourse, perinatal mental health and maternal health care, attachment theory and attachment-based intervention, and ecopsychology and ecotherapy. An attachment-based ecotherapy intervention is introduced, and recommendations are made for future study. The study aims to stimulate new discourse on how ecotherapy can be incorporated in the field of marriage, couple, and family therapy to offer expanded clinical support for marginalized expecting couples.
... This is a major shortfall since nonmaternal multiple caregiving (allomothering) was normative for most of human evolutionary history, yet our understanding of its role in development remains limited (Dagan & Sagi-Schwartz, 2021;Kramer, 2010;Meehan & Hawks, 2013). Other critics have argued that this Western bias has overemphasized the importance of sensitive responsiveness, and generated a narrow conception of optimal caregiving which ignores the importance of cultural context (Keller et al., 2018;Vicedo, 2017). Therefore, understanding what particular form(s) of caregiving, if any, humans are psychologically adapted to requires research outside of WEIRD contexts. ...
... As described above, caregiving and development research has been criticized for its WEIRD bias. It is argued that this has led to the narrow view that sensitive responsiveness is always best, and also a poor understanding of multiple caregiving (Dagan & Sagi-Schwartz, 2021;Keller et al., 2018;Schmidt et al., 2021;Vicedo, 2017). The aim of this study is to further our understanding of the CHAUDHARY, SALALI, AND SWANEPOEL levels of sensitive responsiveness and the structure of multiple caregiving networks that children may be adapted to. ...
... Some cross-cultural psychologists argue that the Western bias in developmental research has produced a narrow conception of optimal caregiving as sensitive and responsive, which largely ignores the importance of cultural context and socialization goals (Keller et al., 2018;Vicedo, 2017). The distinction between proximal versus distal strategies has also been highlighted as relevant to cultural intentions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Attachment theory postulates that there is a particular style of caregiving that, because of its interaction with our evolved psychology, is most likely to result in healthy psychological development. Attachment research has been criticized because most studies have been conducted with Western populations. Critics argue this has (a) overemphasized the importance of sensitive responsive caregiving and (b) limited our understanding of multiple nonmaternal caregiving (allomothering). Here, we analyze the extent of sensitive responsiveness and structural properties of allomothering networks among Mbendjele hunter-gatherers who reside in the Republic of Congo. Humans lived as hunter-gatherers for the majority of our evolutionary history, thus studying contemporary hunter-gatherers can offer insight into the caregiving children may be psychologically adapted to. Based on 12-hr focal follows of 18 children (0–4 years old; 10 male), we constructed caregiving networks across the domains of responding to crying, physical contact, interactive care, and proximity. Crying was virtually always responded to rapidly via comforting and never via scolding. Children received physical contact and care for the majority of the day. Allomothering accounted for 40%–50% of caregiving in each domain. While allomaternal networks were large, they were highly concentrated—the majority of a child’s allocare was provided by just a few caregivers. Due to high caregiver:child ratios, “sharing” of caregivers was limited—a child typically had several allomothers who directed a majority of their allomaternal effort to him/her. These findings add to our understanding of the level and sources of sensitive responsive caregiving that children may be evolutionarily primed to expect.
... Although the role of culture and social ecology in infant attachment has been a topic of a lively debate for a while (Keller, 2016(Keller, , 2017Morelli, 2015;Rothbaum et al., 2000;van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999;Vicedo, 2017), socioecological psychology and adult attachment research have remained largely disconnected from one another. When adult attachment theory was formulated during the late 80s and early 90s, cultural critiques were yet to be integrated into traditional attachment theory (Vicedo, 2017). ...
... Although the role of culture and social ecology in infant attachment has been a topic of a lively debate for a while (Keller, 2016(Keller, , 2017Morelli, 2015;Rothbaum et al., 2000;van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999;Vicedo, 2017), socioecological psychology and adult attachment research have remained largely disconnected from one another. When adult attachment theory was formulated during the late 80s and early 90s, cultural critiques were yet to be integrated into traditional attachment theory (Vicedo, 2017). When cultural work on early life attachment reached a critical peak and provided alternative views to the classical formulation attachment theory during the second half of 2010s, the adult attachment literature had already progressed and matured in its own trajectory (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). ...
... Thinking about one's hometown cultivates a sense of identity with it. This connection is called hometown identity, in which we identify with the local culture and regional spiritual belongings and form a special emotional connection with a particular location (Vicedo, 2017;Greider and Garkovich, 1994). Our hometown holds our warmest and happiest childhood memories. ...
Article
This study examines the relationship between CEO locality and corporate pollution emissions for a sample of Chinese A-share listed firms from 2008 to 2020. When the CEO’s hometown is consistent with the province of firm registration, the local CEOs are more likely to take measures conducive to environmental protection, such as increasing environmental investment to reduce corporate pollution. The results are robust to alternative model specifications, endogeneity issues, sampling criteria, and variable definitions. We identify executive social relations and long-termism as two potential channels. Overall, our findings demonstrate the important role of CEO locality in reducing corporate pollution and fulfilling social responsibilities. JEL classification numbers: H23, M51, O17. Keywords: CEO locality, Environmental governance, Informal institution.
... Attachment theory focuses on difficult transitional periods across the lifespan (Howe, 2011) and identifies how security-seeking behaviours can be significant facilitators of thriving (Cameron, 2008;Cameron et al., 2013;Schore, 2017;Schore & Schore, 2008;Vicedo, 2017). Attachment theory highlights the importance of parental and psychosocial support; thus, it is apposite in examining this critical transitional period in late adolescence (Kochanska & Kim, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated, through an attachment theoretical lens, the relationship between first-year university students’ personal and academic adjustment and 3 psychosocial resources: parental attachment, student resources (parental support, social support, ways of reducing loneliness, emotion regulation, coping strategies, locus of control), and gender. Participants answered questionnaires relating to their psychosocial resources and post-secondary adjustment in first and second term. These data were analysed using a planned regression analysis. In Term 1, paternal attachment predicted students’ emotional adjustment, with social and personal resources accounting for this relationship, and was related to academic adjustment via locus of control. Maternal attachment predicted academic adjustment. Gender and locus of control predicted academic performance (as measured by grade point average [GPA]). In Term 2, parental attachment predicted emotional adjustment, with social support accounting for this relationship, but academic adjustment was no longer related to paternal attachment. Overall, gender and locus of control predicted academic success. Suggestions are made for developing transitional theoretical models that address psychosocial processes that will help shape responsive institutional programming and planning in support of incoming college students. These recommendations include designing more personalized programs to match students and their family systems where possible and keeping parents/guardians informed of helpful supports for students’ experiences when needed.
Article
Until quite recently, investigations of gender/sex development operated from a baseline assumption that gender/sex is dichotomous or binary. Most such studies constructed gender/sex outside of or adjacent to specific cultures, and for the most part studied children from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic (WEIRD) countries. In this paper I first advocate for four research guidelines—inclusivity, epistemic justice, materiality, and empiricism. I sketch out the historical background that still shapes contemporary studies into the psychology of gender/sex identity in infants and toddlers. Next, I point toward methods for the study of gender/sex in infants and toddlers that have the potential to make nonbinary development visible and to develop culturally diverse concepts of identity development. Finally, I challenge psychologists and others to view and operationalize the study of identity as a relational, phenomenological entity rather than a fixed feature of the individual psyche.
Article
There have been many calls for the use of attachment theory in children’s social work practice. Yet, some commentators claim that attachment theory has been uncritically adopted by social workers and has become an unhelpfully dominant perspective. Empirical evidence to support such claims is limited, however. We therefore sought to examine these claims in a qualitative vignette and interview study with twenty-three children’s safeguarding social workers from two English local authorities. We found that many social workers had an anti-theoretical orientation and that, for those who did draw on formal theories, attachment theory was less commonly identified as an influence than systems theory and was typically drawn on alongside other theories and perspectives. We also found examples of social workers taking a critical stance on the theory and its use in practice. The current study findings therefore challenge depictions of attachment theory as problematically dominant in children’s safeguarding social work practice. We argue that future debate on whether attachment theory has an appropriate level of practice influence would benefit from being grounded in more robust research on the current level of influence.
Book
Full-text available
Our book is free to download at the URL below or via the OUP website (search for title; tap on open padlock icon; tap ‘PDF’). It sets out from our research showing babies enjoy participating in groups and group dynamics well before they have formed their first infant-adult attachment. We show how this finding can enrich daycare experiences for both infants and educators, while supplying a new set of assumptions for psychotherapy and the foundations of human psychology. https://global.oup.com/academic/product/babies-in-groups-9780192859518?q=babies%20in%20groups&lang=en&cc=au
Article
Full-text available
The idea that some diseases result from a poor fit between modern life and our biological make-up is part of the long history of what historian of medicine Charles Rosenberg has called the "progress-and-pathology narrative." This article examines a key episode in that history: 1973 Nobel laureate Niko Tinbergen's use of an evolutionary framework to identify autism as a pathogenic effect of progress. Influenced by British psychiatrist John Bowlby's work, Tinbergen and his wife Elisabeth saw autistic children as victims of environmental stress caused mainly by mothers' failure to bond with their children and to protect them from conflicting situations. However, the author argues that their position was not "environmental." For them, autism was due to a failure of socialization but the mechanisms that explain that failure were established by biological evolution. Situating their views within the context of Niko's concern about the derailment of biological evolution by cultural evolution, this article shows that their ideas are of special significance for understanding the persistence of the view that civilization poses a risk to human health.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter examines the history of some challenges to John Bowlby’s and Mary Ainsworth’s ethological attachment theory (EAT). Bowlby and Ainsworth argued that the mother-infant relationship is a natural dyad designed by evolution in which the instinctual responses of one party activate instinctual responses in the other, and that secure attachment is an adaptation. This chapter focuses on EAT’s two fundamental tenets: the universality of attachment patterns and the biological foundations of the attachment system. It shows that several scholars have challenged those tenets over the years and argues that attachment researchers have not addressed those challenges successfully.
Book
Should babies sleep alone in cribs, or in bed with parents? Is talking to babies useful, or a waste of time? A World of Babies provides different answers to these and countless other childrearing questions, precisely because diverse communities around the world hold drastically different beliefs about parenting. While celebrating that diversity, the book also explores the challenges that poverty, globalization and violence pose for parents. Fully updated for the twenty-first century, this edition features a new introduction and eight new or revised case studies that directly address contemporary parenting challenges, from China and Peru to Israel and the West Bank. Written as imagined advice manuals to parents, the creative format of this book brings alive a rich body of knowledge that highlights many models of baby-rearing - each shaped by deeply held values and widely varying cultural contexts. Parenthood may never again seem a matter of 'common sense'.
Chapter
Attachment theory is predicated on the assumption of dyadic relationships between a child and one or a few significant others. Despite its recognition of alloparenting in some cultural environments, current attachment research is heavily biased toward the mother as the major attachment figure in the life of the developing child. This chapter presents evidence that diverse childcare arrangements exist in cultures that differ from Western norms and shows how these are equally normative in their respective cultural contexts. In these settings, alloparenting is neither chaotic nor unstable; it is the norm, not the exception. In all environments, infant care is far more than just an isolated, biopsychological phenomenon: it is an activity deeply imbued with cultural meanings, values, and practices. To account for these multiple levels, the construct of attachment must shift its emphasis away from an individual child toward the network of relationships surrounding a child. Overwhelming evidence on diverse childcare arrangements in non-Western cultures calls the putatively universal model of attachment (derived from the Bowlby-Ainsworth paradigm and still widely applied today) into question. In support of future research, this chapter proposes an inclusive reconceptualization of attachment, informed by research from non-Western cultural settings.
Chapter
Attachment theorists see a capacity for close one-to-one bonding and autonomy as ideal developmental outcomes and tend to see early distrust as the inevitable consequence of largely unavoidable separation anxieties that are part of physical and emotional weaning—a consequence mitigated by secure attachment. For Freud ([1930] 1964), in contrast, socialization creates individual anxieties to syphon energy away from personal fulfillment and redirect it toward socially valued behaviors. A psychodynamic account of development, then, would question if one-to-one bonding and autonomy are really ideal or only normative and would ask if separation anxieties are somehow intrinsic to norms and to their reproduction.
Chapter
The African proverb, “It takes a village to raise a child,”1 expresses an underlying truth. Most societies around the world do not expect mothers, or parents, to rear children alone. Mothers and their young children are usually enmeshed in larger kinship groups and communities that help with child care and other tasks. Dating at least back to Margaret Mead’s (1974) 1928 groundbreaking study of adolescence in Samoa, sociocultural anthropologists have been documenting multiple child care and discussing some of its probable effects upon children’s emotional bonds with their caretakers and other people. The bias toward exclusive mothering that has dominated much of Western psychology—including John Bowlby’s (1969) theory of attachment—has been evident to anthropologists for a long time.