ArticlePDF Available

Nostalgia: past, present, and future

Authors:
  • Archbridge Institute

Abstract

Traditionally, nostalgia has been conceptualized as a medical disease and a psychiatric disorder. Instead, we argue that nostalgia is a predominantly positive, self-relevant, and social emotion serving key psychological functions. Nostalgic narratives reflect more positive than negative affect, feature the self as the protagonist, and are embedded in a social context. Nostalgia is triggered by dysphoric states such as negative mood and loneliness. Finally, nostalgia generates positive affect, increases self-esteem, fosters social connectedness, and alleviates existential threat.
Nostalgia
Past, Present, and Future
Constantine Sedikides,
1
Tim Wildschut,
1
Jamie Arndt,
2
and Clay Routledge
3
1
University of Southampton,
2
University of Missouri, and
3
North Dakota State University
ABSTRACT—Traditionally, nostalgia has been conceptual-
ized as a medical disease and a psychiatric disorder. In-
stead, we argue that nostalgia is a predominantly positive,
self-relevant, and social emotion serving key psychological
functions. Nostalgic narratives reflect more positive than
negative affect, feature the self as the protagonist, and are
embedded in a social context. Nostalgia is triggered by
dysphoric states such as negative mood and loneliness.
Finally, nostalgia generates positive affect, increases self-
esteem, fosters social connectedness, and alleviates exis-
tential threat.
KEYWORDS—nostalgia; positive affect; self-esteem; social
connectedness; existential meaning
The term nostalgia was inadvertedly inspired by history’s most
famous itinerant. Emerging victoriously from the Trojan War,
Odysseus set sail for his native island of Ithaca to reunite with his
faithful wife, Penelope. For 3 years, our wandering hero fought
monsters, assorted evildoers, and mischievous gods. For another
7 years, he took respite in the arms of the beautiful sea nymph
Calypso. Possessively, she offered to make him immortal if he
stayed with her on the island of Ogygia. ‘‘Full well I acknowl-
edge,’’ Odysseus replied to his mistress, ‘‘prudent Penelope
cannot compare with your stature or beauty, for she is only a
mortal, and you are immortal and ageless. Nevertheless, it is she
whom I daily desire and pine for. Therefore I long for my home and
to see the day of returning’’ (Homer, 1921, Book V, pp. 78–79).
This romantic declaration, along with other expressions of
Odyssean longing in the eponymous Homeric epic, gave rise to
the term nostalgia. It is a compound word, consisting of nostos
(return) and algos (pain). Nostalgia, then, is literally the
suffering due to relentless yearning for the homeland. The term
nostalgia was coined in the 17th century by the Swiss physician
Johaness Hofer (1688/1934), but references to the emotion it
denotes can be found in Hippocrates, Caesar, and the Bible.
HISTORICAL AND MODERN CONCEPTIONS OF
NOSTALGIA
From the outset, nostalgia was equated with homesickness. It
was also considered a bad omen. In the 17th and 18th centuries,
speculation about nostalgia was based on observations of Swiss
mercenaries in the service of European monarchs. Nostalgia
was regarded as a medical disease confined to the Swiss, a view
that persisted through most of the 19th century. Symptoms—
including bouts of weeping, irregular heartbeat, and anorexia—
were attributed variously to demons inhabiting the middle brain,
sharp differentiation in atmospheric pressure wreaking havoc in
the brain, or the unremitting clanging of cowbells in the Swiss
Alps, which damaged the eardrum and brain cells.
By the beginning of the 20th century, nostalgia was regarded
as a psychiatric disorder. Symptoms included anxiety, sadness,
and insomnia. By the mid-20th century, psychodynamic ap-
proaches considered nostalgia a subconscious desire to return
to an earlier life stage, and it was labeled as a repressive com-
pulsive disorder. Soon thereafter, nostalgia was downgraded to a
variant of depression, marked by loss and grief, though still
equated with homesickness (for a historical review of nostalgia,
see Sedikides, Wildschut, & Baden, 2004).
By the late 20th century, there were compelling reasons for
nostalgia and homesickness to finally part ways. Adult partici-
pants regard nostalgia as different from homesickness. For ex-
ample, they associate the words warm,old times,childhood, and
yearning more frequently with nostalgia than with homesickness
(Davis, 1979). Furthermore, whereas homesickness research
focused on the psychological problems (e.g., separation anxiety)
that can arise when young people transition beyond the home
environment, nostalgia transcends social groups and age. For
example, nostalgia is found cross-culturally and among well-
functioning adults, children, and dementia patients (Sedikides
et al., 2004; Sedikides, Wildschut, Routledge, & Arndt, 2008;
Zhou, Sedikides, Wildschut, & Gao, in press). Finally, although
homesickness refers to one’s place of origin, nostalgia can refer
Address correspondence to Constantine Sedikides, Center for Re-
search on Self and Identity, School of Psychology, University of
Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, England, U.K.; e-mail:
cs2@soton.ac.uk.
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
304 Volume 17—Number 5Copyright r2008 Association for Psychological Science
to a variety of objects (e.g., persons, events, places; Wildschut,
Sedikides, Arndt, & Routledge, 2006).
It is in this light that we note the contemporary definition of
nostalgia as a sentimental longing for one’s past. It is, moreover, a
sentimentality that is pervasively experienced. Over 80% of
British undergraduates reported experiencing nostalgia at least
once a week (Wildschut et al., 2006). Given this apparent
ubiquity, the time has come for an empirical foray into the
content, causes, and functions of this emotion.
THE EMPIRICAL BASIS FOR UNDERSTANDING
NOSTALGIA
The Canvas of Nostalgia
What is the content of the nostalgic experience? Wildschut et al.
(2006) analyzed the content of narratives submitted voluntarily
by (American and Canadian) readers to the periodical Nostalgia.
Also, Wildschut et al. asked British undergraduates to write a
narrative account of a nostalgic experience. These narratives
were also analyzed for content. Across both studies, the most
frequently listed objects of nostalgic reverie were close others
(family members, friends, partners), momentous events (birth-
days, vacations), and settings (sunsets, lakes).
Nostalgia has been conceptualized variously as a negative,
ambivalent, or positive emotion (Sedikides et al., 2004). These
conceptualizations were put to test. In a study by Wildschut,
Stephan, Sedikides, Routledge, and Arndt (2008), British and
American undergraduates wrote narratives about a ‘‘nostalgic
event’’ (vs. an ‘‘ordinary event’’) in their lives and reflected
briefly upon the event and how it made them feel. Content
analysis revealed that the simultaneous expression of happiness
and sadness was more common in narratives of nostalgic events
than in narratives of ordinary events. Also in Wildschut et al.,
British undergraduates wrote about a nostalgic (vs. ordinary vs.
simply positive) event in their lives and then rated their happi-
ness and sadness. Although the recollection of ordinary and
positive events rarely gave rise to both happiness and sadness,
such coactivation occurred much more frequently following the
recollection of a nostalgic event. Yet, nostalgic events featured
more frequent expressions of happiness than of sadness and
induced higher levels of happiness than of sadness.
Wildschut et al. (2006) obtained additional evidence that
nostalgia is mostly a positively toned emotion: The narratives
included far more expressions of positive than negative affect. At
the same time, though, there was evidence of bittersweetness.
Many narratives contained descriptions of disappointments and
losses, and some touched on such issues as separation and even
the death of loved ones. Nevertheless, positive and negative
elements were often juxtaposed to create redemption, a narrative
pattern that progresses from a negative or undesirable state (e.g.,
suffering, pain, exclusion) to a positive or desirable state (e.g.,
acceptance, euphoria, triumph; McAdams, 2001). For example,
although a family reunion started badly (e.g., an uncle insulting
the protagonist), it nevertheless ended well (e.g., the family
singing together after dinner).
The strength of the redemption theme may explain why, de-
spite the descriptions of sorrow, the overall affective signature of
the nostalgic narratives was positive.
Moreover, Wildschut et al. (2006) showed that nostalgia is a
self-relevant and social emotion: The self almost invariably
figured as the protagonist in the narratives and was almost always
surrounded by close others. In all, the canvas of nostalgia is rich,
reflecting themes of selfhood, sociality, loss, redemption, and
ambivalent, yet mostly positive, affectivity.
The Triggers of Nostalgia
Wildschut et al. (2006) asked participants to describe when they
become nostalgic. The most frequently reported trigger was
negative affect (‘‘I think of nostalgic experiences when I am sad
as they often make me feel better’’), and, within this category,
loneliness was the most frequently reported discrete affective
state (‘‘If I ever feel lonely or sad I tend to think of my friends or
family who I haven’t seen in a long time’’). Given these initial
reports, Wildschut et al. proceeded to test whether indeed
negative mood and loneliness qualify as nostalgia triggers.
British undergraduates read one of three news stories, each
based on actual events, that were intendedto influence their mood.
In the negative-mood condition, they read about the Tsunami that
struck coastal regions in Asia and Africa in December 2004. In the
neutral-mood condition, they read about the January 2005 landing
of the Huygens probe on Titan. In the positive-mood condition,
they read about the November 2004 birth of a polar bear, osten-
sibly in the London Zoo (actually in the Detroit Zoo). Then they
completed a measure of nostalgia, rating the extent to which they
missed 18 aspects of their past (e.g., ‘‘holidays I went on,’’ ‘‘past TV
shows, movies,’’ ‘‘someone I loved’’). Participants in the negative-
mood condition were more nostalgic (i.e., missed more aspects of
their past) than were participants in the other two conditions.
In another study, loneliness was successfully induced by
giving participants false (high vs. low) feedback on a ‘‘loneli-
ness’’ test (i.e., they were led to believe they were either lonely or
not lonely based on the feedback). Subsequently, participants
rated how much they missed 18 aspects of their past. Partici-
pants in the high-loneliness condition were more nostalgic than
those in the low-loneliness condition. These findings were rep-
licated among 9- to 15-year-old Chinese children, Chinese un-
dergraduates, and Chinese factory workers (Zhou et al., in press).
Why might negative mood and loneliness trigger nostalgia?
The psychological significance of nostalgia may reside in its
capacity to counteract distress and restore psychological equa-
nimity. But what are the pathways through which nostalgia exerts
such palliative benefits?
The Psychological Significance of Nostalgia
Wildschut et al. (2006) randomly assigned British undergradu-
ates to a nostalgia or ordinary-event condition. They induced
Volume 17—Number 5 305
C. Sedikides et al.
nostalgia in one of two ways. First, they instructed participants to
think of a nostalgic (vs. ordinary) event from their lives, list four
relevant keywords, and reflect briefly upon the event and how it
made them feel. Second, they provided participants with the
definition of nostalgia and instructed them to bring to mind a
nostalgic autobiographical event, immerse themselves in the
experience, and write about it for 6 minutes. Here, in the control
condition, participants thought about the ordinary event as if
they were observers, imagined the event as though they were
historians recording factual details, and produced a factual ac-
count. (Notably, further studies have additionally contrasted
nostalgia with reflections on positive events, positive future
events, and autobiographical memories.)
Followingsuccessful nostalgia manipulation checks, Wildschut
et al. (2006) assessed affect, self-regard, and social connect-
edness. They assessed affect either with the items ‘‘happy,’’
‘‘content,’’ ‘‘sad,’’ and ‘‘blue’’ or with the Positive and Negative
Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988).
They assessed self-regard either with the items ‘‘significant’’
and ‘‘self-esteem’’ or with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
(Rosenberg, 1965). Finally, they assessed social connectedness
using (a) the items ‘‘loved’’ and ‘‘protected’’ (b) the Revised
Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (Fraley, Waller, &
Brennan, 2000), which indexes attachment anxiety and avoid-
ance; and (c) the Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire
(Buhrmeister, Furman, Wittenberg, & Reis, 1998); the assess-
ment focused on perceived competence in initiating social in-
teractions, self-disclosing, and providing emotional support.
Wildschut et al. (2006) proposed that nostalgia serves as a
repository of positive affect. Nostalgia has been characterized as
a ‘‘joyous’’ experience that gives rise to ‘‘a feeling of elation’’
(Kaplan, 1987, p. 465). Indeed, nostalgic participants reported
more positive (but not negative) affect than did control partici-
pants. Nostalgia generates positive affectivity.
Wildschut et al. (2006) also proposed that nostalgia enhances
positive self-regard. Nostalgia has been theorized to bestow
‘‘an endearing luster’’ on the self and to cast ‘‘marginal, fugitive,
and eccentric facets of earlier selves in a positive light’’
(Davis, 1979, pp. 41–46). Indeed, nostalgic participants
reported higher self-regard than did control participants. Nos-
talgia not only elevates positive self-regard, it also increases the
implicit accessibility of positive self-attributes and attenuates
self-esteem defence (Vess, Arndt, Routledge, Sedikides, &
Wildschut, 2008).
Moreover, Wildschut et al. (2006) proposed that nostalgia
strengthens social bonds. Nostalgia is a social emotion; it has
been said that, during nostalgic reverie, ‘‘the mind is ‘peopled’’’
(Hertz, 1990, p. 195). Symbolic ties with close others are af-
firmed, and close others come to be momentarily part of one’s
present. Indeed, nostalgic participants manifested stronger so-
cial connectedness than did control participants: They felt more
loved and protected, had reduced attachment anxiety and
avoidance, and reported greater interpersonal competence.
Would nostalgia be capable of counteracting the negative ef-
fects of loneliness? Can nostalgia serve a coping function? Zhou
et al. (in press) addressed this question. They found that lone-
liness is associated with, or triggers, perceived lack of social
support. At the same time, loneliness is associated with, or
triggers, nostalgia. Interestingly, nostalgia is associated with, or
triggers, perceptions of social support. Loneliness directly re-
duces perceptions of social support, but indirectly increases
such perceptions via nostalgia: Nostalgia magnifies perceptions
of social support, thus counteracting the effect of loneliness.
Finally, Routledge, Arndt, Sedikides, and Wildschut (2008)
proposed that nostalgia imbues life with meaning, which facil-
itates coping with existential threat. One of the primary human
challenges is carving out a meaningful existence. Yet, awareness
of inevitable mortality presents a major obstacle on the path to
psychological equanimity. According to terror management
theory, one can mitigate existential anxiety through shared be-
liefs about the nature of reality that imbue life with meaning.
Nostalgia can contribute an overall sense of enduring meaning to
one’s life. In several studies testing American undergraduates,
Routledge et al. supported this existential function of nostalgia.
After being reminded of their mortality (relative to an aversive
dental procedure), the more nostalgic participants felt, the more
meaningful they perceived their life to be. Also, after reminders
of mortality (relative to a dental procedure or failing an important
exam), participants who were more prone to nostalgia (e.g., had
reported frequent engagement in nostalgia), or who had received
a nostalgia induction, actually had fewer death-related thoughts.
Nostalgia boosted perceptions of life as meaningful and
assuaged existential threat.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Another key function of nostalgia is that it may facilitate con-
tinuity between past and present selves. Nostalgia may facilitate
use of positive perceptions about the past to bolster a sense of
continuity and meaning in one’s life (Sedikides, Wildschut,
Gaertner, Routledge, & Arndt, 2008). An additional function of
nostalgia may be its motivating potential. Nostalgia may boost
optimism, spark inspiration, and foster creativity (Stephan,
Wildschut, Sedikides, Routledge, & Arndt, 2008). Recent re-
search provides initial evidence for both of these possibilities. Of
course, there may also be complex nuances that merit attention.
Nostalgia may erode a sense of meaning in the present and may
forestall motivation, if the individual is fixated on better days
gone by.
Socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, &
Charles, 1999) proposes that, with advancing age, people come
to view their life span as limited. They shift attention away from
future-oriented, knowledge-related goals and emphasize the
importance of purpose and meaning in life and of being em-
bedded in a social network. This raises two issues about nos-
306 Volume 17—Number 5
Nostalgia
talgia. First, are age-related changes in motivation reflected in
the frequency and content of nostalgia? Older (compared to
younger) adults may experience nostalgia more frequently and
assign a more prominent role to close others in their nostalgic
reverie. Second, does nostalgia acquire greater significance in
older age? Bereavement and declines in physical health may
render older adults especially vulnerable to social isolation,
which, in turn, would impair the formation and maintenance of
intimate friendships and social networks. Nostalgia, then, would
have an important role to play in reestablishing a symbolic
connection with significant others.
CODA
Regarded throughout centuries as a psychological ailment,
nostalgia is now emerging as a fundamental human strength. It is
part of the fabric of everyday life and serves at least four key
psychological functions: It generates positive affect, elevates
self-esteem, fosters social connectedness, and alleviates exis-
tential threat. By so doing, nostalgia can help one navigate
successfully the vicissitudes of daily life. More generally, nos-
talgia is uniquely positioned to offer integrative insights across
such areas of psychology as memory, emotion, the self, and re-
lationships. Nostalgia has a long past and an exciting future.
Recommended Reading
Batcho, K.I. (1995). Nostalgia: A psychological perspective. Perceptual
and Motor Skills,80, 131–143. Reports a measure of nostalgia in
which participants rate the extent to which they missed aspects of
their past.
Davis, F. (1979). (See References). A scholarly introduction of the
construct of nostalgia, and a review of available evidence.
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2008). (See
References). An empirical demonstration of the existential func-
tion of nostalgia.
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., & Baden, D. (2004). (See References). A
review of historical conceptions of nostalgia coupled with specu-
lations about nostalgia’s functions.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C.D. (2006). (See
References). An exploration of the content and triggers of nostalgic
accounts, as well as an empirical foray into the affective, self-re-
gard, and social-connectedness functions of nostalgia.
REFERENCES
Buhrmeister, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M.T., & Reis, H.T. (1998).
Five domains of interpersonal competence in peer relation-
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,55,
991–1008.
Carstensen, L.L., Issacowitz, D.M., & Charles, S.T. (1999). Taking time
seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psy-
chologist,54, 165–181.
Davis, F. (1979). Yearning for yesterday: A sociology of nostalgia. New
York: Free Press.
Fraley, R.C., Waller, N.G., & Brennan, K.A. (2000). An item response
theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,78, 350–365.
Hertz, D.G. (1990). Trauma and nostalgia: New aspects of the coping of
aging holocaust survivors. Israeli Journal of Psychiatry and Re-
lated Sciences,27, 189–198.
Hofer, J. (1934). Medical dissertation on nostalgia. (C.K. Anspach,
Trans.). Bulletin of the History of Medicine,2, 376–391. (Original
work published 1688)
Homer. (1921). The Odyssey (F. Caulfied, Trans.). London: G. Bell and
Sons.
Kaplan, H.A. (1987). The psychopathology of nostalgia. Psychoanalytic
Review,74, 465–486.
McAdams, D.P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General
Psychology,5, 100–122.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2008). A blast
from the past: The terror management function of nostalgia.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,44, 132–140.
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., & Baden, D. (2004). Nostalgia: Conceptual
issues and existential functions. In J. Greenberg, S. Koole, & T.
Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psy-
chology (pp. 200–214). New York: Guilford Press.
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Gaertner, L., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J.
(2008). Nostalgia as enabler of self-continuity. In F. Sani (Ed.),
Self-continuity: Individual and collective perspectives (pp. 227–
239). New York: Psychology Press.
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J. (2008). Nos-
talgia across the life-span. Unpublished data, University of
Southampton.
Stephan, E., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Routledge, C.R., & Arndt, J.
(2008). The magic of the past: Nostalgia inspires. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Southampton.
Vess, M., Arndt, J., Routledge, C., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T.
(2008). Nostalgia as self-esteem resource. Unpublished data, Uni-
versity of Missouri.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and val-
idation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,55,
1063–1070.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C.D. (2006).
Nostalgia: Content, triggers, functions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,91, 975–993.
Wildschut, T., Stephan, E., Sedikides, C., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J.
(2008, February). Feeling happy and sad at the same time: Nos-
talgia informs models of affect. Paper presented at the 9th Annual
Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Al-
buquerque, New Mexico.
Zhou, X, Sedikides, C., Wildschut, C., & Gao, D.-G. (in press). Coun-
teracting loneliness: On the restorative function of nostalgia.
Psychological Science.
Volume 17—Number 5 307
C. Sedikides et al.
... Described as a medical condition-a form of melancholy-in the Early Modern period, it has become an important trope in Romanticism (Boym, 2002). Nostalgia is associated with a longing for the past, its personalities, possibilities, and events, especially the "good old days" or a "warm childhood" (Sedikides, et al, 2008). There is a predisposition, caused by cognitive biases such as rosy retrospection, for people to view the past more favourably and future more negatively (Etchells, January 16, 2015). ...
... [W]e are laid asleep In body, and become a living soul: While with an eye made quiet by the power Of harmony, and the deep power of joy, We see into the life of things. (Wordsworth,(46)(47)(48)(49)(50) Wordsworth is absent from the scene for five years, but his absence does not make him forget the glory of his past memories. Rather his re-visit gives him new pleasure and thought that makes his mind noble. ...
Article
Full-text available
The comparative study between William Wordsworth’s “Tintern Abbey” and William Butler Yeats’ “The Lake Isle of Innisfree” has been made in the paper based on the theoretical points of view of psychoanalysis and ecocriticism. The poets were born and brought up in two different periods. Wordsworth (1770-1850) belongs to the Romantic Age and Yeats (1865-1939) to the Modern Age—the first one wants to live on his past memories while the second one longs for his roots of Irish indigene. Every memory, either sad or happy, grows on an event of life that feeds the troubled mind later years with some retrospective feelings and emotions. Both of the poets have chosen two ingredient elements of romanticism—escapism and nostalgia—to make a sojourn of utopia through romantic recollections staying into two different worlds of romance and reality. Writers of the Romanticist or of the Modernist ideologies, like Wordsworth and Yeats, try to escape to an ideal world either in childhood and adolescent past or in the landscape of serene beauty. The poems we have chosen deal with the romantic worlds of recollections where the writers wish to escape from the troubled materialistic worlds of din and bustle. Being repeatedly disappointed with the mundane life of change and progress, cruelty and corruption, love, distrust and hatred, and above all, continuous fight for livelihood both Yeats and Wordsworth want to flee to their bygone days of sweet memories; Yeats longs for fleeing to the countryside of Ireland, his place of birth, and Wordsworth to his child memories. We have tried a little bit to analyze their nostalgic feelings and emotions through the thematic discussions of the selected poems from psychoanalytical and ecocritical points of view.
... It is also important to note that emotional resonance is not limited to positive emotions. Negative emotions, such as longing or melancholy tied to nostalgia, can also enhance brand attachment when balanced appropriately (Sedikides et al., 2008). Thus, marketers must carefully craft retrothemed content to evoke the right emotional responses to maximize customer engagement without alienating or confusing consumers. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the effects of retro-themed content, emotional resonance, and brand consistency on customer re-engagement. Using a structured questionnaire adapted from established scales, data were collected from 300 respondents and analyzed through descriptive statistics, reliability testing, confirmatory factor analysis, and multiple regression. The findings reveal that retro-themed content positively influences customer re-engagement both directly and indirectly through emotional resonance, which serves as a significant mediator. Brand consistency also contributes positively to customer re-engagement, though to a lesser extent. These results highlight the importance of leveraging nostalgia in marketing strategies while ensuring consistent brand messaging to strengthen emotional connections and encourage repeated interactions. The study provides theoretical and practical insights for marketers seeking to enhance customer loyalty through retro-themed campaigns.
... Norwood et al. Journal of Environmental Psychology 105 (2025) 102634 past, may be triggered by negative affect or states such as loneliness, and although described and experienced as bittersweet, often results in overall positive moods including feelings of social support (Sedikides et al., 2008). Nostalgia can provide a sense of continuity with the self (in this case the pre-injury self), reaffirm identity, and therefore reduce existential anxiety (Juhl et al., 2010;Sedikides et al., 2016). ...
... (2012) conducted a cross-sectional study with 5022 adolescents (ages [11][12][13][14][15][16]. They discovered that although SC had a negative relationship with depression, this relationship diminished when peer attachment and SE were taken into account. ...
Article
The current study investigates the connection between adult attachment styles and how they affect social connectedness and loneliness. Drawing on attachment theory, it examines how secure, anxious, and avoidant attachment patterns affect individuals' social experiences and emotional isolation. The Adult Attachment Scale (AAS), UCLA Loneliness Scale, and Social Connectedness Scale were used to evaluate a sample of 124 adult participants. According to the findings, people with secure attachment styles felt less alone and more connected to others, whereas people with insecure attachment styles—especially those who are anxious and avoidant—felt more alone and less connected. Results showed that individuals with secure attachments reported stronger social connections and lower loneliness, while those with insecure styles, particularly anxious and avoidant, experienced heightened loneliness and reduced social connectedness. These findings underscore the lasting impact of early relational patterns on emotional well-being and highlight the importance of developing secure attachment styles to buffer against loneliness.
... 19). Nostalgia, a compound word consisting of nostos (return) and algos (pain), literally means the suffering caused by an unrelenting yearning for the homeland (Sedikides et al, 2008). It is a complex, multilayered, volatile, emotionally charged story that glorifies romanticized past times, people, objects, sentiments, and events using binary oppositions to juxtapose them against their inferior counterparts in the present (Velikonja, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper critically examines the socio-political landscape of Yugoslav society through the lyrical narratives of two influential musical bands: Azra from Zagreb and Zabranjeno Pušenje from Sarajevo. Amidst rising Yugo-nostalgia and the idealization of the former Yugoslav state, the works of these rock bands provide an alternative perspective on the complexities and challenges of Yugoslav society. By conducting a thematic analysis of their lyrics, this research elucidates the cultural and political sentiments of the era, demonstrating how popular art functioned as a medium for incisive social commentary. The study underscores the importance of Azra and Zabranjeno Pušenje in articulating dissent and shaping public discourse, thereby contributing to a deeper understanding of Yugoslav identity. This examination reveals the enduring impact of their artistic expressions on contemporary interpretations of the Yugoslav socio-political context.
... Nostalgia-themed programs captivate audiences by fostering emotional connections through curated music, storytelling, and relatable themes. Sedikides et al. (2008) describe nostalgia as a psychological tool that enhances mood, reinforces social bonds, and strengthens individual identity. Yasika FM's use of memorable songs from past decades and engaging narratives in the "Greatest Memory" program encourages listeners to relive significant moments, creating a deep sense of attachment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Listener perception is crucial for a program's sustainability. This study aims to understand listener perceptions of the Greatest Memory program on Radio Yasika FM Jogja. Using a descriptive qualitative method and perception analysis, the study involved followers of @greatestmemories_id on Instagram as subjects. Findings reveal that listener perceptions are generally consistent and fall within the latitude of acceptance, indicating positive evaluations. However, statements from informants Ana Musiam and Bagas Fernando fall in the latitude of non-commitment, reflecting varied perceptions based on their individual thoughts, experiences, and views. Overall, most informants express acceptance and favorable perceptions of the program, demonstrating its effectiveness in meeting audience expectations.
Article
Full-text available
This article examines how Dergâh Journal articulated a vision of spiritual nationalism, framing national identity as a continuity from the Ottoman Empire to the Turkish Republic, grounded in cultural inheritance, artistic expression shaped by Ottoman-Sufi traditions, and a deep historical consciousness. Published during the Allied occupation of Istanbul between 1921 and 1923, Dergâh positioned itself outside the dominant nationalist paradigms of its time: on one side, militant resistance; on the other, rigid positivist modernization. Instead, it sought an alternative path to reimagine Turkish nationalism. The journal’s contributors contended that the true foundation of a nation lies not only in political structures but in the ability to preserve and reinterpret its spiritual, cultural, and artistic heritage. Rejecting both passive Western imitation and reactionary nostalgia, Dergâh pursued a dynamic synthesis of past and present, asserting that literature, art, music, and language must evolve organically while remaining anchored in their cultural and spiritual roots. Drawing inspiration from Henri Bergson’s (d. 1941) concepts of intuition (intuition) and duration (durée), the journal envisioned national identity as a living, ever-evolving force. Through its engagement with themes such as linguistic fragmentation, artistic mimicry, and the erosion of historical temporality, Dergâh sought to reclaim national consciousness by reviving a cultural and spiritual sensibility deeply rooted in Ottoman-Sufi traditions, envisioning art and thought as living continuities rather than borrowed imitations. This study situates Dergâh within the broader debates on nationalism and modernization, highlighting its distinctive attempt to reconcile spiritual depth and artistic vitality with the challenges of shaping modern Turkish identity.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Nostalgia, as both a social phenomenon and subjective experience, plays a significant role in product experience, consumer preference and human-product/brand relationships. Despite frequent mention and use, relatively little explicit knowledge of this topic has been generated in relation to design. This paper therefore investigates nostalgic experience from a design perspective. Firstly, it proposes a heuristic model for explaining the underlying process of design-evoked nostalgic experience. Secondly, in order to provide a comprehensive understanding of the experience, it analyses nostalgia from three distinct product experience levels (experience of meaning, aesthetic experience and emotional experience) and highlights the relationship between nostalgic experience and memory retrieval (explicit memory and implicit memory). It then examines the influential factors of nostalgic experience from two perspectives: (1) the user and context; (2) the product and interaction. Finally, nostalgia is placed in a wider context in relation to familiarity and novelty within the overarching research field of "Design and Memory". The ethical dimension of nostalgia is also addressed.
Conference Paper
In this paper I argue that there is a need for game studies to look beyond nostalgia as a period style or form of remediation, and to more carefully consider the role of nostalgia as an affective state experienced by players. Specifically, I argue that nostalgia is a positive emotional response that can be elicited in players without the need to embed period or historical referents in games. Extending this, I argue that the experience of nostalgia might enhance player motivation to explore game spaces, which has repercussions for game design. This paper makes use of existing literature on the psychology and aesthetic qualities of nostalgia to develop an initial theoretical basis for the study. To explore the implications of affective nostalgia, a case study analysis of two recent games is presented. Both of these games are dependent upon player motivation to explore their game worlds.
Article
This study investigated the psychological impact of nostalgia trigged by confectionery consumption. Participants (N = 149, ages 22-79) were instructed to eat caramels and thereafter respond to a scale on moods, the timing of nostalgic evocation, autobiographical-cultural memory, the nostalgic function, questions about remembrance, and free-answer questions. The results showed that eating the test food evoked both autobiographical and cultural memory, suggesting the possibility of using the food as a new trigger to evoke nostalgia. Furthermore, it was shown that nostalgic evocation enhances social ties, temporal continuity of the self, meaning of life, and other psychological functions, as well as facilitates seeing value in traditional products, clarifying that these functions contribute to higher enjoyment and can have various positive effects. It was suggested that evoking nostalgia with the eating of test food can have psychological effects that lead to well-being.
Chapter
Full-text available
In 1969, Crosby, Stills, Nash, and Young released their self-titled album containing the classic song "Suite: Judy Blue Eyes." A particularly striking lyric from this song recommended: "Don't let the past remind us of what we are not now." This evocative line suggests a question with far-reaching social psychological implications. How does a person's sense of the past contribute to (or detract from) the perceived continuity of his or her identity? This chapter entertains that question. We are concerned with the continuity within or between two fundamental sources of identity: the individual and collective selves. In particular, we focus on the temporal continuity between individual selves, between individual and collective selves, and between collective selves. We begin by defining the two types of self, specifying their possible relations, and asking how the seeming continuity within or between them is maintained. We proceed to argue that nostalgia is an important mechanism that enables this continuity, and we support our argument with a review of the empirical literature.
Article
Full-text available
In recent studies of the structure of affect, positive and negative affect have consistently emerged as two dominant and relatively independent dimensions. A number of mood scales have been created to measure these factors; however, many existing measures are inadequate, showing low reliability or poor convergent or discriminant validity. To fill the need for reliable and valid Positive Affect and Negative Affect scales that are also brief and easy to administer, we developed two 10-item mood scales that comprise the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). The scales are shown to be highly internally consistent, largely uncorrelated, and stable at appropriate levels over a 2-month time period. Normative data and factorial and external evidence of convergent and discriminant validity for the scales are also presented. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
According to terror management theory, people turn to meaning-providing structures to cope with the knowledge of inevitable mortality. Recent theory and research suggest that nostalgia is a meaning-providing resource and thus may serve such an existential function. The current research tests and supports this idea. In Experiments 1 and 2, nostalgia proneness was measured and mortality salience manipulated. In Experiment 1, when mortality was salient, the more prone to nostalgia participants were, the more they perceived life to be meaningful. In Experiment 2, when mortality was salient, the more prone to nostalgia participants were, the less death thoughts were accessible. In Experiment 3, nostalgia and mortality salience were manipulated. It was found that nostalgia buffered the effects of mortality salience on death-thought accessibility.
Article
Full-text available
Four studies tested whether nostalgia can counteract reductions in perceived social support caused by loneliness. Loneliness reduced perceptions of social support but increased nostalgia. Nostalgia, in turn, increased perceptions of social support. Thus, loneliness affected perceived social support in two distinct ways. Whereas the direct effect of loneliness was to reduce perceived social support, the indirect effect of loneliness was to increase perceived social support via nostalgia. This restorative function of nostalgia was particularly apparent among resilient persons. Nostalgia is a psychological resource that protects and fosters mental health.
Article
Recent years have witnessed an upsurge of interest among theorists and researchers in autobiographical recollections, life stories, and narrative approaches to understanding human behavior and experience. An important development in this context is D. P. McAdams's life story model of identity (1985; see also records 1993-97296-000 and 1996-06098-001), which asserts that people living in modern societies provide their lives with unity and purpose by constructing internalized and evolving narratives of the self. The idea that identity is a life story resonates with a number of important themes in developmental, cognitive, personality, and cultural psychology. This article reviews and integrates recent theory and research on life stories as manifested in investigations of self-understanding, autobiographical memory, personality structure and change, and the complex relations between individual lives and cultural modernity. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This paper examines the coping mechanism of aging survivors of the Holocaust with the extreme traumatic stressors they experienced in the past. Differences in the role of memories, reminiscences and nostalgic longings are presented. The potential role of sensory stimuli in the development of reminiscences and nostalgic feelings is emphasized. The comparison of the genesis of reminiscences and nostalgic phenomena may offer additional parameters for the understanding of the symptom formation in the elderly.