ChapterPDF Available

Sex Differences in Attachment

Authors:
S
Sex Differences in Attachment
Elaine Scharfe
Trent University, Peterborough, ON, Canada
Definition
There is some evidence that men and women have
moderately different distributions of insecure
adult attachment styles. Generally, men may tend
to report higher avoidance and women may tend
to report higher ambivalence, but these ndings
are somewhat inconsistent and controversial.
Introduction
Although controversial, there is some empirical
evidence that there may be small to moderate sex
differences in adult attachment and that these dif-
ferences may be rst evident during middle child-
hood. Despite the controversy, I would propose
that the suggestion that there may be sex differ-
ences in attachment should not be surprising. It is
well accepted that women and men tend to be
socialized differently from birth (Bem 1993)
and, as a result, men are typically less emotional
and less nurturing than women. Furthermore,
there is support that women and men may per-
ceive social interactions differently and conse-
quently behave differently in their relationships.
Despite the proposal that attachment
representations and gender roles have a joint inu-
ence on behavior of adults, sex differences in
attachment do not seem to be evident during
infancy and preschool years, and there are incon-
sistent reports of sex differences during the school
age years (see Del Giudice 2008 for summary of
this research). Furthermore, sex differences in
adulthood are not consistently reported but tend
to be evident with self-report questionnaire mea-
sures, in particular measures of Bartholomews
four-category model. It does seem clear that the
ability to detect (or not) sex differences in attach-
ment may be associated with the type of assess-
ment, the power to test for differences, and
characteristics of the sample.
Sex Differences in Attachment
Over the past 40 years, attachment researchers
have consistently reported that most individuals
in low risk or nonclinical samples, regardless of
the age of the participants or method of assess-
ment, are secure. Although there are no sex dif-
ferences in the proportion of secure and insecure
individuals, some researchers have reported sex
differences in proportions of the type of insecure
attachment. Furthermore, some researchers have
proposed that these differences may be adaptive
and predictable. For example, Del Giudice (2009)
proposed an evolutionary model that, among
other things, proposed that women develop
insecure-ambivalent attachment in conditions of
#Springer International Publishing AG 2016
T.K. Shackelford, V.A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3592-1
moderate stress in childhood and avoidant attach-
ment under conditions of high stress whereas men
develop insecure-avoidant attachment when they
experience stress in childhood. Interestingly, very
little attention has been paid to the effects of the
gender of the caregiver on the developing attach-
ment relationship. Mothers typically invest more
in the parent-child relationship and are usually a
childs primary caregiver. However, it is unknown
if this primary attachment is more inuential, in
particular, in situations where, for example, the
attachment relationship with the mother is differ-
ent than the attachment relationship with the
father.
Del Giudice and colleagues have proposed that
sex differences in attachment may develop in
middle childhood (see Del Giudice and Belsky
2010) although the proposal is somewhat contro-
versial (see Thompson 2010; van IJzendoorn and
BakermansKranenburg 2010). Early work mea-
suring infant attachment using the Strange Situa-
tion tends not to mention sex differences in
attachment; however, early behavioral work con-
sistently reports that, in preschool aged children,
insecure males tend to be more aggressive
whereas insecure females tend to be more depen-
dent and compliant. These ndings are consistent
with the observed sex difference that when
stressed males tend to engage in ght or ight
behaviors, females tend to engage in tend and
befriendbehaviors.
There are a few studies reporting sex differ-
ences in middle and late childhood and nearly all
report a sex difference in the categorization of the
insecure children: more ambivalence in females
and more avoidance in males. These gender dif-
ferences have been reported in studies using sev-
eral different methods of assessment including
versions of the Doll Story Completion Task, The
Manchester Child Attachment Story Task, and the
Coping Strategies Questionnaire. Although some
researchers continue to insist that these sex differ-
ences are not meaningful, Del Giudice and Belsky
(2010) proposed that the life history perspective
and neurobiological developmental changes may
help to understand why middle childhood may be
an important transition with respect to the devel-
opment of sex differences in attachment.
Del Giudice (2009) proposed an integrated
evolutionary model of the development of attach-
ment, which among other things, may explain
how and why middle childhood acts as a transi-
tional stage in both the development of attachment
and reproductive strategies. He proposed that this
phase of reorganization of attachment may be due
to intrasexual competition in the peer groupand
physiological and hormonal changes associated
with adrenarche both contributing to the devel-
opment of sex differences to improve the adapta-
tion of the insecure child. Although further work
is needed to test his model, it builds upon
Bowlbys original theory, which was rmly
rooted in evolutionary thinking at the time, and
provides insights from modern evolutionary the-
ory to explain why attachment researchers may
observe sex differences in adult attachment.
Sex Difference in Adult Attachment
Early work using the AAI does not suggest sex
differences in attachment, however, as with many
studies using interview assessments; the samples
were typically small and may not have had suf-
cient statistical power to test for differences.
Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn
(2009) summarized the data from over 10,000
AAI interviews addressing issues of base-line pro-
portions in clinical and nonclinical samples, gen-
der distributions, as well as differences in
distributions for adolescents and individuals
from low SES samples, ethnic minorities, and
non-Western countries. Using the AAI four-
category classication of a sample of nonclinical
mothers (n=700) as their baseline, their ndings
indicated that nonclinical fathers were more likely
to be classied as dismissing. Although there
were no signicant differences with the preoccu-
pied category, despite the large number of inter-
views in both groups, the authors only had
sufcient power to test for large differences
between the proportions of individuals catego-
rized as preoccupied. Using the AAI dimensions,
which increases the power to test for differences,
Simpson et al. (2002) reported that men scored
signicantly closer to the dismissing end of the
2 Sex Differences in Attachment
activation continuum whereas women scored sig-
nicantly closer to the preoccupied end.
Although Hazan and Shaver (1987) did not
report sex differences in their original article
using the three-category measure, researchers
consistently report sex differences with
Bartholomews four-category model (see
Bartholomew and Horowitz 1991). Bartholomew
and colleagues have reported that females and
males do not tend to differ on their secure or
fearful ratings; however, females tend to score
higher than males on the preoccupied scale and
lower than males on the dismissing scale. These
sex differences are evident when using both
Bartholomews family and peer interviews as
well as both the categorical and continuous self-
report surveys. Partner- and friend-reports reveal
similar sex differences (see also Scharfe and
Bartholomew 1994). Interestingly, Schmitt
et al. (2003) analyzed the RQ data across 62 coun-
tries and reported small to moderate sex differ-
ences in dismissing attachment across most
cultures in particular, cultures characterized by
low stress and low fertility environments and
concluded that greater levels of dismissingness in
males is near universal.
The introduction of the ECR and the ECR-R
did not include a discussion of sex differences so it
is not known whether or not sex differences were
tested in the original data (see Brennan et al. 1998;
Fraley et al. 2000). Furthermore, there are few
researchers who test (or report) sex differences
in published work using the ECR or the ECR-R,
although it is not clear whether or not there are no
sex differences on the two dimensions or that the
researchers simply did not test for sex differences;
however, there are a few exceptions. For example,
in a recent meta-analysis, Del Giudice (2011)
reported a signicant but small sex difference in
that men scored higher on the avoidance dimen-
sion and lower on the anxiety dimension. Interest-
ingly, he found that these sex differences were
stronger in community samples. Del Giudice
suggested that there are consistent sex differences
in attachment; however, there are variables such
as the distribution and type of the sample, geo-
graphic region, the non-normal distribution of the
data, unreliability of the measures, and effect of
age that may confound the results.
As pointed out by some scholars (see Del
Giudice and Belsky 2010), the fact that
researchers nd sex differences with some mea-
sures, and in some samples, and not with other
measures or samples is likely meaningful and
should not be ignored. As suggested above, it is
well accepted that women and men are socialized
differently and perceive and behave differently in
their relationships and perhaps it would be pro-
ductive to examine the joint inuence of attach-
ment representations and sex role on behavior in
relationships. For example, it is well accepted that
attachment avoidance is related to a greater num-
ber of less-committed relationships for both men
and women whereas attachment anxiety is associ-
ated with dependency and possessiveness for both
men and women. Perhaps a fruitful direction for
research to help to understand the importance of
sex differences would be the examination of
those ndings in which gender boundaries are
crossed. In particular, it may be helpful to examine
the research describing relationship characteris-
tics of anxious-ambivalent men or avoidant
women. The few studies that have highlighted
these unexpected sex differences highlight the
importance of these differences in understanding
the effects of attachment representations on adult
relationships.
There is some evidence that preoccupied or
ambivalent attachment for men and dismissing
or avoidant women may have a more negative
effect on their relationships and well-being. For
example, Simpson et al. (1999) reported that
highly ambivalent mens relationships
(as compared to relationships of other men and
all women) were more likely to have ended over a
4-month period suggesting that highly ambivalent
men may have less successful relationships. Fur-
thermore, Kanninen et al. (2003) reported that
preoccupied men had a heightened affected
response to traumatic events and were proposed
to be vulnerable to the emotions and distress asso-
ciated with past traumatic events suggesting that
highly ambivalent men may have more difculty
coping with difcult life events. Similarly, Bodner
et al. (2014) reported some evidence that, in early
Sex Differences in Attachment 3
adulthood, preoccupied men may experience a
signicant decline in their reports of the meaning
of life. Consistent with the suggestion that women
may develop avoidant attachment under extreme
stress, there is also some support that avoidant
women may have more difculty in their interper-
sonal relationships. For example, Mikulincer and
Florian (1999) reported some evidence that
avoidant women showed weaker attachment to
their fetus and more negative prenatal mental
health. Rholes et al. (1999) reported that more
avoidant women displayed more anger while
waiting for an anxiety provokingsituation
with their partners, and Simpson et al. (2002)
reported that more avoidant women provided
less support to their partners regardless of his
attachment. Although these studies provide some
evidence that anxious-ambivalent men and
avoidant women have more difculty in relation-
ships and coping with life events, very few studies
systematically examine the differential effects of
attachment across gender, and a more comprehen-
sive examination of this issue is needed.
Conclusions
In summary, although the ndings are inconsis-
tent and controversial, there is some support that,
at least in adulthood, men may tend to report
higher avoidance and women may tend to report
higher ambivalence. There are, however, inci-
dences where women report higher avoidance
and men report higher ambivalence and these
ndings might be more interesting and productive
to understand the importance of sex differences in
attachment. As researchers continue to explore
this controversial issue, it will be interesting to
pay particular attention to the effect when individ-
uals go against the norm”–what is the effect
when the insecure female or male is different
than what would be expected. I would suggest that
both the typical and the atypical sex differences
will be informative.
Cross-References
Attachment in Adulthood
Attachment Theory
Individual Variations in Attachment
John Bowlby
Measurement: Categorical Vs. Continuous
References
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H.
(2009). The rst 10,000 adult attachment interviews:
Distributions of adult attachment representations in
clinical and non-clinical groups. Attachment &
Human Development, 11, 223263.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. (1991). Attachment
styles among young adults: A test of a four-category
model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
61, 226244.
Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the
debate on sexual inequality. New Haven: Yale Univer-
sity Press.
Bodner, E., Bergman, Y. S., & Cohen-Fridel, S. (2014). Do
attachment styles affect the presence and search for
meaning in life? Journal of Happiness Studies, 15,
10411059.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-
report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative
overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Attachment theory and close relationships
(pp. 4676). New York: The Guilford Press.
Del Giudice, M. (2008). Sex-biased ratio of avoidant/
ambivalent attachment in middle childhood. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 369379.
Del Giudice, M. (2009). Sex, attachment, and the develop-
ment of reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 32,121.
Del Giudice, M. (2011). Sex differences in romantic attach-
ment: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 37, 193214.
Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Sex differences in
attachment emerge in middle childhood: An evolution-
ary hypothesis. Child Development Perspectives, 4,
97105.
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An
item response theory analysis of self-report measures of
adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 350365.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love concep-
tualized as an attachment process. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 52,511524.
Kanninen, K., Punamäki, R., & Qouta, S. (2003). Adult
attachment and emotional responses to traumatic mem-
ories among Palestinian former political prisoners.
Traumatology, 9, 127154.
4 Sex Differences in Attachment
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1999). Maternalfetal bond-
ing, coping strategies, and mental health during preg-
nancy: The contribution of attachment style. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 255276.
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Oriña, M. M. (1999).
Attachment and anger in an anxiety-provoking situa-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
940957.
Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Reliability and
stability of adult attachment patterns. Personal Rela-
tionships, 1,2343.
Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Allensworth, M., Allik, J., Ault,
L., Austers, I., et al. (2003). Are men universally more
dismissing than women? Gender differences in roman-
tic attachment across 62 cultural regions. Personal
Relationships, 10, 307331.
Simpson, J. A., Ickes, W., & Grich, J. (1999). When accu-
racy hurts: Reactions of anxiousambivalent dating
partners to a relationship-threatening situation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 754769.
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., Oriña, M. M., & Grich,
J. (2002). Working models of attachment, support giv-
ing, and support seeking in a stressful situation. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 598608.
Thompson, R. A. (2010). Attachment and life history the-
ory: A rejoinder. Child Development Perspectives, 4,
106108.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & BakermansKranenburg, M. J.
(2010). Stretched until it snaps: Attachment and close
relationships. Child Development Perspectives, 4,
109111.
Sex Differences in Attachment 5
... With regards to the moderator variables, previous research reports variations in both attachment dimensions and self-compassion, depending on either sex or age (Chopik et al., 2013;Del Giudice, 2009;Hwang et al., 2016;Neff & Vonk, 2009;Scharfe, 2016;Souza & Hutz, 2016;Wren et al., 2012). Associations between self-compassion and attachment did not vary based on the proportion of females assessed in the current meta-analysis, suggesting potential generalisation across sexes for this association. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Attachment might shape the extent to which a person is self‐compassionate. Despite the plethora of research examining attachment and self‐compassion, no previous systematic review has quantified the magnitude of the associations between self‐compassion and different attachment dimensions. Design Random‐effects meta‐analyses examined the magnitude of the associations of self‐compassion with anxious, avoidant, and secure attachment, using correlational effects ( r ‐value). Moderator analyses tested whether the effects varied as a function of participant age, sex, population type (students vs. community sample) and attachment measure used within studies. Methods A systematic search of the literature using SCOPUS, Web of Science, and CINAHL databases retrieved 37 eligible studies. Results The meta‐analyses revealed a medium effect size for the positive association between self‐compassion with secure attachment, r avg = .395, 95% CI [0.248, 0.524], and medium and small effect sizes for the negative associations with anxious attachment, r avg = −.282, 95% CI [−0.329, −0.233], and avoidant attachment, r avg = −.280, 95% CI [−0.320, −0.240]. Moderator analyses indicate that the magnitude of associations with avoidant attachment varied as a function of participant age and population type (students vs. community samples). Conclusions The findings suggest differential associations between self‐compassion and attachment dimensions. Self‐compassion was positively associated with secure attachment, while the reverse was found for insecure attachment. Negative associations between self‐compassion and avoidant attachment were larger for older individuals. Ageing populations may be vulnerable to lower self‐compassion when already more prone to experiencing avoidant attachment. Compassion‐focused therapy may be an effective therapeutic option when working with individuals reliant on anxious or avoidant attachment dimensions.
... Another study by Ripardo & Lordelo (2022) found that maternal control played a predictive role in insecure attachment in women, whereas both paternal care and control were relevant factors. Evidence also suggests that across cultures, men are more likely to exhibit a dismissing-avoidant attachment style, while women are more prone to an anxious-preoccupied style (Scharfe, 2016;Del Giudice, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Attachment is an intrinsic relational mechanism that influences individuals' perceptions, behaviours, and relationships. Extensive research has consistently linked parenting styles to attachment patterns in adulthood. This study aimed to examine the relationship between parenting styles and romantic attachment in adults and whether gender served as a moderating variable. A total of 146 students (128 females, 18 males) from Maldives National University, aged 18 to 49, who were either married or in a dating relationship completed an online survey assessing parenting styles using the Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) and romantic attachment styles using the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) Scale. Results of correlational analyses revealed that perceived authoritarian parenting had a positive association with attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance, suggesting an insecure attachment pattern. Permissive and authoritative parenting styles negatively correlated with these attachment dimensions, reflecting a secure romantic attachment. Contrary to expectations, gender did not moderate these relationships. The study's limitations include a gender imbalance and a focus on perceived maternal parenting styles only. The results have practical implications for therapeutic interventions, parenting practices, and educational programs aimed at fostering secure attachments. Additionally, they highlight the need for future research to explore other moderating variables and longitudinal effects of parenting on attachment across diverse cultural contexts.
... In the general population, according to Vrtbovská (2009, in: Hybenová, 2010, approximately 50-75% of individuals have secure attachment relationships, with Hazan and Shaver (1987) suggesting it's around 60% of adults with secure attachment, with the remainder evenly distributed among insecure attachment types. Despite researchers reporting over the past 40 years that most individuals, regardless of age, are securely attached (Scharfe, 2016), Freeman and Brown (2001) suggest that more than half of adolescents have an insecure attachment style, with Akhtar (2012) indicating a preference among adolescents for an avoidant attachment style. This inconsistency may be attributed to the specificity of the research sample, its geographical context, and the characteristics of contemporary lifestyle or the COVID pandemic. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The aim of this chapter is to provide an overview of selected social factors influencing the level of radicalization in adolescents. It has been confirmed that these factors can significantly impact the radicalization process in adolescents (Beelmann, 2020; Doosje et al., 2016; Wolfowicz et al., 2021). The authors specifically focused on attachment relationships and social isolation. Both constructs are related to how adolescents perceive themselves and the world around them, which can contribute to their vulnerability to radicalizing influences. A secure attachment provides a child with a sense of safety, trust, and support, contributing to healthy emotional and social development. Adolescents with secure attachments are expected to be more resilient to radicalizing influences because they have strong emotional foundations and healthy relationships that provide support and a sense of identity (Delgado et al., 2022). On the other hand, social isolation refers to a state where individuals have limited contact and support from others, which can lead to feelings of loneliness, alienation, and despair. Those who feel socially isolated may be more vulnerable to radicalization as they seek ways to feel accepted and valued, lacking the resources that provide emotional support and diverse perspectives (Alivernini & Manganelli, 2016; Copeland et al., 2018). The addressed social factors share the assumption that individuals have an inherent need to belong to a group or be accepted, a desire for interpersonal relationships, which can help explain human behavior and the motivation to join a group (whether legitimate or radical/extremist) (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Attachment theory emphasizes the need to form and maintain interpersonal bonds, and social isolation also relates to interpersonal connections. Therefore, we believe that attachment relationships and social isolation may be key factors in the radicalization process of adolescents. In the context of the Slovak Republic, these factors have not been sufficiently analyzed concerning radicalization and extremism. Thus, the authors aim to fill this gap by building knowledge based on empirical results. Understanding these mechanisms will help formulate and implement effective preventive measures, thereby contributing to the fight against the radicalization of young people.
... In addition, women tend to have higher participation in social activities compared to men (Hong et al., 2009;Liu et al., 2020). The origin of this difference between men and women is debated, but some argue that women and men may perceive social interactions differently, and consequently behave differently in their relationships (Scharfe, 2017). When facing adversity, women are more likely to seek emotional support from their social network than men; men may turn to their network as well, but interactions are likely to focus on instrumental support (Fiori & Denckla, 2012;Kendler et al., 2005;Matud et al., 2003;Meij et al., 2008;Stroebe & Schut, 2010;Vandervoort, 2000). ...
... Women in the current sample reported higher levels of anxiety, stress, and attachment anxiety than men, revealing patterns that align with prior findings. [24][25][26][27] However, while it is a common pattern that men tend to report higher levels of avoidance, 28,29 the current sample did not find gender difference in attachment avoidance, which was in line with findings reported by Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn 30 and Liu and Ma. 31 As called by Del Giudice, 29 more research is needed to better understand the role of gender in romantic attachment. Note. ...
Article
Background and objectives: The stressors of the COVID-19 pandemic spurred alarming levels of stress and anxiety in college students. It is important to identify factors that attenuate the negative effect of stress on anxiety. Framed by the attachment diathesis-stress process perspective, this study examined how the two dimensions of insecurity in romantic attachment-attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance-buffered the effect of stress on anxiety in a population of college students during the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic. Design and methods: The study employed cross-sectional and correlation designs and used an online survey to gather self-report data from a sample of 453 college students. Data were collected from March 15, 2020, to February 16, 2021. Results: Anxiety, stress, and the two insecurity dimensions were all mutually correlated with one another. Multiple regression analysis revealed that as the level of attachment anxiety increased, the association of stress and anxiety turned stronger. Conclusions: The findings suggest that targeting attachment insecurity may prove to be fruitful when helping college students effectively regulate stress to lessen levels of anxiety.
... In addition, women tend to have higher participation in social activities compared to men (Hong et al., 2009;Liu et al., 2020). The origin of this difference between men and women is debated, but some argue that women and men may perceive social interactions differently, and consequently behave differently in their relationships (Scharfe, 2017). When facing adversity, women are more likely to seek emotional support from their social network than men; men may turn to their network as well, but interactions are likely to focus on instrumental support (Fiori & Denckla, 2012;Kendler et al., 2005;Matud et al., 2003;Meij et al., 2008;Stroebe & Schut, 2010;Vandervoort, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERS) refer to the cognitive processes used to consciously or unconsciously interpret and manage emotionally arousing information. While their role in emotional functioning is well established, their association with attachment styles has received relatively limited attention. This study examines the use of CERS from both the categorical and dimensional perspectives of attachment theory while also focusing on gender differences. A sample of 684 women and 234 men, aged between 18 and 87 years old completed self-report measures, including the Relationship Scale Questionnaire and the short form of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Data were analyzed using t-tests, the Kruskal-Wallis test, and multiple regression with robust errors. Results revealed small gender differences: women scored higher on anxiety attachment dimension, rumination, and catastrophizing, whereas men scored slightly higher on acceptance. No significant differences were found between secure and dismissing-avoidant attachment styles in CERS; however, both groups differed significantly from individuals with preoccupied and fearful attachment styles. For the dimensional model of attachment, compared to the avoidant dimension, the anxiety dimension emerged as the dominant predictor, impairing cognitive processes essential for adaptive strategies and fostering reliance on maladaptive strategies. These findings highlight the pivotal role of attachment anxiety in shaping cognitive emotion regulation in both genders.
Article
The current study aimed to explore peer attachment among Algerian adolescents with particular focus on gender differentials, more specifically, the prevalence and differences in the average degrees of each one of peer attachment determinants (levels, styles, and dimensions peer attachment) among the study sample. In order to achieve this goal, a quantitative research was carried out employing a descriptive analytical approach, through which, the emotional attachment scale for adolescents in its form of attachment to friends by Abdul Rahman and Al Omari (2014) (codified by the researchers) was administered to a sample consisting of (130) adolescents. The study resulted in several noteworthy findings: foremost among them, the „high level‟ of peer attachment, the dimension of „peer trust‟, and the „secure attachment style‟ are the most prevalent peer attachment dimensions among the study sample, and although there were no statistically significant differences in light of gender in all said determinants among the sample members, a statistically significant difference in peer communication is present, in which the differences are in favor of females. Keywords: Adolescents ; Peer Attachment ; Attachment Styles ; Attachment Dimensions ; Gender.
Article
Background: Traumatic bonding, defined as attachment to a perpetrator of intimate partner violence (IPV), offers one explanation as to why many people with abusive romantic partners do not break off these relationships. But what individual-level risk factors make some victims of IPV more likely than others to develop traumatic bonding toward their partners? What is the nature of the potential association between traumatic bonding and PTSD symptoms? Participants: A path model tested the potential roles of childhood maltreatment and attachment insecurity as risk factors for traumatic bonding, as well as the potential association between traumatic bonding and PTSD symptoms, in a high-risk sample of 354 participants in current abusive relationships. Results: As hypothesized, childhood maltreatment and attachment insecurity significantly predicted traumatic bonding over and above the effects of age, gender, and romantic love. In addition, attachment insecurity moderated the association between childhood maltreatment and traumatic bonding, such that at higher levels of attachment insecurity, the association between childhood maltreatment and traumatic bonding was stronger than at mean or lower levels of attachment insecurity. Traumatic bonding was positively associated with PTSD symptoms. Conclusions: Overall, the results support the role of childhood maltreatment as a risk factor for both traumatic bonding and PTSD symptoms and highlight the importance of attachment insecurity in these associations. This was the first study to examine a complex model of risk factors for traumatic bonding. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This article explores the possibility that romantic love is an attachment process--a biosocial process by which affectional bonds are formed between adult lovers, just as affectional bonds are formed earlier in life between human infants and their parents. Key components of attachment theory, developed by Bowlby, Ainsworth, and others to explain the development of affectional bonds in infancy, were translated into terms appropriate to adult romantic love. The translation centered on the three major styles of attachment in infancy--secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent--and on the notion that continuity of relationship style is due in part to mental models (Bowlby's "inner working models") of self and social life. These models, and hence a person's attachment style, are seen as determined in part by childhood relationships with parents. Two questionnaire studies indicated that relative prevalence of the three attachment styles is roughly the same in adulthood as in infancy, the three kinds of adults differ predictably in the way they experience romantic love, and attachment style is related in theoretically meaningful ways to mental models of self and social relationships and to relationship experiences with parents. Implications for theories of romantic love are discussed, as are measurement problems and other issues related to future tests of the attachment perspective.
Article
Full-text available
This research assesses the contribution of pregnant women's attachment style to their reactions to the fetus and pregnancy. In Study 1, 260 women who were at different stages of pregnancy completed measures tapping attachment style and bonding to the fetus. In Study 2, 30 women were classified according to their attachment style, and their bonding to the fetus, mental health, and ways of coping with pregnancy-related problems were followed-up during the 3 trimesters of pregnancy. Secure women were strongly attached to the fetus from the beginning of pregnancy and reported seeking support and positive mental health during the entire pregnancy. Avoidant women showed weak attachment to the fetus and negative mental health in the first and third trimesters of pregnancy, and stronger bonding and better mental health in the second trimester. They also relied on distancing coping during the entire pregnancy. Anxious-ambivalent women showed a gradual increase in bonding to fetus from trimester to trimester, but their reliance on emotion-focused coping and negative mental health remained stable throughout the entire pregnancy. Results are discussed in terms of attachment theory.
Article
Full-text available
Gender differences in the dismissing form of adult romantic attachment were investigated as part of the International Sexuality Description Project-a survey study of 17,804 people from 62 cultural regions. Contrary to research findings previously reported in Western cultures, we found that men were not significantly more dismissing than women across all cultural regions. Gender differences in dismissing romantic attachment were evident in most cultures, but were typically only small to moderate in magnitude. Looking across cultures, the degree of gender differentiation in dismissing romantic attachment was predictably associated with sociocultural indicators. Generally, these associations supported evolutionary theories of romantic attachment, with smaller gender differences evident in cultures with high-stress and high-fertility reproductive environments. Social role theories of human sexuality received less support in that more progressive sex-role ideologies and national gender equity indexes were not cross-culturally linked as expected to smaller gender differences in dismissing romantic attachment.
Article
Full-text available
The association between attachment styles and emotional responses to traumatic memories was examined among 153 Palestinian former political prisoners. Self-report tools revealing adult attachment and intensity and valence of emotional responses were applied. As hypothesized, a high intensity of cognitive appraisal and a low intensity of affective responses characterized the emotional profile of insecure-dismissing men. By contrast, the emotional profile of insecure-preoccupied men was characterized by low cognitive and high affective responses, and intensive behavioral urge to act. Secure men in turn had a moderate and balanced emotional profile involving both cognitive and affective responses. In accordance with the activation hypothesis, when exposed to a high level of torture and ill-treatment, the insecure-preoccupied men showed especially intensive affective and behavioral responses. Contrary to the hypothesis, the insecure-dismissing men showed high-intensive cognitive and low-intensive affective responses independently of the severity of the trauma exposure.
Article
Full-text available
Why are anxious–ambivalent individuals especially likely to have turbulent and unstable relationships? To help answer this question, the authors use 3 theoretical perspectives to examine how heightened empathic accuracy in a relationship-threatening situation is associated with personal and relational distress. Dating couples inferred their partners' thoughts and feelings from a videotaped interaction which they each rated slides of opposite-sex individuals. Highly anxious–ambivalent individuals were more empathically accurate in this relationship-threatening situation; however, their self-reported thoughts and feelings indicated greater distress and less confidence in their partners and relationships. The more anxious–ambivalent women reported a slight decrease in the perceived closeness of the relationships. Four months later, more anxious–ambivalent men's relationships were more likely to have ended. These findings are discussed in terms of their theoretical and applied implications. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The current work examines the connection between attachment theory and meaning in life (MIL) across adulthood, by inspecting attachment style differences on two dimensions of MIL: presence of meaning (PML) and search for meaning (SML). MIL and attachment measures were collected from 992 participants of three age-groups, young adults (21–30), established adults (31–49), and older adults (50–65). Multivariate analyses demonstrated that older adults scored higher on PML, while younger adults reported more SML. In general, securely attached individuals demonstrated more PML and less SML than participants with insecure attachment styles, and individuals with a fearful attachment style displayed more SML than other attachment styles. Age interacted with attachment, as dismissive young adults displayed less SML, and gender differences were revealed in PML among established adults with regard to the preoccupied and fearful attachment styles. Finally, a three-way interaction of attachment × age × gender was found for PML, as in the established adults, both preoccupied men and fearful women reported a decline in PML, while older women with secure attachment reported higher levels of PML. While in accordance with the developing literature in the field of positive psychology, the current findings shed light on the manner by which the connections between attachment styles, age and gender are associated with the presence and the search for MIL.
Article
This study examined how working models of attachment to parents (assessed by the Adult Attachment Interview—AAI) and romantic partners (assessed by the Adult Attachment Questionnaire—AAQ) predicted spontaneous caregiving and care seeking in a stressful situation. Dating couples were videotaped while one partner (the man) waited to do a stressful task. Observers then rated each woman’s support giving and each man’s support seeking. The AAI and the AAQ independently predicted behavioral outcomes. Women with more secure representations of their parents and whose dating partners sought more support provided more support, whereas women with more secure representations of their parents whose partners sought less support provided less. Women who reported being more avoidantly attached to romantic partners provided less support than did less avoidant women, regardless of how much support their partners sought. Attachment orientations did not predict men’s support seeking.
Article
In this book a leading theorist on sex and gender discusses how hidden assumptions embedded in our cultural discourses, social institutions, and individual psyches perpetuate male power and oppress women and sexual minorities. Sandra Lipsitz Bem argues that these assumptions, which she calls the lenses of gender, shape not only perceptions of social reality but also the more material things—like unequal pay and inadequate daycare—that constitute social reality itself. Her . . . examination of these hidden cultural lenses enables us to look at them rather than through them and to better understand recent debates on gender and sexuality. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)