Conference PaperPDF Available

Understanding " Death by GPS " : A Systematic Analysis of Catastrophic Incidents Associated with Personal Navigation Technologies

Authors:
  • University of St.Gallen

Abstract and Figures

Catastrophic incidents associated with GPS devices and other personal navigation technologies are sufficiently common that these incidents have been given a colloquial nickname: " Death by GPS ". While there is a significant body of work on the use of personal navigation technologies in everyday scenarios, no research has examined these technologies' roles in catastrophic incidents. In this paper, we seek to address this gap in the literature. Borrowing techniques from public health research and communication studies, we construct a corpus of 158 detailed news reports of unique catastrophic incidents associated with personal navigation technologies. We then identify key themes in these incidents and the roles that navigation technologies played in them, e.g. missing road characteristics data contributed to over 24% of these incidents. With the goal of reducing casualties associated with personal navigation technologies, we outline implications for design and research that emerge from our results, e.g. advancing " space usage rule " mapping, incorporating weather information in routing, and improving visual and audio instructions in complex situations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Understanding “Death by GPS”: A Systematic
Study of Catastrophic Incidents Associated
with Personal Navigation Technologies
Allen Yilun Lin*, Kate Kuehl**, Johannes Schöning, Brent Hecht*
*Northwestern University, **University of Minnesota, University of Bremen
allen.lin@eecs.northwestern.edu, kuehl088@umn.edu, schoening@uni-bremen.de,
bhecht@northwestern.edu
ABSTRACT
Catastrophic incidents associated with GPS devices and
other personal navigation technologies are sufficiently
common that these incidents have been given a colloquial
nickname: “Death by GPS”. While there is a significant body
of work on the use of personal navigation technologies in
everyday scenarios, no research has examined these
technologies’ roles in catastrophic incidents. In this paper,
we seek to address this gap in the literature. Borrowing
techniques from public health research and communication
studies, we construct a corpus of 158 detailed news reports
of unique catastrophic incidents associated with personal
navigation technologies. We then identify key themes in
these incidents and the roles that navigation technologies
played in them, e.g. missing road characteristics data
contributed to over 24% of these incidents. With the goal of
reducing casualties associated with personal navigation
technologies, we outline implications for design and research
that emerge from our results, e.g. advancing “space usage
rule” mapping, incorporating weather information in routing,
and improving visual and audio instructions in complex
situations.
Author Keywords
GPS; SatNav; personal navigation technologies; map apps
ACM Classification Keywords
H.5.m. Information interfaces and presentation (e.g., HCI):
Miscellaneous;
INTRODUCTION
A tourist drives his rental car across a beach and directly into
the Atlantic Ocean [16]. A person in Belgium intending to
drive to a nearby train station ends up in Croatia [46]. A
family traveling on a dirt road gets stranded for four days in
the Australian outback [45]. These incidents all have one
major factor in common: playing a key role in each incident
was a personal navigation technology, i.e. a GPS device, a
mobile map app (e.g. Google Maps, Apple Maps) or a
SatNav.
Catastrophic incidents associated with personal navigation
technologies are sufficiently common that they have come to
be associated with a colloquial name: “Death by GPS” [34].
While thankfully not all of these incidents involve the loss of
life, it is not uncommon to see media reports of people
endangering themselves or others and/or causing extensive
property damage due in part to their interaction with a
personal navigation technology.
It is tempting to blame these incidents on users and users
alone. Indeed, reports of these incidents are often peppered
with comments from witnesses and observers inquiring as to
why drivers “wouldn’t question driving into a puddle that
doesn’t seem to end” [34] and did not notice “multiple-
language traffic signs” [46]. However, it is our responsibility
as HCI researchers to design better systems that help people
avoid making “user errors [36], especially when these errors
involve such extensive human and financial costs.
The geographic human-computer interaction (GeoHCI”)
[17] literature includes a relatively large body of work that
examines how people use GPS-based navigation
technologies in standard scenarios and in the course of their
everyday lives (e.g. [7,18,21,27,28]). However, no work has
focused on the increasingly large number of catastrophic
incidents associated with these technologies. In other words,
the “Death by GPS” phenomenon has yet to be studied in a
rigorous fashion.
This paper seeks to begin the process of addressing this gap
in the literature. As has been pointed out in the work on
typical interactions with GPS devices [7], a major obstacle
to the systematic analysis of “Death by GPS” incidents is that
no database of these incidents exists. Additionally, methods
that have been used to study interaction with GPS devices in
the past (e.g. lab studies, field studies) are not valid for this
type of analysis.
To overcome these obstacles, we turned to an unlikely source
of data: news articles. This approach is adapted from the
public health literature, where news articles are used as
sensors when the research topic is of sufficient significance
but no authoritative dataset is available. Using rigorous best
practices for building a minimally biased-corpus of news
Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for
personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are
not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies
bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for third-
party components of this work must be honored. For all other uses, contact
the Owner/Author.
Copyright is held by the owner/author(s).
CHI 2017, May 06-11, 2017, Denver, CO, USA
ACM 978-1-4503-4655-9/17/05.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3025453.3025737
stories and expert-led qualitative coding, we collected and
analyzed a dataset of 158 news stories about unique
catastrophic incidents associated with personal navigation
technologies.
In our analyses of this corpus, we had two cascading research
goals:
Goal 1: Identify the patterns that characterize catastrophic
incidents associated with personal navigation technologies.
Goal 2: Use the identified patterns to generate implications
for research and design that can help build safer personal
navigation technologies.
More specifically, for our first goal, we sought to ascertain
themes in both the basic properties of these incidents (e.g.
Who was involved? What happened?) and themes in the
roles that navigation technologies played in the incidents (i.e.
How did the navigation technology specifically fail the
user?). Based on the identified patterns, our second research
goal involved outlining a series of concrete steps that
researchers and practitioners can take to prevent the
reoccurrence of common types of catastrophic incidents (and
save lives).
We find, for instance, that a large number of “Death by GPS”
incidents are single-vehicle collisions (likely far more than
accidents caused by other factors), that stranding events were
the next most common type of incident, and that distraction
by a navigation device was significantly associated with
more serious incidents. With regard to the roles of
technology, we observed that missing road characteristics
attributes (e.g. road surface types and current condition) had
a substantial effect, as did the failure to correctly infer
routing preferences (among a series of other factors).
The implications for research and design that emerge from
our findings span the spectrum of “GeoHCI” topical
domains. For example, we discuss how our results highlight
the importance of (1) incorporating vehicle type and weather
information into routing algorithms, (2) improving
navigation guidance in the face of complex geographies, and
(3) developing separate interfaces for tourists and locals.
More generally, our results show that navigation devices can
be more intelligent about safety than their current state-of-
the-art: telling users to pay attention to their environment
when the device is turned on. Blanket warnings like these are
known to be ineffective in HCI [35], and our results show a
path forward towards improved approaches.
In summary, this paper makes the following contributions:
1. We perform the first research that systematically
characterizes catastrophic incidents associated with
personal navigation technologies and the role that these
technologies played in these incidents. We identify
1
The dataset can be downloaded here: https://goo.gl/8vE14V
major themes in the incidents themselves and in the roles
played by technology.
2. With the goal of preventing the patterns we identified in
these catastrophic incidents from reoccurring, we
outline a series of implications for research and design
that can help us develop safer personal navigation
technologies.
To further research on this topic, we are also releasing the
core dataset we developed for this paper
1
. This dataset
consists of the complete corpus of 158 news stories along
with all the codes we applied to each story in the process
described below. To make our findings more accessible, we
are also releasing an interactive web map version of the
corpus, which allows users to see the approximate location
of each incident and further information about the incident
2
.
A Note on Terminology: The subject of this research
resulted in several terminological challenges. The core
technologies of interest to this paper GPS devices, SatNav
devices, and mobile map applications like Google Maps and
Apple Maps are often referred to using the term GPS”.
This term ignores the diverse positioning techniques (e.g.
Wi-Fi positioning), routing algorithms, and cartography built
into these technologies, so we felt it was imprecise to use this
more casual language given the nature of this paper. As such,
we use the term personal navigation technology
(sometimes shortened to navigation technology or
“navigation device”). Similarly, given the diversity of the
types of incidents in our corpus, assigning this class of
incidents a formal name was not straightforward. We chose
the term “catastrophic incidents” in accordance with the
“extremely unfortunate or unsuccessful” definition of
“catastrophic” [50].
RELATED WORK
This work’s core motivation primarily emerges from two
areas in the “GeoHCI” literature: (1) work that has examined
the use of personal navigation technologies in standard
scenarios and (2) research that has looked at the long-term
behavioral and cognitive effects of using these technologies.
Navigation Technologies in Standard Scenarios
Researchers began to investigate HCI issues associated with
in-car navigation systems almost as soon as these
technologies were first commercialized [10,11,48]. This
thread of research covers a diverse set of topics including
attention demands [11,23,48], cartography [27,33,42],
different modes of output [9,21] and age-related variation
[2], all with a focus on everyday usage scenarios. For
instance, Kun et al. [23] conducted a lab simulation study and
found that graphical GPS interfaces distracted users from the
primary task of driving. Medenica et al. [33] coupled
augmented reality with in-car GPS navigators and showed
that this combination reduced drivers’ distractions. Jensen et
2
The interactive map is available here: https://goo.gl/jlQ8S4
al. [21] compared different interaction modes of in-car GPS
navigators and concluded that the combination of audio-
visual output is preferred by drivers, but did not significantly
reduce device-related distractions.
The projects in this research thread that most directly
motivated our work are those of Hipp et al. [18] and Brown
and Laurier [6]. Both studies considered the troubles
drivers encountered with in-car GPS devices in typical
driving situations. Hipp et al. [18] conducted a traditional
user interface evaluation to compare the performances of
different types of in-car navigation systems on the same
route. They identified unexpressed routing preferences,
failure to understand intentional detours from planned routes,
and the lack of real-time traffic information as the common
interaction weakness of commercial navigators (with the
latter now being fixed in most modern navigation
technologies). Brown and Laurier [7] carried out an
interaction analysis in which they observed and interviewed
drivers about their daily uses of in-car GPS to understand
their navigation practices. They outlined five types of
“normal troubles” of using in-car GPS navigators in
everyday driving: destination, routing, maps and sensors,
timing of instructions and inflexibility of the technology.
This work is distinguished from that above in that instead of
studying the use of personal navigation technologies in
standard scenarios, we focus on catastrophic incidents that
involved these technologies. Some of the roles that these
technologies play in catastrophic incidents are similar to
those identified in the literature on standard scenarios, and
other roles are new to the literature (as are the resulting
design implications). We discuss the relationship between
our findings and the findings from prior work in detail below.
Long-term Impact of Navigation Technology Use
Another class of relevant research focuses on understanding
the behavioral and cognitive changes produced by personal
navigation technologies. For instance, Leshed et al. [28]
conducted an ethnography-based study and showed that
drivers using GPS-based navigation technologies are
disengaged from their surrounding environment. Aporta and
Higgs [3] examined the long-term impact of navigation
technology at a larger scale, arguing that the adoption of
navigation technologies has alienated many Inuit hunters
from the traditional wayfinding skills they have depended on
for thousands of years. Other studies have looked at the
cognitive impact of navigation systems. For instance,
Gardony et la. [13] conducted a lab-based simulation study
and demonstrated that these devices may impair users’
ability to record information about the environment and their
spatial orientation. The findings of this line of work inform
this paper’s research and design implications, specifically
those related to the multifaceted relationships between a
navigation technology, its user, and the environment.
METHODS
Although catastrophic incidents associated with personal
navigation technologies are sufficiently noteworthy to have
been given a moniker “Death by GPS” no authoritative
dataset of these incidents exists. The high stakes of these
incidents make them worthy of study, but the lack of
available data and relative rarity of these incidents make it
difficult to analyze them. Additionally, lab experiments or
other simulations are not currently well-suited to this
research area.
Fortunately, the domain of public health has significant
experience studying phenomena with the same core
properties as “Death by GPS” incidents, i.e. relatively rare
phenomena of media interest for which no authoritative
dataset is available and for which simulations are not
currently tractable. Specifically, to examine these
phenomena, researchers in this domain have followed a two-
step pipeline: (1) build a corpus of news stories describing
these incidents and (2) analyze the corpus using expert-led
qualitative coding techniques. For example, in the absence of
a national surveillance system for homicide-suicide in the
United States, Malphurs and Cohen [31] collected and coded
related news articles from 191 national newspapers to
identify the number and subtypes of such incidents. This
approach of relying on newspapers to summarize the
characteristics of homicide-suicide incidents has also been
applied in the Netherlands [29] and Italy [40]. Similarly, to
study the collisions between wheelchairs and motor vehicles,
a type of accident that is not distinguished in police reports,
LaBan and Nabity [24] gathered 107 news articles using
LexisNexis. They analyzed this corpus to understand gender
incidence ratios, proportion of different types of motor
vehicles, the time of incidents, and other characteristics of
these incidents.
In this paper, we adopt this approach from the public health
literature. To do so, we first verified that no relevant
authoritative dataset exists by contacting several local police
departments and national agencies, including the
Minneapolis Police Department (USA), the Aachen Police
Department (Germany), National Transportation Safety
Board (USA) and National Highway Traffic Safety
Administration (USA). We then implemented the pipeline
from public health, using the process described in more detail
below.
Phase 1: Corpus Development
One of the key challenges in the public health-based
approach is gathering the corpus of news articles. Most prior
work has relied on one of two methods: (1) an exhaustive
search in the local newspaper of a specific study site (e.g.
[5,38]) or (2) unstructured but extensive querying of news
search engines (e.g. [24,31]). Since our work is not well-
suited to a specific study site, we implemented a more robust
version of the latter approach using best practices from
communication studies for sampling news stories with
minimal bias [25,44].
The first step in this minimal bias sampling approach
involves leveraging prior research in this space (i.e. the
literature covered in the Related Work section) to seed a set
of keywords, which is then grown using a variety of
structured strategies (e.g. synonym generation). These
keywords are then used to iteratively query a news database
(in our case LexisNexis), with the set of keywords refined at
each step. Achieving acceptable precision for one’s
keywords is particularly important given that these databases
often have strict limits on the total number of news stories
that can be returned for a given query (e.g. LexisNexis’s is
set to 1000 stories). We were able to achieve a precision of
40.8%, which is within acceptable parameters [44]. This left
us with 408 articles that were related to catastrophic
incidents associated with personal navigation technologies.
We note that we restricted our search to stories published in
2010 or later to ensure that our findings are relevant to
modern personal navigation technologies (e.g. smartphone
map apps) rather than early-generation devices.
Additionally, due to language constraints with respect to the
database and the coders, we only searched for stories written
in English, a subject we cover in more detail in the limitation
section.
Two challenges remained before we could begin the next
stage of the pipeline. First, many of the articles were opinion
columns about the “Death by GPS” phenomenon and did not
describe specific incidents. Second, some incidents were
described by two different journalists in two different
publications. To remove these articles from our dataset, two
researchers conducted an exhaustive search, reading each
article and evaluating its validity for our study and matching
duplicates (we kept the more detailed of any two stories on
the same incident; disagreements were resolved through
discussion).
In the end, we were left with a corpus that contains 158 news
stories, each describing a unique catastrophic incident
associated with personal navigation technologies. For
replication purposes and for researchers who may want to
apply this method in other contexts, we have included
additional detail about how we implemented our corpus-
building procedure in the documentation of our coded
dataset. We also discuss the one minor change we had to
make to the standard procedure to adapt it to the goals of our
research project: we could not simply use the keywords from
prior research on interaction with navigation technologies
because these only focused on standard scenarios. As such,
we used a slightly more iterative keyword generation method
in which researchers identified keywords from small samples
of actual relevant news stories.
Phase 2: Expert-led Coding
The second stage of the public health pipeline involves
employing a relatively standard qualitative coding procedure
with an important exception: coders are domain experts. This
expertise enables coders to map properties of incidents
reported in news articles to pre-existing topics in the
literature of interest, or to new challenges when relevant. In
our case, our two coders (members of our research team) had
extensive expertise in both geography and HCI, the two
fields most associated with our research questions. More
specifically, each coder had both a Masters’ degree in
geography or geoinformatics and a Masters’ degree in
computer science (with a focus on HCI).
The specifics of the coding process were as follows: using a
small seed corpus, knowledge of our research goals, and
expertise in the research domain, our coders jointly
established a series of coding dimensions. Next, using a
random sample of 10 articles, the coders jointly developed a
list of codes for each dimension and developed a
corresponding codebook (this is included in our dataset).
Both coders then evaluated a set of 40 overlapping articles to
assess each dimension for interrater reliability. Importantly,
when it was not possible to assess an article for a particular
coding dimension, coders left the value blank.
The Cohen’s Kappa of coders’ results on all dimensions
ranged from 0.69 to 0.95, which indicates “substantial
agreement” [26]. Of particular note, we achieved a Cohen’s
Kappa of 0.79 for the Technological Cause dimension,
which is the basis for a set of key findings below. As the
Cohen’s Kappa was sufficiently high for all dimensions,
coders evaluated the remaining articles on an individual
basis.
Beyond Technological Cause, other major coding
dimensions that were considered included the Seriousness of
the incident (e.g. Was death involved?), the Incident Type
(e.g. Was it a single-vehicle collision? Did a vehicle get
stranded?), Weather, Road Surface (e.g. Was it on a dirt road
or a paved road?), whether Distraction was explicitly noted
as an issue in the article and whether the driver was a Local
Driver or Non-local Driver to the area of the incident. A
complete list of dimensions and their corresponding specific
codes is included in our public dataset. For the major coding
dimensions, coders were able to assign codes for over 90%
of incidents with the exception of Local Driver, in which
37% of incidents could not be coded.
Interpretation of Results
As described above, there is a consensus (e.g.
[22,24,29,31,38,40]) in the public health literature that when
no authoritative data is available, the news article-based
pipeline we employ here can provide valuable early insight
about a phenomenon of interest. However, news article-
derived data has its share of limitations, as is the case with
many datasets considered in public health (e.g. even
authoritative crime datasets have been criticized for
potentially strong racial biases [39], an issue the computing
community has been facing in association with predictive
policing technologies [20]). To the best of our knowledge,
our use of news article-derived data is novel to the HCI
literature. As such, we believe that highlighting the known
limitations of this type of data early in the paper is important
so that our results can be interpreted in proper context.
The most significant limitation of news article-derived data
is a risk of “newsworthiness” bias, or an overrepresentation
of incidents that are in alignment with the incentive
structures of news organizations. While at least one study
has found no such bias (e.g. [38]), others have found
newsworthiness bias to manifest as an overrepresentation of
(1) accidental incidents (e.g. fewer suicides, more unusual
events) or (2) more fatal incidents (e.g. more murders, fewer
assaults) [12]. All incidents that we examine are accidental
in nature, making the accidental bias less relevant [12,38].
However, a potential bias towards fatal incidents is important
to consider when examining our results below.
To minimize further risk of bias, we employ robust statistical
tests when making comparisons between types of incidents.
In most cases, we are able to simply use Pearson’s Chi-
squared test of independence. However, in circumstances
where the assumptions of Chi-squared distribution are
violated due to relatively small sample size, we used a
likelihood ratio G test of independence, a best practice
suggested by [1,32]. All p-values reported in the paper have
been subject to Bonferroni correction.
Newsworthiness bias mainly affects proportional results (i.e.
comparisons between incident types), which are a small
percentage of the results we present below. The bulk of our
results are either qualitative descriptions of incidents or
absolute values (e.g. raw counts of incidents of certain
types). Our absolute results should be interpreted in the
context of a limited understanding of the size of the incident
population, i.e. we do not know what share of catastrophic
incidents associated with personal navigation technologies
are included in our news corpus. However, even if the
incidents in our sample are close to the entire population, the
aggregate devastation to blood and treasure of just these
incidents make them worthy of analysis and discussion in the
HCI literature, which does not often examine such high-cost
interactions with technology. In order to add additional
context and further this discussion, we provide qualitative
descriptions of incidents wherever space allows.
RESULTS
In this section, we provide an overview of the major results
that emerged from our coding process. In doing so, we seek
to address our first research goal: characterizing patterns in
catastrophic incidents associated with personal navigation
technologies. We organize our thematic findings into two
groups (1) themes in the basic properties of these incidents
and (2) themes in the technological causes of these incidents.
We discuss each group of findings in turn below.
Basic Properties
Many People Have Died in Incidents Associated with
Personal Navigation Technologies
Table 1 shows the results of our coding for the Seriousness
of the incidents with respect to human and financial cost.
Clear in Table 1 is that navigation technologies have been
associated with some truly tragic events: our corpus
3
News article numbers refer to the index in our released dataset.
describes the deaths of 52 people in total, including two
children. These deaths occurred across 45 incidents, or 28%
of our corpus. Additionally, our corpus contains 23 incidents
(15%) that resulted in significant bodily harm, but not death.
Although the proportion of fatal incidents in our corpus may
be exaggerated due to the aforementioned newsworthiness
bias, the absolute number of deaths (and injuries) associated
with navigation technologies that we have identified is
alarming. GPS devices, mobile maps, and other navigation
technologies provide us with tremendous benefits, but these
results indicate that they also have a set of costs that had not
yet been systematically enumerated. These results also
highlight the importance of better understanding catastrophic
incidents like those studied here, as well as using this
understanding to design safer technologies.
Table 1 also shows that “Death by GPS” is not the ideal term
to describe incidents associated with navigation technologies
that have serious implications. Over 50% of the incidents in
our corpus did not involve death or significant injury, with
the damage in these cases being primarily of a financial or
other nature. Examples of these incidents include a group of
skiers who intended to go to La Plagne, a famous ski resort
in the Alps, but ended up arriving at Plagne, a town in
southern France that is 715 km away (article #46
3
). Another
example involved five men who drove onto a nuclear power
plant’s property at the behest of their navigation device and
were suspected of terrorism (article #95).
The Most Common Incident Type is a Single-Vehicle Crash,
but There is Substantial Incident Type Diversity
Table 2 depicts the results of our coding for Incident Type
and shows that the most common type of incident in our
corpus is car crashes. However, the table also shows that
crashes are far from the only type of incident we
encountered. For instance, almost 20% of incidents resulted
in cars being stranded in very rural areas and over 15%
involved people going on substantial detours. We were also
surprised by the number of reports (7) of people driving on
the wrong side of the road for an extended distance. Such
examples include a person who drove 48km on the wrong
side of a highway after following her device’s instructions to
enter the wrong freeway ramp (article #90) and a 37-year-old
man who was caught driving the wrong way on an Australian
Seriousness of Incidents
#
%
Major (deaths)
44
28%
Major (injuries)
23
15%
Medium (e.g. property damage, legal
consequences)
52
33%
Low (e.g. significant inconvenience)
39
25%
highway for more than 10 km and attributed the error to his
navigation device (article #12).
In Table 2, we also show subtypes of the Crashes incident
type. We found that single-vehicle collisions comprised the
majority of crashes (51 cases, 32% of overall incidents), with
crashes with other vehicles (26 cases, 17%) and crashes with
pedestrians and bikes (13 cases, 8%) making up the
remainder of crash incidents. To understand single-vehicle
collisions in more detail, we did an additional round of
coding to identify more detailed themes (this was done by a
single expert coder). Here we found that vehicles colliding
with buildings, walls, and guardrails due to excessively
narrow roads were the most common type of single-vehicle
incident. Crashing with low overhead bridges is another
common occurrence in our corpus, with a diverse array of
other objects in the environment being the subject of the
remainder of the single-car crashes.
Personal Navigation Technology-related Crashes Appear to
Be Proportionally Different Than Typical Crashes
To put the above results in context, we utilized The National
Automotive Sampling System General Estimates System
(NASS GES) dataset from the U.S. National Highway
Traffic Safety Administration [49]. The NASS GES dataset
contains a representative sample of vehicle crashes of all
types as reported by police. While not directly comparable to
our corpus, the NASS GES can provide a sense of whether
personal navigation technology-related crashes are
distributionally similar to the population of car crashes, or
whether the role played by navigation technology manifests
in different types of crash outcomes.
Our results suggest that the latter is the case: crashes
associated with personal navigation technologies appear to
be different in type relative to typical crashes. For instance,
only 15% of car crashes in the NASS GES dataset are single-
vehicle collisions, whereas the same type accounts for 57%
of crashes in our corpus (Table 2). Moreover, crashes
associated with building/walls/guardrails and overhead
bridges are much less common in the NASS GES dataset,
comprising less than 2% of crashes overall, while in our
corpus they account for 42% of all crashes. Among other
implications, this result provides further evidence that simply
adopting standard recommendations from traditional traffic
safety research will not be able to address the safety concerns
associated with personal navigation technologies.
Unfamiliarity with One’s Surroundings Plays a Key Role
A substantial percentage of the incidents in our corpus
occurred when users of personal navigation technologies
were outside of their home region. Specifically, 78% percent
of the incidents involved non-locals and only 22% percent
involved locals. Some examples of incidents involving non-
locals include one story in which a person drove her car into
a swamp (article #23). The driver was quoted as saying This
was the road it told me to take … I don’t know the area at all,
so I just thought it was okay”. Another incident consisted of
a driver hitting and killing a pedestrian, with the
corresponding news article reporting that “the driver was
unfamiliar with the area and was adjusting her GPS
navigational system” (article #71).
While the use of navigation technologies likely increases
outside of one’s home region, this result does suggest that
user interfaces for navigation technologies may want to
encourage more caution and support users in different ways
when they are in their home region and when they are
traveling. We discuss these implications for design in more
detail below.
Distraction Leads to More Serious Incidents
We identified a significant association between our
Distraction coding dimension and our Seriousness
dimension, with distraction leading to many deadly incidents
(  ). Examining the 21 deadly incidents
that involved distraction in more detail, we found that in five
cases, people were using non-critical features of their
navigation device. For instance, a driver killed a cyclist while
“using the zoom-in function” (article #33) and another driver
from Springfield injured a bicyclist while “looking for place
to eat on GPS” (article #40).
Stranding Risk Increases with Dirt Roads, Bad Weather, and
Especially Both at the Same Time
We observed significant associations between our Road
Surface coding dimension and our Incident Type dimension.
In particular, if vehicles were traveling on a dirt road, there
were more than the expected number of stranding incidents
(  ). This was especially the case when
weather was a factor. Examples include a medical student
from Quebec who followed GPS and got stranded on a
logging road for three days in the snow (article #4) and a
British couple and their children who were stranded for four
days on an unsealed road that was made muddy by torrential
rain (article #115). Interestingly, the latter family thought
that their in-car GPS device was suggesting a significant
shortcut and followed its instructions as a result, a point we
return to later.
More generally, we found significant interaction between
disaster type dimension and the weather dimension (
  ). Specifically, there are more than the expected
Types of Incidents
# (%)
Trespass (violate space usage rules)
5 (3%)
Wrong way (opposite side)
7 (4%)
Detour (e.g. wrong address)
25 (16%)
Stranded/stuck (e.g. in the wildness, on railroad
tracks)
31 (20%)
Crashes
90 (57%)
Crashes with pedestrians/bikes
13 (8%)
Crashes with vehicles
26 (17%)
Single-vehicle collisions
51 (32%)
number of stranding incidents under severe weather, as one
might anticipate.
Technological Causes
Table 3 shows the results of our coders assessing each article
for its Technological Cause. In this section, we discuss the
themes in the distribution of these results, as well as the
findings from a more detailed coding that we conducted to
understand important trends.
Attributes that are Missing or Incorrect Are a Major Problem
Geographic information, like that which is used in routing
for personal navigation technologies, consists of two
components: spatial information and attributes of that spatial
information [14]. Broadly speaking, in a routing context, the
spatial information is the location of a road and the attributes
consist of key properties of the road (e.g. the speed limit).
Our results in Table 3 suggest that missing and incorrect
attributes play a major role in the catastrophic incidents in
our corpus, being in part responsible for 64 (53%) of these
incidents. To better understand the types of attributes
involved, one expert conducted a second round of coding to
determine the types of attributes that were most often
missing or incorrect and the results are also included in Table
3. The physical characteristics of the road (e.g. width,
surface) (30 incidents) and clearance height (17 incidents)
were by far the most common type of attributes that were
missing or incorrect. Indeed, stories about routing algorithms
neglecting the road characteristics and the heights of
overpasses are pervasive in our corpus. For example, as
noted above, failure to incorporate road surface information
led multiple sedans to be stranded on unpaved roads (often
in a very dangerous fashion) (e.g. article #4, #115) and
multiple trucks ran into serious trouble due to low-clearance
roads (e.g. article #6, #34, #36). Indeed, we found trucks
were more susceptible to suffer from attribute related issues
due to this problem as evidenced by the significant
interaction between our Vehicle Type coding dimension and
the Technological Cause dimension (   
).
Another theme present in the attribute types in Table 3 is the
notion of “space usage rules” (SURs) [41], or regulations
associated with the use of a certain area (in this case, a road).
For instance, in one incident, a truck that traveled on truck-
prohibited road killed a father and a daughter in a sedan
(article #27). In another, an in-car GPS device guided a
driver up a private driveway, and the driver ended up in a
physical confrontation with the owners of the property
(article #102).
Cartographic and Audio Instructions Are Not Capable of
Handling Complex Geographic Contexts
Table 3 shows that almost 18 incidents involved an issue
with routing guidance, either in visual (cartographic) or
audio form. Past work on the use of GPS devices in standard
scenarios identified that excessive instructions are a
significant problem with GPS usability [2,7]. While we did
observe this problem in our corpus, many of the incidents
given this code by our experts related to a different issue: the
inability of the personal navigation technology to help
drivers navigate complex geographic contexts.
For example, in one story in our corpus, a person who was
driving at night was faced with a freeway on-ramp that was
immediately parallel to a railroad track (article #146). Figure
1 shows a Street View image of the exact location of the
incident. When the driver’s navigation device asked him to
turn right, the driver turned onto the railroad tracks as the
instructions were ambiguous. Ten kilometers later, the
driver’s car was destroyed by an oncoming train, but
fortunately the driver survived by jumping out of the car.
Similarly, article #66 tells a tragic story in which a bicyclist
was hit by a driver who ignored a “Yield” sign at a non-
typical intersection because the driver’s navigation device
screen simply instructed her to “go straight”. Wrong-way
driving was particularly (and significantly; 
   ) associated with cartographic and navigation
instruction issues, and complex geographies were common
in these cases. For instance, one report in our corpus (article
#39) describes the story of a driver who followed her
navigation device’s instructions to “take the first left turn” at
a roundabout. However, the actual first left turn (not the first
legal left turn) was the exit ramp of a freeway, and the driver
who was on the road at night entered the freeway driving
in the wrong direction. This driver sadly lost her life.
Standard Scenarios versus Catastrophic Incidents
As noted above, past work has done a rigorous job of
identifying and categorizing problems encountered by users
Technological Causes
# (%)
Missing or incorrect geographic objects
5 (4%)
Geocoding (i.e. associating toponym and its
coordinates)
7 (6%)
Incorrect toponym disambiguation (i.e. select
similar but wrong destination)
8 (7%)
Instructions/visualization
18 (16%)
Non-transparent/wrong route preference
18 (16%)
Missing or incorrect attributes
64 (53%)
Physical characteristics of the road (e.g. road
surface, road widths)
30 (25%)
Clearance height
17 (14%)
Traffic rules (e.g. no left turn)
5 (4%)
Temporary blockage
3 (3%)
Geopolitical boundary (e.g. country border)
2 (2%)
Private area
2 (2%)
Ferry line as road
3 (3%)
Bridge limitation
2 (2%)
Table 3. Distribution of Technological Cause. Note: The #
does not add up to 158 because coders did not enter a code
when there was not enough information in given news story to
make a certain type of assessment.
of personal navigation technologies in standard usage
scenarios. While the issues discussed above have not been
highlighted in prior work, one additional contribution of the
results in Table 3 is to add gravity to many of the previously-
identified issues. For instance, in a study of challenges
encountered in standard GPS device usage, Brown and
Laurier [7] found that route preferences, out-of-date spatial
data, the timing of navigation guidance, and positioning
errors were key sources of user frustration. Some of these
issues appear in Table 3, meaning that they were in part
responsible for a number of catastrophic incidents in addition
to more everyday usability issues.
Of particular note are Brown and Laurier’s findings with
respect to route preference. Route preference issues played a
role in 18 (16%) of the news stories in our corpus, indicating
they are a significant issue in catastrophic incidents as well
as everyday usage scenarios. However, the route selection
issues present in our corpus are of a substantially different
character than those identified by Brown and Laurier.
Specifically, while participants in Brown and Laurier’s study
wanted more route choice, people in our corpus were given
too many choices (i.e. at least one was dangerous). For
example, in one incident a Canadian couple got lost in rural
Nevada after selecting the “shortest path” route option
suggested by their navigation device, which included a little-
maintained road. They were stranded in Nevada for 49 days,
during which time the husband sadly lost his life (article #9).
We return to this case and the issue of strict “shortest path”
routing and route selection in the implications section.
With respect to prior work, it is also interesting to examine
Table 3 for what is not common or present at all in our
corpus. It appears that some issues with everyday use of
navigation technologies do not play a role in catastrophic
incidents associated with these technologies. For instance,
positioning inaccuracies and the lack of adaptability to
intentional “detours” were the sources of major usability
challenges in the work of Brown and Laurier. However,
neither appeared in our corpus. Similarly, missing spatial
data was not a major issue in our corpus it played a role in
only 5 (4%) of incidents but has been identified as a
significant issue in standard usage scenarios. For
catastrophic incidents, the issue appears to be attributes
rather than the spatial data itself, a subject we discuss
immediately below.
IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND DESIGN
In this section, we turn our attention to our second research
goal: helping to identify solutions to the problems we found
in our results section by enumerating a series of implications
for both research and design. Some of these implications
suggest improvements to the design of existing systems,
while other present important new challenges for the
GeoHCI research community. We have organized these
implications into two high-level categories corresponding to
two broad areas of the GeoHCI research space: implications
related to spatial computing (e.g. routing algorithms, missing
attributes) and implications related user interaction issues.
Spatial Computing Implications
Geometries without Attributes Can Be Dangerous
A major finding above is that missing attributes play a
substantial role in the catastrophic incidents in our corpus.
This suggests that road network geometries may be “getting
ahead” of the corresponding attributes. That is, data
providers are adding road segments to their networks faster
than they are adding the attributes to those segments that are
necessary to facilitate safe routing.
These results suggest that data providers may not want to
integrate road segments into their networks unless those
segments have high-quality data for a core set of attributes.
Based on our findings, these attributes should include the
type of the road (e.g. dirt, asphalt) and the clearance height
of the road (as defined by any overpasses, tunnels, and other
obstacles) at minimum.
Incorporate Vehicle Type into Routing Decisions
Even when high-quality attributes are included, however,
they must be used intelligently by routing algorithms.
Returning to Table 3, a key theme emerges in this respect:
many of the incidents included in this table could have been
prevented if routing algorithms can understand the
limitations of the vehicle that they are routing. For instance,
it is often not safe for sedans to drive down rough country
roads, and trucks should not drive down roads with low
clearance heights. Coupled with good coverage of attributes,
incorporating vehicle type information would be a
straightforward and effective way to maintain good coverage
of possible routes (e.g. allowing SUVs to drive down rough
country roads), while at the same time increasing safety.
Extend Space Usage Rule Mapping Efforts to Road Networks
We identified that the lack of space usage rules (i.e. usage
regulations) is a common missing attribute associated with
the catastrophic incidents in our corpus. Space usage rules
(SURs) have been a topic of growing interest in the GeoHCI
research community in the past few years (e.g. [19,41,43]),
but this literature has focused on mapping rules associated
with regions rather than roads. For example, a common
Figure 1. A Google Street View image depicting the complex
geography of the location of the incident in article #146.
research challenge in SUR mapping is identifying regions in
which smoking is legal or illegal [41].
Our research suggests that more effort should be spent on the
identification of SURs for road networks. In particular,
improving data related to the maximum clearance of roads,
whether roads are public or private, and improved
recognition of traffic rules are particularly important.
Fortunately, unlike many SUR mapping challenges that
require multifaceted approaches (e.g. natural language
processing, crowdsourcing), it is likely that much of the work
here can be done using computer vision (CV) approaches.
The automated detection of traffic rules in this fashion is
already underway [4]. It is likely that private property signs
would present unique challenges for CV algorithms due to
their diversity, but this is a contained problem that can likely
be at least partially addressed with current state-of-the-art
CV techniques.
The Weather Matters When Routing
Our results suggest that routing algorithms should consider
weather information when generating routes, and should do
so in concert with vehicle type information. A substantial
number of the stranding incidents in our corpus would have
been avoided with relatively straightforward weather- and
vehicle-aware routing approaches. For instance, if it has
rained 20 centimeters in the past day, routing algorithms
should not send drivers of sedans down dirt roads. Similarly,
if it has snowed 20 centimeters and it has stayed below
freezing, routing algorithms should recommend that sedan
drivers stick to main thoroughfares, which are plowed more
quickly and more often (and should perhaps consider
increasingly available information in many cities about
which roads have been plowed since the last major snow).
The Downsides of Map Matching
We observed in our corpus that map matching techniques
[15] can backfire. These techniques are designed to mitigate
GPS noise by “snapping” vehicle locations to the closest
road network geometry. However, they were likely involved
in the three incidents in which a person drove on a train track
parallel to a road (article #17, #32, #116) and also a few
incidents in which people drove on the wrong side of the
divided road (e.g. article #12, #90) (all cases happened in
evening). In these cases, map matching algorithms likely
“snapped” the driver’s position to the nearest or the correct
side of the road, making the driver believe that they were on
right track (which may be difficult to assess at night).
Although more work is needed to understand this issue in
detail, one potential improvement is to make map matching
algorithms more error-sensitive in situations in which the
distance between geometries is smaller than the error
tolerance. Specifically, when an algorithm notices that there
are multiple parallel linear geometries (e.g. a divided
highway or a railroad parallel to a road), it can reduce the
tolerance of its map matching radius. When observing a
small, persistent mismatch for a short period, GPS devices
could immediately prompt users about this mismatch and ask
the driver to look at the environment to confirm that the
vehicle is on a legal road.
User Interaction Implications
Route Preference Must Be Accompanied with Adequate
Information to Make an Educated Choice
Past research on the use of navigation technology in standard
scenarios has advocated for providing greater route
preference for users. Our results suggest that this preference
must be accompanied with adequate information for users to
make safe decisions. Current navigation devices often offer
multiple routing preferences such as “fastest”, “shortest”, or
“eco mode”. At the very least, these technologies should
warn users that certain choice may involve traversing unsafe
territory, as was the case with the Canadian couple that chose
the “shortest path” through Nevada without understanding
the consequences of doing so.
As mentioned above, in addition to the usability problem of
excessive instructions with bad timing found by previous
studies, we identified a new type of guidance-related
problem: instructions that are too simple for the spatial
decisions that the user has to make. Two research challenges
emerge from this issue: (1) automatically detecting complex
geographies and (2) developing interfaces to better support
users in these contexts. With regard to the first challenge,
public crash datasets (e.g. [49]) can provide ground truth
information to help develop regression models that assess the
complexity of a routing context based on the topology of the
surrounding road network (and likely other information, such
as railroads). The second challenge might be at least partially
addressed through the use of image-based navigation, i.e. by
annotating Street View imagery with arrows and labels.
Image-based navigation is known to have benefits over most
other approaches [47] but needs to be updated frequently to
reflect any potential changes in the environment..
Local Mode and Non-Local Mode
Our results suggest that non-local drivers are at substantially
greater risk for catastrophic incidents associated with
navigation technologies than local drivers. These findings
advocate for the development of customized features for
each of these populations, i.e. a “local mode” and a “non-
local mode”. For instance, neuroscience research has shown
that more attention is required when driving in an unfamiliar
environment [30]. As such, designers should investigate
strategies for reducing interaction with drivers when drivers
are outside their home region(s). Additionally, routing
algorithms could provide non-local drivers with an “easiest”
route that prioritizes highways and avoids complex
intersections to minimize the turn-by-turn instructions and
general information load. Similarly, GPS devices could
disable non-essential functionality (e.g. searching for local
restaurants) while in unfamiliar territory and re-enable those
functions only when drivers come to a complete stop (or
return to their home areas).
DISCUSSION
In this paper, we provided the first characterization of the
patterns in catastrophic incidents associated with the use of
personal navigation technologies. We have also outlined a
series of implications for design and research that emerge
from these patterns. Below, we highlight several discussion
points associated with this research.
First, it is interesting to reflect on the design implications in
the context of automated vehicles. Some of the implications
will clearly become moot if a human is not behind the wheel
(e.g. those related to improved instructions), as will be the
case for many of the core functionalities of navigation
devices [6]. However, other implications may become
significantly more important. For instance, adding attribute
information to geometries, improving understanding of
space usage rules and incorporating weather information will
be critical to helping automated cars avoid dangerous
navigation decisions. The same would likely apply in the
nearer term with semi-automated cars, as recent work
suggests that there may be excessive deference to automated
routing approaches given the attentional challenges of partial
automation [37]. Similarly, the research community has
pointed out the need to keep drivers engaged when behind
the wheel of a mostly-automated vehicle. Prompting users in
the case of persistent map matching issues and engaging
them in other difficult navigation-related tasks may be one
way to accomplish this goal.
Second, the news article-based pipeline we use here may be
able to help HCI researchers examine other difficult-to-study
phenomena. As noted above, our public health-based
approach is best suited to phenomena that share three
properties: (1) no authoritative dataset is available, (2)
instances are too rare to observe in large numbers in the wild
and cannot be replicated in a lab setting, and (3) instances are
frequently covered by journalists. Some additional HCI
phenomena that share these properties include criminal
events in the sharing economy and safety concerns related to
location-based games like Pokémon GO [8]. To make it
easier for researchers to employ our methodology, we have
provided a step-by-step description of our approach in the
documentation that is included with our coded dataset.
It is important to note that our coded dataset contains much
more data than we could fully describe in this paper. While
we have highlighted what we as researchers in the
geographic HCI domain believe to be the most important
themes in our results, other researchers may benefit from
examining our data from a different perspective. One
particularly interesting avenue of exploration that we are
working to investigate is using the spatial locations of each
incident (available in the dataset) to try to develop predictive
models of the types of areas in which the use of navigation
technologies might be particularly risky.
While we believe it is important for the HCI community to
examine and learn from catastrophic incidents associated
with the use of computing technologies, it is also important
to put the relative incidence of these catastrophes in context.
While we identified that GPS devices and related
technologies played a role in at 158 catastrophic incidents
involving 52 deaths, these technologies have also likely
played a role in saving the lives of many people (e.g. guiding
people to emergency resources, preventing people from
getting lost). With this in mind, the design and research
suggestions we make above are careful to be augmentative
of existing navigation technology functionality rather than
substantially altering current functionality.
Limitations
In addition to the drawbacks of the news article-based
pipeline discussed above, this paper is also subject to several
additional limitations. For instance, while our incident
corpus is the first agglomeration of its type of any scale,
future work should seek to increase this size by either finding
more news stories or collecting data on incidents that are not
reported in the news. With respect to identifying unreported
incidents, crowdsourcing has been proven effective for
building databases of technology failures in the domain of
aviation [51]. This may be an approach that is feasible in this
domain as well. Similarly, a related limitation of our dataset
is that it that 97% of our articles came from either the U.S.,
the U.K., Canada, New Zealand, or Australia (due to the
focus on English articles). It is reasonable to assume that
patterns in other countries might be different, and future
work should examine these patterns.
The issue of survivor bias should also be considered. It is
likely that navigation technologies have played a role in a
significant number of deadly accidents for which there was
no witness or exogenous information to identify the role of
the technology (the 44 deadly incidents considered here had
one or both of these). Interestingly, survivor bias could
counteract the fatality bias discussed above.
CONCLUSION
In this paper, we have extended prior work on user
interaction with navigation technologies to consider
catastrophic incidents associated with these technologies.
We have characterized key patterns that exist in these
incidents and enumerated implications for research and
design that emerge from these patterns. This research
increases our understanding of how the navigation
technologies that we design cause serious harm, as well as
provides a path towards developing safer navigation
technologies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to thank our colleagues in GroupLens and our
anonymous reviewers for their feedback. This work was
funded in part by the U.S. National Science Foundation (IIS-
1421655, CAREER IIS-1552955) and by the Volkswagen
Foundation through a Lichtenberg Professorship.
REFERENCES
1. Alan Agresti. 1996. An introduction to categorical data
analysis. Wiley New York.
2. Abdullah Al Mahmud, Omar Mubin, and Suleman
Shahid. 2009. User experience with in-car GPS
navigation systems: comparing the young and elderly
drivers. 1. https://doi.org/10.1145/1613858.1613962
3. Claudio Aporta and Eric Higgs. 2005. Satellite Culture:
Global Positioning Systems, Inuit Wayfinding, and the
Need for a New Account of Technology. Current
Anthropology 46, 5: 729753.
https://doi.org/10.1086/432651
4. Vahid Balali, Armin Ashouri Rad, and Mani Golparvar-
Fard. 2015. Detection, classification, and mapping of
U.S. traffic signs using google street view images for
roadway inventory management. Visualization in
Engineering 3, 1: 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40327-
015-0027-1
5. J. Baullinger, L. Quan, E. Bennett, P. Cummings, and K.
Williams. 2001. Use of Washington State newspapers
for submersion injury surveillance. Injury Prevention 7,
4: 339342. https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.7.4.339
6. David Beattie, Lynne Baillie, Martin Halvey, and
Roderick McCall. 2015. Adapting SatNav to meet the
demands of future automated vehicles. In CHI 2015
Workshop on Experiencing Autonomous Vehicles:
Crossing the Boundaries between a Drive and a Ride.
Retrieved June 14, 2016 from
http://strathprints.strath.ac.uk/52245/
7. Barry Brown and Eric Laurier. 2012. The Normal
Natural Troubles of Driving with GPS. In Proceedings
of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in
Computing Systems (CHI ’12), 1621–1630.
https://doi.org/10.1145/2207676.2208285
8. Ashley Colley, Jacob Thebault-Spieker, Yilun Lin,
Jonna Häkkilä, Nuno Nunes, Dirk Wenig, Kate Kuehl,
Benjamin Fischman, Valentina Nisi, Nina Runge,
Donald Degraen, Brent Hecht, and Johannes Schöning.
2017. The Geography of Pokémon GO: Beneficial and
Problematic Effects on Places and Movement. In
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human
Factors in Computing Systems.
9. P. Dalton, P. Agarwal, N. Fraenkel, J. Baichoo, and A.
Masry. 2013. Driving with navigational instructions:
Investigating user behaviour and performance. Accident
Analysis & Prevention 50: 298303.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.05.002
10. Thomas A. Dingus and Melissa C. Hulse. 1993. Some
human factors design issues and recommendations for
automobile navigation information systems.
Transportation Research Part C: Emerging
Technologies 1, 2: 119131.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0968-090X(93)90009-5
11. Thomas A. Dingus, Melissa C. Hulse, Jonathan F.
Antin, and Walter W. Wierwille. 1989. Attentional
demand requirements of an automobile moving-map
navigation system. Transportation Research Part A:
General 23, 4: 301315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-
2607(89)90013-7
12. Philip R. Fine, Chester S. Jones, J. Michael Wrigley, J.
Scott Richards, and Matthew D. Rousculp. 1998. Are
newspapers a viable source for intentional injury...
Southern Medical Journal 91, 3: 234.
13. Aaron L. Gardony, Tad T. Brunyé, Caroline R.
Mahoney, and Holly A. Taylor. 2013. How Navigational
Aids Impair Spatial Memory: Evidence for Divided
Attention. Spatial Cognition & Computation 13, 4: 319
350. https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2013.792821
14. Michael F. Goodchild. 2001. A Geographer Looks at
Spatial Information Theory. In Spatial Information
Theory, Daniel R. Montello (ed.). Springer Berlin
Heidelberg, 113. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-45424-
1_1
15. Joshua S. Greenfeld. 2002. Matching GPS observations
to locations on a digital map. In Transportation
Research Board 81st Annual Meeting.
16. Hilary Hanson. 2012. GPS Leads Japanese Tourists To
Drive Into Australian Bay. The Huffington Post.
Retrieved September 12, 2016 from
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2012/03/19/gps-tourists-
australia_n_1363823.html
17. Brent Hecht, Johannes Schöning, Thomas Erickson, and
Reid Priedhorsky. 2011. Geographic Human-computer
Interaction. In CHI ’11 Extended Abstracts on Human
Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA ’11), 447–450.
https://doi.org/10.1145/1979742.1979532
18. Markus Hipp, Florian Schaub, Frank Kargl, and
Michael Weber. 2010. Interaction Weaknesses of
Personal Navigation Devices. In Proceedings of the 2Nd
International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces
and Interactive Vehicular Applications (AutomotiveUI
’10), 129–136.
https://doi.org/10.1145/1969773.1969796
19. Konstantin Hopf, Florian Dageförde, and Diedrich
Wolter. 2015. Identifying the Geographical Scope of
Prohibition Signs. In Spatial Information Theory, Sara
Irina Fabrikant, Martin Raubal, Michela Bertolotto,
Clare Davies, Scott Freundschuh and Scott Bell (eds.).
Springer International Publishing, 247267.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-23374-1_12
20. White House. 2016. Big Data: A Report on Algorithmic
Systems, Opportunity, and Civil Rights. Washington,
DC: Executive Office of the President, White House.
21. Brit Susan Jensen, Mikael B. Skov, and Nissanthen
Thiruravichandran. 2010. Studying Driver Attention and
Behaviour for Three Configurations of GPS Navigation
in Real Traffic Driving. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems
(CHI ’10), 1271–1280.
https://doi.org/10.1145/1753326.1753517
22. John D. Kraemer and Connor S. Benton. 2015.
Disparities in road crash mortality among pedestrians
using wheelchairs in the USA: results of a capture
recapture analysis. BMJ Open 5, 11: e008396.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008396
23. Andrew L. Kun, Tim Paek, Željko Medenica, Nemanja
Memarović, and Oskar Palinko. 2009. Glancing at
Personal Navigation Devices Can Affect Driving:
Experimental Results and Design Implications. In
Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on
Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular
Applications (AutomotiveUI ’09), 129–136.
https://doi.org/10.1145/1620509.1620534
24. Myron M. LaBan and Thomas S. Nabity. 2010. Traffic
Collisions Between Electric Mobility Devices
(Wheelchairs) and Motor Vehicles: Accidents, Hubris,
or Self-destructive Behavior? American Journal of
Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation 89, 7: 557560.
https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0b013e3181d8a346
25. Stephen Lacy, Brendan R. Watson, Daniel Riffe, and
Jennette Lovejoy. 2015. Issues and Best Practices in
Content Analysis. Journalism & Mass Communication
Quarterly: 1077699015607338.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699015607338
26. J. Richard Landis and Gary G. Koch. 1977. The
Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical
Data. Biometrics 33, 1: 159174.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310
27. Talia Lavie, Tal Oron-Gilad, and Joachim Meyer. 2011.
Aesthetics and usability of in-vehicle navigation
displays. International Journal of Human-Computer
Studies 69, 12: 8099.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2010.10.002
28. Gilly Leshed, Theresa Velden, Oya Rieger, Blazej Kot,
and Phoebe Sengers. 2008. In-car Gps Navigation:
Engagement with and Disengagement from the
Environment. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference
on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’08),
16751684. https://doi.org/10.1145/1357054.1357316
29. M. C. A. Liem and F. Koenraadt. 2007. Homicide-
suicide in the Netherlands: A study of newspaper
reports, 1992 - 2005. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry &
Psychology 18, 4: 482493.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14789940701491370
30. Mandy Mader, André Bresges, Reyhan Topal,
Alexander Busse, Michael Forsting, and Elke R.
Gizewski. 2009. Simulated car driving in fMRI
Cerebral activation patterns driving an unfamiliar and a
familiar route. Neuroscience Letters 464, 3: 222227.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.08.056
31. Julie E. Malphurs and Donna Cohen. 2002. A
newspaper surveillance study of homicide-suicide in the
United States. The American Journal of Forensic
Medicine and Pathology 23, 2: 142148.
32. Mary L. McHugh. 2013. The Chi-square test of
independence. Biochemia Medica 23, 2: 143149.
https://doi.org/10.11613/BM.2013.018
33. Zeljko Medenica, Andrew L. Kun, Tim Paek, and Oskar
Palinko. 2011. Augmented Reality vs. Street Views: A
Driving Simulator Study Comparing Two Emerging
Navigation Aids. In Proceedings of the 13th
International Conference on Human Computer
Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services
(MobileHCI ’11), 265–274.
https://doi.org/10.1145/2037373.2037414
34. Greg Milner. 2016. Pinpoint: How GPS Is Changing
Technology, Culture, and Our Minds. W. W. Norton &
Company.
35. Jakob Nielsen. 2001. Error message guidelines. Nielsen
Norman Group: 0624.
36. Donald A. Norman. 2013. The Design of Everyday
Things: Revised and Expanded Edition. Basic Books.
37. Stephen M. Casner Norman Edwin L. Hutchins, Don.
The Challenges of Partially Automated Driving.
Retrieved December 21, 2016 from
http://cacm.acm.org/magazines/2016/5/201592-the-
challenges-of-partially-automated-driving/fulltext
38. David Y. Rainey and Carol W. Runyan. 1992.
Newspapers: a source for injury surveillance? American
journal of public health 82, 5: 745746.
39. Robert M. Regoli and John D. Hewitt. 2008. Exploring
Criminal Justice. Jones & Bartlett Learning.
40. Paolo Roma, Antonella Spacca, Maurizio Pompili,
David Lester, Roberto Tatarelli, Paolo Girardi, and
Stefano Ferracuti. 2012. The epidemiology of homicide
suicide in Italy: A newspaper study from 1985 to 2008.
Forensic Science International 214, 13: e1e5.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.forsciint.2011.06.022
41. Pavel Andreevich Samsonov, Xun Tang, Johannes
Schöning, Werner Kuhn, and Brent Hecht. 2015. You
Can’T Smoke Here: Towards Support for Space Usage
Rules in Location-aware Technologies. In Proceedings
of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors
in Computing Systems (CHI ’15), 971–974.
https://doi.org/10.1145/2702123.2702269
42. Julia Schreiber. 2009. Bridging the gap between useful
and aesthetic maps in car navigation systems. In
Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on
Human-Computer Interaction with Mobile Devices and
Services, 51. Retrieved September 19, 2016 from
http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1613922
43. Marcus Soll, Philipp Naumann, Johannes Schöning,
Pavel Samsonov, and Brent Hecht. 2016. Helping
Computers Understand Geographically-Bound Activity
Restrictions. 24422446.
https://doi.org/10.1145/2858036.2858053
44. Jo Ellen Stryker, Ricardo J. Wray, Robert C. Hornik,
and Itzik Yanovitzky. 2006. Validation of Database
Search Terms for Content Analysis: The Case of Cancer
News Coverage. Journalism & Mass Communication
Quarterly 83, 2: 413430.
https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900608300212
45. By Bonnie Malkin in Sydney. 2010. Britons stranded in
Australian Outback for four days after satnav error.
Retrieved September 12, 2016 from
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/worldnews/australiaan
dthepacific/australia/7925649/Britons-stranded-in-
Australian-Outback-for-four-days-after-satnav-
error.html
46. Bruno Waterfield. 2013. GPS failure leaves Belgian
woman in Zagreb two days later. Retrieved September
12, 2016 from
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/worldnews/europe/bel
gium/9798779/GPS-failure-leaves-Belgian-woman-in-
Zagreb-two-days-later.html
47. Dirk Wenig, Alexander Steenbergen, Johannes
Schöning, Brent Hecht, and Rainer Malaka. 2016.
ScrollingHome: Bringing Image-based Indoor
Navigation to Smartwatches. In Proceedings of the 18th
International Conference on Human-Computer
Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services
(MobileHCI ’16), 400–406.
https://doi.org/10.1145/2935334.2935373
48. Walter W. Wierwille, Jonathan F. Antin, Thomas A.
Dingus, and Melissa C. Hulse. 1988. Visual Attentional
Demand of An In-car Navigation Display System.
Retrieved September 13, 2016 from
https://trid.trb.org/view.aspx?id=927088
49. Traffic Safety Facts 2014: A Compilation of Motor
Vehicle Crash Data from the Fatality Analysis
Reporting System and the General Estimates System.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration.
Retrieved from
https://crashstats.nhtsa.dot.gov/Api/Public/ViewPublicat
ion/812261
50. Definition of Catastrophic. Retrieved from
https://www.google.com/search?q=catastrophic+inciden
ts&ie=utf-8&oe=utf-8#q=catastrophic
51. Aviation Safety Reporting System. NASA. Retrieved
from
http://akama.arc.nasa.gov/ASRSDBOnline/QueryWizar
d Filter.aspx.
... However, where does the user responsibility lay in the context of achieving responsible AI? As users familiarize themselves with these novel LLM services it is expected that the novelty of anecdotal misuses [81] will fade, as so did the novelty of first-time GPS users who made headlines by following GPS instructions blindly and crashing their cars [82]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Large Language Models (LLMs) are predictive probabilistic models capable of passing several professional tests at a level comparable to humans. However, these capabilities come with ethical concerns. Ethical oversights in several LLM-based products include: (i) a lack of content or source attribution, and (ii) a lack of transparency in what was used to train the model. This paper identifies four touchpoints where ethical safeguards can be applied to realize a more responsible AI in LLMs. The key finding is that applying safeguards before the training occurs aligns with established engineering practices of addressing issues at the source. However, this approach is currently shunned. Finally, historical parallels are drawn with the US automobile industry, which initially resisted safety regulations but later embraced them once consumer attitudes evolved.
... The consequences of navigation errors can be severe for various user groups and use contexts. For example, navigation errors with GPS devices have led to several catastrophic incidents, colloquially referred to as "death by GPS" (Lin et al., 2017). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Even though they are day-to-day activities, humans find navigation and wayfinding to be cognitively challenging. To facilitate their everyday mobility, humans increasingly rely on ubiquitous mobile maps as navigation aids. However, the over-reliance on and habitual use of omnipresent navigation aids deteriorate humans' short-term ability to learn new information about their surroundings and induces a long-term decline in spatial skills. This deterioration in spatial learning is attributed to the fact that these aids capture users' attention and cause them to enter a passive navigation mode. Another factor that limits spatial learning during map-aided navigation is the lack of salient landmark information on mobile maps. Prior research has already demonstrated that wayfinders rely on landmarks—geographic features that stand out from their surroundings—to facilitate navigation and build a spatial representation of the environments they traverse. Landmarks serve as anchor points and help wayfinders to visually match the spatial information depicted on the mobile map with the information collected during the active exploration of the environment. Considering the acknowledged significance of landmarks for human wayfinding due to their visibility and saliency, this thesis investigates an open research question: how to graphically communicate landmarks on mobile map aids to cue wayfinders' allocation of attentional resources to these task-relevant environmental features. From a cartographic design perspective, landmarks can be depicted on mobile map aids on a graphical continuum ranging from abstract 2D text labels to realistic 3D buildings with high visual fidelity. Based on the importance of landmarks for human wayfinding and the rich cartographic body of research concerning their depiction on mobile maps, this thesis investigated how various landmark visualization styles affect the navigation process of two user groups (expert and general wayfinders) in different navigation use contexts (emergency and general navigation tasks). Specifically, I conducted two real-world map-aided navigation studies to assess the influence of various landmark visualization styles on wayfinders' navigation performance, spatial learning, allocation of visual attention, and cognitive load. In Study I, I investigated how depicting landmarks as abstract 2D building footprints or realistic 3D buildings on the mobile map affected expert wayfinders' navigation performance, visual attention, spatial learning, and cognitive load during an emergency navigation task. I asked expert navigators recruited from the Swiss Armed Forces to follow a predefined route using a mobile map depicting landmarks as either abstract 2D building footprints or realistic 3D buildings and to identify the depicted task-relevant landmarks in the environment. I recorded the experts' gaze behavior with a mobile eye-tracer and their cognitive load with EEG during the navigation task, and I captured their incidental spatial learning at the end of the task. The wayfinding experts' exhibited high navigation performance and low cognitive load during the map-aided navigation task regardless of the landmark visualization style. Their gaze behavior revealed that wayfinding experts navigating with realistic 3D landmarks focused more on the visualizations of landmarks on the mobile map than those who navigated with abstract 2D landmarks, while the latter focused more on the depicted route. Furthermore, when the experts focused for longer on the environment and the landmarks, their spatial learning improved regardless of the landmark visualization style. I also found that the spatial learning of experts with self-reported low spatial abilities improved when they navigated with landmarks depicted as realistic 3D buildings. In Study II, I investigated the influence of abstract and realistic 3D landmark visualization styles on wayfinders sampled from the general population. As in Study I, I investigated wayfinders' navigation performance, visual attention, spatial learning, and cognitive load. In contrast to Study I, the participants in Study II were exposed to both landmark visualization styles in a navigation context that mimics everyday navigation. Furthermore, the participants were informed that their spatial knowledge of the environment would be tested after navigation. As in Study I, the wayfinders in Study II exhibited high navigation performance and low cognitive load regardless of the landmark visualization style. Their visual attention revealed that wayfinders with low spatial abilities and wayfinders familiar with the study area fixated on the environment longer when they navigated with realistic 3D landmarks on the mobile map. Spatial learning improved when wayfinders with low spatial abilities were assisted by realistic 3D landmarks. Also, when wayfinders were assisted by realistic 3D landmarks and paid less attention to the map aid, their spatial learning improved. Taken together, the present real-world navigation studies provide ecologically valid results on the influence of various landmark visualization styles on wayfinders. In particular, the studies demonstrate how visualization style modulates wayfinders' visual attention and facilitates spatial learning across various user groups and navigation use contexts. Furthermore, the results of both studies highlight the importance of individual differences in spatial abilities as predictors of spatial learning during map-assisted navigation. Based on these findings, the present work provides design recommendations for future mobile maps that go beyond the traditional concept of "one fits all." Indeed, the studies support the cause for landmark depiction that directs individual wayfinders' visual attention to task-relevant landmarks to further enhance spatial learning. This would be especially helpful for users with low spatial skills. In doing so, future mobile maps could dynamically adapt the visualization style of landmarks according to wayfinders' spatial abilities for cued visual attention, thus meeting individuals' spatial learning needs.
... Driver Assistance Technology: Issues with other assistance technologies, such as navigation, crash avoidance, and automation level also played a role in accidents between VRUs and AVs. For example, Lin et al. (2017) identified the key reasons for accidents caused by navigation systems, such as missing road characteristics data, weather conditions, poor audio and visual instructions from 158 news reports. Good et al. (2017) analyzed how well a crash avoidance system performed in bicycle crash scenarios. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Automated vehicles (AVs) are of great potential in reducing crashes on the road. However, it is still complicated to eliminate all the possible accidents, especially those with vulnerable road users (VRUs), who are among the greater risk than vehicle occupants in traffic accidents. Thus, in this paper, we conducted a systematic review of safety-critical scenarios between AVs and VRUs. We identified 39 papers in the literature and typical safety-critical scenarios between AVs and VRUs. They were further divided into three categories, including human factors, environmental factors, and vehicle factors. We then discussed the development, challenges, and possible solutions for each category. In order to further improve the safety of VRUs when interacting with AVs, multiple stakeholders should work together to 1) improve AI and sensor technologies and vehicle automation, 2) redesign the current transportation infrastructure, 3) design effective communication technologies and interfaces between vehicles and between vehicles and VRUs, and 4) design effective simulation and testing methods to support and evaluate both infrastructure and technologies.
Article
Full-text available
The increased usage of navigation technologies has caused conflicts in local traffic management, resulting in congested residential areas among other challenges for residents. This paper uses content analysis to investigate such negative social externalities within local communities and neighbourhoods. Through a corpus of 90 news articles about traffic incidents caused by navigation technologies, we identified negative traffic and safety-related externalities, including congestion, damage, pollution, and accidents. We also report on countermeasures by local communities and governments, including street closures, speed limit reduction, and turn bans. Based on our results, we discuss the implications for designing mobile navigation technologies that reduce negative social externalities.
Article
Social media platforms use permanent suspension as a measure of last resort to intervene with users who spread harmful or misleading content. However, permanent suspension does not signify the end of a user's online presence, but rather on that specific platform. This issue is particularly salient for influential users with large audiences, as they have the potential to cause substantial shifts in the overall social media information landscape when suspended. Our work employs a mixed methods approach to study the context around, and behavioral patterns after, permanent suspension. We find that migration is a common step after suspension, and characterize a number of behavioral strategies and patterns that occur after influential users are suspended. By focusing on consequences of suspension across more holistically, we have identified numerous opportunities for design and future research to mitigate the potential negative effects of permanent suspensions on the broader social media information landscape.
Article
Spatial navigation is more difficult and effortful for older than younger individuals, a shift which occurs for a variety of neurological, physical, and cognitive reasons associated with aging. Despite a large body of evidence documenting age‐related deficits in spatial navigation, comparatively less research addresses how to facilitate more effective navigation behavior for older adults. Since navigation challenges arise for a variety of reasons in old age, a one‐size‐fits‐all solution is unlikely to work. Here, we introduce a framework for the variety of spatial navigation challenges faced in aging, which we call LOST — Location, Orientation, Spatial mapping, and Transit . The LOST framework builds on evidence from the cognitive neuroscience of spatial navigation, which reveals distinct components underpinning human wayfinding. We evaluate research on navigational aids—devices and depictions—which help people find their way around; and we reflect on how navigation aids solve (or fail to solve) specific wayfinding difficulties faced by older adults. In summary, we emphasize a bespoke approach to improving spatial navigation in aging, which focuses on tailoring navigation solutions to specific navigation challenges. Our hope is that by providing precise support to older navigators, navigation opportunities can facilitate independence and exploration, while minimizing the danger of becoming lost. We conclude by delineating critical knowledge gaps in how to improve older adults' spatial navigation capacities that the novel LOST framework could guide to address. This article is categorized under: Psychology > Development and Aging Neuroscience > Cognition Neuroscience > Behavior
Article
Automated vehicles (AVs) are of great potential to reduce crashes on the road. However, it is still challenging to eliminate all the possible crashes, especially those with vulnerable road users (VRUs). Thus, in this paper, we conducted a systematic review of challenging scenarios involving AVs and VRUs. We identified 39 relevant papers in the literature and categorized the challenging scenarios into three categories: human factors, environmental factors, and vehicle factors. For each category, we summarized the development, challenges, and potential solutions from the literature. Our findings aim to enhance the safety of VRUs when interacting with AVs, and multiple stakeholders must collaborate to 1) improve AI and sensor technologies and vehicle automation, 2) redesign the current transportation infrastructure, 3) design effective communication technologies and interfaces between vehicles, and between vehicles and VRUs, and 4) design effective simulation and testing methods to support and evaluate both infrastructure and technologies.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The widespread popularity of Pokémon GO presents the first opportunity to observe the geographic effects of location-based gaming at scale. This paper reports the results of a mixed methods study of the geography of Pokémon GO that includes a five-country field survey of 375 Pokémon GO players and a large scale geostatistical analysis of game elements. Focusing on the key geographic themes of places and movement, we find that the design of Pokémon GO reinforces existing geographically-linked biases (e.g. the game advantages urban areas and neighborhoods with smaller minority populations), that Pokémon GO may have instigated a relatively rare large-scale shift in global human mobility patterns, and that Pokémon GO has geographically-linked safety risks, but not those typically emphasized by the media. Our results point to geographic design implications for future systems in this space such as a means through which the geographic biases present in Pokémon GO may be counteracted.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Providing pedestrian navigation instructions on small screens is a challenging task due to limited screen space. As image-based approaches for navigation have been successfully proven to outperform map-based navigation on mobile devices, we propose to bring image-based navigation to smartwatches. We contribute a straightforward pipeline to easily create image-based indoor navigation instructions that allow users to freely navigate in indoor environments without any localization infrastructure and with minimal user input on the smartwatch. In a user study, we show that our approach outperforms the current state-of-the art application in terms of task completion time, perceived task load and perceived usability. In addition, we did not find an indication that there is a need to provide explicit directional instructions for image-based navigation on small screens.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The lack of certain types of geographic data prevents the development of location-aware technologies in a number of important domains. One such type of “unmapped” geographic data is space usage rules (SURs), which are defined as geographically-bound activity restrictions (e.g. “no dogs”, “no smoking”, “no fishing”, “no skateboarding”). Researchers in the area of human- computer interaction have recently begun to develop techniques for the automated mapping of SURs with the aim of supporting activity planning systems (e.g. one-touch “Can I Smoke Here?” apps, SUR-aware vacation planning tools). In this paper, we present a novel SUR mapping technique – SPtP – that outperforms state-of-the-art approaches by 30% for one of the most important components of the SUR mapping pipeline: associating a point observation of a SUR (e.g. a ’no smoking’ sign) with the corresponding polygon in which the SUR applies (e.g. the nearby park or the entire campus on which the sign is located). This paper also contributes a series of new SUR benchmark datasets to help further research in this area.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Recent work has identified the lack of space usage rule (SUR) data -- e.g. "no smoking", "no campfires" -- as an important limitation of online/mobile maps that presents risks to user safety and the environment. In order to address this limitation, a large-scale means of mapping SURs must be developed. In this paper, we introduce and motivate the problem of mapping space usage rules and take the first steps towards identifying solutions. We show how computer vision can be employed to identify SUR indicators in the environment (e.g. "No Smoking" signs) with reasonable accuracy and describe techniques that can assign each rule to the appropriate geographic feature.
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study aims to quantify and describe the burden of fatal pedestrian crashes among persons using wheelchairs in the USA from 2006 to 2012. Design The occurrence of fatal pedestrian crashes among pedestrians using wheelchairs was assessed using two-source capture-recapture. Descriptive analysis of fatal crashes was conducted using customary approaches. Setting Two registries were constructed, both of which likely undercounted fatalities among pedestrians who use wheelchairs. The first used data from the Fatality Analysis Reporting System, and the second used a LexisNexis news search. Outcome measures Mortality rate (per 100 000 person-years) and crash-level, driver-level and pedestrian-level characteristics of fatal crashes. Results This study found that, from 2006 to 2012, the mortality rate for pedestrians using wheelchairs was 2.07/100 000 person-years (95% CI 1.60 to 2.54), which was 36% higher than the overall population pedestrian mortality rate (p=0.02). Men's risk was over fivefold higher than women's risk (p<0.001). Compared to the overall population, persons aged 50–64 using wheelchairs had a 38% increased risk (p=0.04), and men who use wheelchairs aged 50–64 had a 75% increased risk over men of the same age in the overall population (p=0.006). Almost half (47.6%; 95% CI 42.8 to 52.5) of fatal crashes occurred in intersections and 38.7% (95% CI 32.0 to 45.0) of intersection crashes occurred at locations without traffic control devices. Among intersection crashes, 47.5% (95% CI 40.6 to 54.5) involved wheelchair users in a crosswalk; no crosswalk was available for 18.3% (95% CI 13.5 to 24.4). Driver failure to yield right-of-way was noted in 21.4% (95% CI 17.7 to 25.7) of crashes, and no crash avoidance manoeuvers were detected in 76.4% (95% CI 71.0 to 81.2). Conclusions Persons who use wheelchairs experience substantial pedestrian mortality disparities calling for behavioural and built environment interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Background Maintaining an up-to-date record of the number, type, location, and condition of high-quantity low-cost roadway assets such as traffic signs is critical to transportation inventory management systems. While, databases such as Google Street View contain street-level images of all traffic signs and are updated regularly, their potential for creating an inventory databases has not been fully explored. The key benefit of such databases is that once traffic signs are detected, their geographic coordinates can also be derived and visualized within the same platform. Methods By leveraging Google Street View images, this paper presents a new system for creating inventories of traffic signs. Using computer vision method, traffic signs are detected and classified into four categories of regulatory, warning, stop, and yield signs by processing images extracted from Google Street View API. Considering the discriminative classification scores from all images that see a sign, the most probable location of each traffic sign is derived and shown on the Google Maps using a dynamic heat map. A data card containing information about location and type of each detected traffic sign is also created. Finally, several data mining interfaces are introduced that allow for better management of the traffic sign inventories. Results The experiments conducted on 6.2 miles of I-57 and I-74 interstate highways in the U.S. –with an average accuracy of 94.63 % for sign classification– show the potential of the method to provide quick, inexpensive, and automatic access to asset inventory information. Conclusions Given the reliability in performance shown through experiments and because collecting information from Google Street View imagery is cost-effective, the proposed method has potential to deliver inventory information on traffic signs in a timely fashion and tie into the existing DOT inventory management systems. Such spatio-temporal representations provide DOTs with information on how different types of traffic signs degrade over time and further provides useful condition information necessary for predicting sign replacement plan.
Article
Full-text available
This article discusses three issues concerning content analysis method and ends with a list of best practices in conducting and reporting content analysis projects. Issues addressed include the use of search and databases for sampling, the differences between content analysis and algorithmic text analysis, and which reliability coefficients should be calculated and reported. The “Best Practices” section provides steps to produce reliable and valid content analysis data and the appropriate reporting of those steps so the project can be properly evaluated and replicated.
Article
AUTONOMOUS CARS PROMISE to give us back the time we spend in traffic, improve the flow of traffic, reduce accidents, deaths, and injuries, and make personal car travel possible for everyone regardless of their abilities or condition. But despite impressive demonstrations and technical advances, many obstacles remain on the road to fully autonomous cars.20 Overcoming the challenges to enabling autonomous cars to safely operate in highly complex driving situations may take some time. Manufacturers already produce partially automated cars, and a spirited competition to deliver the most sophisticated ones is under way. Cars that provide high levels of automation in some circumstances (such as highway driving) have already arrived in the marketplace and promise to be in the hands of a large number of car owners in the next few years.
Conference Paper
This paper questions whether satellite navigation systems (SatNav) in their current form will be useful in future automated vehicles. We highlight the current challenges to the design of SatNav systems and point out how aspects of these may no longer be essential during the transition towards more highly automated vehicles. Finally we pose a number of research questions that may be helpful to understand the evolution of SatNav systems in automated vehicles.