Content uploaded by Francisco Alonso
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Francisco Alonso on Sep 26, 2018
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Sociology and Anthropology, 2017, Vol. 1, No. 1, 1-7
Available online at http://pubs.sciepub.com/jsa/1/1/1
©Science and Education Publishing
DOI:10.12691/jsa-1-1-1
Shouting and Cursing while Driving: Frequency,
Reasons, Perceived Risk and Punishment
Francisco Alonso1,*, Cristina Esteban1, Andrea Serge1, Mª Luisa Ballestar2
1DATS (Development and Advising in Traffic Safety) Research Group, INTRAS (University Research Institute on Traffic and Road
Safety), University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
2METRAS (Measurement, Evaluation, Analysis, and Data Processing of Traffic Accidents and Road Safety) Research Group, INTRAS
(University Research Institute on Traffic and Road Safety), University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
*Corresponding author: datspublications@gmail.com
Abstract Traffic accidents are a major cause of death and injury in the world. Generally speaking about
aggression, evidence has shown that drivers who usually express aggressive behaviors more frequently tend, at the
same time, to have higher rates of road crashes or traffic incidents. Furthermore, in most cases, the situations in
which aggressive behaviors appear are typical of normal current traffic conditions, turning this behavior into
something very common, and into a very serious road safety issue. This has also been related with the clear lack of
Road Safety Education that is evident in most of the countries. The aim of this study was to describe the factors and
perceptions related to the aggressive behavior of verbally insulting and shouting while driving. In this study, an
extensive list of behaviors, that experts more or less unanimously consider as aggressive driving, was described; one
of them was labeled shouting and insulting. The sample was obtained from a random sampling proportional to and
representative of the segments of the population by age, sex, region and size of the municipality. The survey was
aimed at Spanish drivers over 14 years. The starting sample size was 1,100 surveys. As a result, shouting and
insulting is not considered such a dangerous offense as it is driving under the influence of alcohol, but we cannot
deny that there are many types of bad or risky maneuvers that could be banned from a legal point of view. The
degree of social tolerance towards such behavior is variable. Some individuals merely ignore them, accepting them
as something inevitable. Multiple types of risky maneuvers and deliberated misbehaviors, which are (formally and
informally) already forbidden from a legal point of view, make other drivers and pedestrians uncomfortable and
restrict their movements, creating violent, stressful and risky situations, and they are still performed by drivers. In
short, aggression in driving is one of them. As a conclusion, there is a high prevalence of this phenomenon among
Spanish drivers. Furthermore, most of the aggressive expressions related to shouting and cursing on the road are
preceded by subjective factors such as stress, fatigue and personality traits, which may be intervened through the
strengthening of road safety education and road safety campaigns.
Keywords: smoking, driving, road safety, infraction, normative, driving misbehaviors
Cite This Article: Francisco Alonso, Cristina Esteban, Andrea Serge, and Mª Luisa Ballestar, “Shouting and
Cursing while Driving: Frequency, Reasons, Perceived Risk and Punishment.” Journal of Sociology and
Anthropology, vol. 1, no. 1 (2017): 1-7. doi: 10.12691/jsa-1-1-1.
1. Introduction
Traffic accidents represent a major cause of death and
injury in the world. According to the World Health
Organization, 1.23 million people worldwide die each
year because of a traffic accident [1,2]. Generally
speaking about aggression, we could define this concept
as any behavior through which people try to harm or
inflict injury (physical, moral, psychological or social) to
another person or other people. In this regard, "aggressive
driving" is defined as the behavior of an angry or
impatient driver who intentionally endangers the life of
another driver, passenger or pedestrian, in response to an
argument, dispute or grievance in traffic. In most cases,
the situation in which aggressive behaviors appear are
typical of normal current traffic conditions, making this
behavior into something very common and a very serious
road safety issue [3,4,5]. It has been also related with the
clear lack of Road Safety Education that is evident in most
of the countries [6,7].
The causes of aggressive driving are very complex and
this may be due to multiple factors [8,9], on the other hand,
aggressive reactions are always preceded by an emotional
state, which may originate in people’s own personal
circumstances [10,11], or be triggered by the external
environment, or by the behaviors observed in other drivers
or pedestrians. A nervous or angry driver may be more
susceptible, have lower tolerance for frustration or be less
tolerant of the behavior of others [5,8]. Moreover, and as
any emotional reaction, aggressiveness is also modulated
by the subjective interpretation of the situation. For the
sake of simplicity, when explaining the origin of
aggressive behavior in general, and particularly those
manifested in driving, scientific studies have distinguished
2 Journal of Sociology and Anthropology
between internal causes specific to each individual and
their personal circumstances linked to internal and
external causes, from the subjective context as well as
from social circumstances [8,9,12].
Among the external causes there are several environmental
factors that in certain circumstances can contribute
to an aggressive reaction, or increase its probability of
occurrence [12]. Annoying physical conditions that
directly affect the comfort, and can negatively affect mood,
encouraging the emergence of aggressive behavior.
Among these factors it could be worth mentioning
elements such as noise and heat. In relation to the
temperature of the environment, for example, it has been
shown that the incidence of violent crime increases during
the summer months, so if the atmosphere is hot and humid,
the driver is more likely to get frustrated or angry, and
adopt aggressive behaviors. Some studies also show that
noise pollution can lead to aggressive reactions, especially
if the subject has no control over the size or duration
of the noise [13]. Another external condition that is
most often associated with aggressive driving is traffic
congestion, so characteristic of big cities. To these factors
we could add many others that have to do with one's own
driving situation and the stress generated by the urgency,
the frustration, or the actions of other drivers [14,15].
A number of aggressive behaviors of drivers, especially
young people, are rooted in the observation of violent
models in film and television. Just think of any of the
chase scenes or street racing, where cars are literally
shattered, or used violently, or employed as a hallmark of
an aggressive character. In real life, the everyday example
offered by many users of public roads is also influent,
especially when you consider that many of these
aggressive behaviors are never sanctioned [16,17].
If we find that a person shouts or insults, we may be
inclined to imitate such behavior in order to reach our
destination more quickly. In this sense, we might consider
aggressive driving as a form of self-behavior of our
culture, ingrained since childhood, first learned as a
passenger, observing older people, and later put into
practice and reinforced by the media. It must not be
forgotten that in our society there is a widespread
tendency to represent the vehicle as a private territory on
the road, a kind of home on wheels moving with oneself
and whose integrity must be maintained at all costs. In this
sense, it seems justifiable to point out that the aggressive
impulse may represent innate feelings of territorial rights,
serving as a basis for many dangerous and inconsiderate
behavior on the roads [18].
Although there is no single profile of the aggressive
driver, we know from the statistics that most aggressive
drivers are relatively young men, poorly educated, with
criminal records, histories of violence and problems with
alcohol and drugs. Most aggressive behaviors often occur
in drivers who are 18 to 26 years old, but we can also find
a good percentage of cases between 26 and 50 years, and
later in smaller proportion between 50 and 75 years [19].
Many of these individuals have recently had a strong
emotional setback, such as job loss, loss of a loved one, a
divorce or breakup, or have suffered an injury or accident.
Finally, numerous studies have found links between
aggression and the difficulties to contain the anger and
hostility toward others, and the tendency to take risks at
the wheel, committing offenses and getting involved in
traffic accidents. The more aggressiveness and hostility,
the higher the number of offenses and traffic accidentes,
the more the risk of subsequent recurrence increases
[21,22].
1.1. Study Framework
Law, and all its related aspects, plays an essential role
that comes from legal science. Moreover, law applies to
individuals and societies, so it has a lot to do with
sociology and psychology. Individuals and societies may
or may not know the laws, they may or may not accept
them, they may or may not share their principles, and they
may or may not obey them. In order for laws to be applied
and obeyed, different sciences must be involved when
developing them. In addition, the law is not the only thing
to take into account; rules make no sense unless there
are consequences when they are not obeyed. From
this approach, traffic laws have to be treated from a
comprehensive perspective.
Moreover, it is important to understand legislation and
everything it involves, and to regulate the drivers’
behavior since reckless behavior not only affects the
driver him/herself but but also other people (drivers and
pedestrians on the road). Therefore, it means preserving
one’s life and the life of others. So, this is why the
framework of this article was a largescale project based on
“traffic laws and road safety” with the purpose of raising
people’s awareness regarding this matter [23,24]. This
global research on traffic laws and road safety used a
questionnaire made up of a set of items in different sections.
An important aspect of the questionnaire is the order of the
questions. The objective of these items was not to influence
the answers in a particular direction. First of all, the
questionnaire was used to collect sociodemographic data
(such as age, gender, occupation, etc.).
In addition, other descriptive factors relevant to road
safety were also taken into account in order to classify
drivers: main motive of the journey, driving frequency,
professional drivers, driving experience, kilometers per
year, type of journey, most frequently used type of road,
and record of accidents and penalties.
There were also subsections used to collect information
related to the following areas: unsafe/risky behaviors
(speeding, inappropriate speed in specific situations, unsafe
following distance, shouting or verbally insulting while
driving, driving under the influence of alcohol, driving
without a seat belt, smoking while driving, driving without
insurance, driving without the required vehicle inspection).
It was also interesting to learn about the beliefs,
knowledge, and attitudes of participants towards the areas
of “legislation”, “penalties”, “law enforcement”, “law and
traffic laws”, and the “effectiveness of the measures to
prevent traffic crashes”. In this section of the
questionnaire, participants were asked to provide
information about aggressive behavior while driving:
reasons and frequency, risk of expressing driving anger,
severity of the penalty, estimated probability of penalty,
type of penalties, and penalties received (evaluation and
effectiveness).
Journal of Sociology and Anthropology 3
1.2. Objectives
The aim of this study was to describe the factors and
perceptions related to the aggressive behavior of verbally
insulting and shouting while driving. So, this study aimed
at obtaining information about the views Spanish people
have on this conduct
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
The sample was obtained from a simple random sample
(SRS) based on gender, age, habitat and the region. The
criteria for the distribution of the sample are: The election
of households in samples proportional to the universe by
Autonomous Community and habitat. For the election of
individuals: proportional to the population studied by age
group and sex.
The survey is aimed at drivers with driving license. The
proportion of subjects is a reflection of the census; it
includes drivers from 14 years to over 65 years. In terms
of age (as shown in Table 1), it can be clearly seen how
the percentage distribution is proportional to the general
census of drivers. So, the age group most represented is
the group between 30 and 44 years old (38.01%), and
people between 14 and 17 years are the least represented.
The sample size was 1,100 surveys and it consisted of
678 men (61.60%) and 422 women (38.40%), representing
a margin of error for the general information of ± 3 with a
confidence interval of 95% in the most unfavorable case
of p=q=50%, and a level of significance of 0.05. The
gender distribution is closely related to age, the older the
age, the more the proportion of women decreases. From
age 45, the percentage of women is reduced, as in the
driving population.
Table 1. Distribution of the number of drivers and sample based on
age
Age Census Distribution Sample
14-17 248.62 1,21 13
18-24 1.987.05 9,67 106
25-29 2.635.76 12,83 141
30-44 7.809.78 38,01 418
45-65 6.158.15 29,97 331
> 65 1.706.37 8,31 91
Total 20.545.73 100 1.100
2.2. Procedure and Design
This observational cross-sectional study, consisted in
the administration of a questionnaire, in which people
were questioned about their views on the behavior of
verbally insulting and shouting while driving.
The questionnaire includes the knowledge, attitudes and
behaviors of users regarding traffic and road safety. Its
comments refer to both assessment of current traffic rules
as assessment of the behavior on the road scenario. The
survey consists of a series of questions structured around a
few different sections which address the objectives
pursued in the investigation. The questionnaire was
applied through a semi-structured telephone interview
with a maximum duration of 20 minutes by staff of
EMER-GfK The staff responsible for conducting the
survey countries followed the instruction of the research
team. The average duration of the interview was 20 minutes,
with some variability due to individual differences
themselves.
To achieve the proposed aims, the following variables
were taken into account:
Demographic variables: sociodemographic factors, as
age and education level.
Driving behavior: Subsequently, the drivers were asked
about their opinions on the following behaviors: “not
maintaining a safe distance”, “driving after drinking”,
“driving without insurance” and “driving without seat belt
in the rear seats and in the city”," shouting or verbally
insulting while driving".
Information on driving behavior: the information was
obtained from these variables: behavior frequency,
performance reasons, reasons why it is not done,
perception of the accident risk, and type of road.
With these variables and the previously described
demographic information, five questions were designed:
first of all, the frequency of “verbally shouting and
insulting” was evaluated. The response format ranged
from "never" to "almost always", in a Likert format.
The second question evaluated the reason by which the
behavior “Shouting and verbally insulting” was performed;
the response format was open, since the subject had to
provide reasons.
The third question assessed the reasons why the actions
of shouting and coursing are not performed. The response
format was open.
The fourth question evaluated the risk perceived by
the subjects in the behaviors of “speeding”, "driving at an
inappropriate speed", "not keeping the safe distance",
"shouting or verbally insulting while driving", "driving
after drinking an alcoholic beverage" and “driving without
seat belt in the rear seats and in the city”. The possible
answers for this question were presented in a scale from 0
to 10, where 0 means that the risk, as a cause of accident,
is zero and 10 is maximum.
The fifth question asked the participants to state on
which type of road they perceived the highest level of risk,
on a scale ranging from 0 to 10.
2.3. Data Processing
Once the data was obtained, the relevant statistical
analyses were carried out with the Statistical Package for
the Social Sciences (SPSS). For the comparison of mean
values, the One-way ANOVA test for the General Linear
Model (GLM) was conducted, followed by Bonferroni's
post-hoc test. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.
2.4. Ethics
For this type of study, ethical approval and formal
consent are not required. The research type described in
the manuscript did not require the official intervention
of the Ethics Committee in Experimental Research,
(consultative and advisory body of the University of
4 Journal of Sociology and Anthropology
Valencia), as no personal data are used and the
participation was anonymous. However, the Research
Ethics Committee for Social Science in Health of the
University Research Institute on Traffic and Road Safety
at the University of Valencia was consulted, certifying
that the research subject to analysis responds to the
general ethical principles, currently relevant to research in
Social Science, and issued a favorable opinion to carry out
our research in Spain.
3. Results
This study analyzed the results obtained in multiple
behaviors that occur in the field of driving and road safety.
As is shown in Figure 1, 26.4% of drivers recognize that
they shout or insult while driving; while 66.4% say they
have never or almost never performed this type of
behavior at the wheel (see Figure 1).
In addition, some reasons provided by the participants
for carrying out the studied behaviors are the following:
43.9% of drivers who recognize that they shout or insult
while driving do it in reaction to another driver, either
because the other driver does not meet their standards or
because they have to face a dangerous maneuver.
Approximately, 27% of respondents state that they
shout or insult while driving when other drivers cause a
risky or stressful situation. In fact, the remaining
percentage is relatively lower (see Figure 2).
On the other hand, 24.2% of people who drive
without shouting and insulting say it is their way of being,
that they are quiet, 17.3% think it is a waste of time
and about 15% believe it is a matter of habit and
education.
Figure 1. Percentage distribution of drivers depending on the frequency of performing the behavior "shouting or insulting"
Figure 2. Percentage distribution of reported reasons given for the realization of behavior “shout or insult while driving".
2.2 5
26.4
24.8
41.6
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
30%
35%
40%
45%
Always
Many times
Sometimes
Almost never
Never
How often do drivers insult or shout?
22.5
21.4
13.6 13.6 12.6
4.8 3.2 2.7 1.3
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
Reaction to breakinf of
a rule
Reaction to dangerous
maneuvers
The other driver puts
me in danger
Stress
Others
I do not realize
Anger
Irresponsability of
others
Everybody does it
Percentage
Reasons to insult or shout
Journal of Sociology and Anthropology 5
Regarding factors that drivers usually associate with an
implied higher risk of accidents, on a scale from 0 to 10,
the most scored ones are alcohol and speed (see
Figure 3). In the case of driving after drinking any
alcoholic drinks, about 60% gives the maximum score
when assessing risk or danger of this behavior as a cause
of accident; while over 75% of drivers valued at 8 or more
the risk of accident by speeding. For what concerns the
behavior of not adjusting the speed to the condition of
traffic, road, weather etc, even though the scores are well
distributed, only 1,4% of the drivers think that it does
not imply any risk (giving scores below five -5- ). As we
have seen, not maintaining a safe distance ranks fourth in
terms of perceived risk. In this regard, over 65% of drivers
give a value of 8 (on a scale of 0-10), reflecting the higher
risk considerations associated with this behavior as a
cause of accidents. It is very worrying that 11.2% of
drivers do not perceive any risks in the conduct of driving
without seat belts even in the back seats and in a
city. Also, only 25% give the maximum risk. Thus, it
seems necessary to carry out interventions to promote a
change in attitudes about it. Regarding the status of the
vehicle, one out of three drivers do not consider that it is a
factor or element of risk (scores below five), when the
reality is that many accidents are the result of the poor
condition of the vehicle, caused by a lack of concern in
maintaining its security levels. Finally, there are few
drivers who believe that shouting while driving carries
some risk.
If we analyze the relationship between the risk
perceived by drivers in each one of the studied behaviors
as a cause of accidents and in the type of road they use for
their trips, those who mainly do urban journeys attributed,
on average, higher scores to all the behavior, while the
average scores of other participants only differ
significantly in the case of shouting or insulting while
driving F(3,1086)=7.29; p<.001(see Figure 4 and Table 2).
The analysis of mean differences through Bonferroni
test, as shown in Table 2, indicates that drivers shout or
verbally insult depending on the type of road.
Subjects that drive on urban roads or highway give
higher scores and are statistically more significant than
those driving on turnpikes. Finally, there are also higher
and statistically significant results in subjects who drive
on conventional roads, in alignment with the group of
subjects who travel on a turnpike.
Figure 3. Level of risk perception according to different potential misbehaviors
Figure 4. Distribution of the risk perceived in the behavior “shouting or insulting when driving" depending on the road normally used while traveling
4.8 4.6
2.6
4.8
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
Urban
Conventional road
Turnpike
Freeway
Mean
Where do you perceive that insulting or screaming is
dangerous?
6 Journal of Sociology and Anthropology
Table 2. Bonferroni test
Dep. variable (I) (J)
Mean diff.
SE Sig.
CI 95%
(I-J)
LL
UL
Shouting or
insulting verbally
while driving
Urban
Turnpike
1.97
.48
<.001
.93
3.49
Conventional road
Turnpike
1.97
.49
<.001
.68
3.26
Freeway
Turnpike
2.20
.49
<.001
.89
3.52
I= type of road normally used for trips; J = type of road normally used for trips.
Table 3. Bonferroni test
Dep. variable (I) (J)
Mean diff.
SE Sig.
(I-J)
Shouting or insulting verbally
while driving
Almost never Many always 1.54 .41 .002
Almost never Sometimes .99 .23 <.001
Never Many always 1.92 .39 <.001
Never Sometimes 1.37 .21 <.001
I= "How often participants shout or insult while driving:"; J= "How often participants shout or insult while driving:"
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for reported means of shouting or
insulting while driving among Spanish drivers
Frequency n M SD
Always 22 4.09 3.64
Many times
54 3.35 2.86
Sometimes 291 3.90 2.63
Almost never
273
4.89
2.55
Never
448 5.27 2.88
Total 1088 4.69 2.82
The frequency in which drivers perform the behaviors
studied is also maintained in each risk. Subjects who show
a high risk perception perform these behaviors to a lesser
extent. If we focus on shouting or insulting behavior
while driving, we obtain statistically significant results
F(4,1083)=14.79, p<.001.g Applying the post-hoc Bonferroni
we found, as it can be seen in Table 3, that drivers who
say they often carry out this behavior are also those who
perceive the lowest risk in doing so, and their mean values
are statistically different from those of drivers who say
they never or almost never shout or insult while driving
(see Table 4).
Moreover, while virtually all drivers say that driving at
an excessive speed, driving with levels of alcohol higher
than what is legally allowed and driving without insurance
are punishable behaviors, it is noteworthy that 14% think
that driving without seat belts is not punishable. The 8% of
drivers report that driving at an inappropriate speed is not
sanctionable; in the case of not keeping a safe distance,
25% believe that it is not sanctionable either, and the same
applies to 65% of drivers for what concerns shouting and
insulting while driving. Also, it is noteworthy how 65% of
them think that smoking while driving is not sanctionable.
Finally, if we analyze the type of sanctions that drivers
associated with each behavior, it can be observed that
between 81% and 96% of drivers believe that all
behaviors can be sanctioned with a fine, 70% said that
driving with excessive alcohol levels can be punished with
prison, while about 90% of drivers said that driving at an
excessive speed, at an inappropriate speed for the existing
conditions (85%) or exceeding the alcohol limits (96.4%)
may involve a temporary or full suspension of the driving
license; also, 78% said that driving without an insurance
may be subject to penalty.
4. Discussion
Anger reactions and expressions are a commonly
observed phenomenon on the road. Children observe,
react and internalize swearing, screaming, obscene
gestures or violent abuses of drivers: this role model
distorts the attitudes about what is dangerous, and gives
children the perception that the existence of aggressive
drivers on the road is normal, which increases the risk for
everyone. Also, role models of aggressive driving in the
media can contribute to the lack of respect towards people
and towards the traffic regulation. Through this model, the
risky driver lowers the threshold for expressing disrespect
and endangering others, making shouting and insulting a
socially acceptable behavior. Role models of aggressive
drivers may wear a sense of social responsibility as key
road users for the health and safety of others [25].
In this study we have found that aggressive driving is a
normal behavior for 26.4% of respondents. On the other
hand, 66.4% of people do not often perform or have never
performed behaviors such as shouting or insulting other
drivers on the road.
Also, the assessment of the perceived risk of violent
behaviors like yelling or insulting in relation to other risky
behaviors that occur on the roads allows us to predict and
improve the existing measures of intervention on this
issue [9,19,23]. Thus, there are few drivers who perceive
some risk in this type of action, situating "shouting" behind
other behaviors such as "driving after consuming alcohol",
"driving without adapting to road conditions," "driving
faster than permitted", “not keeping the safe distance", “not
using a seat belt "and" having a vehicle in poor condition”.
These results are consistent with other studies, which
also emphasize that there is a tendency to underestimate
the perceived risk and, at the same time, to overestimate
the risk assumed in the case of many of these behaviors
[26]. The problem is that, often, drivers are right. If
someone talks to any taxi driver or with someone who
spent most of his/her working day behind the wheel, this
person will not hesitate to say that traffic is like a jungle,
where the strongest ones prevail and where one should not
lower his or her guard. They are the first to be always on
the defensive, and sometimes the best defense is a good
offense. Antisocial driving is in many senses “contagious”,
and it is becoming the statistical and social norm in big
Journal of Sociology and Anthropology 7
cities, becoming an evident predictor of risky behaviors
while driving [27]. In the worst cases, driving seems to
bring out the worst of people. As soon as drivers get into
the car and sit behind the wheel, they are transformed, and
almost never for good. Many people stop being
polite and become selfish, hostile and aggressive and,
simultaneously, more dangerous to other road users [28].
5. Conclusions
Multiple types of risky maneuvers and deliberated
misbehaviors, which are (formally and informally)
already forbidden from a legal point of view, make other
drivers and pedestrians uncomfortable, and restrict their
movements, creating violent situations related to higher
objective risk for every road user. The degree of social
tolerance towards such behavior is variable. Some
individuals merely ignore them them, accepting them
as something inevitable. Others, however, react with
indignation, unleashing all their lexicon of profanity and
swearing, cursing and accompanying their words with
relevant nonverbal communication, gestures of reproach,
and sometimes reaching direct confrontation. Some people
are frightened or feel anxious about this. Fear causes are
removed, leading to coercion. Anxiety makes people
nervous, makes them become indecisive or hesitant, thus
giving rise to situations of risk or endangering both them
and others.
In general, aggressive behaviors observed on the road
are preceded by subjective factors such as stress, fatigue
and personality traits. However, for the specific case of
shouting and cursing, as specifically addressed in this
study, there is a higher prevalence of these misbehaviors
in urban areas and on highways, and they are more related
to the observed behaviors of other road users, such as
breaking the rules and performing risky maneuvers.
Finally, regarding the intervention strategies that could
be used to prevent this kind of aggressive expressions, it
has been demonstrated that the articulation of road safety
education and road safety campaigns can strengthen the
growth of a road safety culture among road users.
References
[1] World Health Organization. “Global Status Report on Road
Safety”. WHO: Geneva. 2015.
[2] Salamati, P., Moradi, A., Soori, H., Amiri, M. and Soltani, M.
(2015). High crash areas resulting in injuries and death in Tehran
traffic áreas from november 2011 throught february 2012: A
geographic information system analysis. Medical Journal of the
Islamic Republic of Iran, 29: 214.
[3] Jenenkova, O. (2014). Personal Characteristics of Aggressive
Drivers in the Perception of Drivers and Road Traffic Inspectors.
Psychological Thought, 7(1), 80-92.
[4] Nesbit, S. M., Blankenship, K. L., and Murray, R. A. (2012). The
influence of just‐world beliefs on driving anger and aggressive
driving intentions. Aggressive behavior, 38(5), 389-402.
[5] Vallières, E. F., Vallerand, R. J., Bergeron, J., and McDuff, P.
(2014). Intentionality, anger, coping, and ego defensiveness in
reactive aggressive driving. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
44(5), 354-363.
[6] Alonso, F., Esteban, C., Useche, S.A. and Manso, V. (2016).
“Analysis of the State and Development of Road Safety Education
in Spanish Higher Education Institutions”. Higher Educational
Research, 1(2). 2016. [Publication in advance]
[7] Alonso, F., Esteban, C., Useche, S.A. and Manso, V. (2016).
“Determinants and Stakeholders Influencing Children’s Road
Safety Education”. International Journal of Elementary Education
(IJEEDU), 2016. [Publication in advance].
[8] Danaf, M., Abou-Zeid, M., and Kaysi, I. (2015). Modeling anger
and aggressive driving behavior in a dynamic choice–latent
variable model. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 75, 105-118.
[9] Useche, S.A., Serge, A. and Alonso, F. “Risky Behaviors and
Stress Indicators between Novice and Experienced Drivers”.
American Journal of Applied Psychology, 3(1), pp. 11-14. 2015.
[10] Jovanović, D., Lipovac, K., Stanojević, P. and Stanojević, D.
(2011). The effects of personality traits on driving-related anger
and aggressive behaviour in traffic among Serbian drivers.
Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour,
14(1), 43-53.
[11] Kovácsová, N., Rošková, E. and Lajunen, T. (2014).
Forgivingness, anger, and hostility in aggressive driving.
AccidentAnalysis and Prevention, 62, 303-308.
[12] Balogun, S. K., Shenge, N. A. and Oladipo, S. E. (2012).
Psychosocial factors influencing aggressive driving among
commercial and private automobile drivers in Lagos metropolis.
The Social Science Journal, 49(1), 83-89.
[13] Deffenbacher, J.L., Lynch, R.S., Filetti, L.B., Dahlen, E.R. and
Oetting, E.R. (2003a). Anger, aggression, risky behavior, and
crash-related outcomes in three groups of drivers. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 41(3), 333-349.
[14] Deffenbacher, J.L., Deffenbacher, D.M., Lynch, R.S. and Richards
T.L. (2003b). Anger, aggression, and risky behavior: a comparison
of high and low anger drivers. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
41(6), 701-718.
[15] Efrat, K. and Shoham, A. (2013). Aggressive driving, influenced
by the proneness of driving aggression. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 59, 459-465.
[16] Paleti, R., Eluru, N. and Bhat, C. R. (2010). Examining the
influence of aggressive driving behavior on driver injury severity
in traffic crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42(6), 1839-
1854.
[17] Parker, D., Lajunen, T., and Summala, H. (2002). Anger and
aggression among drivers in three European countries. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 34(2), 229-235.
[18] Perinelli, E. and Gremigni, P. (2016). Use of social desirability
scales in clinical psychology: A systematic review. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 72(6), 534-551.
[19] Mizell, L. and Co. (1997). Aggressive Driving. Aggressive
Driving: Three Studies. AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety
Resources, 1997.
[20] Alonso, F., Esteban, C., Useche, S.A. and López de Cózar, E.
(2016). Prevalence of Physical and Mental Fatigue Symptoms on
Spanish Drivers and Its Incidence on Driving Safety. Advances in
Psychology and Neuroscience, 1(2): 10-18.
[21] Alonso, F., Esteban, C. Sanmartin, J. and Useche, S.A. (2016).
Consistency Between the Subjective Perception of Feeling
Indisposed, the Decision to Drive and Driving Performance.
Science Journal of Public Health, 4(6): 482-488.
[22] Useche, S. A. (2012). “Cómo evaluar e intervenir sobre la
conducta antisocial desde la psicología jurídica? Acta Colombiana
de Psicología, 15(2):149-152.
[23] Alonso, F., Esteban, C., Calatayud, C., Medina, J. E. and Alamar,
B. (2005). La Justicia en el Tráfico. Análisis del ciclo
legislativoejecutivo a nivel internacional. Cuadernos de Reflexión
Attitudes, Attitudes, Barcelona.
[24] Alonso, F., Sanmartín, J., Calatayud, C., Esteban, C., Alamar, B.
and Ballestar, M. L. (2005a). La justicia en el Tráfico.
Conocimiento y valoración de la población española. Cuadernos
de Reflexión Attitudes, Attitudes, Barcelona.
[25] James, L. and Nahl, D. (2000). Aggressive driving is emotionally
impaired driving. Conference Paper.
[26] Luna Blanco, R. (2013). Risk perception and safety driving.
Carreteras: Revista técnica de la Asociación Española de la
Carretera, 189: 48-56.
[27] Zhang, T., Chan, A.H. and Zhang, W. (2015). Dimensions of
driving anger and their relationships with aberrant driving.
Accident Analysis & Prevention, 81:124-33.
[28] Qu, W., Dai, M., Zhao, W., Zhang, K. and Ge Y. (2016).
Expressing Anger Is More Dangerous than Feeling Angry when
Driving. PLoS ONE, 11(6):e0156948.