Conference PaperPDF Available

L2 buoyancy: Exploring learners’ everyday resilience in the language classroom

Authors:
L2 buoyancy:
Exploring learners’ everyday resilience
in the language classroom
Saerom Yun Phil Hiver
International Graduate School of English
Ali Al-Hoorie
University of Nottingham
2016 Psychology of Language Learning 2
Jyväskylä, Finland
(Dörnyei & Ryan, 2015; Dörnyei, 2005; Gardner & Lambert, 1972;
Gardner, 2007; Martin, 2002; Masgoret & Gardner, 2003; Staw, 1989)
ENGLISH ENGLISH
Successful L2 learner Motivated L2 learner
In L2 Learning
Why are even motivated students likely to be debilitated
by adversity and setbacks experienced in a school setting?
(e.g., poor performance, study pressure, any other academic stress)
Motivational states of language learners are rather fluctuating than steady.
Motivation tends to decline at a certain level once learners gradually began to lose their
desire and interests in learning a new language.
(Dörnyei, 2000; Ushioda, 2008)
Motivation is variable.
What should be more importantly considered?
1. Maintenance of motivational states against various distracting language learning
circumstances
2. Insufficiency of motivation to solely explain successful L2 learning.
Another complementary concept is needed.
Buoyancy
L2 motivation
Main Concepts
Positive
Psychology
Resilience Buoyancy
In Educational
Psychology
Individual learners ability to manage their
everyday academic setbacks and
difficulties inevitable in the ordinary school
life such as poor grades, exam pressure,
and difficult schoolwork.
(Martin & Marsh, 2008; 2009)
Buoyancy
Math
(Martin & Marsh, 2008)
Buoyancy in learning mathematics
𝒌
𝒏
𝒌
Buoyancy
Successful L2 learning
!
L2 achievement
L2 achievement
For the success of L2 learning,
learners probably need ability
to overcome all difficulties
in the course of L2 learning.
L2 stress
L2 pressure
L2 difficulties
Poor L2 outcomes
…. etc.
Successful L2 learning
1) to sustain motivation to obtain successful performance despite some academic
difficulties.
2) to help students proactively bounce back and step forward from adversities and
underachievement.
L2 buoyancy
Research Questions
Does buoyancy exist in second language learning?
What are the typical outcomes or profiles of buoyancy for second language learners
at college level?
What is the relationship between buoyancy and L2 achievement?
Hypothesized Predictors
L2 buoyancy
Self
-efficacy
Students’ beliefs and confidence in their ability in their L2 work
Self
-regulation
Students’ certainty about how to do well in L2 learning
Ideal L2 self
Students’ future images of themselves as competent L2 speakers
L2 persistence
Students’ abilities to follow through with completing L2 tasks
L2 anxiety
Student’s nervousness about not doing well in L2 learning
T
-S relationship
Students’ perceptions toward their L2 teachers
(Martin & Marsh, 2006, 2008)
L2 buoyancy
L2 buoyancy
+
(In L2 learning)
Self-efficacy
Self-regulation
Ideal self
Persistence
Anxiety
T-S Relationship
L2 Achievement
Predictors
Outcome variable
Profiles
1. Participant
2. Procedure & Results
2-1. Cluster Analysis
2-2. CFA & SEM
METHODS & RESULTS
Participants
For Questionnaire survey based data
6Universities
in Seoul & Gyeonggi-do, Korea
878Respondents
Research Questions
Does buoyancy exist in second language learning?
What are the typical outcomes or profiles of buoyancy for second language learners
at college level?
What is the relationship between buoyancy and L2 achievement?
Cluster Analysis
Questionnaire Survey Cluster Analysis
Cluster Solution
Two-step clustering
One-way ANOVA
Post Hoc comparisons /Tukey’s HSD test
k-means clustering
Multinomial logistic regression
6-point Likert Scale
Method 1
Procedure
Variables Entered into Cluster Analysis
Validational
Variable
L2
self-efficacy L2
self-regulation Ideal
L2 self L2
persistence L2
anxiety T-S
relationship L2 buoyancy
Cluster 1 (n = 111)
Ideally
buoyant L2 learners 5.16 4.53 5.22 4.75 2.08 5.11 4.98
Cluster 2 (n = 317)
Buoyant L2 learners 4.05 3.82 4.36 3.99 3.25 4.46 4.18
Cluster 3 (n = 227)
Minimally
buoyant L2 learners 3.53 3.41 3.77 3.41 3.59 3.69 3.60
Cluster 4 (n = 186)
Dependent L2 learners 2.80 3.16 2.96 3.04 4.63 3.69 3.03
Cluster 5 (n = 38)
Fragile L2 learners 3.14 2.70 1.82 1.69 3.34 1.81 1.79
Result 1
Cluster Analysis
1.79 3.03 3.60 4.18 4.98
Ideally buoyant L2 learners Fragile L2 learners
Result 1
Cluster Analysis
<Buoyancy Level>
Cluster 1
Ideally buoyant L2 learners
5.16
4.53
5.22
L2 self-efficacy
4.75
2.08
5.11
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 persistence
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
4.98 L2 buoyancy
Male n=64
Female n=47
----------------
n=111
12.6% of the total
12.6
36.1
25.7
21.2
4.3
4.3
65.8
13.5
7.2
13.5
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
21.6
6.3
3.6
27.0
33.3
4.5
3.6
English
Other Languages
Humanities
Engineering
Social Science
Natural Science
Others
Gender
Year
65.8 (n=73)
13.5 (n=15)
7.2 (n= 8)
13.5 (n=15)
Major
21.6 (n=24)
6.3 (n= 7)
3.6 (n= 4)
27.0 (n=30)
33.3 (n=37)
4.5 (n= 5)
3.6 (n= 4)
Result 1
Cluster Profiles
Cluster 2
Buoyant L2 learners
4.05
3.82
4.36
L2 self-efficacy
3.99
3.25
4.46
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 persistence
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
4.18 L2 buoyancy
12.6
36.1
25.7
21.2
4.3
Male n=182
Female n=135
----------------
n=317
36.1% of the total
65.0
18.6
8.5 7.9
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
65.0 (n=206)
18.6 (n=59)
8.5 (n=27)
7.9 (n=25)
10.7
4.77.9
33.1
29.7
7.9 6.0
English
Other Languages
Humanities
Engineering
Social Science
Natural Science
Others
10.7 (n=34)
4.7 (n=15)
7.9 (n=25)
33.1 (n=105)
29.7 (n=94)
7.9 (n=25)
6.0 (n=19)
Gender
Year
Major
Result 1
Cluster Profiles
Cluster 3
Minimally buoyant L2 learners
3.53
3.41
3.77
L2 self-efficacy
3.41
3.59
3.69
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 persistence
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
3.60 L2 buoyancy
Male n=110
Female n=116
----------------
n=226
25.7% of the total
12.6
36.1
25.7
21.2
4.3
4.3
63.7
19.9
8.4 8.0
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
7.5
4.06.2
36.3
30.9
10.2 4.9
English
Other Languages
Humanities
Engineering
Social Science
Natural Science
Others
63.7 (n=144)
19.9 (n=45)
8.4 (n=19)
8.0 (n=18)
7.5 (n=17)
4.0 (n= 9)
6.2 (n=14)
36.3 (n=82)
30.9 (n=70)
10.2 (n=23)
4.9 (n=11)
Gender
Year
Major
Result 1
Cluster Profiles
Cluster 4
Dependent L2 learners
2.80
3.16
2.96
L2 self-efficacy
3.04
4.63
3.69
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 persistence
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
3.03 L2 buoyancy
Male n=87
Female n=99
----------------
n=186
21.2% of the total
12.6
36.1
25.7
21.2
4.3
4.3
55.9
19.9
11.8
12.4
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
4.95.4
8.1
26.8
36.5
12.4
5.9
English
Other Languages
Humanities
Engineering
Social Science
Natural Science
Others
55.9 (n=104)
19.9 (n=37)
11.8 (n=22)
12.4 (n=23)
4.9 (n= 9)
5.4 (n=10)
8.1 (n=15)
26.8 (n=50)
36.5 (n=68)
12.4 (n=23)
5.9 (n=11)
Gender
Year
Major
Result 1
Cluster Profiles
Cluster 5
Fragile L2 learners
3.14
2.70
L2 self-efficacy
3.34
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 persistence
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
L2 buoyancy
1.79
1.82
1.69
1.81
Male n=25
Female n=13
----------------
n=38
4.3% of the total
12.6
36.1
25.7
21.2
4.3
81.6
7.9
5.3 5.3
1st
2nd
3rd
4th
2.6
0
5.3
71.1
10.5
7.9
2.6
English
Other Languages
Humanities
Engineering
Social Science
Natural Science
Others
81.6 (n=31)
7.9 (n= 3)
5.3 (n= 2)
5.3 (n= 2)
2.6 (n= 1)
- (n= 0)
5.3 (n= 2)
71.1 (n=27)
10.5 (n= 4)
7.9 (n= 3)
2.6 (n= 1)
Gender
Year
Major
Result 1
Cluster Profiles
Hypothesized model for SEM CFA & SEM
Construct Reliability
With finalized scales
Convergent validity
Discriminant validity
Method 2
Procedure
Research Questions
Does buoyancy exist in second language learning?
What are the typical outcomes or profiles of buoyancy for second language learners
at college level?
What is the relationship between buoyancy and L2 achievement?
CFA and SEM
Result 2
CFA and SEM
Path βBSE CR
-efficacy
→ L2 Buoyancy
.31 .33 .046 7.26***
-regulation .39 .42 .044 9.49***
.29 .27 .038 7.01***
.07 .06 .034 1.72
-S relationship .17 .16 .037 4.26***
→ L2 Achievement
.24 .21 .052 4.02***
-efficacy .05 .04 .035 1.23
-regulation .01 .01 .042 0.32
.08 .06 .030 2.13*
Result 2
CFA and SEM
L2 achievement
L2 buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
L2 anxiety
T-S
relationship
Ideal L2 self
.31
.39
.29
.07
.17
.01
.08
.05
.24
[The Structural Model]
Note. Statistically significant (p < .05) standardized (β) coefficients
for the structural model predicting L2 achievement
Path βBSE CR
Self
-efficacy
→ L2
Buoyancy
.31 .33 .046
7.26***
Self
-regulation .39 .42 .044
9.49***
Ideal L2 self
.29 .27 .038
7.01***
Anxiety
.07 .06 .034 1.72
T
-S relationship .17 .16 .037
4.26***
L2 buoyancy
→ L2
Achievement
.24 .21 .052
4.02***
Self
-efficacy .05 .04 .035 1.23
Self
-regulation .01 .01 .042 0.32
Ideal L2 self
.08 .06 .030 2.13*
DISCUSSION
Discussion 1
Role of L2 self in L2 buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 anxiety
T-S
relationship
L2
buoyancy
L2 achievement
L2 buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
L2 anxiety
T-S
relationship
Ideal L2 self
.31
.39
.29
.07
.17
.01
.08
.05
.24
[The Structural Model]
Note. Statistically significant (p < .05) standardized (β) coefficients for the structural model predicting L2 achieve
ment and general learning achievement (GPA).
Discussion 1
Role of L2 self in L2 buoyancy
Cluster 1
Ideally buoyant L2 learners
5.16
4.53
5.22
L2 self-efficacy
2.08
5.11
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
4.98 L2 buoyancy
Cluster 5
Fragile L2 learners
3.14
2.70
L2 self-efficacy
3.34
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
L2 buoyancy
1.79
1.82
1.81
Discussion 1
Role of L2 self in L2 buoyancy
All three L2 self constructs are conceptually linked to L2 buoyancy.
They play powerful roles in developing L2 buoyancy.
L2 buoyancy may require a robust L2 self
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
Discussion 1
Role of L2 self in L2 buoyancy
L2 anxiety
Discussion 2
Role of T-S relationship in L2 Buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2
buoyancy
T-S
relationship
L2 achievement
L2 buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
L2 anxiety
T-S
relationship
Ideal L2 self
.31
.39
.29
.07
.17
.01
.08
.05
.24
[The Structural Model]
Note. Statistically significant (p < .05) standardized (β) coefficients
for the structural model predicting L2 achievement
Discussion 2
Role of T-S relationship in L2 Buoyancy
Cluster 1
Cluster 2
Cluster 3
Cluster 4
Cluster 5
df Tukey’s post-hoc tests F-value / p η2
L2 self
-efficacy 5.16 4.05 3.53 2.80 3.14 4 1 > 2 > 3 > 5 > 4
351.5/.0001
.62
L2 self
-regulation 4.53 3.82 3.41 3.16 2.70 4 1 > 2 > 3 > 4 > 5
148.9/.0001
.41
Ideal L2 self
5.22 4.36 3.77 2.96 1.82 4 1 > 2 > 3 > 4 > 5
269.7/.0001
.55
L2 persistence
4.75 3.99 3.41 3.04 1.69 4 1 > 2 > 3 > 4 > 5
255.2/.0001
.54
L2 anxiety
2.08 3.25 3.59 4.63 3.34 4 4 > 3 , 5 , 2 > 1
149.7/.0001
.41
T
-S relationship 5.11 4.46 3.69 3.69 1.81 4 1 > 2 > 3 , 4 > 5
222.8/.0001
.51
Discussion 3
Role of L2 Anxiety in L2 buoyancy
Cluster 2
Buoyant L2 learners
4.05
3.82
4.36
L2 self-efficacy
3.25
4.46
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
4.18 L2 buoyancy
Cluster 5
Fragile L2 learners
3.14
2.70
L2 self-efficacy
3.34
L2 self-regulation
Ideal L2 self
L2 anxiety
T-S relationship
L2 buoyancy
1.79
1.82
1.81
Discussion 3
Role of L2 Anxiety in L2 buoyancy
Learners who have sufficient high level of L2 self seem to have an ability
to cope with negative emotional states.
Strengthening L2 learners’ positive psychological factors in the face of
the external threat may more successfully help learners develop ability to
overcome everyday stress and setbacks in L2 learning context.
“Successful learners can utilize their specific strengths and compensate for their pos
sible weaknesses in adjustment to the particular learning environment.
(Dörnyei & Ryan, 2015, p. 28)
L2 learners who have a certain degree of anxiety can be buoyant!
L2 anxiety
Discussion 3
Role of L2 Anxiety in L2 buoyancy
Discussion 4
Role of Buoyancy in L2 Achievement
L2
buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy L2
Achievement
L2
self-
regulation
Mediator
L2 self constructs may only be able to indirectly affect L2 achievement mediated through L2 buoyancy.
Ideal L2 self
L2 achievement
L2 buoyancy
L2
self-efficacy
L2
self-regulation
L2 anxiety
T-S
relationship
Ideal L2 self
.31
.39
.29
.07
.17
.01
.08
.05
.24
[The Structural Model]
Note. Statistically significant (p < .05) standardized (β) coefficients for the structural model predicting L2 achieve
ment and general learning achievement (GPA).
Discussion 4
Role of Buoyancy in L2 Achievement
Path βBSE CR
-efficacy
→ L2 Buoyancy
.31 .33 .046 7.26***
-regulation .39 .42 .044 9.49***
.29 .27 .038 7.01***
.07 .06 .034 1.72
-S relationship .17 .16 .037 4.26***
→ L2 Achievement
.24 .21 .052 4.02***
-efficacy .05 .04 .035 1.23
-regulation .01 .01 .042 0.32
.08 .06 .030 2.13*
References
Aida, Y. (1994). Examination of Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope’s construct of foreign language anxiety: The case of students of Japanese. The Modern Language Journal, 78(2), 155168
Dörnyei, Z. (2000). Motivation in action: Towards a process-oriented conceptualisation of student motivation. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 519538.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dörnyei, Z., & Ryan, S. (2015). The psychology of the language learner revisited. New York, NY: Routledge.
Gardner, R. C. (2007). Motivation and Second Language Acquisition 1. Porta Linguarum, 8, 920.
Gardner, R. C., & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and motivation in second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Gottfried, A. E. (1982). Relations between academic intrinsic motivation and anxiety in children and young adolesents. Journal of School Psychology Psychology, 20, 205215.
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety. Modern Language Journal, 70(2), 125132.
Kohn, P. M., Lafreniere, K., & Gurevich, M. (1991). Hassles, health, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 47882.
MacIntyre, P. D., & Gardner, R. C. (1991). Methods and results in the study of anxiety and language learning : A review of the literature. Language Learning, 41(1), 85117.
MacIntyre, P. D., & Gardner, R. C. (1994). The subtle effects of language anxiety on cognitive processing in the second langauge. Language Learning, 44(2), 283305
Marsh, H. W. (1990). A multidimensional, hierarchical model of self-concept: Theoretical and empirical justification. Educational Psychology Review, 2(2), 77172.
Martin, A. J. (2002). Motivation and academic resilience: Developing a model for student enhancement. Australian Journal of Education, 46(I), 3449.
Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2008). Academic buoyancy: Towards an understanding of students’ everyday academic resilience. Journal of School Psychology, 46(1), 5383.
Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2009). Academic resilience and academic buoyancy: Multidimensional and hierarchical conceptual framing of causes, correlates and cognate constructs.
Oxford Review of Education, 35(3), 353370.
Masgoret, A. M., & Gardner, R. C. (2003). Attitudes, motivation, and second language learning: A metaanalysis of studies conducted by Gardner and associates. Language Learning,
53(1), 123163.
Shirom, A. (1986). Students’ stress. Higher Education, 15(6), 667676.
Smith, P. L., & Fouad, N. A. (1999). Subject-matter specificity of self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, interests, and goals: Implications for the social-cognitive model.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46(4), 461471.
Staw, B. M. (1989). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In H. J. Leavitt, L. R. Pondy, & D. M. Boje (Eds.), Readings in managerial psychology (4th ed., pp. 3671).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Ushioda, E. (2008). Motivation and good language learners. In C. Griffiths (Ed.), Lessons from good lanauge learners (pp. 1934). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zeidner, M. (1992). Sources of academic stress: the case of first year Jewish and Arab college students in Israel. Higher Education, 24(1), 2540.
THANK YOU
Saerom Yun Philip Hiver Ali-Al-Hoorie
Article
Full-text available
Anxiety is a state of mind of intense worry that can affect students’ performance both positively and negatively. Internationally, recent studies show high percentages of students diagnosed with anxiety and depression. This paper seeks to quantitatively analyze how anxiety is related to academic performance in Ecuadorian students. An Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) model with school fixed effects estimates an educational production function. The results confirm that, up to certain levels of anxiety, students' scores improve. However, once anxiety levels are elevated -a threshold that is higher for male students- scores are negatively affected. These findings are relevant for two reasons. First, during adolescence, experiences and interpersonal relationships impact personality construction and influence the development of cognitive, non-cognitive and emotional skills. Second, given the current post-pandemic situation, in which anxiety is a frequent scenario. This research concludes that it is necessary to reflect on the importance of rethinking pedagogical processes in schools and to analyze strategies to introduce mental health in the school curriculum.
Article
Full-text available
Academic buoyancy is developed as a construct reflecting everyday academic resilience within a positive psychology context and is defined as students' ability to successfully deal with academic setbacks and challenges that are typical of the ordinary course of school life (e.g., poor grades, competing deadlines, exam pressure, difficult schoolwork). Data were collected from 598 students in Years 8 and 10 at five Australian high schools. Half-way through the school year and then again at the end of the year, students were asked to rate their academic buoyancy as well as a set of hypothesized predictors (self-efficacy, control, academic engagement, anxiety, teacher-student relationship) in the area of mathematics. Multilevel modeling found that the bulk of variance in academic buoyancy was explained at the student level. Confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling showed that (a) Time 1 anxiety (negatively), self-efficacy, and academic engagement significantly predict Time 1 academic buoyancy; (b) Time 2 anxiety (negatively), self-efficacy, academic engagement, and teacher-student relationships explain variance in Time 2 academic buoyancy over and above that explained by academic buoyancy at Time 1; and (c) of the significant predictors, anxiety explains the bulk of variance in academic buoyancy.
Article
Full-text available
This paper explores a model of motivation and introduces the concept of academic resilience. Although motivation is critical to academic success, academic gains that students make can be lost if they are not resilient to setback, study pressure, and stress in the school setting. It is therefore important that students are motivated and resilient to academic pressures. A problem with motivation theory and research, however, is that it has not been formulated in a way that provides educators and students with a common language with which to develop motivation and academic resilience in the classroom. This paper draws together seminal motivation theory, posits clear constructs that represent these theories, and then repackages them into a model that can be used by educators and understood by students. Such a model also holds direct implications in the classroom and counselling contexts, and the strategies for enhancing motivation and academic resilience are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
‘Academic resilience’ refers to a student’s capacity to overcome acute or chronic adversities that are seen as major assaults on educational processes. Although intersecting with highly vulnerable and important populations, academic resilience does not map onto the many students who are faced with setbacks, challenges and pressures that are part of more regular academic life. This, it is argued, reflects ‘academic buoyancy’ that maps onto the many students who must negotiate the ups and downs of everyday academic life as distinct from acute and chronic adversities relevant to more traditional constructions of academic resilience. Inherent in this argument, then, is a proposed hierarchical framework in which academic buoyancy is a necessary but not sufficient condition for academic resilience. Such a hierarchical framework, therefore, has the potential to speak to all students and so represents an encompassing framework that can more fully explain the nature and extent of adversities and challenges that are part of academic life. We further contend that academic resilience and academic buoyancy require multidimensional approaches to their conceptualising and measurement in order to most effectively differentiate the factors that are (and are not) components, causes, correlates and cognate to them. We conclude by proposing a number of conceptual and empirical approaches to a next generation of research into academic resilience and academic buoyancy, develop the notion of ‘leading’ and ‘lagging’ indicators of buoyancy and resilience, and identify the implications of our framework for intervention and policy in the academic domain and beyond.
Article
Research results over the past decades have consistently demonstrated that a key reason why many second language learners fail--while some learners do better with less effort--lies in various learner attributes such as personality traits, motivation, or language aptitude. In psychology, these attributes have traditionally been called "individual differences." The scope of individual learner differences is broad--ranging from creativity to learner styles and anxiety--yet there is no current, comprehensive, and unified volume that provides an overview of the considerable amount of research conducted on various language learner differences, until now.
Article
It almost goes without saying that good language learners are motivated. Common sense and everyday experience suggest that the high achievers of this world have motivation, a word which derives from the Latin verb movere meaning to move. Thus, simply defined, we might say that motivation concerns what moves a person to make certain choices, to engage in action, and to persist in action. The need for personal motivation is a message that resonates across so many stories of major and minor human endeavor, whether in the single-minded dedication of an athlete pursuing an Olympic dream, the drive and ambition of a young executive aiming for the top of the corporate ladder, or the willpower and self-discipline of someone determined to lose weight or to give up smoking. Without motivation, success will be hard to come by, and the case of learning a second or foreign language is little different. Motivation is listed by Rubin (1975) among the three essential variables on which good language learning depends. As Corder (1967, p. 164) famously put it forty years ago, “Let us say that, given motivation, it is inevitable that a human being will learn a second language if he is exposed to the language data.” Yet however commonsensical this general observation might be, the pursuit of its empirical verification has exercised language acquisition scholars for decades and generated an enormous amount of research.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Book
Over the past decade, the focus of inquiry into the psychology of SLA has shifted from the analysis of various characteristics within individuals towards a greater consideration of individuals’ dynamic interactions with diverse contexts. This revisit of the bestselling The Psychology of the Language Learner reflects on these developments by challenging some of the assumptions upon which the original text was based, maintaining the familiar structure of the original, while situating the discussion within a very different theoretical framework.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.