ArticlePDF Available

Adaptable Categorization of Hands and Tools in Prosthesis Users

Authors:
Psychological Science
1 –4
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797616685869
journals.sagepub.com/home/pss
Short Report
Some theories propose that tools become incorporated
into the neural representation of the hands (a process
known as tool embodiment; Maravita & Iriki, 2004).
Others suggest that conceptual body representation is
rigid and that experience with one’s own body is insuf-
ficient for adapting bodily cognition, as shown in indi-
viduals born without hands (Vannuscorps & Caramazza,
2016) and in amputees with persistent phantom hand
representation (Kikkert et al., 2016). How sharp is the
conceptual boundary between hands and tools? This
question is particularly relevant for individuals who have
lost one hand and use prosthetic hands as tools to sup-
plement their missing hand function. Although both con-
genital one-handers (i.e., amelia patients) and one-handed
amputees are encouraged to use prostheses, the former
show a greater tendency than the latter to use prosthetic
hands in daily tasks (Jang et al., 2011). One-handers have
a fully functional remaining hand (allowing them to use
handheld tools, etc.), which makes them less likely to
show semantic distortions in hand and tool representa-
tion. However, their bodies and their interactions with
their environment are fundamentally altered by their dis-
ability (Makin et al., 2013; Makin, Wilf, Schwartz, &
Zohary, 2010).
To determine how real-world experience shapes con-
ceptual categorization of hands, tools, and prostheses,
we recruited one-handers with congenital or acquired
unilateral hand loss to take part in a study involving a
priming task. We predicted that one-handers, particularly
congenital one-handers, would show more conceptual
blurring between hands and tools than control partici-
pants would, as a result of less experience with a hand
and more reliance on prostheses (which are essentially
tools) for typical hand functions. We further predicted
that individual differences in prosthesis usage would be
reflected in implicit categorization of hands, manual
tools, and prostheses.
Method
Twenty-four one-handers (12 born without a hand and
12 who lost a hand through amputation) and 21 matched
control participants performed a visual priming task in
which they verbally categorized target images of hands
and tools (Fig. 1a). On each trial, participants saw a prime
stimulus followed by a target stimulus, each of which
appeared for 32 ms (stimulus onset asynchrony = 600
ms). On baseline trials, the prime was always a scram-
bled image, and on experimental trials, the prime could
be an image of a hand, a tool, or a prosthesis. On all tri-
als, the target was either a hand or a tool. Participants
were instructed to ignore the prime and to verbally report
whether the target image was a hand or a tool. Time from
the start of the target display to voice onset was recorded
as the participants’ reaction time (RT). Participants com-
pleted 40 baselines trials and four blocks (60 trials each)
of experimental trials.
Ten different exemplars were used as prime and target
items in each category (for example stimuli, see Fig. S1 in
the Supplemental Material available online). Hand and
prosthesis images showed the side of the participants’
missing hand (one-handers) or nondominant hand (con-
trols). When possible, images were of the participant’s
own prosthesis, so daily prosthesis usage (assessed both
by asking how often participants wore their prosthesis
and by using an adapted version of the Motor Activity
Log; Makin et al., 2013) generally reflected individuals’
experience with the prime prosthesis image presented to
them (see the Supplemental Material for additional meth-
odological details).
685869PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616685869van den Heiligenberg et al.Flexible Categorization
research-article2016
Corresponding Author:
Tamar R. Makin, University College London, Institute of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London, WC1N 3AZ, United
Kingdom
E-mail: tmakin@ucl.ac.uk
Adaptable Categorization of Hands
and Tools in Prosthesis Users
Fiona M. Z. van den Heiligenberg1, Nick Yeung2, Peter Brugger3,
Jody C. Culham4, and Tamar R. Makin1,5
1FMRIB Centre, Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford; 2Department of
Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford; 3Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Neurology,
University Hospital Zurich, Switzerland; 4Brain and Mind Institute, Department of Psychology,
University of Western Ontario; and 5Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London
Received 1/18/16; Revision accepted 12/3/16
2 van den Heiligenberg et al.
Results
We first examined the prime-target congruency effect on
trials with hand and tool primes in one-handers and con-
trol participants. RTs for controls were slower when the
prime and target were congruent (i.e., from the same
category; e.g., hand prime and hand target) than when
they were incongruent (i.e., from different categories;
e.g., hand prime and tool target), which indicates same-
category prime interference in target processing (Boy &
Sumner, 2010; see also Vainio, 2011, for negative stimu-
lus-response compatibility for hand images). Next, a 2
(prime) × 2 (target) × 2 (group) repeated measures analy-
sis of variance revealed a significant three-way interac-
tion, F(1, 43) = 5.37, p = .025; this indicates that, unlike
the results for control participants, the priming effect was
absent in one-handers (for further analysis, see Results
and Fig. S2 in the Supplemental Material). In control par-
ticipants, we found a significant Prime × Target interaction,
F(1, 20) = 11.24, p = .003. Further, planned comparisons
using paired-samples t tests revealed a significant RT
difference between congruent and incongruent trials for
bothhand targets, t(20) = 2.19, p = .041, d = 0.175, and
tool targets, t(20) = 3.31, p = .003, d = 0.261. However, in
one-handers, there was no significant Prime × Target
interaction (F < 1), which suggests that the conceptual
hand-tool category boundary is blurred in one-handers,
compared with control participants.
We calculated each participant’s congruency effect by
subtracting mean RTs on incongruent trials from mean
RTs on congruent trials. Given our finding that control
participants’ RTs were slower on congruent than on
incongruent trials, a greater congruency effect reflects
greater dissociation between hands and tools. Although
one-handers did not show a significant congruency
effect, there was evidence that their categorization behav-
ior was modulated by their case histories, specifically the
age at which one-handed amputees lost their hand and
their habitual prosthesis usage. We found a significant
correlation between age at hand loss and congruency
effect, r(10) = .65, p = .022 (Fig. 1b): Hand loss earlier in
life related to weaker congruency effects, whereas ampu-
tees who lost a hand later in life (and therefore had more
experience with the now-missing hand) showed greater
congruency effects. These findings suggest that the con-
ceptual distinction between hands and tools develops
through experience with natural hands. We also found
that one-handers who used their prosthesis more tended
to show weaker congruency effects than those who
used prostheses less frequently—correlation between
Baseline Trials
Experimental Trials
Scrambled
Prime
Prosthesis
Prime
Tool
Prime
Hand
Prime
Hand Target Tool Target
600-ms SOA
32 ms 32 ms
a
Age at Hand Loss (years)
Hand-Tool Congruency Effect (ms)
4540 N/A
150
100
50
–50
–100
–150
0
35302520150
b
One-Handed Amputees
Controls
Congenital One-Handers
Fig. 1. Experimental procedure and results. On each trial (a), participants saw a 32-ms prime stimulus followed by a 32-ms target stimulus, with a
600-ms stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Participants were asked to make a speeded, forced-choice verbal response as to whether the target was a
hand or a tool. Baseline trials differed from experimental trials only in that the prime was a scrambled image rather than an image of a hand, tool,
or prosthesis. Reaction times on incongruent trials (in which prime and target stimuli were from different categories) were subtracted from reaction
times on congruent trials (in which prime and target stimuli were from the same category) to calculate the hand-tool congruency effect. The scatter
plot (b; with best-fitting regression line) shows the mean hand-tool congruency effect as a function of age at hand loss for congenital one-handers
and one-handed amputees, along with the mean congruency effect for control participants.
Flexible Categorization 3
congruency effect and prosthesis usage: r(22) = −.38, p =
.068—such that the hand-tool category boundary
(reflected in the congruency effect) tended to blur with
the regularity of prosthesis usage.
Theories of tool embodiment state that prosthesis
usage should result in categorization of the prosthesis as
a hand (Murray, 2008). In the final set of analyses, we
assessed the degree to which prosthesis primes affected
responses to hands and tools as a function of prosthesis
experience. Given that categorical similarity resulted in
slower responses for congruent prime-target pairs than
for incongruent prime-target pairs, slowing of RTs for
prosthesis primes can be taken to reflect the conceptual
similarity between prostheses and hands or tools. To
investigate this, we ran a backwards regression analysis
on RTs for prosthesis-hand trials using age at hand loss,
years since hand loss, prosthesis usage, congruency
effect size, and mean RT on baseline trials as predictors.
The final model for hand-target trials, F(2, 21) = 35.08,
p< .001, R = .88, adjusted R2 = .75, included age at hand
loss, β = −0.29, t(23) = −2.72, p = .013, and baseline RT,
β = 0.78, t(23) = 7.28, p < .001 (see Fig. S3 in the Supple-
mental Material). This analysis revealed that people who
lost their hand earlier in life showed greater conceptual
similarity between the prosthesis and hands. We also ran
a backwards regression analysis on RTs for prosthesis-tool
trials using the same parameters as for the previous set of
backwards regressions. The final model, F(2, 21) = 42.48,
p < .001, R = .90, adjusted R2 = .78, included prosthesis
usage, β = 0.24, t(23) = 2.39, p = .026, and baseline RT, β =
0.83, t(23) = 8.36, p < .001 (Fig. S4 in the Supplemental
Material). This analysis showed that, in opposition to the
previous regression, the conceptual relationship between
prostheses and tools was best predicted by prosthesis usage,
with participants who used their prostheses more showing
greater conceptual similarity between prostheses and tools.
Conclusion
Together, our findings demonstrate that categorization of
hands and tools in one-handers depends on both prior
experience with a natural hand before amputation and
later artificial-hand usage. Specifically, dissociation bet-
ween hands and tools (exemplified by the congruency
effect) depends on the degree of experience with that
hand. Moreover, the representation of prostheses as hands
and tools depends on daily life experience. Given the
relatively limited semantic-category deficit but profoundly
changed body experience resulting from hand loss, we
suggest that the adaptable conceptual relationship
between hands, tools, and prostheses is embodied. Never-
theless, because high-level lexico-semantic processing may
implicitly depend on body representation (Rueschemeyer,
Pfeiffer, & Bekkering, 2010), further studies are necessary
to elucidate the underlying process.
Action Editor
Jamin Halberstadt served as action editor for this article.
Author Contributions
All authors contributed to the development of the study con-
cept. F. M. Z. van den Heiligenberg, N. Yeung, and T. R. Makin
designed the study. Testing and data collection were performed
by F. M. Z. van den Heiligenberg. F. M. Z. van den Heiligenberg
and T. R. Makin analyzed and interpreted the data. All authors
contributed to drafting the manuscript and provided critical
revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manu-
script for submission.
Acknowledgments
We thank our participants and Opcare for their help and Scott
Macdonald for assistance with data collection.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
This work was supported by the Cogito Foundation and by the
Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (Grant No. 104128/Z/14/Z).
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information can be found at http://journals
.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/0956797616685869
Open Practices
All data and materials have been made publicly available via
the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://
osf.io/ck22q/. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this
article can be found at http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/
suppl/10.1177/0956797616685869. This article has received
badges for Open Data and Open Materials. More information
about the Open Practices badges can be found at http://www
.psychologicalscience.org/publications/badges.
References
Boy, F., & Sumner, P. (2010). Tight coupling between posi-
tive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 36, 892–905. doi:10.1037/a0017173
Jang, C. H., Yang, H. S., Yang, H. E., Lee, S. Y., Kwon, J. W.,
Yun, B. D., . . . Jeong, H. W. (2011). A survey on activi-
ties of daily living and occupations of upper extremity
amputees. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine, 35, 907–921.
doi:10.5535/arm.2011.35.6.907
4 van den Heiligenberg et al.
Kikkert, S., Kolasinski, J., Jbabdi, S., Tracey, I., Beckmann,
C. F., Johansen-Berg, H., & Makin, T. R. (2016). Revealing
the neural fingerprints of a missing hand. eLife, 5, Article
e15292. doi:10.7554/eLife.15292
Makin, T. R., Cramer, A. O., Scholz, J., Hahamy, A., Henderson
Slater, D., Tracey, I., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2013).
Deprivation-related and use-dependent plasticity go hand
in hand. eLife, 2, Article e01273. doi:10.7554/eLife.01273
Makin, T. R., Wilf, M., Schwartz, I., & Zohary, E. (2010). Amputees
“neglect” the space near their missing hand. Psychological
Science, 21, 55–57. doi:10.1177/0956797609354739
Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema).
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86. doi:10.1016/j
.tics.2003.12.008
Murray, C. D. (2008). Embodiment and prosthetics. In P. Gallagher,
D. Desmond, & M. MacLachlan (Eds.), Psychoprosthetics (pp.
119–130). London, England: Springer.
Rueschemeyer, S.-A., Pfeiffer, C., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Body
schematics: On the role of the body schema in embodied
lexical–semantic representations. Neuropsychologia, 48,
774–781.
Vainio, L. (2011). Negative stimulus-response compatibility
observed with a briefly displayed image of a hand. Brain
and Cognition, 77, 382–390. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.09.007
Vannuscorps, G., & Caramazza, A. (2016). Typical action
perception and interpretation without motor simulation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 113,
86–91.
... Perhaps the most likely candidates to achieve embodiment are substitution devices, such as artificial limbs (van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017). Artificial limb technologies attempt to increasingly mirror the appearance and function of the human body (Vujaklija et al., 2016), with hopes that greater similarity between the artificial and natural limbs will enable users to achieve ''technological embodiment'' (Makin et al., 2017). ...
... Perhaps the most likely candidates to achieve embodiment are substitution devices, such as artificial limbs (van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017). Artificial limb technologies attempt to increasingly mirror the appearance and function of the human body (Vujaklija et al., 2016), with hopes that greater similarity between the artificial and natural limbs will enable users to achieve ''technological embodiment'' (Makin et al., 2017). Embodiment has been suggested to promote intuitive control, learning, and comfort when using new tools, thus providing unique opportunities to improve the user interface for devices such as artificial limbs (Makin et al., 2017). ...
... Artificial limb technologies attempt to increasingly mirror the appearance and function of the human body (Vujaklija et al., 2016), with hopes that greater similarity between the artificial and natural limbs will enable users to achieve ''technological embodiment'' (Makin et al., 2017). Embodiment has been suggested to promote intuitive control, learning, and comfort when using new tools, thus providing unique opportunities to improve the user interface for devices such as artificial limbs (Makin et al., 2017). However, there is currently little empirical evidence to show that embodiment actually relates to everyday behavior with artificial limbs, let alone that embodiment benefits users (Bekrater-Bodmann, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
When people talk, they move their hands to enhance meaning. Using accelerometry, we measured whether people spontaneously use their artificial limbs (prostheses) to gesture, and whether this behavior relates to everyday prosthesis use and perceived embodiment. Perhaps surprisingly, one- and two-handed participants did not differ in the number of gestures they produced in gesture-facilitating tasks. However, they did differ in their gesture profile. One-handers performed more, and bigger, gesture movements with their intact hand relative to their prosthesis. Importantly, one-handers who gestured more similarly to their two-handed counterparts also used their prosthesis more in everyday life. Although collectively one-handers only marginally agreed that their prosthesis feels like a body part, one-handers who reported they embody their prosthesis also showed greater prosthesis use for communication and daily function. Our findings provide the first empirical link between everyday prosthesis use habits and perceived embodiment and a novel means for implicitly indexing embodiment.
... Prosthesis usage was assessed by combining two, highly correlated measurements of usage: prosthesis wear frequency and a prosthesis activity log (PAL) [29,88]. Participants rated their prosthesis wear frequency on a scale: 0 = never, 1 = rarely, 2 = occasionally, 3 = daily (<4 hours), 4 = daily (4-8 hours), 5 = daily (>8 hours). ...
... We chose to use multiple configurations of hands and prostheses to probe the experience of congenital one-handers and control participants (who mostly see prostheses/hands-of-the-missing-side from a third person perspective, respectively). We note that previous studies probing visual hand representation in similar populations [88], including specifically in OTC [59], used a similar approach. We further note that the few studies finding differences between egocentric/ allocentric [90] or self/others [91] visual hand representation in OTC identified lateralised effects to the right OTC, whereas our effects are comparable across hemispheres. ...
Article
Full-text available
The potential ability of the human brain to represent an artificial limb as a body part (embodiment) has been inspiring engineers, clinicians, and scientists as a means to optimise human–machine interfaces. Using functional MRI (fMRI), we studied whether neural embodiment actually occurs in prosthesis users’ occipitotemporal cortex (OTC). Compared with controls, different prostheses types were visually represented more similarly to each other, relative to hands and tools, indicating the emergence of a dissociated prosthesis categorisation. Greater daily life prosthesis usage correlated positively with greater prosthesis categorisation. Moreover, when comparing prosthesis users’ representation of their own prosthesis to controls’ representation of a similar looking prosthesis, prosthesis users represented their own prosthesis more dissimilarly to hands, challenging current views of visual prosthesis embodiment. Our results reveal a use-dependent neural correlate for wearable technology adoption, demonstrating adaptive use–related plasticity within the OTC. Because these neural correlates were independent of the prostheses’ appearance and control, our findings offer new opportunities for prosthesis design by lifting restrictions imposed by the embodiment theory for artificial limbs.
... Visual information about the prosthesis has direct application for amputees. Indeed, prostheses are categorised differently compared to tools, but also biological limbs, which is reflected in activity in the occipitotemporal cortex (OTC) areas, as well as greater functional coupling between OTC and sensorimotor hand areas (van den J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Heiligenberg et al., 2017). OTC is known to contain distinct visual representations of different object categories, which has direct application to prostheses. ...
Article
Full-text available
Our interaction with the world rests on the knowledge that we are a body in space and time, which can interact with the environment. This awareness is usually referred to as sense of embodiment. For the good part of the past 30 years, the rubber hand illusion (RHI) has been a prime tool to study embodiment in healthy and people with a variety of clinical conditions. In this paper, we provide a critical overview of this research with a focus on the RHI paradigm as a tool to study prothesis embodiment in individuals with amputation. The RHI relies on well-documented multisensory integration mechanisms based on sensory precision, where parietal areas are involved in resolving the visuo-tactile conflict, and premotor areas in updating the conscious bodily representation. This mechanism may be transferable to prosthesis ownership in amputees. We discuss how these results might transfer to technological development of sensorised prostheses, which in turn might progress the acceptability by users.
... Consider for instance a blind person and how she interacts with her cane: studies have demonstrated that the cane is treated as a part of the body (Malafouris, 2008;Maravita and Iriki, 2004). The effect is even more pronounced with artificial limbs (van den Heiligenberg, 2018;van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017), and this was likely true with stone tools as they became integral to the livelihood of prehistoric Homo (Malafouris, 2020;Haggard et al. 2003). ...
Article
Clark and Fischer argue that humans treat social artifacts as depictions. In contrast, theories of distributed cognition suggest that there is no clear line separating artifacts from agents, and artifacts can possess agency. The difference is likely a result of cultural framing. As technology and artificial intelligence grow more sophisticated, the distinction between depiction and agency will blur.
... The use of prosthetic limbs offer a unique insight into the plasticity of body representation, as they bridge the conceptual gap between a limb and a tool (van den Heiligenberg, Yeung, Brugger, Culham, & Makin, 2017). Here, we present the first experiments to examine how the use of an upper-limb prosthesis affects the experience of an object's weight in the context of real and illusory weight differences. ...
Preprint
Humans routinely use tools to extend their repertoire of skilled behavior, using their action capabilities to enhance reach, dexterity, or strength. Little is known, however, about how using a tool affects our experience of object properties, such as heaviness. To better understand how using a tool to manipulate our environment affects perception, participants (n=44) lifted and reported the felt heaviness of objects which varied independently in size and mass. In a blocked repeated-measures design, they lifted with their anatomical hands and with a grasping tool. Contrary to our hypotheses, which was based on earlier work with prosthetic hands (Buckingham et al., 2018), participants experienced real and illusory weight differences just as robustly with a grasping tool as they did with their anatomical hands. These findings add to the growing body of work examining perception tool use and suggest that grasping tools may be incorporated differently into the representation of the body in action than prosthetic hands.
... Prosthetic limb users, of course, represent a group who have a particular type of experience, having actively used a prosthetic limb, possibly embodying it (Murray, 2004;Niedernhuber, Barone, & Lenggenhager, 2018). Indeed, there have been reports that the use of a prosthetic limb fundamentally changes one's haptic experience of object weight (Buckingham et al., 2018), categorization ability (van den Heiligenberg, Yeung, Brugger, Culham, & Makin, 2017), and even visual perception (Nico, Daprati, Rigal, Parsons, & Sirigu, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
The “uncanny phenomenon” describes the feeling of unease associated with seeing an image that is close to appearing human. Prosthetic hands in particular are well known to induce this effect. Little is known, however, about this phenomenon from the viewpoint of prosthesis users. We studied perceptions of eeriness and human-likeness for images of different types of mechanical, cosmetic, and anatomic hands in upper-limb prosthesis users (n=9), lower-limb prosthesis users (n=10), prosthetists (n=16), control participants with no prosthetic training (n=20), and control participants who were trained to use a myoelectric prosthetic hand simulator (n=23). Both the upper- and lower-limb prosthesis user groups showed a reduced uncanny phenomenon (i.e., significantly lower levels of eeriness) for cosmetic prosthetic hands compared to the other groups, with no concomitant reduction in how these stimuli were rated in terms of human-likeness. However, a similar effect was found neither for prosthetists with prolonged visual experience of prosthetic hands nor for the group with short-term training with the simulator. These findings in the prosthesis users therefore seem likely to be related to limb absence or prolonged experience with prostheses.
Article
Full-text available
The study of artificial arms provides a unique opportunity to address long-standing questions on sensorimotor plasticity and development. Learning to use an artificial arm arguably depends on fundamental building blocks of body representation and would therefore be impacted by early-life experience. We tested artificial arm motor-control in two adult populations with upper-limb deficiencies: a congenital group - individuals who were born with a partial arm, and an acquired group - who lost their arm following amputation in adulthood. Brain plasticity research teaches us that the earlier we train to acquire new skills (or use a new technology) the better we benefit from this practice as adults. Instead, we found that although the congenital group started using an artificial arm as toddlers, they produced increased error noise and directional errors when reaching to visual targets, relative to the acquired group who performed similarly to controls. However, the earlier an individual with a congenital limb difference was fitted with an artificial arm, the better their motor control was. Since we found no group differences when reaching without visual feedback, we suggest that the ability to perform efficient visual-based corrective movements is highly dependent on either biological or artificial arm experience at a very young age. Subsequently, opportunities for sensorimotor plasticity become more limited.
Preprint
Full-text available
The study or artificial-arms provides a unique opportunity to address long-standing questions on sensorimotor plasticity and development. Learning to use an artificial-arm arguably depends on fundamental building blocks of body representation and would therefore be impacted by early-life experience. We tested artificial-arm motor-control in two adult populations with upper-limb deficiency: congenital one-handers – who were born with a partial arm, and amputees – who lost their biological arm in adulthood. Although one-hander started using an artificial-arm at an early age, they produced increased error noise and directional errors when reaching to visual targets, relative to amputees who performed similarly to controls. However, the earlier a one-hander was fitted with an artificial-arm the better their motor control was. We suggest that visuomotor integration, underlying the observed deficits, is highly dependent on either biological- or artificial-arm experience at very young age, Subsequently, opportunities for sensorimotor plasticity become more limited.
Preprint
Full-text available
When people talk, they move their hands to enhance meaning. Here we ask whether people spontaneously use their artificial limbs (prostheses) to gesture, and whether prosthesis gesture behaviour relates to everyday prosthesis use and perceived embodiment. One-handed participants with congenital and acquired hand loss and two-handed controls participated in gesture-facilitating tasks, measured using acceleration monitors and further validated with offline video coding. Everyday functional prosthesis use and perceived prosthesis embodiment were assessed using questionnaires. Perhaps surprisingly, one- and two-handed participants did not differ in the amount of gestures they produced. However, they did differ in their gesture profile. One-handers performed more, and bigger, movements with their intact hand while gesturing relative to their prosthesis, whereas two-handers produced more equal movements across hands. Importantly, one-handers who incorporated their prosthesis more into gesturing, that is - produced gestures that were more similar to their two-handed counterparts - also showed more frequent prosthesis use in day-to-day life. Although as a group, one-handers only marginally agreed that their prosthesis feels like a body-part, people reporting positive embodiment also showed great prosthesis habits, both for communication and daily function. We propose that measuring gesture behaviour in prosthesis-users can be used as an implicit and objective clinical tool to monitor and assess successful prosthesis adoption.
Article
Full-text available
The 'embodied cognition' framework proposes that our motor repertoire shapes visual perception and cognition. But recent studies showing normal visual body representation in individuals born without hands challenges the contribution of motor control on visual body representation. Here, we studied hand laterality judgements in three groups with fundamentally different visual and motor hand experiences: two-handed controls, one-handers born without a hand (congenital one-handers) and one-handers with an acquired amputation (amputees). Congenital one-handers, lacking both motor and first-person visual information of their missing hand, diverged in their performance from the other groups, exhibiting more errors for their intact hand and slower reaction-times for challenging hand postures. Amputees, who have lingering non-visual motor control of their missing (phantom) hand, performed the task similarly to controls. Amputees' reaction-times for visual laterality judgements correlated positively with their phantom hand's motor control, such that deteriorated motor control associated with slower visual laterality judgements. Finally, we have implemented a computational simulation to describe how a mechanism that utilises a single hand representation in congenital one-handers as opposed to two in controls, could replicate our empirical results. Together, our findings demonstrate that motor control is a driver in making visual bodily judgments.
Article
Full-text available
The hand area of the primary somatosensory cortex contains detailed finger topography, thought to be shaped and maintained by daily life experience. Here we utilise phantom sensations and ultra high-field neuroimaging to uncover preserved, though latent, representation of amputees' missing hand. We show that representation of the missing hand's individual fingers persists in the primary somatosensory cortex even decades after arm amputation. By demonstrating stable topography despite amputation, our finding questions the extent to which continued sensory input is necessary to maintain organisation in sensory cortex, thereby reopening the question what happens to a cortical territory once its main input is lost. The discovery of persistent digit topography of amputees' missing hand could be exploited for the development of intuitive and fine-grained control of neuroprosthetics, requiring neural signals of individual digits.
Article
Full-text available
Significance How do we perceive and interpret others’ actions? In recent years, the dominant view on this issue is that efficient perceptual processing of others’ actions cannot be achieved by visual analysis of the movements alone but requires unconscious, covert motor simulation of the observed movements. This idea has developed a large following in many disciplines such as psychology, neuroscience, neurology, psychiatry, and philosophy of mind and has started to influence the study and treatment protocols of patients suffering from conditions affecting the perception and interpretation of action (such as autism, schizophrenia, and various dementias). In this paper, however, we report evidence that efficient perception and interpretation of actions can be achieved without motor simulation.
Article
Full-text available
To assess prosthetic use by upper extremity amputees, and their difficulties with prostheses in activities of daily living and occupations. This study is based on a survey of 307 subjects, who were using prostheses manufactured in the Center of Prosthetics and Orthotics. The survey questionnaire included items about general demographic characteristics, side and level of amputation, type of prosthesis and its use, and difficulties in the activities of daily living, employment and driving. The most common type of prosthesis was the cosmetic hand type (80.2%). There were no statistically significant correlations between satisfaction with prosthesis and the amputation level or type of prosthesis. The most common difficulties in daily living activities experienced by amputees were lacing shoes, removing bottle-tops with a bottle opener, and using scissors. Only 7.3% of amputees received rehabilitation services. Less than half of the amputees (44.7%) used their prostheses for eight or more hours a day, and 76.9% used their prostheses for regular or irregular cosmetic purposes. After amputation, most of the respondents (69.0%) became unemployed or changed workplaces. In our study, respondents preferred cosmetic usage to functional usage. Only 30.0% of respondents reported satisfaction with their prostheses. Many of the amputees had difficulties in complex tasks and either changed jobs or became unemployed. Clerical workers were the occupation group, which was most likely to return to work. The development of a more functional prosthetic hand and additional rehabilitation services are required.
Article
Full-text available
When associations between certain visual stimuli and particular actions are learned, those stimuli become capable of automatically and unconsciously activating their associated action plans. Such sensorimotor priming is assumed to be fundamental for efficient responses, and can be reliably measured in masked prime studies even when the primes are not consciously perceived. However, when the delay between prime and target is increased, reversed priming effects are often found instead (the negative compatibility effect, NCE). The main accounts of the NCE assume that it too is a sensorimotor phenomenon, predicting that it should occur only when the initial positive priming phase also occurs. Alternatively, reversed priming may reflect a perceptual process entirely independent from positive motor priming (which is simply evident at a different temporal delay), in which case no dependency is expected between the NCE and positive priming. We tested these predictions while new sensorimotor associations were learned, and found a remarkable symmetry between positive and reversed priming during all such learning phases, supporting the idea that reversed priming is a sensorimotor process that automatically follows the positive priming phase.
Article
Full-text available
What happens in our brain when we use a tool to reach for a distant object? Recent neurophysiological, psychological and neuropsychological research suggests that this extended motor capability is followed by changes in specific neural networks that hold an updated map of body shape and posture (the putative "Body Schema" of classical neurology). These changes are compatible with the notion of the inclusion of tools in the "Body Schema", as if our own effector (e.g. the hand) were elongated to the tip of the tool. In this review we present empirical support for this intriguing idea from both single-neuron recordings in the monkey brain and behavioural performance of normal and brain-damaged humans. These relatively simple neural and behavioural aspects of tool-use shed light on more complex evolutionary and cognitive aspects of body representation and multisensory space coding for action.
Article
Manual responses can be primed by viewing an image of a hand. The left-right identity of the viewed hand reflexively facilitates responses of the hand that corresponds to the identity. Previous research also suggests that when the response activation is triggered by an arrow, which is backward-masked and presented briefly, the activation manifests itself in the negative priming effect. The present study showed that response activation, which is produced by an identity of a briefly presented image of a hand, can be similarly associated with a negative priming effect. However, in contrast to the previously reported negative priming effects, the hand stimuli produced negative priming even when the hand was not backward-masked and did not contain task-relevant information. The study supports the view that the automatic inhibition of motor activation triggered by briefly viewed objects is a general and basic functional principle in exogenous motor control processes.
Article
Words denoting manipulable objects activate sensorimotor brain areas, likely reflecting action experience with the denoted objects. In particular, these sensorimotor lexical representations have been found to reflect the way in which an object is used. In the current paper we present data from two experiments (one behavioral and one neuroimaging) in which we investigate whether body schema information, putatively necessary for interacting with functional objects, is also recruited during lexical processing. To this end, we presented participants with words denoting objects that are typically brought towards or away from the body (e.g., cup or key, respectively). We hypothesized that objects typically brought to a location on the body (e.g., cup) are relatively more reliant on body schema representations, since the final goal location of the cup (i.e., the mouth) is represented primarily through posture and body co-ordinates. In contrast, objects typically brought to a location away from the body (e.g., key) are relatively more dependent on visuo-spatial representations, since the final goal location of the key (i.e., a keyhole) is perceived visually. The behavioral study showed that prior planning of a movement along an axis towards and away from the body facilitates processing of words with a congruent action semantic feature (i.e., preparation of movement towards the body facilitates processing of cup.). In an fMRI study we showed that words denoting objects brought towards the body engage the resources of brain areas involved in the processing information about human bodies (i.e., the extra-striate body area, middle occipital gyrus and inferior parietal lobe) relatively more than words denoting objects typically brought away from the body. The results provide converging evidence that body schema are implicitly activated in processing lexical information.
Deprivation-related and use-dependent plasticity go hand in hand
  • T R Makin
  • A O Cramer
  • J Scholz
  • A Hahamy
  • Henderson Slater
  • D Tracey
  • I Johansen-Berg