ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Introduction. For some time, the issue of participating in high-risk sports, including skydiving, has been linked to the trait of sensation seeking, but skydivers do not constitute a homogeneous group in terms of this factor. The aim of the study was to determine the role of the need for sensation in performing skydiving and to examine whether the importance of this factor differs depending on gender. Material and methods. The study included a total of 143 skydivers (98 men and 45 women) aged from 17 to 49 years with different levels of expertise in skydiving. In total, 73 respondents were categorised as novices, and 70 were considered experts. Novice skydivers were defined as having completed no more than 10 jumps in their lives. Expert skydivers were persons who had made at least 100 jumps in their lives and had a licence to perform skydiving independently, without instructor supervision. The need for stimulation was measured using the Sensation Seeking Scale IV by Zuckerman, in its Polish version by Oleszkiewicz-Zsurzs. Results. Since a high proportion of individuals with a strong need for sensation was found among both men and women, it can be concluded that it is an important factor in primary selection in skydiving (when the sport is undertaken), regardless of gender. Conclusions. The significance of sensation seeking as a factor in secondary selection in skydiving (when the sport is being performed) differs depending on the particular dimension of sensation seeking and gender. Susceptibility to boredom is probably a significant factor in secondary selection in women.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Copyright © 2016 by the Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw, Faculty of Physical Education and Sport in Biała Podlaska
Pol. J. Sport Tourism 2016, 23, 94-98
DOI: 10.1515/pjst-2016-0011 94
SENSATION SEEKING AS ONE OF THE MOTIVATING FACTORS
FOR PERFORMING SKYDIVING
AGNIESZKA BOŁDAK1, MONIKA GUSZKOWSKA2
Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw, Faculty of Tourism and Recreation, Departament
of Tourism1, Faculty of Rehabilitation, Chair of Psycho-social Foundations of Rehabilitation and Bioethics,
Department of Psychology of Rehabilitation and Special Pedagogy2
Mailing address: Agnieszka Bołdak, Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education, 34 Marymoncka Street,
00-968 Warszawa, tel.: +48 605102585, fax: +48 22 8651080, e-mail: agnieszka.boldak@gmail.com
Abstract
Introduction. For some time, the issue of participating in high-risk sports, including skydiving, has been linked to the trait of
sensation seeking, but skydivers do not constitute a homogeneous group in terms of this factor. The aim of the study was to de-
termine the role of the need for sensation in performing skydiving and to examine whether the importance of this factor diers
depending on gender. Material and methods. The study included a total of 143 skydivers (98 men and 45 women) aged from
17 to 49 years with dierent levels of expertise in skydiving. In total, 73 respondents were categorised as novices, and 70 were
considered experts. Novice skydivers were dened as having completed no more than 10 jumps in their lives. Expert skydivers
were persons who had made at least 100 jumps in their lives and had a licence to perform skydiving independently, without
instructor supervision. The need for stimulation was measured using the Sensation Seeking Scale IV by Zuckerman, in its Polish
version by Oleszkiewicz-Zsurzs. Results. Since a high proportion of individuals with a strong need for sensation was found
among both men and women, it can be concluded that it is an important factor in primary selection in skydiving (when the sport
is undertaken), regardless of gender. Conclusions. The signicance of sensation seeking as a factor in secondary selection in
skydiving (when the sport is being performed) diers depending on the particular dimension of sensation seeking and gender.
Susceptibility to boredom is probably a signicant factor in secondary selection in women.
Key words: skydiving, psychology, high risk sports, gender, motivation
Introduction
In recent years, we have observed growing interest in vari-
ous forms of activity that are associated with providing partici-
pants with strong emotions and intense experiences [1, 2]. Prac-
tising some sports carries health risks and even the risk of death.
Such activities are often referred to as extreme sports or high-
risk sports. According to the denition by Breivik, a high-risk
sport is a discipline in which one has to accept the possibility
of serious injury or death as an inherent part of the activity [3].
For some time, the issue of participating in high-risk sports
has been linked to the personality trait of sensation seeking [4,
5, 6]. Zuckerman described sensation seeking as the need for
new, varied, and complex experiences and the readiness to take
physical and social risk in order to meet this need [7]. Accord-
ing to the author, sensation seeking consists of four dimensions.
The rst dimension, identied as thrill and adventure seeking,
describes the need to engage in physical activity that provides
extraordinary physical sensations, including sports such as
skydiving, mountain climbing, and scuba diving. The second
dimension, identied as experience seeking, manifests itself in
the tendency to seek cognitive stimulation by engaging in ac-
tivities that provide intense sensory experiences, including mu-
sic, art, and travel, as well as leading non-conformist lifestyles
and making friends with like-minded unconventional people.
The third dimension, disinhibition, is connected with sensation
seeking through hedonistic attitudes towards life, including
having risky sexual relationships, abusing alcohol, and gam-
bling. Inner or outer constraints do not stop individuals from
engaging in such behaviour. The fourth dimension, susceptibil-
ity to boredom, manifests itself as an aversion to monotonous
activities, as well as restlessness and impatience when a person
is feeling bored.
Sensation seekers accept risk as a possible result of provid-
ing themselves with an optimal level of arousal. They enjoy be-
ing involved in activities and situations that have elements of
novelty and stimulation and satisfy their hedonistic needs [8].
Such people often do not pay attention to whether their behav-
iour is socially approved [7]. However, this does not mean that
sensation seekers always undertake activities that exceed or are
barely within the bounds of laws, social norms, or moral norms.
A satisfactory level of stimulation can be achieved by selecting
highly stimulating lifestyles and social relationships, as well as
highly stimulating professions, such as those of a Special Forces
soldier, mountain rescuer, or re ghter [7, 9, 10]. Sports activity
(skydiving, mountain climbing, scuba diving, paragliding, etc.)
also oers a socially acceptable source of intense sensations [7,
8].
According to many researchers [9, 10, 11, 12], a strong need
for sensation is a distinguishing feature of skydivers. A study by
Bołdak and Guszkowska, however, has found this group to be
heterogeneous in terms of personality traits, including sensa-
tion seeking [13]. It is thus worth investigating whether or not
sensation seeking is a motivating factor for undertaking skydiv-
ing.
Selection may take place already when skydiving is under-
taken. It can be assumed that skydiving training is taken mainly
by people with a strong need for stimulation. In this case, sensa-
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 10/20/16 6:05 PM
Bołdak and Guszkowska: SENSATION SEEKING AS ONE ... 95
Pol. J. Sport Tourism 2016, 23, 94-98
tion seekers should dominate already among novices. The selec-
tion which takes place at the time of undertaking the sport can
be called “primary selection.
Selection can also take place in the later stages of perform-
ing skydiving. This can be called “secondary selection”. People
with a lower need for sensation may discontinue a strongly
stimulating activity due to having exceeded the optimum level
of arousal [14]. According to Breivik, due to the selection present
in risky sports, only individuals whose personalities “match”
a given form of activity will decide to continue the sport [15].
However, research results are not consistent with respect to this
issue. Some authors have suggested that there is a greater need
for stimulation in less experienced persons [16]. Others have
reported that novices have a lower need for sensation: for ex-
ample, expert skydivers scored higher on the scale of sensation
seeking than novices and people who completed the skydiving
course, but then they did not jump with a parachute [9].
According to Zawadzki, selection through temperament
traits in risky sports is stronger in the case of women, as they
are characterised by a lower need for stimulation than men [14].
Sensation seeking is one of the dimensions of temperament. In
the general population, men engage in risky behaviour more of-
ten than women [7, 17]. Women who engage in high-risk sport
have a higher-than-average need for stimulation [7, 18].
Inconsistencies in the results of previous studies raise sev-
eral questions about the importance of sensation seeking in pri-
mary and secondary selection in skydiving, concerning whether
or not the need for sensation is actually a motivating factor in
skydiving, the direction of the relationship, and whether or
not the importance of this trait diers depending on gender. If
primary selection does take place, skydivers should be domi-
nated by people with a strong need for stimulation (regardless
of their level of expertise in skydiving). If the need for sensa-
tion is a factor in secondary selection, sensation seeking should
be signicantly dierent between novice and expert skydivers.
With increasing levels of expertise, the group should become
more and more homogeneous. However, selection in the oppo-
site direction cannot be ruled out. Extreme sensation seekers
may eventually stop practising the discipline due to the fact that
after losing its novelty, it ceases to be suciently stimulating
for them. If sensation seeking is an important factor in primary
and secondary selection only in women, only female skydivers
should manifest a stronger need for sensation in comparison to
the population norms, whereas female novices should be char-
acterised by a smaller need for sensation than female experts.
These dierences should not occur in men.
Material and methods
The study included a total of 143 skydivers (98 men and 45
women) aged from 17 to 49 years (mean = 30.07; SD = 7.10)
with dierent levels of expertise (number of jumps in the entire
group: mean = 333.56; SD = 636.94). The subjects were arbi-
trarily classied into one of the two groups according to their
level of expertise, after consultation with an experienced and
professionally active skydiving instructor from the Aero Club of
Warsaw.
In total, 73 respondents were considered novices, which
means they had done no more than 10 jumps (mean = 5) in their
lives. The group of expert jumpers included individuals having
a valid Certicate of Qualication issued by the President of the
Polish Civil Aviation Authority. According to Polish law, this li-
cence is mandatory in order for a person to be able to perform
skydiving independently, and it requires passing a practical and
theoretical examination. The group of expert skydivers com-
prised 70 participants, who had done at least 100 jumps (mean
= 675) in their lives. There were no statistical age dierences be-
tween the two groups.
The research was conducted in two Polish aviation clubs (in
Warsaw and Bialystok). Participation in the survey was volun-
tary, and the respondents did not receive any remuneration. All
the respondents practised amateur skydiving and did not be-
long to sporting clubs or the national team. The research project
was accepted by the Senate Research Ethics Committee of the
Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw.
The need for stimulation was measured using the Sensation
Seeking Scale (SSS) by Zuckerman [8] (version IV), in its Polish
version [19]. The questionnaire consists of 68 questions that re-
quire choosing between two extremes, for example, “I don’t like
to skydive at all” and “I would love to try skydiving”. The research
tool was adapted to Polish conditions by Oleszkiewicz-Zsurzs
[19, 20]. The Polish version of the SSS-IV scale diers from the
original in that it has a smaller number of items. The theoreti-
cal variables were determined using the following scales: the
General Sensation Seeking (GSS) scale: 0-20; the Thrill and Ad-
venture Seeking (TAS) scale: 0-14; the Experience Seeking (ES)
scale: 0-15; the Disinhibition (Dis) scale: 0-17; and the Boredom
Susceptibility (BS) scale: 0-18. In the original version, the scale
has satisfactory psychometric properties [8]. However, attempts
to adapt the tool to Polish conditions have not been completely
satisfactory; hence, one should exercise caution while interpret-
ing the results. The reliability indices of the scale (internal con-
sistency) varied from 0.77 for the Dis scale to 0.87 for the ES
scale [19]. The diagnostic accuracy of the tool is not satisfying
according to the author of its Polish adaptation [19]. There has
also been an attempt to adapt a newer version of the scale (SSS-
V), but its outcomes were not published, and they are believed
to have been unsatisfactory as well [21]. Therefore, a decision
was taken to use version SSS-IV, which has been frequently used
for research in Poland. Applying the SSS scale made it possi-
ble to make comparisons between the outcomes of the current
study and the outcomes of previous research.
All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical
IBM package SPSS19 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).
Results
In order to determine the intensity of sensation seeking in
the group of skydivers, point scores were converted to the stand-
ard ten (sten) scale, according to the norms for the Polish popu-
lation. The distributions of sten scores at three levels – of low
scores (up to a sten score of 4), average scores (sten scores of
5-6), and high scores (a sten score higher than 7) – are present-
ed in table 1. In women, there was a clear overrepresentation of
high scores on all SSS scales, especially ES, TAS, and Dis (more
than half of the respondents had high scores). As far as men are
concerned, nearly half of the respondents obtained high scores
only on the Dis scale. However, regardless of gender, the pro-
portion of persons with a strong need for sensation was greater
among skydivers than in the general population.
Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used in
order to determine the variation of scores depending on gender
and the level of expertise in skydiving. A signicant main gen-
der eect was found for the scores of the entire scale (F = 3.306;
p = 0.005; η2 = 0.129). The interaction between group (novices
and experts) and gender reached the level of a trend (F = 2.005,
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 10/20/16 6:05 PM
Bołdak and Guszkowska: SENSATION SEEKING AS ONE ...
96 Pol. J. Sport Tourism 2016, 23, 94-98
p = 0.069, η2 = 0.082). The group eect was not statistically
signicant (F = 0.027, p = 0.714, η2 = 0.027). These data suggest
that the level of expertise does not have an impact on sensation
seeking among skydivers, but the importance of this factor may
vary depending on gender.
Adjusted models proved to be signicant in the case of ES
(F = 3.468, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.070), BS (F = 2.831, p = 0.041,
η2 = 0.058), and general sensation seeking GSS (F = 2.719,
p = 0.047, η2 = 0.055).
The analyses of individual dimensions did not prove a sig-
nicant group eect (tab. 2). The eect reached the level of
a trend in ES – slightly higher scores were found among novices.
The eect of gender was signicant only for ES, with higher
scores in women. In the case of BS, there was a signicant inter-
action between group and gender (g. 1). In the group of novices,
women were characterised by higher BS, while in the group of
expert skydivers, there were no gender-related dierences. The
level of expertise signicantly dierentiated BS among women,
and it was higher in novices. There were no dierences in BS
among men. A signicant interaction between gender and the
level of expertise was also observed in the case of general sensa-
tion seeking (GSS) (g. 2), and the direction of the relationship
was the same. There were no signicant eects of gender or the
level of expertise or any interactions between them in the case
of TAS and Dis.
Table 1. Sten scores in general sensation seeking and the dimensions of
sensation seeking for men (n = 98) and women (n = 45)
Variable Low scores Average
scores High scores
Men Women Men Women Men Women
General sensation
seeking (GSS) 13% 4% 58% 58% 29% 38%
Thrill and adventure
seeking (TAS) 8% 7% 55% 33% 37% 60%
Experience seeking
(ES) 11% 7% 47% 24% 42% 69%
Disinhibition (Dis) 13% 2% 39% 47% 48% 51%
Boredom Susceptibility
(BS) 20% 18% 48% 42% 32% 40%
Table 2. Two-way analysis of variance (group x gender) of general sensation seeking and the dimensions of sensation seeking
Variable
Group Gender ANOVA
Novices Experts Men Women Group Gender Interaction
M SD M SD M SD M SD F p η2* F p η2F p η2
General sensation
seeking (GSS) 12.21 3.23 11.77 2.75 11.77 2.84 12.49 3.30 2.67 0.105 0.019 1.30 0.256 0.009 5.66 0.019 0.039
Thrill and adventure
seeking (TAS) 11.33 2.01 10.96 2.47 11.27 2.33 10.89 2.07 0.66 0.419 0.005 0.91 0.340 0.007 0.17 0.677 0.001
Experience seeking
(ES) 8.63 2.90 7.97 2.52 7.92 2.60 9.13 2.86 3.14 0.079 0.022 5.45 0.021 0.038 2.33 0.129 0.016
Disinhibition (DIS) 9.58 3.51 9.19 2.77 9.46 3.15 9.22 3.23 1.38 0.242 0.010 0.30 0.585 0.002 1.56 0.214 0.011
Boredom Susceptibility
(BS) 9.73 3.59 9.46 3.25 9.34 3.35 10.16 3.529 1.76 0.187 0.012 1.32 0.253 0.009 6.50 0.012 0.045
* η2 – strength of relationship.
Figure 1. Interaction between group x gender (Boredom Susceptibility, BS)
Figure 1. Interaction between group x gender
(Boredom Susceptibility, BS)
Figure 2. Interaction between group x gender (General Sensation Seeking, GSS)
Figure 2. Interaction between group x gender
(General Sensation Seeking, GSS)
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 10/20/16 6:05 PM
Bołdak and Guszkowska: SENSATION SEEKING AS ONE ... 97
Pol. J. Sport Tourism 2016, 23, 94-98
Discussion
The purpose of the study was to determine the role of sensa-
tion seeking in performing skydiving and to examine whether
its importance diers depending on gender.
Since a high proportion of persons with a strong need for
sensation in its dierent dimensions was found among both
men and women, it can be concluded that this variable acts as a
factor in primary selection, already at the initial stage of under-
taking the sport. This has been conrmed by previous studies
in which authors found that the trait of sensation seeking is a
signicant factor that determines taking up high-risk sports [3].
However, the importance of the dimensions of sensation seek-
ing varies, and the biggest role is probably played by experience
seeking, especially among women.
The dimensions of thrill and adventure seeking, disinhibi-
tion, and boredom susceptibility probably do not play a great
role in secondary selection (while the sport is being performed)
– no signicant main eects of the level of expertise in skydiv-
ing were found for these factors in our study. This means that
none of these dimensions seems to be related to whether or not
a person chooses to continue practising skydiving. Experience
seeking was the only dimension that diered slightly depending
on the subjects’ level of expertise. The results of the research
carried out by Breivik et al., which suggest that experts’ need for
sensation is stronger than that of novices and those who stop
practising the sport, have not been conrmed either [9]. It is
worth emphasising, though, that the aforementioned studies
included only men.
Experience seeking seems to be a particularly important
motivating factor in women. Compared to men, women practis-
ing skydiving are characterised by a greater inclination to seek
sensations and new experiences by engaging in activities that
provide intense sensory sensation. The direction of gender dif-
ferences in skydivers is, therefore, opposite to the direction in
the general population. The signicance of this factor was also
conrmed by the distribution of sten scores: almost 70% of the
women in the study received high scores. These results are con-
sistent with the ndings of Hromatko and Butković [22], who
found intergender dierences in sensation seeking. Men were
characterised by higher scores in disinhibition, while women
were characterised by higher scores in experience seeking. In
her examination of paragliders, skydivers, scuba divers, and
mountaineers, Blenner found that, compared to young men,
young women (under the age of 30) were characterised by high-
er scores in three dimensions of sensation seeking (all except
disinhibition) [23].
What distinguishes female skydivers is the very strong need
for experience of a mental nature and, therefore, for emotions
as well. However, data on intergender dierences in sensation
seeking in persons who participate in high-risk sports are still
divergent. A study of extreme kayakers and mountaineers did
not nd any dierences between women and men with regard
to the need for stimulation [24, 25, 26].
According to Woodman et al., the strong emotions experi-
enced during a jump may be an eective means to regulate emo-
tions in people with alexithymia [27]. Alexithymia is described
as the inability to understand or identify emotions and to name
and express them, which leads to a lack of discharge of tension
and anxiety [28]. Research has shown that skydivers with alex-
ithymia, women in particular, deliberately become involved in
situations connected with strong arousal and even fear [29, 30].
This is because they lose the ability to feel pleasure in any other
way than through such intense, sometimes negative feelings.
The dimension of boredom susceptibility, which manifests
itself as an aversion to performing monotonous activities and
responding to them with restlessness and impatience, may be
another potentially important factor. This dimension seems to
play a minor role in secondary selection among men: there were
no dierences in this dimension depending on the level of ex-
pertise among the male subjects in the study. In women, on the
other hand, this dimension was a factor in primary selection.
In the group of novices, women reached the same level as men
in terms of boredom susceptibility (which did not happen in
the general population). What is more, women obtained signi-
cantly higher scores. This variable is also likely to be a factor in
secondary selection in women, but working in the opposite di-
rection than expected: women who discontinue skydiving have
the highest levels of boredom susceptibility. In this respect, fe-
male novices were characterised by higher scores than expert
female skydivers. In the group of expert skydivers, intergender
dierences were not statistically signicant. Therefore, this
component of the need for sensation seems to be an important
factor in primary and secondary selection only in women.
It may be assumed that skydiving attracts people seeking
strong sensations and intense emotions. However, it appears
that in women the trait of boredom susceptibility in its ex-
treme intensity may interfere to some extent with the aspects
of skydiving that are associated with thoroughness, conscien-
tiousness, and psychological resistance. People with high scores
in BS quickly reach a state of impatience when they are forced
to perform routine, repetitive tasks that are part of skydiving
training. After the rst stage of fascination, during which many
elements are novel, such persons may discontinue skydiving
and look for new sources of stimulation. Women susceptible to
boredom to a degree similar to that in men (and higher than
that in average women) are able to satisfy their need for stimula-
tion in regular skydiving.
In conclusion, the signicance of sensation seeking in pri-
mary and secondary selection in skydiving diers depending on
the dimension of this trait and gender. The variables connected
with this trait act as primary selection factors in both genders,
but their importance is greater among women. Susceptibility to
boredom is probably a signicant factor in secondary selection
in women, though in our study it was found to work in the op-
posite direction to the one postulated by Breivik [3].
It should be mentioned, however, that the present study has
some limitations. One of them is a relatively small sample size,
especially when it comes to women. Men dominate among sky-
divers in Poland, so the proportion of men and women in the
study reected their proportion in the population. The second
limitation of the study is a certain arbitrariness of classifying par-
ticipants into the group of expert skydivers. In the study, persons
who had completed at least 100 times jumps were considered
expert skydivers. Nowadays, it takes much less time to make 100
jumps than before. These criteria could be changed: for example,
those who have jumped more than 500 times could be consid-
ered experts. It should also be pointed out that the conclusions
about the importance of sensation seeking as a motivating factor
were drawn somewhat indirectly through comparing the scores
of skydivers to population norms, as well as through comparing
novices to experts, but a precise determination of their motivat-
ing role is possible only in longitudinal studies.
The importance of sensation seeking as a motivating factor
proved to vary depending on gender and the component of the
trait. This suggests that further analyses should be conducted
separately for each component. It would be interesting to deter-
mine the signicance of the components of sensation seeking
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 10/20/16 6:05 PM
Bołdak and Guszkowska: SENSATION SEEKING AS ONE ...
98 Pol. J. Sport Tourism 2016, 23, 94-98
in practising other high-risk sports. In particular, monitoring
them over several years would allow for an eective assessment
of the factors inuencing the decision to undertake a sport and
continue or discontinue it. Expanding research beyond investi-
gating the need for sensation seems worthwhile. Recent studies
by Barlow et al. suggest that dierent motives (the sensations of
the activity, emotion regulation, agency functions, etc.) may be
signicant in various disciplines, such as skydiving, mountain-
eering, and ocean rowing [31, 32].
Literature
1. Lyng S. (1990). Edgework: A social psychological analysis of
voluntary risk taking. American Journal of Sociology 95(4),
851-886.
2. Monasterio E., Mulder R., Frampton C., Mei-Dan O.
(2012). Personality characteristics of BASE jumpers. Jour-
nal of Applied Sport Psychology 24(4), 391-400. DOI:
10.1080/10413200.2012.666710.
3. Breivik G. (1995). Personality, sensation seeking and arousal
in high risk sports. Oslo: Norwegian University of Sports.
4. Lafollie D., Le Scan C. (2008). Some clues to sensation
seeking, disinhibition and the practice of risky sports.
Annales Medico Psychologiques 166(10), 794-798. DOI:
10.1016/j.amp.2006.11.005.
5. Ruedl G., Abart M., Ledochowski L., Burtscher M., Kopp
M. (2012). Self reported risk taking and risk compensation
in skiers and snowboarders are associated with sensation
seeking. Accident Analysis and Prevention 48, 292-296.
DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.01.031.
6. Próchniak P. (2013). Adventure seekers in extreme weath-
er. Psychological proles and determinants. Poznań:
Wydawnictwo Naukowe Contact. [in Polish]
7. Zuckerman M. (2006). Sensation seeking and risky behav-
ior. Washington: American Psychological Association.
8. Zuckerman M. (1979). Sensation seeking: beyond the opti-
mal level of arousal. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
9. Breivik G., Roth W.T., Jørgensen P.E. (1998). Personality,
psychological states and heart rate in novice and expert
parachutists. Personality and Individual Dierences 25(2),
365-380. DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00058-0.
10. Goma-i-Freixanet M. (1991). Personality prole of subjects
engaged in high physical risk sports. Personality and Indi-
vidual Dierences 12(10), 1087-1093. DOI: 10.1016/0191-
8869(91)90038-D.
11. Guszkowska M., Bołdak A. (2010). Temperament traits
among men practising high risk sports. Psychologia – Eto-
logia – Genetyka 22, 7-26. [in Polish]
12. Guszkowska M., Bołdak A. (2010). Sensation seeking in
males involved in recreational high risk sports. Biology of
Sport 27(3), 157-162. DOI: 10.5604/20831862.919331.
13. Boldak A., Guszkowska M. (2013). Are skydivers a homogenous
group? Analysis of features of temperament, sensation seek-
ing, and risk taking. The International Journal of Aviation Psy-
chology 23(3), 197-212. DOI: 10.1080/10508414.2013.799342.
14. Zawadzki B. (1991). Temperament: selection or compensa-
tion? In T. Tyszka (ed.), Psychology and sport (pp. 85-112).
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo AWF. [in Polish]
15. Breivik G. (1999). Personality, sensation seeking and risk
taking among top level climbers, parachute jumpers and
white water kayakers. In G. Breivik (ed.), Empirical studies
of risk sport (pp. 8-26). Oslo: Norges Idrettshogskole, Insti-
tutt for Samfunnsfag.
16. Watson A.E., Pulford B.D. (2004). Personality dierences
in high risk sports amateurs and instructors. Perceptual and
Motor Skills 99(1), 83-94. DOI: 10.2466/pms.99.1.83-94.
17. Roth M., Schumacher J., Brähler E. (2005). Sensation seek-
ing in the community: Sex, age and sociodemographic com-
parisons on a representative German population sample.
Personality and Individual Dierences 39(7), 1261-1271. DOI:
10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.003.
18. Kajtna T., Tusak M. (2004). Some psychological studies of
high risk sports. Kinesiologia Slovenica 10, 96-104.
19. Oleszkiewicz-Zsurzs Z. (1985). Polish adaptation of M.
Zuckerman’s Sensation Seeking Scale. Przegląd Psycholog-
iczny 28(4), 1123-1128. [in Polish]
20. Oleszkiewicz Z. (1982). Demand for stimulation and voca-
tional preferences. Polish Psychological Bulletin 13, 185-195.
21. Strelau J. (2006). Psychology of individual dierences.
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar. [in Polish]
22. Hromatko I., Butković A. (2009). Sensation seeking and
spatial ability in athletes: an evolutionary account. Jour-
nal of Human Kinetics 21, 5-13. DOI: 10.2478/v10078-09-
0001-x.
23. Blenner J.L. (1993). Visual evoked potential stimulus in-
tensity modulation and sensation seeking in thrill seekers.
Journal of Personality and Individual Dierences 14(3), 455-
463. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90315-T.
24. Campbell J.B., Tyrrell D.J., Zingaro M. (1993). Sensation
seeking among white water canoe and kayak paddlers. Per-
sonality and Individual Dierences 14(3), 489-491. DOI:
10.1016/0191-8869(93)90319-X.
25. Burnik S., Jug S., Kajtna T. (2008). Sensation seeking in
Slovenian female and male mountain climbers. Acta Uni-
versitatis Palackianae Olomucensis. Gymnica 38(3), 15-19.
26. Llewellyn D.J., Sanchez X. (2008). Individual dierences and
risk taking in rock climbing. Psychology of Sport and Exer-
cise 9(4), 413-426. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.07.003.
27. Woodman T., Huggins M., Le Scan C., Cazenave N.
(2009). Alexithymia determines the anxiety experienced
in skydiving. Journal of Aective Disorders 116(1-2), 134-138.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.11.022.
28. Mikolajczak M., Luminet O. (2006). Is alexithymia aected
by situational stress or is it a stable trait related to emotion
regulation? Personality and Individual Dierences 40(7),
1399-1408. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.020.
29. Cazenave N., Le Scan C., Woodman T. (2007). Psycho-
logical proles and emotional regulation characteristics
of women engaged in risk-taking sports. Anxiety, Stress, &
Coping 20(4), 421-35. DOI: 10.1080/10615800701330176.
30. Woodman T., Cazenave N., Le Scan C. (2008). Skydiving
as emotion regulation: The rise and fall of anxiety is moder-
ated by alexithymia. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology
30(3), 424-433.
31. Woodman T., Hardy L., Barlow M., Le Scan C. (2010). Mo-
tives for participation in prolonged engagement high-risk
sports: An agentic emotion regulation perspective. Psychol-
ogy of Sport and Exercise 11(5), 345-352. DOI: 10.1016/j.
psychsport.2010.04.002.
32. Barlow M., Woodman T., Hardy L. (2013). Great expecta-
tions: Dierent high-risk activities satisfy dierent motives.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105(3), 458-75.
DOI: 10.1037/a0033542.
Submitted: February 4, 2016
Accepted: April 18, 2016
Unauthenticated
Download Date | 10/20/16 6:05 PM
... This would suggest that the different background implies less safety-oriented training and implementation of performance skills. A further explanation could come from the propensity of this category of pilots for the phenomenon of sensation seeking; indeed, this appears to be associated with a lower perception of risk (Zuckerman, 1994) and is shared by those who engage in extreme sporting activities (Allison et al., 2012;Bołdak and Guszkowska, 2016;Dicle et al., 2018), of which ultralight flight is an example (Blenner, 1993;Wagner and Houilhan,1994). A further interesting result was regarding the sex of pilots; in particular, from the correlation analysis it seems that males had higher levels of Self-Confidence in in-flight performance than women. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Aviation psychology is very interested in understanding how personological and psychological variables influence flight performances. Indeed, risk attitudes have been considered as a risk factor for aviation accidents. In this context, emotions and coping style are key variables which could influence concentration by affecting cognition and attention. In addition, the specific training backgrounds seemed to be associated with differences in in-flight accident rates. The aim of the present study was to investigate the association between age, sex, flight experience, emotional dysregulation, coping styles, flight licenses, and pilots’ risk attitudes. Methods Eighty pilots completed an online survey composed of ad hoc questionnaire for sociodemographic and work-related information’s and self-report questionnaires that assessed emotional dysregulation, coping styles, and risk attitudes. Results Results showed that older age and emotional dysregulation were associated with higher risk attitudes in pilots. Moreover, emotional dysregulation seemed to promote worse self-confidence. Ultralight pilots appeared to be more risk-oriented and less self-confident than civil pilots, while more flight experience appeared to favorite greater self-confidence. Discussion In conclusion, the study suggests the importance of promoting interventions based on sharing pilots’ difficulties and emotions and promoting safe attitudes with special attention to ultralight pilots, age, and sex differences.
... It suggests that people who engage in HRS and ES are characterized by higher levels of this feature than individuals who engage in other, safer sports disciplines and those who do not engage in sports at all [6]. However, more recent research suggests that individuals who engage in HRS and ES are not a homogenous group in terms of their temperament [7][8][9]. Kerr and Mackenzie [10] suggest that these discrepancies could be explained by the individual character of different HRS or ES-some activities last for only a few seconds (e.g., bungee jumping), while others may last for months (e.g., mountaineering or sailing solo around the world). Thus, sports in which the performance lasts a few seconds can be considered a source of strong stimulation, while sports in which the action takes much longer are more often associated with the ability to endure discomfort and sometimes even avoid stimulation [11]. ...
Article
Full-text available
(1) Background: In this paper, we attempt to identify which personality and motivational variables significantly influence the sense of quality of life of individuals who practice extreme sports (ES) and high-risk sports (HRS). In addition, we examined to what extent these relationships are moderated by the athletes’ gender and experience in a given discipline. (2) Methods: A total of 363 individuals who practice ES or who practice HRS took part in the study. All of the participants were from Poland. Standardized questionnaires were used to measure the investigated personality traits (extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism), motives, and satisfaction with life, characteristic of the practiced sports. (3) Results: A positive relationship was observed between levels of extroversion and sense of satisfaction with life in the groups of women who engage in HRS (p = 0.045) and men who engage in ES (p = 0.002). The tendency towards addiction was significantly negatively associated with sense of satisfaction with life only in men who engage in ES (p = 0.015). Individuals who engage in ES and HRS did not differ in levels of tendency towards addiction. (4) Conclusions: Individuals who practice ES differ from individuals who practice HRS in terms of personality features, motivations, and the determinants of their sense of satisfaction with life.
Article
Full-text available
Мета. Виявлення індивідуальних психологічних характеристик, які визначають стійкість до стресів у спортсменів екстремальних видів спорту. У цьому дослідженні взяли участь 60 альпіністів Київського відділення Федерації альпінізму і скелелазіння (26 жінок і 34 чоловіки). Було показано, що альпіністи сприймають велике коло ситуацій як загрозливі і реагують на них зростанням тривожності. Для них характерні високі психологічна мінливість і адаптивність до зовнішніх умов, пов’язані з низьким рівнем емоційної стабільності (нейротизм), високою стабільністю і лабільністю. Опір стресу посилюється за рахунок високої самооцінки спортсменів. Це дозволяє зробити висновок про те, що стресостійкість спортсменів екстремальних видів спорту визначається особистими детермінантами, високою психологічною мінливістю і пристосованістю до зовнішніх умов
Article
Full-text available
Los participantes en deportes extremos usualmente han sido estudiados desde la perspectiva de riesgo. El presente estudio busca alejarse de dicha preconcepción y tiene como objetivo estudiar la experiencia subjetiva emocional. Se empleó una metodología cualitativa con diseño fenomenológico-hermenéutico tomando como herramienta sistemas conversacionales. Los informantes fueron 8 deportistas extremos pertenecientes a un mismo grupo. Dentro de los deportes extremos se incluyen: Escalada de montaña, rapel, puenting, montañismo y bungee jumping. Los resultados indican que el miedo es generador de libertad, puede ser útil, es gratificante y también es promotor de transformaciones personales. Esta investigación otorga valor teórico al porqué del involucramiento en deportes extremos. Participants in extreme sports have usually been studied from a risk perspective. The present study seeks to move away from this preconception and aims to study the emotional subjective experience. A qualitative methodology with phenomenological-hermeneutic design was used taking as a tool conversational systems. The informants were 8 extreme athletes belonging to the same group. Extreme sports included were: Mountain climbing, rappelling, bungee jumping, and mountaineering.The results indicate that fear is a generator of freedom, it can be useful, it is rewarding and it is also a promoter of personal transformations. This research gives theoretical value to the reason for involvement in extreme sports. Participantes em esportes radicais geralmente têm sido estudados a partir de uma perspectiva de risco. O presente estudo busca afastar-se desse preconceito e visa estudar a experiência emocional subjetiva. Uma metodologia qualitativa com desenho fenomenológico-hermenêutico foi utilizada, utilizando sistemas conversacionais como ferramenta. Os informantes eram 8 atletas extremos pertencentes ao mesmo grupo. Esportes radicais incluem: alpinismo, rapel, bungee jumping, montanhismo e bungee jumping. Os resultados indicam que o medo é um gerador de liberdade, pode ser útil, gratificante e também promotor de transformações pessoais. Esta pesquisa dá valor teórico ao motivo do envolvimento em esportes radicais.
Article
Full-text available
This work analyses the perception of self-efficacy (PA) in paragliding pilots, as one of the most relevant variables involved in the risk assumption that comes with practising this sport. The sample consists of 197 pilots (age: M 41.76 years ± 9.9), 19 women and 178 men. A paragliding-specific self-efficacy scale was developed, following Bandura's indications (2006), and the effects of variables such as the pilot's sex, experience level, competition level and injuries sustained were analysed. The results show that paragliding pilots have a high PA (more so men than women), especially in relation to their decision-making capabilities. PA increases with experience and is greatest in pilots at high levels of competition. PA is not associated with pilot accidents or injuries.
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Este trabajo analiza la percepción de autoeficacia (PA) en pilotos de parapente, como una de las variables más relevantes implicadas en la asunción del riesgo que conlleva la práctica de este deporte. La muestra la componen 197 pilotos (edad: M 41.76 años ± 9.9), 19 mujeres y 178 hombres. Se elaboró una escala de autoeficacia específica del dominio del parapente, siguiendo las indicaciones de Bandura (2006), y se analizaron los efectos de variables como el sexo del piloto, el nivel de experiencia, el nivel de competición y las lesiones sufridas. Los resultados nos muestran que los pilotos de parapente tienen una PA elevada (más los hombres que las mujeres), sobre todo en relación con sus capacidades de toma de decisión. La PA aumenta con la experiencia y es mayor en los pilotos de niveles altos de competición. La PA no se asocia a los accidentes o lesiones de los pilotos.
Article
Full-text available
Athletes engaged in high-risk sports (skydiving, hang-gliding, paragliding, microlighting, and rock climbing)will confront significantly higher levels of stress. The purpose of our study is to define the individual psychological characteristics that determine stress resistance inherent in climbers-sportsmenfacing high risks. 60 climbers from the Kyiv branch of the Federation of Mountaineering and Climbing and the climbing section of the National Aviation University (26 women and 34 men, aged 18 to 30 years old, M age = 24, SD = 1.57) are involved in this study. The researchers have used the following tests: Cattell's Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire,Eysenck Personality Inventory, Diagnostics of Stress-Resistance Level ('Forecast'), the "Personality Differential" technique. High and average levels of stress resistance were detected in 42 rock climbers (70%) while most adults involved in non-extreme sports had the average levels of stress resistance. It is shown that climbers perceive a large number of situations as threatening and provoking the anxiety (the self-preservation instinct). High psychological variability and adaptability to external conditions are associated with low levels of neuroticism, high stability, and lability. Increasing the self-esteem of athletes boosts stress resistance. These research results allow us to conclude that the structure of stress resistance of athletes facing high risksis balanced and defined by one's systemic volitional capabilities (strength and stability), self-control behavior (self-esteem) and features of the nervous system which are characterized by susceptibility to external events (neuroticism).
Article
Full-text available
Skydiving, a so-called risky sport, has become a more and more accessible and popular form of recreation. The question arises whether it attracts people of common psychological constitution. The aim of the study described in this article was to determine whether parachutists are a homogenous group in the sense of temperament features, need for stimulation, and risk. The research involved 143 parachutists (98 men and 45 women) between 17 and 49 years old, including 73 beginners with not more than 10 jumps and 70 advanced parachutists who had at least 100 jumps and qualifications to practice skydiving without the supervision of an instructor. In the research, the following questionnaires were applied: Formal Characteristics of Behavior–The Temperament Inventory of Zawadzki and Strelau (1997), Sensation Seeking Scale of Zuckerman (1979), the Multifactorial Scale of Risky Behaviors of Studenski (2006), State–Trait Anxiety Inventory of Spielberger et al. (1983), and the University of Wales Institute of Science and Technology Mood Adjective Check List of Matthews, Chamberlain, and Jones (1990). Cluster analysis identified 3 groups of parachutists, which we label risk avoiders, sensation seekers, and moderates. These 3 groups differ significantly in terms of all diagnosed constant psychical properties, mood, and the level of anxiety before and after a jump.Parachutists are a heterogeneous group, differing significantly in terms of the temperament features needed for sensation and risk taking. This suggests that their motives for engagement in the activity connected with risk are different. Recognition of these features in the process of training and subsequent individualization of training could limit the risk of accidents during parachute jumps.
Article
Full-text available
BASE jumping is an extreme sport, which is associated with significant risk of injury and death. We aimed to determine whether BASE jumpers have different personality characteristics to an age-matched, normal control population. An international population of BASE jumpers was examined using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). Mean age was thirty-four years; 59 (87%) jumpers were male; 28 (41%) jumpers had sustained a significant injury. A substantial proportion of the BASE jumper presented extremely low scores in the temperament measure of Harm Avoidance (p < 0.001); the extent of which has not been reported in any other population.
Article
While there seems to be general agreement among members of contemporary American society about the value of reducing threats to individual well-being, there are may who actively seek experiences that involve a high potential for personal injury or death. High-risk sports such as hang gliding, skydiving, scuba diving, rock climbing, and the like have enjoyed unprecedented growth in the past several decades even as political institutions in Western societies have sought to reduce the risks of injury in the workplace and elsewhere. The contradiction between the public agenda to reduce the risk of injury and death and the private agenda to increase such risks deserves th attention of sociologists. A literature review is presented that points to a number of shortcomings in existing studies, most of which are associated with the psychological reductionism that predominates in this area of study. An effort is made to provide a sociological account of voluntary risk taking by (1) introducing a new classifying concept- edgework-based on numerous themes emerging from primary and secondary data on risk taking and (2) explaining edgework in terms of the newly emerging social psychological perspective produced from the synthesis of the Marxian and Meadian frameworks. The concept of edgework highlights the most sociologically relevant features of voluntary risk taking, while the connections between various aspects of risk-taking behaviour and structural characteristic of modern American society at both the micro and macro levels. This approach ties together such factors as political economic variables, at one end of the continuum, and individual sensations and feelings, at the other end.
Article
Thirty-four male and 20 female members of two canoe clubs completed Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale, Form V. Respondents also rated their paddling level, highest difficulty level of the rivers they paddled with regularity, highest difficulty level of the rivers they would like to paddle, and anxiety level prior to put-in. As predicted, male and female paddlers had significantly higher Thrill and Adventure Seeking scores than the comparable normative scores. Thrill and Adventure Seeking scores correlated negatively with anxiety level, and their correlation with difficulty level of the rivers paddlers would like to attempt approached significance. Similar results were obtained when questionnaire responses were dichotomized and analyses of variance were conducted with sensation seeking scores as dependent variables.
Article
La recherche de sensations est à l’origine de comportements variés. Le parachutiste, l’artiste, le drogué ou le criminel auraient le même besoin de stimulations. Existe-t-il cependant des parachutistes toxicomanes ? La recherche de sensations est-elle assouvie d’une seule ou de plusieurs façons ? Une étude de la personnalité des individus permet d’apporter un élément de réponse à cette question en comprenant le but de cette recherche de sensations. Les « déprimés » réguleraient leur affectivité négative avec la consommation de substances ; au contraire les « hédonistes » extravertis rechercheraient un plaisir varié dans différents domaines (sports à risque, désinhibition) afin de conforter leur affectivité positive. Enfin, les « compensateurs » aventuriers chercheraient une valorisation uniquement dans des sports à risque.