ArticlePDF Available

The Early Indicators of Functional Decrease in Mild Cognitive Impairment

Authors:
  • Centre universitaire de formation et de recherche Jean-François Champollion, Albi, France
  • Inserm U1093 Cognition Action and Sensorimotor Plasticity, Dijon, France

Abstract and Figures

Abstract: Background: Postural activities involved in balance control integrate the anticipatory postural adjustments (APA) that stabilizen balance and posture, facilitating arm movements and walking initiation and allowing an optimal coordination between posture and movement. Several studies reported the significant benefits of virtual reality (VR) exercises in frail older adults to decrease the anxiety of falling and to induce improvements in behavioural and cognitive abilities in rehabilitation processes. The aim of this study was thus to test the efficiency of a VR system on the enhancement of the APA period, compared to the use of a Nintendo Wii system. Methods: Frail older adults (n = 37) were included in this study who were randomized and divided into a VR exercises group (VR group) or a control group using the Nintendo Wii system (CTRL group). Finally, 22 patients were included in the data treatment. APA were studied through muscular activation timings measured with electromyographic activities. The functional reach test, the gait speed, and the time up and go were also evaluated before and after a 3-week training phase. Results and discussion: As the main results, the training phase with VR improved the APA and the functional reach test score along the antero-posterior axis. Together, these results highlight the ability of a VR training phase to induce neuromuscular adaptations during the APA period in frail older adults. Then, it underlines the effective transfer from learning carried out during the VR training movements to control balance abilities in a more daily life context. Keywords: cognitive and motor deficits; postural control; rehabilitation exercise; virtual reality
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 12 August 2016
doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00193
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Edited by:
Milica S. Prostran,
University of Belgrade, Serbia
Reviewed by:
Matthieu P. Boisgontier,
Katholieke Universiteit (KU) Leuven,
Belgium
Ramesh Kandimalla,
Emory University, USA
*Correspondence:
Alexandre Kubicki
alexandre.kubicki@u-bourgogne.fr
Received: 14 June 2016
Accepted: 29 July 2016
Published: 12 August 2016
Citation:
Kubicki A, Fautrelle L, Bourrelier J,
Rouaud O and Mourey F (2016) The
Early Indicators of Functional
Decrease in Mild Cognitive
Impairment.
Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:193.
doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00193
The Early Indicators of Functional
Decrease in Mild Cognitive
Impairment
Alexandre Kubicki 1, 2, 3*, Lilian Fautrelle 1, 4, 5, Julien Bourrelier 1, 2 , Olivier Rouaud1, 6 and
France Mourey 1, 7
1Unité 1093, Cognition, Action et Plasticité Sensorimotrice, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Dijon,
France, 2Université de Bourgogne Franche Comté, Unité de Formation et de Recherche (UFR) Sciences et Techniques des
Activités Physiques et Sportives (STAPS), Dijon, France, 3Institut de Formation aux Métiers de la Santé (IFMS), Nord
Franche-Comté, Hôpital Nord Franche-Comté, Montbéliard, France, 4Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense, Unité de
Formation et de Recherche (UFR) Sciences et Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives (STAPS), Nanterre, France,
5Centre de Recherche sur le Sport et le Mouvement, CeRSM, Unité de Formation et de Recherche (UFR) Sciences et
Techniques des Activités Physiques et Sportives (STAPS), Nanterre, France, 6Centre Mémoire Ressources et Recherche,
Centres Hospitaliers Universitaires (CHU), Dijon-Bourgogne, Dijon, France, 7Unité de Formation et de Recherche (UFR),
Santé, Université de Bourgogne Franche Comté, Dijon, France
Objectives: Motor deficiency is associated with cognitive frailty in patients with Mild
Cognitive Impairments (MCI). In this study we aimed to test the integrity in muscle
synergies involved in an arm-pointing movement in functionally unimpaired MCI patients.
We hypothesized that early motor indicators exist in this population at a preclinical level.
Methods: Electromyographic signals were collected for 11 muscles in 3 groups: Young
Adults (YA), Older Adults (OA), and MCI patients. The OA and MCI groups presented
the same functional status. Each subject performed 20 arm-pointing movements from a
standing position.
Results: The main differences were (1) an earlier activation of the left Obliquus internus
in MCI compared with OA group, (2) an earlier activation for the MCI compared with both
OA and YA. The temporal differences in muscle synergies between MCI and OA groups
were linked with executive functions of MCI patients, assessed by the trail making test.
Moreover, the results show a delayed activation of the right Biceps Femoris and the right
Erector Spinae at l3 in MCI and OA compared with YA.
Interpretation: The motor program changes highlighted in our patient MCI group
suggest that discrete modifications of the motor command seem to exist even in the
absence of functional impairment. Instead of showing an indication of delayed muscle
activation in the MCI patients, our results highlight some early activation of several trunk
muscles.
Keywords: Mild Cognitive Impairments, muscle synergy, Anticipatory Postural Adjustments, motor control,
cognitive functions
BACKGROUND
Mild Cognitive Impairments (MCI) is an intermediate stage between typical age-related cognitive
changes and dementia. There is growing evidence in the literature about the association of
sensory-motor dysfunction with cognitive frailty in aging (Albers et al., 2015). Several research
teams focused on the broad correlation between motor and cognitive capacities in aging.
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
In patients with MCI, there is a solid body of evidence supporting
the motor-related changes. These studies have reported some
gait and balance deficiencies (Boyle et al., 2005; Aggarwal et al.,
2006; Verghese et al., 2008; Gras et al., 2015), which become even
more significant in dual-task conditions (Montero-Odasso et al.,
2014), and a greater fall rate in this population (Doi et al., 2014).
Interestingly, a value of the gait speed test that predicts evolution
toward cognitive frailty has been also highlighted (Buracchio
et al., 2010).
These studies assessed overall motor functions (i.e., functional
abilities) in MCI patients. Motor function involves the peripheral
musculoskeletal system (information capture and torque
production), the peripheral nervous system (information
conduction) and the central nervous system (CNS; motor
planning and programming) (Caffarra et al., 2010; Cisek and
Kalaska, 2010). As the symptoms of MCI are mainly cognitive,
and thus related to central modifications, it could be interesting
to identify potential impairments affecting this central part of
motor behavior in MCI patients.
To assess the integrity of this central part of motor control,
researchers have usually focused on the preparatory period,
which includes motor program processes, and well-known as
Anticipatory Postural Adjustments (APA; Belen’kii et al., 1967;
Massion, 1992; Desmurget and Grafton, 2000; Maloney and
Mamassian, 2009). Indeed, our CNS has the ability to coordinate
posture and movement efficiently, especially by means of feed-
forward processes that allow the anticipated recruitment of
several muscles before the beginning of any self-generated
perturbation of our balanced-system (Massion, 1992). These APA
are mainly involved in maintaining the integrity of the balance
function and can be challenged in several situations (Horak,
2006). Actually, anticipatory activations of trunk muscles, such as
transversus abdominis and multifidus, were found to be delayed
in chronic low-back-pain patients (Hodges and Richardson,
1996). In the same vein, acute and experimentally-induced
pain seems to shift the anticipation activations from a deep
(transversus abdominis and internal obliquus) to a superficial
muscular layer (external obliquus) (Moseley and Hodges, 2005).
Normal aging also challenges these APA (Man’kovskii et al.,
1980; Inglin and Woollacott, 1988; Rogers et al., 1992; Woollacott
and Manchester, 1993; Bleuse et al., 2006; Kanekar and Aruin,
2014), with greater severity in frail elderly adults (Kubicki et al.,
2012a). Neurological disorders such as stroke or Parkinson’s
disease also impair the ability of the CNS to produce efficient APA
(Latash et al., 1995; Garland et al., 1997; Elble and Leffler, 2000).
Nonetheless, there are very few studies about APA in
cognitively impaired patients in the literature. Elble and Leffler
reported that postural and focal reaction times were slower
in Alzheimer Disease (AD) patients, but without specific
impairment in the postural preparatory period, than in age-
matched controls without AD (Elble and Leffler, 2000). To our
knowledge, there is no information in the literature about APA
in MCI patients.
This study is warranted by the lack of data about this
important question, especially when considering the link between
APA and balance function (Robinovitch et al., 2013). Our aim
was to test APA integrity in MCI patients, without functional
impairments, during an arm raising task. Our hypothesis was
that MCI patients would have delayed anticipation activations
compared with age-matched controls, but that the global synergy
of muscle activations would be respected. To better understand
these potential changes, and to take into account the APA
impairment highlighted in normal aging, we included a group of
young adults (YA) in our experiment.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Participants (n=42) were divided into 3 groups: MCI (Mild
Cognitive Impairment group); OA (Older Adults group), and YA
(Young Adults group). All the participants were right handed
as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield,
1971).
MCI subjects (n=14) were included at the Memory,
Resources and Research Centre (CMRR) of Dijon University
Hospital, France, during their annual medical consultation.
Inclusion criteria were (1) a recent diagnosis of amnestic MCI
according to the criteria of NIA-OA (Albert et al., 2011), (2) an
absence of a diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease,
(3) to be aged 60 years or over, (4) no other neurological disease,
such as Parkinsonism syndrome or pyramidal deficiency, (5) no
musculoskeletal deficiency that caused pain, balance deficit or
restricted function. To be more precise, the Alzheimer’s disease
and the Parkinsonism syndrome were clinically diagnosed by an
experienced neurologist. The Alzheimer’s disease was diagnosed
following the “recommendations from the National Institute
on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic
guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease” (Albert et al., 2011), and
the Parkinsonism syndrome following the recommendations
from the “MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s
disease” (Postuma et al., 2015). One more time, both were
retained as exclusion criteria. These subjects were recruited
during their annual medical consultation, during which
they provided their written consent to participate in the
study.
MCI was diagnosed by an experienced neurologist (OR) by
means of the criteria of NIA-OA (Albert et al., 2011). A few
patients underwent associated tests, such as MRI T2 flair to detect
hyper-signals (Wahlund et al., 2001). Their neuropsychological
assessment included the following tests for the executive and
attentional functions: the Trail Making Test (TMT A and TMT
B) (Reitan, 1958; Corrigan and Hinkeldey, 1987; Gaudino et al.,
1995; Lezak et al., 2004) and the Digit Span (Forward and
Backward). The Free Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT)
and the Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS 48) were done to
assess their memory functions. At inclusion, the mean scores
were 4.85 (Digit Span Forward); 3.71 (Digit Span Backward);
46.81 s (TMT A); 128 s (TMT B); 18.6 (FCSRT RL, on 48); 34.5
(FCSRT RT, on 48); 92.8 % (DMS 48, Set 1); 90.7 (DMS 48, Set 2).
OA subjects (n=14) were volunteers who had no
previous experience as subjects in experimental research. These
individuals had no medical diagnosis of or self-assessed cognitive
deficiency, such as memory loss or executive problems, and met
the same (3-4-5) inclusion criteria as MCI patients (see above).
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
YA subjects (n=14) were recruited at the Sport Sciences
Department of the University of Dijon (Burgundy University,
France). They were recruited with respect to the (4-5) inclusion
criteria, they had no medical diagnosis of or self-assessed
cognitive deficiency, and were aged between 20 and 35 years.
This work has been carried out in accordance with The Code
of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of
Helsinki) for experiments involving humans. The privacy rights
of human subjects have been observed at all times.
Experimental Device and Procedure
As a prerequisite, the gait speed test was done at the beginning
of the session to test the functional ability of each patient. All
participants performed a short warm-up of the shoulder muscles
of 1 min duration consisting of global circumduction movements
prior to the start of the experiment. They were instructed to copy
the same movements of the experimenter who performed these
movements in front of the participants.
The starting position was the following: Participants stood
upright on the floor (feet were oriented at 15on both sides of
the sagittal plane, with 15 cm between the two medial malleoli),
the left arm was positioned in alignment with the trunk and the
right index finger pointing toward the ground, with an angle of
35between the arm and trunk (Figure 1). The subjects were
required to keep their eyes fixed on a horizontal bar placed at
2 m from the floor and 2.5 m from the participants’ feet. Two
diodes, 120 cm apart, were fixed on this horizontal bar. The
central point between the two diodes was situated exactly in front
of the participants’ right shoulder. Participants were told to point
their index finger at the diode (left or right) which was switched
on intermittently.
Participants were unaware of the location (left or right) of
the visual stimuli. They were asked to raise their arm as fast as
possible and to start as quickly as possible after the appearance
of the visual stimuli. This complex reaction time task, with a
relative uncertainty about the target location (and thus the motor
program to initiate), made it possible to generate greater APA
than a simple reaction time task (Slijper et al., 2009; Kubicki et al.,
2012b). They were told to point to the diode, to keep their arm
raised for a few seconds, and then lower their arm and move their
index finger back to the initial starting position. The participants
performed 20 pointing trials. All the included participants were
able to understand these instructions, to complete the warm-up
sequence and the 20 reaching movements.
Participant Equipment
Two systems were used to accurately measure (i) the movements
performed and (ii) timing of muscle activations during these
movements.
(i) Right upper limb kinematics were recorded using the
Vicon R
system (Oxford metrics group, UK). Participants
wore 5 motion sensors on the following anatomical sites: nail
of the index finger; dorsal aspect of the scaphoid bone; lateral
aspect of the elbow (lateral epicondyle); anterior aspect of the
shoulder (acromion).
FIGURE 1 | Experimental arm raising task. View of the experimental
set-up for the arm raising task showing a participant in initial position and the
two possible targets. The central point of the bar between the two targets was
situated exactly in front of the participants’ right shoulder. Participants were
asked to point their index finger at the target (left or right) which was lit
intermittently.
(ii) Surface Electromyographic (EMG) activity of 11 muscles was
collected. According to the EMG literature concerning the
detection of APA (Ng et al., 1998; Moseley and Hodges,
2005), we focused our measurements on the lower limbs
and trunk muscles in reference to the main muscle involved
in arm-pointing: the Anterior Deltoid (Crenna and Frigo,
1991). See Figure 4C displaying the electrodes placement.
EMG was recorded on both sides of each subject (right =r,
left =l) for the rectus femoris (RF), biceps femoris (BF), obliquus
internus (OI), erector spinae at the third lumbar vertebra (ESl3),
erector spinae at the seventh dorsal vertebra (ESd7), and on
the right side for the anterior portion of the deltoidus (DA).
To determine the positioning of the surface electrodes, the
participants were instructed how to selectively activate each
recorded muscle individually (Hislop and Montgomery, 2009).
These electrodes were placed parallel to the muscle fibers with an
interelectrode distance of 2.4 cm.
Data Recording and Statistical Analysis
Gait speed (m.s1) was collected on a 4.5 m distance according
to Fried et al. (2001). All EMG signals were preamplified at the
source (VICON R
, Oxford Metrics Group) and were recorded at
a frequency of 2000 Hz. Raw EMG signals were first bandpass
filtered between 5 and 400 Hz and then full-wave rectified and
filtered using a no-lag averaging moving-window algorithm
(window size: 10 ms). As previously defined by Fautrelle et al.
(2010), in this study, we determined the initiation time as the
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
delay between illumination of the diode (the “go-signal”) and
the beginning of significant muscle activation (Fautrelle et al.,
2010). To do this for the 11 recorded muscles, the EMG values
after the illumination of the diode and the EMG baselines
were compared for each value using t-tests for each muscle in
each group. The EMG baselines were computed as the mean
integrated activity of each muscle from 2 to 1 s before the
first diode was lit and when the participants maintained the
initial position. The first instant at which the P-value was lower
than 0.05 for a minimum duration of 50 ms determined the
beginning of muscle activation necessary to perform the pointing
movements.
We chose to calculate the timing of each muscle activation
with reference to activation of the Anterior Deltoid and we
named this parameter the activation timing. Thus, the activation
timing of muscles involving in the APA had negative values
and positive values refer to post-deltoid contractions. This EMG
synergy allowed us to clearly identify the muscles involved
principally in APA in each group, without confounding factors
associated with the electro-mechanical delays (Zhou et al.,
1995). A last aim of this study was to investigate some
potential relationships between muscle activation timings (i.e.,
physiological parameters from motor control) and the results of
clinical tests in MCI group. In this way, the “absolute difference
score” (ADS) in milliseconds (ms) were calculated between
MCI and OA participants for the muscles showing significant
differences in their activation timings between MCI and OA
groups only. Finally, a cumulative score of these differences
(Total ADS) was calculated by summing the 2 ADS scores in
absolute value.
Statistical Analysis
Concerning activation timings, in order to determine potential
differences between groups, we first checked that each variable
was normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk W-test, all the p-values
are >0.1) and had equivalent variances (Levene’s test, all the p-
values are >0.2). Outliers detected using extreme studentized
deviate tests (ESD tests; Rosner, 1983) were removed. For more
precision, these outliers were due to rare and furtive loss of
contact between EMG sensors and elderly frail skin (24/280 for
MCI, 8/280 for OA groups) or excessive sudation (30/280 for
YA group, with a maximum of 7/20 for one participant due to
excessive sudation). Then a one-way ANOVA was conducted in
each recording muscle while the experimental group (x3: YA,
OA, MCI) remained the categorical factor (i.e. the independent
variable).
Concerning muscular activation rates which were reported
in percentage for each participant, Shapiro–Wilk W-tests
showed non-normal (but log-normal) distributions for the 10
tested muscles. Consequently, transformations were performed
(Bartlett, 1947) before investigate the potential differences in the
muscular activation rates using 10 different one-way ANOVA,
similarly to the analyses of the activation timings.
Post-hoc analyses were done with Scheffe tests when necessary.
For all these statistical treatments, the significance level was set
at p<0.05. Moreover, according to Cohen (Bartlett, 1947), the
effect size was specified by the partial eta squared (ηp2), and a
value 0.14 was considered as a large effect, 0.6 as a medium
effect, and 0.1 as a small effect (Cohen, 1988; Sink and Stroh,
2006).
Concerning some potential relationships between muscle
activation timings and the results of well-known clinical tests
in MCI group, a multiple regression model was applied to
the Total ADS (as a dependent variable) with the following
explanatory (independent) variables: Mini-Mental State score
(MMS), Walhund score, Gait speed score, Part A and Part B of
the Trail Making Test scores (TMT A and TMT B). According
to Cohen (1988) and Sink and Stroh (2006), the effect size of
the amount of variance accounted for was specified by the R
squared (R2) score. A R20.14 was considered as a large
effect.
RESULTS
Participants Characteristics
Participants’ characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Women
accounted for 35.71, 64.28, and 57.14% in the YA, OA, and MCI
groups, respectively. The MCI and OA groups were no different
for age (p=0.353) and maximal velocity of the index movement
(p=0.069). Consistent with previous data showing the slow-
down of movement in aging (Ketcham et al., 2002), there was a
significant difference between aged participants of both the MCI
and OA groups and the YA group concerning the index maximal
velocity [F(2,728) =165.91, p<0.001, ηp2=0.89]. Moreover, the
functional status of the MCI and OA were equivalent, as attested
by the comparable Gait Velocity (p=0.448).
Finally, The ANOVA showed a main Group effect for
AD (anterior deltoid muscle) reaction time [F(2,728) =9.367,
TABLE 1 | Patient characteristics (Mean ±Standard Deviation) for each group: Young Adults (YA); Older Adults(OA); Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI).
Parameter YA group OA group MCI group p-value for HY/OA p-value for OA/MCI p-value for HY/MCI
Age (Years) 28.72 ±5.65 70.62 ±4.14 70.15 ±7.17 <0.001 0.353 <0.001
Height (cm) 177 ±3.67 166 ±4.82 165 ±4.23 0.112 0.487 0.214
Weight (Kg) 71 ±5.48 79 ±6.91 76 ±7.33 0.081 0.192 0.154
Gait Speed (m.s1) 1.11 ±0.05 0.94 ±0.11 0.91 ±0.13 <0.001 0.448 <0.001
Index MV (m.s1) 6.781 ±1.58 4.889 ±1.04 4.67 ±1.46 <0.001 0.069 <0.001
p-values are presented for between-group differences. LDS fisher-tests (post-hoc) were done on the Group effects showed by the ANOVA (see Material and Methods section for further
details). “Index MV” means Index Maximal Velocity.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
p<0.001, ηp2=0.91]. The post-hoc decomposition showed that
AD reaction time was significantly shorter in the YA group than
in both the MCI and OA groups (p<0.001) but there was no
difference between the OA and MCI groups (p=0.103).
Muscle Activation Timing between Groups
To clarify the results and highlight the potential differences
between groups, muscle synergies were studied, with data for YA
subjects, considered optimal, as the reference. Figure 2 shows the
raw EMG data obtained for 20 trials for a typical subject of each
group.
Figure 3 shows the scatter of the overall dataset of the timing
of muscle activations (all the trials of all participants in the three
experimental groups).
In the YA group, 3 muscles participated in the APA in the
following order: lESl3, lESd7, and rBF.
The ANOVA showed a main Group effect for several muscles:
lESl3 [F(2,693) =14.92, p<0.001, ηp2=0.15]; rBF [F(2,647) =
4.226, p=0.015, ηp2=0.14]; rRF [F(2,266) =3.7367, p=0.025,
ηp2=0.23]; rESl3 [F(2,383) =9.2164, p<0.001, ηp2=0.22]; lOI
[F(2,459) =6.8433, p=0.0012, ηp2=0.44].
Comparing the timing of muscle activation between YA and
OA groups, we found 1 significant difference: the activation
timing of the rBF (p=0.008), was delayed for the OA group
compared with the YA group.
Interestingly, the post-hoc analysis revealed earlier activations
of the lESl3 (p<0.001) and the lOI (p<0.05) in MCI compared
with OA group. This chronological difference was also significant
between MCI and YA groups for the lESl3 (p=0.019).
Logically, activation timings of the MCI group were
significantly delayed compared with the YA group for the rBF (p
=0.014), the lRF (p=0.027), and rESl3 (p=0.028).
All the average muscle activations, with the ANOVA main
effect and the post-hoc analysis between the three groups are
presented on the Figure 4A.
Muscle Activation Rates between Groups
To determine the robustness of muscle synergy objectively, the
activation rate for each muscle in each group was calculated. For
each muscle, this rate corresponded to the percentage of trials
showing significant muscle activation. Differences were found for
only two muscles. The rESl3 [F(2,9489) =13.51, p<0.0001, ηp2=
0.92] and the rESD7 [F(2,2422) =7.59, p=0.001, ηp2=0.89]
were less frequently activated in the MCI group compared with
the OA group (p<0.001 and p=0.002 respectively) and the YA
group (p<0.001 and p=0.001 respectively). These results are
shown in Figure 4B.
Muscle Activation Timing and Clinical Data
in the MCI Group
The ADS was calculated to highlight potential links between
muscle activation timings and clinical data in MCI patients, in
milliseconds (ms), between MCI and OA subjects, for each of
the 2 muscles showing significant differences in their activation
timing (see Section Muscle Activation Timing between Groups
and Figure 4A): the lESl3 and the lOI. Please note here that the
decision was made to exclude the lRF muscle from this analysis,
as its timing was also modified in the MCI group compared to the
OA group, because this muscle activation presented an important
variability (see on Figure 3 lRF’ graph, and Figure 4A) and a
poor activation rate (see on Figure 4B). A cumulative score of
these differences (Total ADS) was calculated by summing the
2 ADS scores in absolute value. A multiple regression model
was applied to this Total ADS (as a dependent variable) with
the following explanatory (independent) variables: Mini-Mental
State score (MMS), Walhund score, Gait speed score, Part A and
Part B of the Trail Making Test scores (TMT A and TMT B).
The result of this complementary analysis is that the variance
explained by the model was R2=0.819, with only one significant
explanatory variable: The TMT A (Beta =1.08; p=0.03). To
represent this relationship graphically, the Total ADS was plotted
with the TMT A scores for all patients of the MCI group (see
Figure 5).
DISCUSSION
The aim of this study was to measure differences in muscle
synergies used by MCI patients during an arm-raising task,
especially in the preparatory period of postural control (APA)
associated with arm movement. In young adults (YA group),
our results showed -EMG- APA sequences originating from
3 muscles (among the 10 postural muscles we assessed): two
contralateral trunk muscles (lESl3 and lESD7) and one ipsilateral
lower limb muscles (rBF), which was in accordance with the
main literature about APA organization associated with arm
movements (Cordo and Nashner, 1982; Friedli et al., 1984;
Ketcham et al., 2002; Bonnetblanc et al., 2004; Bouisset and Do,
2008).
Effects of Aging on Muscle Synergy in Arm
Raising
Our results clearly showed a delay of ipsilateral Biceps Femoris
(right) in the older adults compared with the YA. This significant
difference probably reflect the first impairment of APA in
these active aged adults. This impairment of motor prediction
mechanisms has been highlighted in the literature through
several studies using EMG signals (Man’kovskii et al., 1980;
Inglin and Woollacott, 1988; Rogers et al., 1992; Woollacott and
Manchester, 1993; Bleuse et al., 2006; Kanekar and Aruin, 2014).
In these studies, the authors showed several delays in the timing
of muscles that participate in APA: postural muscles closer to
the muscle directly involved in the movement were recruited.
The worst case was described by Woollacott and Manchester
(1993). In this study, postural muscle activation occurred after
the arm movement had begun. Indeed, the authors showed a clear
impairment of the APA produced by contralateral erector spinae
muscles in normal aged adults, and a decreased activation rate
for the ipsilateral quadriceps muscles. By contrast, in our study
concerning the same aged population (69.53 ±3.12 years), we
only found a delay for one muscles (rBF), and no impairment
in the recruitment rate of any muscle compared with our young
subjects. It is essential to point out that in our study and contrary
to the Woollacott study, the participants were instructed to point
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
FIGURE 2 | EMG activities of the 11 recorded muscles (line) for one typical participant in each group (column), from left to right: Young Adults (YA);
Older Adults(OA); Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). Each graph represents the EMG signals of every trials from the illumination of the diode (t=0) until 1 s. The
EMG activities (V) were bandpass filtered between 5 and 400 Hz and then full-wave rectified. For the sake of clarity in this figure, EMG signals were normalized by the
maximum EMG value (V) for each muscle and for each participant and were represented in arbitrary units (AU).
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
FIGURE 3 | Scatter plot of the activation timings raw data for all the trials of every participant in the three experimental groups. These activation timings
(ms) were computed for each tested muscle with reference to the anterior deltoid activation. The timing of muscles involving in the APA had negative values and
positive values refer to post-deltoid contractions. In every graph, the activation timings of the young adults (YA) were reported in the left column (diamond markers),
the older adults (OA) in the middle column (cross markers), and the mild cognitive impairment group (MCI) in the right column (circle markers).
to the target as fast as possible; consequently, the older adults
pointed more slowly than the YA, the APA differences were
thus not attributable to aging processes alone, but also to the
differences of inertial forces associated with these self-generated
perturbations.
Effects of MCI on Muscle Synergy in Arm
Raising
This study highlighted two main results about muscle synergy in
arm raising in MCI patients.
First, our results show clearly that the muscle recruitment
in MCI subjects is no more delayed than those of the subjects
belonging to the OA group. On the contrary, two muscle
activations are programmed even earlier: the lESl3, which is
the first muscle involved in the APA in the YA subjects, and
the lOI, which was one of the last muscles activated in both
YA and OA groups. Concerning the lESl3, it is interesting
to note that this muscle recruitment, corresponding to the
first muscle anticipation, occurred earlier in our patient group
compared with both the control subjects participating to this
study (YA and OA). The lOI muscle, mainly involved in trunk
stability, is also recruited early by the MCI patients, compared
with the OA subjects. We have to recall here that the only
difference between these two groups was the presence (MCI)
or the lack (OA) of cognitive dysfunction, mainly involving
memory loss (amnestic MCI). The maximal velocity of the
arm movement was no different between these two groups.
Consequently, these results indicate that cognitive symptoms of
MCI patients are accompanied by fine motor changes mainly
expressed by an earlier activation of two trunk muscles. We
interpreted these results as follows: superficial trunk muscles are
probably recruited earlier to increase the postural steadiness of
the trunk and therefore to be able to compensate for the balance
perturbation caused by the arm movement. This kind of trunk
compensation has already been highlighted in a study by Morris
and Allison or by Moseley and Hodges for low-back-pain patients
(Moseley and Hodges, 2005; Morris and Allison, 2006), and could
be interpreted as compensatory behavior of over-protection, as
an over-estimation of the self-generated perturbation that will
be induced by the movement. Our results are in accordance
with these studies. In the same manner, MCI patients seem to
adopt a more cautious mode of motor control, adapting their
feedforward control to better stabilize the locomotor system.
Secondly, it is important to note that MCI patients presented
the same activation rates as the other groups for most muscles
studied in our task (see Section Muscle Activation Rates
between Groups and Figure 4B), except for the rESl3 and
rESD7. Therefore, these two muscles activated after the deltoid
contraction, were also less frequently activated. Concerning the
rESl3, the OA and MCI subjects are in late compared with the YA
group. However, only the MCI subjects show a less robust use of
this muscle, maybe by adaptation of their early activation of lESl3.
Except for these two muscles, the robustness of muscle synergy
seemed to be preserved in MCI patients throughout all the trials.
These small modifications in the motor command robustness are
in accordance with the lack of motor function impairment in
this sample of patients, shown by identical gait speeds and index
velocities in the two groups of aged adults.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
FIGURE 4 | (A) Muscle synergy used in the pointing movement. From left to right: Young Adults (YA); Older Adults (OA); Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). On the
y-axis, muscles were represented from the bottom up in the chronological order of their activations in the YA group. On the x-axis, timing of muscle activations (ms)
were represented with reference to that of the Anterior Deltoid (Mean ±Standard Deviation). The ANOVA results were displayed on the left (ns =p>0.05; *p<0.05).
Post-hoc results were displayed (when necessary) from left to right for the YA/OA; the OA/MCI and the MCI/YA analysis (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001). (B)
Muscle activation rates: The percentages of trials with significant EMG activation were represented with box plots including median and quartiles, in the same layout
than above. The ANOVA results were displayed on the left (ns =p>0.05; *p<0.05). Post-hoc results were displayed (when necessary) from left to right for the
YA/OA; the OA/MCI and the MCI/YA analysis (*p<0.05). (C) For the sake of clarity, the muscle abbreviations, names and locations were reported here.
Taken together, these results strongly suggest that overall
motor prediction ability is not impaired in MCI patients.
Rather, the anticipated motor command is modified
toward an early recruitment of trunk muscles, probably
aimed to increase its steadiness in a more cautious motor
behavior.
Interestingly, the multiple regression model applied to the
Total ADS (see Section Muscle Activation Timing and Clinical
Data in the MCI Group), representing the muscular timing
differences between OA and MCI patients, highlighted a
relationship with the Trail Making Test, part A (TMT A). In
other words, the higher the cumulative differences in muscle
recruitment in absolute value, the higher the TMT A score. An
increased TMT A score reflects poor executive functions, mainly
in the processing speed of information (Reitan, 1958; Corrigan
and Hinkeldey, 1987; Gaudino et al., 1995; Lezak et al., 2004), and
have to be assessed in MCI patients since an early impairment of
these functions have been highlighted in AD patients (Amieva
et al., 2008). Consequently, our results indicated that TMT A
score reflects both an aspect of the cognitive independency
(processing speed of information) and an aspect of the physical
independency: the ability to accurately coordinate posture and
movement during self-generated perturbations.
This work presents several limits. Indeed, the three groups did
not point with the same maximal velocity. Both the OA and the
MCI subjects pointed more slowly than YA subjects. However,
we have to consider two main aspects: First, this difference did
not rule out comparisons of APA sequences between OA and
MCI subjects. The YA subjects were mainly studied to validate
the optimal command organization in light of previous work.
Secondly, one may wonder whether or not it may have been
more interesting to collect different set pointing speeds from our
participants in order to avoid this velocity difference. However,
voluntarily slowing the reaching movement could also modify the
associated APA sequence (Krakauer and Shadmehr, 2007). With
the instruction used in this study (“Please point to the illuminated
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
FIGURE 5 | Relationship between the cumulative Absolute Difference
Score (Total ADS, x-axis, ms) and the Trail Making Test score, part A
(TMT A score, y-axis, s) for each MCI patient. The Pearson and associated
p-value were displayed at the top of the graph. Black line represented the
regression line, and dotted lines represented the 95% confident interval.
diode as fast as possible”), the movement was probably more
ecological.
Moreover, it is important to note a potential confounding
factor associated with subject gender in our results. Indeed, the
male/female ratio was not the same in the three groups, with a
male preponderance in the YA group. However, if a gender bias
exists, it did not concern the comparison between the OA and
the MCI participants, but only the comparison between the YHA
group and the two groups of aged adults.
Given these various elements, we can speculate that the
changes in the motor program could precede the beginning of
the motor-function impairment highlighted in the literature for
the MCI population. These early motor indicators could be tested
as others biomarkers in order to better predict the evolution of
the dementia (Petersen et al., 2013). Further studies will be done
to explore this hypothesis. In this vein, the included MCI patients
will be followed longitudinally, in order to answer, at least in part,
this interesting question.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
AK, Conception and design, Collection and assembly of data,
Analysis and interpretation of the data, Drafting of the article,
Final approval of the article, Critical revision of the article
for important intellectual content, Statistical expertise. LF,
Conception and design, Collection and assembly of data, Analysis
and interpretation of the data, Drafting of the article, Final
approval of the article, Critical revision of the article for
important intellectual content, Statistical expertise. JB, Collection
and assembly of data, Critical revision of the article for important
intellectual content, Final approval of the article. OR, Provision
of study materials or patients, Critical revision of the article for
important intellectual content, Final approval of the article. FM,
Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of the data,
Critical revision of the article for important intellectual content,
Administrative, technical, or logistic support, Final approval of
the article.
FUNDING
This work was supported by the ANR MAAMI project (Maladie
d’Alzheimer et Apprentissage Moteur Implicite). Research
National Agency (ANR), France.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Clare Doyle, Geoffroy
Petrement, Philip Bastable, and Sophie Garnier-Carronnier for
their precious help.
REFERENCES
Aggarwal, N. T., Wilson, R. S., Beck, T. L., Bienias, J. L., and Bennett,
D. A. (2006). Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the
risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 12, 1763–1769. doi:
10.1001/archneur.63.12.1763
Albers, M. W., Gilmore, G. C., Kaye, J., Murphy, C., Wingfield, A.,
Bennett, D. A., et al. (2015). At the interface of sensory and motor
dysfunctions and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers. Dement. 1, 70–98. doi:
10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.514
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox,
N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to
Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-
Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s
disease. Alzheimers Dement. 3, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
Amieva, H., Le Goff, M., Millet, X., Orgogozo, J. M., Pérès, K., Barberger-Gateau,
P., et al. (2008). Prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: successive emergence of the
clinical symptoms. Ann. Neurol. 5, 492–498. doi: 10.1002/ana.21509
Bartlett, M. S. (1947). The use of transformations. Biometrics 3, 39–52. doi:
10.2307/3001536
Belen’kii, V. E., Gurfinkel,’ V. S., and Pal’tsev, E. I. (1967). Control elements of
voluntary movements. Biofizika 12, 135–141.
Bleuse, S., Cassim, F., Blatt, J. L., Derambure, P., Guieu, J. D., and Defebvre, L.
(2006). Effect of age on anticipatory postural adjustments in unilateral arm
movement. Gait Posture 24, 203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.09.001
Bonnetblanc, F., Martin, O., and Teasdale, N. (2004). Pointing to a target
from an upright standing position: anticipatory postural adjustments are
modulated by the size of the target in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 3, 181–184. doi:
10.1016/j.neulet.2004.01.020
Bouisset, S., and Do, M. C. (2008). Posture, dynamic stability, and voluntary
movement. Neurophysiol. Clin. 6, 345–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2008.10.001
Boyle, P. A., Wilson, R. S., Aggarwal, N. T., Arvanitakis, Z., Kelly, J., Bienias,
J. L., et al. (2005). Parkinsonian signs in subjects with mild cognitive
impairment. Neurology 12, 1901–1906. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000188878.
81385.73
Buracchio, T., Dodge, H. H., Howieson, D., Wasserman, D., and Kaye, J. (2010).
The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol.
8, 980–986. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.159
Caffarra, P., Gardini, S., Vezzadini, G., Bromiley, A., and Venner, A. (2010).
The ideation of movement is supported by fronto-temporal cortical
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
regions involved in the retrieval of semantic knowledge. Acta Biomed. 1,
21–29.
Cisek, P., and Kalaska, J. F. (2010). Neural mechanisms for interacting with
a world full of action choices. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 269–298. doi:
10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavior Sciences, 2nd Edn.,
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cordo, P. J., and Nashner, L. M. (1982). Properties of postural adjustments
associated with rapid arm movements. J. Neurophysiol. 2, 287–302.
Corrigan, J. D., and Hinkeldey, M. S. (1987). Relationships between parts A and B
of the Trail Making Test. J. Clin. Psychol. 4, 402–409.
Crenna, P., and Frigo, C. (1991). A motor program for the initiation of
forward-oriented movements in humans. J. Physio. 437, 635–653. doi:
10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018616
Desmurget, M., and Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modeling allows feedback
control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 423–431. doi:
10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01537-0
Doi, T., Shimada, H., Park, H., Makizako, H., Tsutsumimoto, K., Uemura, K., et al.
(2014). Cognitive function and falling among older adults with mild cognitive
impairment and slow gait. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 8, 1073–1078.
Elble, R. J., and Leffler, K. (2000). Pushing and pulling with the upper extremities
while standing: the effects of mild Alzheimer dementia and Parkinson’s disease.
Mov. Disord. 2, 255–268.
Fautrelle, L., Prablanc, C., Berret, B., Ballay, Y., and Bonnetblanc, F. (2010).
Pointing to double-step visual stimuli from a standing position: very
short latency (express) corrections are observed in upper and lower limbs
and may not require cortical involvement. Neuroscience 2, 697–705. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.014
Fried, L. P., Tangen, C. M., Walston, J., Newman, A. B., Hirsch, C., Gottdiener, J.,
et al. (2001). Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J. Gerontol. A
Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 3, 146–156. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146
Friedli, W. G., Hallett, M., and Simon, S. R. (1984). Postural adjustments associated
with rapid voluntary arm movements 1. Electromyographic data. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 6, 611–622. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.47.6.611
Garland, S. J., Stevenson, T. J., and Ivanova, T. (1997). Postural responses to
unilateral arm perturbation in young, elderly, and hemiplegic subjects. Arch.
Phys. Med. Rehabil. 10, 1072–1077. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90130-1
Gaudino, E. A., Geisler, M. W., and Squires, N. K. (1995). Construct validity in
the Trail Making Test: what makes Part B harder? J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 4,
529–535. doi: 10.1080/01688639508405143
Gras, L. Z., Kanaan, S., McDowd, J., Colgrove, Y. M., Burns, J., and Pohl, P. S.
(2015). Balance and gait of adults with very Mild Alzheimer Disease. J. Geriatr.
Phys. Ther. 1, 1–7. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000020
Hislop, H., and Montgomery, J. (2009). Le Bilan Musculaire de Daniels et
Worthingham: Techniques de Testing Manuel, 9th Edn., Los Angeles, CA:
Masson.
Hodges, P. W., and Richardson, C. A. (1996). Inefficient muscular stabilization of
the lumbar spine associated with low back pain. A motor control evaluation
of transversus abdominis. Spine 22, 2640–2650. doi: 10.1097/00007632-
199611150-00014
Horak, F. B. (2006). Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to
know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing 2, ii7–ii11.
doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl077
Inglin, B., and Woollacott, M. (1988). Age-related changes in anticipatory postural
adjustments associated with arm movements. J. Gerontol. 43, M105–M113. doi:
10.1093/geronj/43.4.m105
Kanekar, N., and Aruin, A. S. (2014). The effect of aging on anticipatory postural
control. Exp. Brain Res. 4, 1127–1136. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3822-3
Ketcham, C. J., Seidler, R. D., Van Gemmert, A. W., and Stelmach, G. E. (2002).
Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size,
and movementamplitude. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 1, 54–64. doi:
10.1093/geronb/57.1.P54
Krakauer, J. W., and Shadmehr, R. (2007). Towards a computational
neuropsychology of action. Prog. Brain Res. 165, 383–394. doi:
10.1016/S0079-6123(06)65024-3
Kubicki, A., Bonnetblanc, F., Petrement, G., Ballay, Y., and Mourey, F.
(2012a). Delayed postural control during self-generated perturbations in
the frail older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 7, 65–75. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S
28352
Kubicki, A., Petrement, G., Bonnetblanc, F., Ballay, Y., and Mourey, F. (2012b).
Practice-related improvements in postural control during rapid arm movement
in older adults: a preliminary study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2, 196–203.
doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr148
Latash, M. L., Aruin, A. S., Neyman, I., Nicholas, J. J., and Shapiro, M. B. (1995).
Feedforward postural adjustments in a simple two-joint synergy in patients
with Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2, 77–89. doi:
10.1016/0924-980X(94)00272-9
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., and Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological
Assessment. 4th Edn., New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Maloney, L. T., and Mamassian, P. (2009). Bayesian decision theory as a model of
human visual perception: testing Bayesian transfer. Vis. Neurosci. 1, 147–155.
doi: 10.1017/s0952523808080905
Man’kovskii, N. B., Mints, A. Y., and Lysenyuk, V. P. (1980). Regulation of the
preparatory period for complex voluntary movement in old and extreme old
age. Hum. Physiol. 6, 46–50.
Massion, J. (1992). Movement, posture and equilibrium: interaction and
coordination. Prog Neurobiol. 1, 35–56. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)
90034-C
Montero-Odasso, M., Oteng-Amoako, A., Speechley, M., Gopaul, K., Beauchet, O.,
Annweiler, C., et al. (2014). The motor signature of mild cognitive impairment:
results from the gait and brain study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 11,
1415–1421. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu155
Morris, S. L., and Allison, G. T. (2006). Effects of abdominal muscle fatigue on
anticipatory postural adjustments associated with arm raising. Gait Posture 3,
342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.10.011
Moseley, G. L., and Hodges, P. W. (2005). Are the changes in postural control
associated with low back pain caused by pain interference? Clin. J. Pain 4,
323–329. doi: 10.1097/01.ajp.0000131414.84596.99
Ng, J. K., Kippers, V., and Richardson, C. A. (1998). Muscle fibre orientation
of abdominal muscles and suggested surface EMG electrode positions.
Electromyogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1, 51–58.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the
Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)
90067-4
Petersen, R. C., Aisen, P., Boeve, B. F., Geda, Y. E., Ivnik, R. J., Knopman, D. S., et al.
(2013). Mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer disease in the community.
Ann. Neurol. 2, 199–208.
Postuma, R. B., Berg, D., Stern, M., Poewe, W., Olanow, C. W., Oertel, W., et al.
(2015). MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord.
30, 1591–1601. doi: 10.1002/mds.26424
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making test as an indicator of
organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.
8.3.271
Robinovitch, S. N., Feldman, F., Yang, Y., Schonnop, R., Leung, P. M., Sarraf,
T., et al. (2013). Video capture of the circumstances of falls in elderly people
residing in long-term care: an observational study. Lancet 9860, 47–54. doi:
10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61263-X
Rogers, M. W., Kukulka, C. G., and Soderberg, G. L. (1992). Age-related
changes in postural responses preceding rapid self-paced and reaction
time arm movements. J. Gerontol. 5, M159–M165. doi: 10.1093/geronj/
47.5.m159
Rosner, B. (1983). Percentage Points for a Generalized ESD Many-Outlier
Procedure, Technometrics. 25, 165–172. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1983.104
87848
Sink, C. A., and Stroh, H. R. (2006). Practical significance: the use of effect
sizes in school counseling research. Prof. Sch. Couns 9, 401–411. doi:
10.5330/prsc.9.4.283746k664204023
Slijper, H., Richter, J., Over, E., Smeets, J., and Frens, M. (2009). Statistics predict
kinematics of hand movements during everyday activity. J. Mot. Behav. 41, 3–9.
doi: 10.1080/00222895.2009.10125922
Verghese, J., Robbins, M., Holtzer, R., Zimmerman, M., Wang, C., Xue, X.,
et al. (2008). Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes.
J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 7, 1244–1251. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.
01758.x
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 10 August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
Kubicki et al. Discrete Motor Changes in MCI Patients
Wahlund, L. O., Barkhof, F., Fazekas, F., Bronge, L., Augustin, M., Sjögren,
M., et al. (2001). European task force on age-related white matter changes.
European task force on age related white matter changes. A new rating scale
for age-related white matter changes applicable to MRI and CT. Stroke 6,
1318–1322. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1318
Woollacott, M. H., and Manchester, D. L. (1993). Anticipatory postural
adjustments in older adults: are changes in response characteristics due to
changes in strategy? J. Gerontol. 2, M64–M70. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.2.M64
Zhou, S., Lawson, D. L., Morrison, W. E., and Fairweather, I. (1995).
Electromechanical delay in isometric muscle contractions evoked by voluntary,
reflex and electrical stimulation. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 2,
138–145. doi: 10.1007/BF00361541
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research
was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial
relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of
interest.
Copyright © 2016 Kubicki, Fautrelle, Bourrelier, Rouaud and Mourey. This is an
open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted,
provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.
No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 11 August 2016 | Volume 8 | Article 193
... Motor behavior may be a potential biomarker that addresses these needs, as complex upper-limb movements have been associated with AD severity [9][10][11]. Recent work has demonstrated that a rapid, easy-toadminister upper-limb motor task involving adaptive fine motor skill can predict disease progression [12] and is more sensitive to cognitive status than other simple motor assessments [13] while requiring no computer hardware/software. ...
... The task is feasible for amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) cohorts to perform [12,14], and with repeated exposure can show better, more consistent performance (low variability) that suggests learning [14][15][16][17]. This is in contrast to other motor tasks that require technology (e.g., movement sensors [11], motion capture [10], electromyography [9], or transcranial magnetic stimulation [18]) and often show a ceiling effect. Given the task's association with disease progression and cognitive status, this short report tested the upper-limb motor task's relationship with hippocampal volume across the AD spectrum (i.e., cognitively unimpaired, aMCI, and mild AD). ...
... Although several complex upper-limb tasks have been shown to be sensitive to disease severity [9,11], this task is among the first to associate motor behavior with an AD biomarker. This work highlights the value of evaluating multiple trials of a motor task, rather than a "one-and-done" approach in which a single attempt could mask relevant differences, which is consistent with extensive work showing the clinical utility of cognitive practice effects [31][32][33][34][35]. Furthermore, these findings are consistent with behavioral data linking practice effects on this motor task with visuospatial scores [16,36,37], suggesting a potential mechanism underlying the relationship to hippocampal volume shown here. ...
Article
Full-text available
Hippocampal atrophy is a widely used biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease (AD), but the cost, time, and contraindications associated with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) limit its use. Recent work has shown that a low-cost upper extremity motor task has potential in identifying AD risk. Fifty-four older adults (15 cognitively unimpaired, 24 amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and 15 AD) completed six motor task trials and a structural MRI. Several measures of motor task performance significantly predicted bilateral hippocampal volume, controlling for age, sex, education, and memory. Thus, this motor task may be an affordable, non-invasive screen for AD risk and progression.
... Motor behavior may be a potential biomarker that addresses these needs, as complex upper-limb movements have been associated with AD severity [9][10][11]. Recent work has demonstrated that a rapid, easy-to-administer upper-limb motor task involving adaptive fine motor skill can predict disease progression [12] and is more sensitive to cognitive status than other simple motor assessments [13] while requiring no computer hardware/software. ...
... It is feasible for amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI) cohorts to perform [12,14], and with repeated exposure it can show within-session practice effects (i.e., motor task acquisition) that indicate intact learning ability [14][15][16] (consistent with [17]). This is in contrast to other motor tasks that require technology (e.g., movement sensors [11], motion capture [10], electromyography [9], or transcranial magnetic stimulation [18]) and often show a ceiling effect. ...
... Although several complex upper-limb tasks have been shown to be sensitive to disease severity [9,11], this is among the first to associate motor behavior with an AD biomarker. This work highlights the value of evaluating multiple trials of a motor task, rather than a "one-anddone" approach in which a single attempt could mask relevant differences. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Hippocampal atrophy is a widely used biomarker for Alzheimer's disease (AD), but the cost, time, and contraindications associated with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) limit its use. Recent work has shown that a low-cost upper extremity motor task has potential in identifying AD risk. Fifty-four older adults (15 cognitively unimpaired, 24 amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment, and 15 AD) completed six motor task trials and a structural MRI. Motor task acquisition significantly predicted bilateral hippocampal volume, controlling for age, sex, education, and memory. Thus, this motor task may be an affordable, non-invasive screen for AD risk and progression.
... L'altération des fonctions exécutives serait le paramètre le plus fortement associé au risque de chute (94) , mais il existerait aussi un lien avec les capacités d'attention ou le score de QI verbal (95) . Récemment, un lien a aussi été suggéré entre les capacités de planification du mouvement et les capacités cognitives (96,97) . L'étude de Stöckel et al. montre que les capacités réduites de planification chez des individus âgés peuvent en partie être expliquées par un déclin des capacités cognitives, en particulier la rapidité des processus cognitifs et la flexibilité cognitive (96) . ...
... Récemment, un lien a aussi été suggéré entre les capacités de planification du mouvement et les capacités cognitives (96,97) . L'étude de Stöckel et al. montre que les capacités réduites de planification chez des individus âgés peuvent en partie être expliquées par un déclin des capacités cognitives, en particulier la rapidité des processus cognitifs et la flexibilité cognitive (96) . ...
... L'étude de Yan et al., portant sur des mouvements de pointage dans le plan horizontal, suggère des capacités de planification amoindries par rapport aux sujets jeunes (110) . Plus récemment, des études montrent que l'activité motrice anticipatoire est altérée chez la personne âgée (111) , d'autant plus lorsqu'un trouble cognitif est présent (96,97) . Cependant, jusqu'à récemment (112) , aucune étude ne s'est encore spécifiquement intéressée aux effets du vieillissement sur la planification de mouvements adaptés à l'environnement gravitaire. ...
Article
La chute est une problématique de santé majeure de la population âgée. Sur un plan mécanistique, la gravité est la seule responsable de la chute des corps sur Terre. L’être humain, au cours de son évolution, a appris à percevoir et à utiliser la force gravitaire pour son déplacement et son orientation. Dans cette revue, nous tentons d’établir un lien entre l’adaptation à l’environnement gravitaire et le phénomène de chute chez la personne âgée. Dans un premier temps, nous passons en revue les paradigmes d’évaluation et les mécanismes de perception de la gravité. En particulier, nous nous attardons sur le rôle des systèmes visuel, vestibulaire et somesthésique. Nous évoquons ensuite les modifications sensorimotrices survenant avec l’âge et susceptibles d’altérer ces mécanismes de perception. Le concept de désadaptation à l’environnement gravitaire peut constituer un cadre théorique intéressant dans la compréhension du phénomène de chute lié au vieillissement et pourrait, à terme, éclaircir la prévention et la prise en charge de ce phénomène. Fall risk, a major health concern in the aging population, is inherently linked to gravity. Humans, through evolution, have learned to perceive gravity in order to successfully orientate and move their bodies. Here, we attempt to link gravity-related sensorimotor control to fall phenomenon in older people. We review evaluation paradigms of gravity perception, as well as physiological mechanisms. Specifically, we focus on the role of vestibular, visual and somesthetic systems. We discuss age-related sensorimotor modifications that are prone to alter perception processes. We propose that the concept of adaptation to gravitational environment could provide an interesting theoretical framework for the understanding of age-related falls and eventually clarify prevention and management of this phenomenon.
... For example, previous studies have shown that motor planning adapts reaching trajectories to changing inertial and gravitational constraints in young adults ( Vu et al., 2016a). There are pieces of evidence suggesting that aging alters motor planning (Kanekar and Aruin, 2014;Kubicki et al., 2016;Casamento-Moran et al., 2017;Stöckel et al., 2017;Wunsch et al., 2017), and that altered motor planning may cause falls (Lord and Fitzpatrick, 2001;Lyon and Day, 2005;Robinovitch et al., 2013;Tisserand et al., 2016). Since fall is inherently linked to gravity, understanding how older adults adapt their motor planning to gravity is crucial. ...
... We compared arm trajectories between upward and downward movements and between age-groups to test whether older adults produce trajectories that are similar to young adults. According to results suggesting that motor planning deteriorates during aging (Kanekar and Aruin, 2014;Kubicki et al., 2016;Casamento-Moran et al., 2017;Stöckel et al., 2017;Wunsch et al., 2017), one could predict that older adults present with a decreased capability to plan optimal arm movements that minimize muscle effort. Such impairment should be reflected by a decreased directiondependence of arm kinematics in older adults compared to young ones. ...
... The present results add to the existing literature suggesting that motor planning is modified with aging (Kanekar and Aruin, 2014;Kubicki et al., 2016;Casamento-Moran et al., 2017;Stöckel et al., 2017;Wunsch et al., 2017). Neuroscientists have first interpreted motor planning modifications as a deterioration of feedforward processes (proactive strategies) that urges older adults to favor feedback processes (reactive strategies). ...
Article
Full-text available
Several sensorimotor modifications are known to occur with aging, possibly leading to adverse outcomes such as falls. Recently, some of those modifications have been proposed to emerge from motor planning deteriorations. Motor planning of vertical movements is thought to engage an internal model of gravity to anticipate its mechanical effects on the body-limbs and thus to genuinely produce movements that minimize muscle effort. This is supported, amongst other results, by direction-dependent kinematics where relative durations to peak accelerations and peak velocity are shorter for upward than for downward movements. The present study compares the motor planning of fast and slow vertical arm reaching movements between 18 young (24 ± 3 years old) and 17 older adults (70 ± 5 years old). We found that older participants still exhibit strong directional asymmetries (i.e., differences between upward and downward movements), indicating that optimization processes during motor planning persist with healthy aging. However, the size of these differences was increased in older participants, indicating that gravity-related motor planning changes with age. We discuss this increase as the possible result of an overestimation of gravity torque or increased weight of the effort cost in the optimization process. Overall, these results support the hypothesis that feedforward processes and, more precisely, optimal motor planning, remain active with healthy aging.
... Aging affects postural control, increasing both the risk of falls and the fear of falling 15 . Previous studies have reported that the elderly population presented the delayed onset of postural muscles during APAs 7,14,16 , decreased APAs and/or increased CPAs postural muscle activation 7 , different muscle patterns or strategies to maintain posture 8,14,17 , and delayed COP onset during APAs, when compared with young people 8,14 . ...
... T 0 moment (i.e., the beginning of the movement) was defined as the onset of DEL. After the onset of each trial, we calculated the timing of each muscle activation with reference to the DEL onset 16,18 . Triggers of kinematics and force platform data were provided to two channels of the EMG to permit data synchronization and offline analysis using MatLab programs (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Chronic low back pain (CLBP) is associated with postural control impairments and is highly prevalent in elderly people. The objective of this study is to verify whether anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs) and compensatory postural adjustments (CPAs) are affected by CLBP in elderly people by assessing their postural control during a self-initiated perturbation paradigm induced by rapid upper arm movement when pointing to a target. The participants’ lower limb muscle onset and center of pressure (COP) displacements were assessed prior to perturbation and throughout the entire movement. T0 moment (i.e., the beginning of the movement) was defined as the anterior deltoid (DEL) onset, and all parameters were calculated with respect to it. The rectus femoris (RT), semitendinosus (ST), and soleous (SOL) showed delayed onset in the CLBP group compared with the control group: RF (control: − 0.094 ± 0.017 s; CLBP: − 0.026 ± 0.012 s, t = 12, p
... By studying the onset of the postural muscles, investigators have been able to assess muscle synergy in postural control, necessary to perform movement. Few studies, however, have discussed the differences in the order of the recruitment or the patterns of muscle recruitment between younger and older participants [11,13,14,18,29,35]. In general, studies have reported that these muscles have a similar behavior (i.e., distal to proximal and reciprocal activation distal) in younger adults with significant anticipation, compared to older adults [11,13,14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs) are a feedforward mechanism triggered in advance to a predictable perturbation, to help the individual counteract mechanical effects that the disturbance may cause. Whether or not this strategy is compromised in the elderly is not a consensus in the literature. Methods In this systematic review with meta-analysis, we investigated aging effects on postural control, based on anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs). We selected 11 eligible articles of the following databases: Lilacs, SciELO, PubMed, Cochrane Central, Embase, and CINAHL, involving 324 research participants, assessing their methodological quality and extracting electromyographic, posturographic, and kinematic measurements. We included studies that investigated the occurrence of APAs in healthy younger and older adults, published before 10th August 2022, in English. Studies involving participant with conditions that may affect balance or that did not report measures of onset or amplitude of electromyography (EMG), COP, or kinematics were excluded. To analyze the aggregated results from these studies, we performed the analysis based on the outcome measures (EMG, COP, or kinematic measures) used in individual studies. We calculated differences between younger and older adult groups as the mean differences between the groups and the estimated effect. Egger’s test was conducted to evaluate whether this meta-analysis had publication bias. Results Through this review, older adults showed no significant difference in the velocity to perform a movement compared to the younger adults (MD 0.95, 95% CI −0.86, 2.76, I² = 82%), but both muscle onset and center of pressure (COP) onset were significantly more delayed in older than in younger adults: erector spinae (MD −31.44, 95% CI −61.79, −1.09, I² = 95%); rectus abdominis (RA) (MD −31.51, 95% CI −70.58, −3.57, I² = 85%); tibialis anterior (TA) (MD −44.70, 95% CI −94.30, 4.91, I² = 63%); soleus (SOL) (MD −37.74, 95% CI −65.43, −10.05, I² = 91%); gastrocnemius (GAS) (MD −120.59, 95% CI −206.70, −34.49, I² = 94%); quadriceps (Q) (MD −17.42, 95% CI −34.73, −0.12, I² = 0%); biceps femoris (BF) (MD −117.47, 95% CI −192.55, −42.70, I² = 97%); COP onset (MD −45.28, 95% CI −89.57, −0.98, I² = 93%), and COP apa (COPapa) (MD 2.35, 95% CI −0.09, 4.79, I² = 64%). These changes did not seem to be linked to the speed of movement but possibly to age-related physiological changes that indicated decreased motor control during APAs in older adults. Conclusions Older adults use different postural strategies that aim to increase the safety margin and stabilize the body to perform the movement, according to the requirements imposed, and this should be considered in rehabilitation protocols. Systematic review registration PROSPERO CRD420119143198
... Main results showed that control centrality was negatively correlated with age indicating a global trend of node controllability reduction in older brains. The presence of sporadic positive correlations in the frontal and central areas should be further investigated for possible compensatory mechanisms occuring in later age as well as in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease (Kubicki et al. 2016;Behfar et al. 2020;Guillon et al. 2019). Instead, brain regions in the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) were significantly impacted by aging, in terms of relative loss of control centrality (Fig. 3). ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding how few distributed areas can steer large-scale brain activity is a fundamental question that has practical implications, which range from inducing specific patterns of behavior to counteracting disease. Recent endeavors based on network controllability provided fresh insights into the potential ability of single regions to influence whole brain dynamics through the underlying structural connectome. However, controlling the entire brain activity is often unfeasible and might not always be necessary. The question whether single areas can control specific target subsystems remains crucial, albeit still poorly explored. Furthermore, the structure of the brain network exhibits progressive changes across the lifespan, but little is known about the possible consequences in the controllability properties. To address these questions, we adopted a novel target controllability approach that quantifies the centrality of brain nodes in controlling specific target anatomo-functional systems. We then studied such target control centrality in human connectomes obtained from healthy individuals aged from 5 to 85. Main results showed that the sensorimotor system has a high influencing capacity, but it is difficult for other areas to influence it. Furthermore, we reported that target control centrality varies with age and that temporal-parietal regions, whose cortical thinning is crucial in dementia-related diseases, exhibit lower values in older people. By simulating targeted attacks, such as those occurring in focal stroke, we showed that the ipsilesional hemisphere is the most affected one regardless of the damaged area. Notably, such degradation in target control centrality was more evident in younger people, thus supporting early-vulnerability hypotheses after stroke.
... Although complex movements involving multilimb coordination have been associated with disease severity [4][5][6] , recent work has also demonstrated that such movement may be sensitive to disease progression 7 when assessed with a timed motor task. To minimize cost and assessment time and improve portability, we developed an upper extremity motor task that i) does not require any hardware or software; ii) can differentiate between cognitively intact and cognitively impaired individuals 8 better than other simple motor tasks (i.e., grip strength, see 9 ); and iii) is feasible for amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) cohorts 7,10 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Cortical amyloid deposition is one of the hallmark biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease. However, given how cost- and time-intensive amyloid imaging can be, there is a continued need for a lowcost, non-invasive, and accessible enrichment strategy to pre-screen individuals for their likelihood of amyloid prior to imaging. Previous work supports the use of coordinated limb movement as a potential screening tool, even after controlling for cognitive and daily function. Thirty-six patients diagnosed with amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment over the age of 65 underwent 18F-Flutemetamol amyloid-positron emission tomography imaging, then completed a timed motor task involving upper limb coordination. This task takes ~5 minutes to administer and score. Multivariate linear regression and Receiver Operator Characteristic analyses showed that including motor task performance improved model prediction of amyloid burden. Results support the rationale for including functional upper extremity motor assessment as a cost- and time-effective means to screen participants for amyloid deposition.
... Because regions of the sensorimotor system -such as paracentral lobule -are not directly affected by the atrophy process (Agosta et al., 2010), we speculate that possible compensatory mechanisms could have therefore taken place. In line with this hypothesis, recent findings suggest that more efficient motor commands in mild cognitive impaired patients could trigger the later functional decline (Kubicki et al., 2016). Longitudinal studies involving healthy subjects converting into AD will be fundamental to confirm or reject this prediction (Dubois et al., 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the progressive atrophy leads to aberrant network reconfigurations both at structural and functional levels. In such network reorganization, the core and peripheral nodes appear to be crucial for the prediction of clinical outcome due to their ability to influence large-scale functional integration. However, the role of the different types of brain connectivity in such prediction still remains unclear. Using a multiplex network approach we integrated information from DWI, fMRI and MEG brain connectivity to extract an enriched description of the core-periphery structure in a group of AD patients and age- matched controls. Globally, the regional coreness - i.e., the probability of a region to be in the multiplex core - significantly decreased in AD patients as a result of the randomization process initiated by the neurodegeneration. Locally, the most impacted areas were in the core of the network - including temporal, parietal and occipital areas - while we reported compensatory increments for the peripheral regions in the sensorimotor system. Furthermore, these network changes significantly predicted the cognitive and memory impairment of patients. Taken together these results indicate that a more accurate description of neurodegenerative diseases can be obtained from the multimodal integration of neuroimaging-derived network data.
Article
Full-text available
Background Falls in elderly people are a major health burden, especially in the long-term care environment. Yet little objective evidence is available for how and why falls occur in this population. We aimed to provide such evidence by analysing real-life falls in long-term care captured on video. Methods We did this observational study between April 20, 2007, and June 23, 2010, in two long-term care facilities in British Columbia, Canada. Digital video cameras were installed in common areas (dining rooms, lounges, hallways). When a fall occurred, facility staff completed an incident report and contacted our teams so that we could collect video footage. A team reviewed each fall video with a validated questionnaire that probed the cause of imbalance and activity at the time of falling. We then tested whether differences existed in the proportion of participants falling due to the various causes, and while engaging in various activities, with generalised linear models, repeated measures logistic regression, and log-linear Poisson regression. Findings We captured 227 falls from 130 individuals (mean age 78 years, SD 10). The most frequent cause of falling was incorrect weight shifting, which accounted for 41% (93 of 227) of falls, followed by trip or stumble (48, 21%), hit or bump (25, 11%), loss of support (25, 11%), and collapse (24, 11%). Slipping accounted for only 3% (six) of falls. The three activities associated with the highest proportion of falls were forward walking (54 of 227 falls, 24%), standing quietly (29 falls, 13%), and sitting down (28 falls, 12%). Compared with previous reports from the long-term care setting, we identified a higher occurrence of falls during standing and transferring, a lower occurrence during walking, and a larger proportion due to centre-of-mass perturbations than base-of-support perturbations. Interpretation By providing insight into the sequences of events that most commonly lead to falls, our results should lead to more valid and effective approaches for balance assessment and fall prevention in long-term care.
Article
Full-text available
In response to the increasing call by professional organizations, journal editors, and statisticians to include not only derived significance levels from quantitative statistical procedures but also measures of effect sizes (ES), this article first provides a rationale for school counseling-related researchers to include these key indices in their studies. We contend that with this information, readers are better able to contextualize and properly interpret research findings and conclusions. Second, the three major ES families and the indices associated with each one are reviewed. Next, the influence of various research designs and differing sample sizes on the size of these effects is discussed, followed by an introduction to ES computation, reporting, and interpretation, particularly as these issues relate to school counseling settings. Practical examples, ES summary tables, and supplementary resources are overviewed as well.
Article
Full-text available
Background. Early motor changes associated with aging predict cognitive decline, which suggests that a “motor signature” can be detected in predementia states. In line with previous research, we aim to demonstrate that individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have a distinct motor signature, and specifically, that dual-task gait can be a tool to distinguish amnestic (a-MCI) from nonamnestic MCI. Methods. Older adults with MCI and controls from the “Gait and Brain Study” were assessed with neurocognitive tests to assess cognitive performance and with an electronic gait mat to record temporal and spatial gait parameters. Mean gait velocity and stride time variability were evaluated under simple and three separate dual-task conditions. The relationship between cognitive groups (a-MCI vs nonamnestic MCI) and gait parameters was evaluated with linear regression models and adjusted for confounders. Results. Ninety-nine older participants, 64 MCI (mean age 76.3±7.1 years; 50% female), and 35 controls (mean age 70.4±3.9 years; 82.9% female) were included. Forty-two participants were a-MCI and 22 were nonamnestic MCI. Multivariable linear regression (adjusted for age, sex, physical activity level, comorbidities, and executive function) showed that a-MCI was significantly associated with slower gait and higher dual-task cost under dual-task conditions. Conclusion. Participants with a-MCI, specifically with episodic memory impairment, had poor gait performance, particularly under dual tasking. Our findings suggest that dual-task assessment can help to differentiate MCI subtyping, revealing a motor signature in MCI.
Article
This document presents the Movement Disorder Society Clinical Diagnostic Criteria for Parkinson's disease (PD). The Movement Disorder Society PD Criteria are intended for use in clinical research but also may be used to guide clinical diagnosis. The benchmark for these criteria is expert clinical diagnosis; the criteria aim to systematize the diagnostic process, to make it reproducible across centers and applicable by clinicians with less expertise in PD diagnosis. Although motor abnormalities remain central, increasing recognition has been given to nonmotor manifestations; these are incorporated into both the current criteria and particularly into separate criteria for prodromal PD. Similar to previous criteria, the Movement Disorder Society PD Criteria retain motor parkinsonism as the core feature of the disease, defined as bradykinesia plus rest tremor or rigidity. Explicit instructions for defining these cardinal features are included. After documentation of parkinsonism, determination of PD as the cause of parkinsonism relies on three categories of diagnostic features: absolute exclusion criteria (which rule out PD), red flags (which must be counterbalanced by additional supportive criteria to allow diagnosis of PD), and supportive criteria (positive features that increase confidence of the PD diagnosis). Two levels of certainty are delineated: clinically established PD (maximizing specificity at the expense of reduced sensitivity) and probable PD (which balances sensitivity and specificity). The Movement Disorder Society criteria retain elements proven valuable in previous criteria and omit aspects that are no longer justified, thereby encapsulating diagnosis according to current knowledge. As understanding of PD expands, the Movement Disorder Society criteria will need continuous revision to accommodate these advances. © 2015 International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society.
Article
Study Design: The contribution of transversus abdominis to spinal stabilization was evaluated indirectly in people with and without low back pain using an experimental model identifying the coordination of trunk muscles in response to a disturbance to the spine produced by arm movement. Objectives: To evaluate the temporal sequence of trunk muscle activity associated with arm movement, and to determine if dysfunction of this parameter was present in patients with low back pain. Summary of Background Data: Few studies have evaluated the motor control of trunk muscles or the potential for dysfunction of this system in patients with low back pain. Evaluation of the response of trunk muscles to limb movement provides a suitable model to evaluate this system. Recent evidence indicates that this evaluation should include transversus abdominis. Methods: While standing, 15 patients with low back pain and 15 matched control subjects performed rapid shoulder flexion, abduction, and extension in response to a visual stimulus. Electromyographic activity of the abdominal muscles, lumbar multifidus, and the contralateral deltoid was evaluated using fine‐wire and surface electrodes. Results: Movement in each direction resulted in contraction of trunk muscles before or shortly after the deltoid in control subjects. The transversus abdominis was invariably the first muscle active and was not influenced by movement direction, supporting the hypothesized role of this muscle in spinal stiffness generation. Contraction of transversus abdominis was significantly delayed in patients with low back pain with all movements. Isolated differences were noted in the other muscles. Conclusions: The delayed onset of contraction of transversus abdominis indicates a deficit of motor control and is hypothesized to result in inefficient muscular stabilization of the spine.
Article
AimTo examine the association of the combination of slow gait and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) with cognitive function and falling in community-dwelling older people.Methods Participants were selected from the Obu Study of Health Promotion for the Elderly (n = 3400), and underwent gait examination and a battery of neuropsychological examinations, including the Mini-Mental State Examination and the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology Functional Assessment Tool (tablet version of Trail Making Test Part A and B, Symbol Digit Substitution Task, Figure selection task, Word memory and Story memory), and were interviewed with a series of questionnaires including medical history, physical activity, geriatric depression scale and fall history.ResultsParticipants were classified into control (n = 2281), slow gait speed (SG; n = 278), MCI (n = 673) and MCI with SG (MCI+SG; n = 168) groups. All cognitive functions were significantly affected by the group factor, even adjusting for participant characteristics as covariates (P < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis showed that the control group had better performance than the other groups, and the MCI+SG group had worse performance than the other groups in all cognitive functions (all P < 0.05). In multiple logistic regression analysis, SG and MCI were independently associated with falling (all P < 0.05), and MCI+SG had a higher odds ratio for falling (adjusted OR 1.99, 95% CI 1.08–3.65).Conclusions Our findings support the idea that slow gait and MCI were related, and concurrently associated with falling. Motor function among MCI subjects should be focused on to assess profile risks. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2014; ●●: ●●–●●.
Article
Recent evidence indicates that sensory and motor changes may precede the cognitive symptoms of Alzheimer's disease (AD) by several years and may signify increased risk of developing AD. Traditionally, sensory and motor dysfunctions in aging and AD have been studied separately. To ascertain the evidence supporting the relationship between age-related changes in sensory and motor systems and the development of AD and to facilitate communication between several disciplines, the National Institute on Aging held an exploratory workshop titled “Sensory and Motor Dysfunctions in Aging and AD.” The scientific sessions of the workshop focused on age-related and neuropathologic changes in the olfactory, visual, auditory, and motor systems, followed by extensive discussion and hypothesis generation related to the possible links among sensory, cognitive, and motor domains in aging and AD. Based on the data presented and discussed at this workshop, it is clear that sensory and motor regions of the central nervous system are affected by AD pathology and that interventions targeting amelioration of sensory-motor deficits in AD may enhance patient function as AD progresses.