ChapterPDF Available

Latin causativization in typological perspective

Authors:

Abstract

Causativization has a position in an intricate network of lexical relations and valency operations, including deagentive and passive, whose general function it is to express a given predicate in construction with different constellations of central participants. The corresponding tasks can be solved by (1) coding the causative or non-causative relation of an actant to the rest of the clause, or (2) leaving it to inference.
CLIPP
Christiani Lehmanni inedita, publicanda, publicata
titulus
Latin causativization in typological perspective
huius textus situs retis mundialis
http://www.christianlehmann.eu/publ/
lehmann_latin_causativization.pdf
dies manuscripti postremum modificati
08.06.2013
occasio orationis habitae
13ème Colloque International de Linguistique Latine,
Bruxelles, 4 – 8 avril, 2005
volumen publicationem continens
Poccetti, Paolo (ed.), Latinitatis rationes. Proceedings of the
17th International Colloquium on Latin Linguistics, Rome,
2013
annus publicationis
2016
paginae
918-943
Latin causativization in typological perspective
Christian Lehmann
University of Erfurt
Abstract
Causativization has a position in an intricate network of lexical relations and valency
operations, including deagentive and passive, whose general function it is to express a
given predicate in construction with different constellations of central participants. The
corresponding tasks can be solved by (1) coding the causative or non-causative relation of
an actant to the rest of the clause, or (2) leaving it to inference.
In the first case, the relation may be coded (1.1) on the verb or (1.2) on the actants.
If the relation is coded on the verb, this may be done (1.1.1) at the lexical level, by a
paradigm of converse verb stems, (1.1.2) at the derivational level by deriving a verb stem
with a different valency, or (1.1.3) at the syntactic level by a construction involving
another verb.
In the cases 1.1, the causative construction may be more marked compared with a base of
lesser valency, or else the deagentive construction may be more marked compared with a
base of higher valency.
In case 1.2, a given stem is converted into a different valency frame according to the
syntactic environment. The causative constellation may then be coded by a special case
either on the causer or on the causee.
Latin may be characterized as a language that makes very little use of the strategy that is
cross-linguistically the most common one, viz. 1.1.2. Instead, it relies heavily on 1.1.1,
1.2 and 2. This is in consonance with its general aversion against derivational valency
operations and with its dependent-marking type.
1 Introduction
1
1.1 General prerequisites
Causativization is a regular derivational process in many languages of the world. A good
example is Turkish, which has a causative suffix -dir with a number of allomorphs – as
illustrated in E1.
E1. a. Caesar haber-i bil-iyor
T
URK
Caesar news-ACC know-IMPFV
‘Caesar knows the news’
b. Caesar-e haber-i bil-dir-di-k
Caesar-DAT news-ACC know-CAUS-PST-1.PL
‘we made the news known to Caesar’
1
Thanks are due to Concepción Cabrillana for helpful comments on this paper.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 2
E1.a represents the base situation, with an actor of its own. E1.b is a causative version of it,
where the base verb is provided with a causative suffix. This augments its semantic valency
by an agent, the causer, which occupies the subject slot. The subordinate subject, representing
the causee, is demoted to the highest syntactic function available, which here is the indirect
object. The process is very regular in Turkish, and many agentive verbal meanings
represented by simple transitive verbs in languages such as Latin are represented by
causatives of intransitive bases in Turkish, as shown by the single example of T1.
T1. Transparency of agentive verbs in Turkish and Latin
language
verb Turkish Latin English
intransitive
art-mak cresc-ere grow
agentive
art-ır-mak aug-ere make grow
While causativization is a prominent topic in any Turkish grammar, it does not figure in
standard Latin grammars and has not been frequently treated in Latin linguistics. Examples
like the above make us understand why: Latin does not have a productive morphological
process for the formation of causative constructions. A translation of E1 into Latin yields E2.
E2. a. Caesar res gestas nouit.
L
AT
‘Caesar knows the news’
b. Caesarem de rebus gestis certiorem fecimus.
‘we made the news known to Caesar’
While E2.b may be considered the agentive counterpart to E2.a, clearly the two sentences
have no grammatical or derivational paradigmatic relationship.
The aim of this contribution is to characterize the formation of causative constructions in
Classical Latin in general terms. The main questions to be asked are:
Which are the preferred strategies to fulfill the relevant subfunctions of causativization?
In this choice, how does Latin compare with other languages; in other words, in which
respects is the Latin grammar of causativization like the grammar of causativization of
most or even all languages, and in which respects is it peculiar?
This presupposes a functional theory of causativization and a theory of strategies that can be
employed in this domain at the typological level. These will be provided in turn in section 2.
1.2 Participation
At the cognitive level, an elementary situation consists of a set of participants related to each
other by a network of relations which cross-cut at an immaterial center called the situation
core. At the semantic level, the situation core is represented by a predicate, participants are
represented by terms that act as arguments of predicates. This is true regardless of whether
the participant is coded as a naked NP, a cased NP or an NP governed by an adposition. In
principle and disregarding governed cases for the moment, case relators cases and
adpositions – are predicates at the semantic level (cf. Lehmann 2006). This means that even a
simple clause is represented semantically by a complex proposition comprising several
predicates if it codes arguments that are not inherent in the predicate representing the situation
core. E3 serves as a simple example (a corpus example of the same structure is in Hor. Sat. 1,
9, 30).
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 3
E3. serua domino canticum cecinit
L
AT
‘the slave sang a song for the master’
At the cognitive level, E3 is a complex situation with three participants. At the semantic level,
we have a central predicate coded by cecinit, which has two arguments, serva and canticum.
The benefactive relation of the participant dominus to the situation – coded by the dative case
– may be represented, at the semantic level, by a co-predicate
GIVE
(serva, e, dominus), where
the object given is the proposition based on the central predicate, here represented by an event
variable e, and domino is the recipient of the co-predicate (see Shibatani 1996 for such an
analysis and E6.a below for an example).
2 Typology of causativization
2.1 Functional basis
Causativity may be defined as in T2 and visualized as in S1:
T2. Causativity
I. A situation C is causative iff it is complex in the following way:
there is a situation B such that C includes B,
there are at least two participants a, b ... n,
C includes a, b ...n,
B includes b ... n; a is not an element of B,
a controls C,
b is the participant that has most control in B,
C\B can be more or less autonomous with respect to B, in the limiting case an
autonomous situation with participants (esp. a) of its own,
accordingly the participation of a in C can be more or less peripheral,
accordingly influence of a on B (und also on b) can be more or less mediate.
a: causer
b: causee
B: base situation
C\B: causing situation
C: causative situation
II. Derivatively, a construction which expresses C and is derived from a construction
expressing B is causative.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 4
S1. Causative situation
The predicate of C\B is symbolized by the left-upper explosion. It will be called the cause
predicate (and, if a verb, the cause verb). The predicate of B, symbolized by the right-lower
explosion, will be called the base predicate (and, if a verb, the base verb). In E4, a illustrates
the base situation, b illustrates the corresponding causative situation.
E4. a. die Studenten schreiben morgen eine Klausur
G
ERM
‘the students will write a test tomorrow’
b. ich lasse die Studenten morgen eine Klausur schreiben
‘I will have the students write a test tomorrow’
A verb that combines the meanings of the base predicate and the cause predicate in a
transparent way, as do the above Turkish examples, is a causative verb. A verb whose
meaning contains these two components, but is merely in a lexical paradigmatic relationship
with the base predicate without bearing a structural relation to it, as does Latin augeo
contrasted with cresco in T1, is just an agentive verb which may be called semantically
causative.
The causer is constitutive of a causative situation. If it is removed from the conceptual
level, as in E4.a, only the base situation remains, and the whole situation is no longer
causative. The causee, on the other hand, is optional at the conceptual level (i.e. apart from its
being optional in particular syntactic constructions). A situation lacking it is illustrated in
E5.b.
E5. a. ich schreibe morgen eine Klausur
G
ERM
‘I will write a test tomorrow’
b. ich lasse morgen eine Klausur schreiben
‘I will have a test written tomorrow’
Here the causer is the only agent in C; but it is not the immediate agent of the base predicate.
Such a causative sentence expresses mediate agency of the causer in B. If we compare, in
this respect, E5.b with E4.b, mediate agency appears as omission of the causee. If, instead, we
compare it with E5.a, where the same participant is an immediate agent, mediate agency
appears as distantiation of the agent.
The parameters on which causative constructions vary follow from the definition in T2
(cf. Comrie 1985 and Dixon 2000:61-74). They may be grouped as follows:
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 5
The control exerted on B by the causer may be stronger or weaker. With strongest
control, it
2
forces B by direct participation. With weakest control, it just lets B happen.
Thus, causativization oscillates between coercion and permission (see Talmy 1976 for
relevant theoretical foundation).
The causer shares control of C with the causee. Its stronger or weaker control is therefore
complemented by the weaker or stronger control of the causee. With least control, the
causee is just the direct patient of the causer’s action. With most control, it remains the
agent in B, with the causer’s permission.
3
The causer’s involvement in B may be more or less central. With central involvement, the
causer acts directly on the causee; with distant involvement, it does something that
induces B.
This entails that the cores of the two situations B and C may remain disjoint or may
merge. If they remain disjoint, the causer performs some activity appropriate to bring B
about (indirect causation). If they merge, the causer engages actively in B (direct
causation).
Central involvement of the causer entails its strong control; but indirect causation is
compatible with both coercion and permission. T3 summarizes how control and affectedness
of the causee depend on control and involvement on the part of the causer.
T3. Interdependence of parameters of causation
causer’s control causer’s
involvement strong weak
central direct impingement
causee controlled and affected
marginal coercion
causee controlled permission
causee controlling, not affected
Seen in the perspective of T3 and E5.b, causativization marks the fact that, contrary to
expectations and to the default case, the participant that has highest control in the situation is
not centrally involved (is not the direct agent of the base predicate), but only marginally
involved.
2.2 Strategies of causativization
As said before, participant relations are represented by predicates at the semantic level. These
may be made explicit by full verbs, as when an instrumental relation is coded by the verb
‘use’, the benefactive relation is coded by a verb ‘favor’ and a causative relation by a verb
meaning ‘cause, effectuate’. We will see such cases in Latin causativization below. Such
strategies of the lexical-syntactic level are always available and do not contribute to
structuring the grammar of a language. What is of interest here is the grammaticalization of
such participant relations. They are then represented at the structural level by relational
grammatical or derivational formatives. The main alternative here is between the association
of the formative with either the verb representing the situation core or with the NP
2
Participants are treated grammatically as neutral entities.
3
See Biville 1995 for Latin.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 6
representing the participant. These will be called the verbal and the nominal strategy,
respectively.
4
We will first illustrate them for the role of the beneficiary, taking up the
introductory example of section 1.2. As with the causative situation, we may speak of a base
situation typically containing an agent and a patient, and a benefactive situation distinguished
from the former by an additional participant, the beneficiary, which, as it were, receives the
base situation. Tamil may employ either a verbal or a nominal strategy to code this, as shown
in E6.
E6. a. naan avan-ukku ttuwisakaravanʈi-yai ttirutti kuʈu-tt-een
T
AM
1.SG 3.SG.M-DAT bike-ACC repair give-PST-1.SG
‘I repaired him the bike’ (Lehmann et al. 2004:78)
b. naan avan-ukk-aaka ttuwisakaravanʈi-yai ttirutti-n-een
1.SG 3.SG.M-DAT-BEN bike-ACC repair-PST-1.SG
‘I repaired the bike for him’ (Lehmann et al. 2004:76)
E6.a presents the benefactive situation as a donative situation, coding the benefactive relation
by a verb meaning ‘give’, the beneficiary as its recipient and the benefactum, i.e. the agent’s
deed, as the transferred object. Contrariwise, E6.b codes the beneficiary as depending from a
specific case relator meaning something like ‘in favour of’. While some languages do admit
of such variation, Hindi would only use the verbal strategy, Latin only the nominal strategy
(as in E3) in benefactive constructions.
The causative situation allows for the same coding alternative as the benefactive situation.
While the causer is coded as an NP, the cause predicate may be coded in two main ways. The
first alternative is for it to be coded as a verbal formative, e.g. a kind of causative function
verb or causative verb affix, or else as a nominal formative, e.g. a kind of agentive case on the
causer. E7 from Turkish illustrates the first alternative, E8 from Lezgian the second one.
E7. a. Orhan öl-dü
T
URK
Orhan die-PST ‘Orhan died’
b. Hasan Orhan-ı öl-dür-dü
Hasan Orhan-ACC die-CAUS-PST ‘Hasan killed Orhan’
E8. a. k
h
ic’ q’ena
L
EZG
dog(ABS) died ‘the dog died’
b. gada-di k
h
ic’ q’ena
boy-ERG dog(ABS) died ‘the boy killed the dog’ (Kittilä 2002:159)
Both for the verbal and for the nominal strategy, there is a continuum of structural means
which differ by their degree of explicitness or reduction.
A. For the verbal strategy, reduction means that the causative formative may either be
grammaticalized to a causative function verb (“support verb”) or a morphological operator on
the base predicate, or it may be lexicalized together with the base predicate. The following
four main strategies are commonly distinguished on this bifurcating reduction continuum (see
S2 below and cf. Comrie 1985, section 2):
4
In studies of valency operations, it is customary to restrict the attention to verbal strategies. Shibatani
(2006:229) is among the few to argue explicitly for consideration of nominal strategies.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 7
Complex causative sentence: The causative construction consists of a main clause
containing the cause predicate and a finite subordinate clause depending on the latter. E9 is an
example.
E9. sol efficit [ut omnia floreant]
L
AT
‘the sun makes everything blossom’ (Cic.n.d. 2, 41)
Periphrastic (analytic) causative construction: The causative construction is in one clause
which, however, is complex in that the cause predicate is coded by a function verb, while the
base predicate is coded as a non-finite verb form depending on the latter. This construction is
illustrated by E10 from French.
E10. le soleil fait tout fleurir
(ditto)
Derivational (synthetic) causative construction: The main verb of the sentence is based on
the stem of the base predicate which is modified by some morphological process mostly a
suffix coding the cause predicate, from which a causative verb results. This was already
illustrated by E1 and E7 above.
Lexical causative alternation: There is a lexical paradigmatic relationship between two
verbs that are synonymous except that one appears in valency frame FC and the other in
valency frame FB, where FC has one argument more than FB which carries the role of an
agent in the subject/ergative function, while the argument corresponding to the
subject/ergative of FB is demoted in FC. A Latin example was already provided in T1.
B. In the nominal strategies, the base predicate is left untouched, and instead the cause
predicate is coded as a case relator. The reduction of the latter may be described by the
grammaticalization path leading from some lexical base via an adposition to a concrete and
finally a grammatical case. A relatively early stage of this path, the coverb strategy, is
illustrated by the Mandarin ba construction as it appears in E11.b.
E11. a. chuán fān le
M
AN
boat capsize TEL
“the boat capsized”
b. Wáng bǎ chuán fān le
Wang ACC boat capsize TEL
“Wang capsized the boat” (I. Wild p.c.)
While other ba constructions have a simpler counterpart lacking ba, the main verb cannot be
transitive in constructions such as E11.b, and consequently ba may not be omitted here
(
*
Wang fan le chuan). E11.b is, thus, a genuine causative construction. Structurally, the
relator ba associates, like a preposition, with the causee NP. It is, however, grammaticalized
from a verb meaning ‘take’ and still behaves as a coverb rather than a case marker in that it
does not so much mark the case function of the undergoer NP as it codes the control cline
between the actor and the undergoer, in general, and thus, between the causer and the causee
in this construction.
With grammaticalization proceeding, the path of the nominal strategies bifurcates, and the
case relator either marks the actor (causer) or the undergoer (causee) in the construction. The
first alternative was illustrated by E8.b. Latin examples of both alternatives are in section 3.4.
The verbal and the nominal strategies become indistinguishable when there is no specific
agentive or deagentive marker on either the predicate or its dependents. This is the case in the
last strategy, causative valency conversion: A verb stem appears in two valency frames FC
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 8
and FB which differ in that FC has one argument more than FB which carries the role of an
agent in the subject/ergative function, while the argument corresponding to the
subject/ergative of FB is demoted in FC. For plurivalent bases, this strategy may be identified
(as distinct from the nominal strategy) both in accusative and in ergative systems. For
monovalent bases, as in English break (tr./intr. verb), it can be identified only in accusative
systems, since in ergative systems it amounts to the addition of an ergative actant as
illustrated in E8.
S2. Scales of causativization strategies
reduction
association
lexical-
syntactic analytic synthetic fusional zero
verbal complex
sentence periphrastic derivational alternation
nominal adposition case
valency
conversion
The verbal and nominal reduction continua may be united in the form of S2. This is a scale of
decreasing sentential complexity and of increasing fusion of the two propositions, mirroring
iconically the directness of the causer’s involvement as defined in T3.
2.3 Complexity in causative constructions
Keeping control of a situation gets increasingly difficult in proportion to two factors: the
number of participants it contains and the degree of control of the causee. Consequently,
semantic complexity of a causative construction increases along these two parameters: An
additional higher agent is both more expected and easier to accommodate in a situation the
fewer participants this already contains and the less control these already have. Structural
complexity of causative constructions increases along the same lines: the ensuing upheaval of
the base verb valency is less radical, and the resulting valency has better chances to fit into an
existent base-verb model, the lesser the valency of the base and the better the argument with
the highest syntactic function fits an undergoer role. Therefore, we have the implicational
hierarchy shown in S3.
S3. Base predicate hierarchy for causativization
adjective
inactive
intransitive verb
active
intransitive verb
transitive verb
multivalent verb
The interpretation of S3 is as follows: If a strategy of S2
5
forms causative constructions from
bases at some point of S3, then it forms causative constructions from bases left to that point of
S3.
6
Here it should be noted that verbs formed by providing an adjectival base with an agent
causing the base argument to be in the state or property designated by the adjective are
traditionally called factitive, not causative. This distinction is justified because many
languages, among them the Indo-European and the Mayan languages, make a structural
5
Strategies here have to be conceived at the level of abstraction of S2, i.e. without taking into consideration the
particular formatives employed. There may be variation in these, as there is between the first two positions of S3
in the languages mentioned presently.
6
Some of the implicational relationships are already alluded to in Dixon 2000:31, 61f.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 9
distinction between causativization of verbal bases and factitivization. From an
onomasiological perspective, however, factitivization belongs into the first position of S3.
S3 may be briefly illustrated. Yucatec Maya has productive factitivization and
causativization of inactive intransitive bases. German has productive factitivization and a
dozen of synthetic causative verbs (of the type sitzen ‘sit’ – setzen ‘set’) only from intransitive
bases, with one exception (trinken ‘drink’ tränken ‘water’, which however confirms the
rule, as the underlying direct object can practically not be accommodated in the valency of the
causative verb. Turkish has derivational factitivization and causativization over the entire
gamut of S3.
S2 and S3 correlate significantly, viz. inversely as regards their left-right orientation: the
farther the base predicate is on the right of S3, the more likely a strategy near the start of S2
will be used (cf. Comrie 1985, section 2 and Hoffmann 2007, section 2.1).
2.4 Causativization and other valency-changing operations
Agentivization is an operation which adds a highest agent, the causer, to the base situation.
The base situation is presupposed as a starting point; the situation that contains an added
highest agent is arrived at as the result of the operation.
In language, complex constructions arising as the result of an operation presuppose the
existence of simple constructions with the same functional (cognitive, communicative)
properties, which may serve as a model of the target of the operation (cf. Dik 1985). For
instance, agentivization aims at a constellation that may also be expressed, in an elementary
way, by simple agentive (transformative or transport) transitive verbs, e.g. parse, bring. Such
elementary situations, in their turn, may form the input to an operation which outputs that
kind of situation which served as the input to the first operation. In the case at hand, the
converse operation consists in suppressing the agent in a situation so that the resulting
situation happens without the intervention of an agent, i.e. by itself, as in this string parses
easily. This converse of agentivization is deagentivization. Their structural implementation
in terms of deverbal verb derivation is known as causative and anticausative. The pair is
mirrored by a pair of operations concerning the addition and the blocking of a patient
argument (cf. Lehmann 2002, section 3).
It is important to note that the relationship between these operations is not only converse
but also complementary. Whether my linguistic system provides me with intransitive ‘break’
plus an operation to derive causative ‘break’ from it, or it provides me with transitive ‘break’
plus an operation to derive anticausative ‘break’ from it, ultimately the same purpose is
served. Languages do indeed differ in that some pair a large inventory of monovalent stems
with a productive set of transitivization operations, while others pair a large inventory of
polyvalent stems with a productive inventory of detransitivization operations. This point will
be taken up in section 3.5.
3 Causativization in Latin
A large number of strategies for the linguistic manifestation of a causative situation are
available in Latin, of which only the most important ones will be considered here.
7
We will
7
Some of them are discussed in Simone & Cerbasi 2001, §3 and Hoffmann 2007. Among the strategies not
discussed here is preverbation, as in miser ‘miserable’ – immisero ‘make miserable’.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 10
first analyze a strategy that relies on pure inference and then review the coding strategies by
passing through S2 from left to right. Particular attention will be paid to the nominal
strategies.
3.1 Base verb strategy
The first strategy to be considered here does not figure in S2 because it does not involve any
coding of causation. Mediate agency is mostly left unexpressed in Latin, i.e. not
distinguished from direct agency, as in E12 (facere) and E13 (Kühner & Stegmann 1962:100).
E12. (Piso) cum uellet sibi anulum facere, aurificem iussit uocari (Cic. Verr. 4, 56)
L
AT
‘when Piso wanted to have a ring made, he had the goldsmith called’
E13. complures pauperes mortuos ... suo sumptu extulit (Nep. 5, 4, 3)
L
AT
‘he had several poor dead people bury at his own cost’
The base verb strategy does not code the specific cognitive constellation, but leaves it to
inference. The resulting construction is thus, structurally, not a causative construction. Latin
shares it with other ancient Indo-European languages, in particular Greek. In modern German,
too, E5.a may mean E5.b. As we shall see in section 3.4, the nominal strategies are essentially
based on the base verb strategy.
3.2 Complex causative sentence
The construction of a complex causative sentence is the maximally explicit coding strategy. It
is at the lexical-syntactic level, thus universally available, not grammaticalized and therefore
not specific to Latin. T4 shows a selection of cause verbs with the complex sentence
constructions that they may be used in.
T4. Lexical-syntactic causative constructions
cause verb subordinate clause causee
form meaning finite:
ut + subj.
accusativus
cum infinitivo
(prep. +)
gerund = object
efficio bring about
+
curo see to it + +
compello force + +
impello move + + +
induco induce + + +
suadeo advise + + + +
iubeo order + + +
persuadeo
persuade + + +
sino let (+) + (+)
facio make + +
As may be seen, the criteria chosen for the characterization of these constructions do not
correlate. This wide variation of syntactic constructions for the caused subordinate clause
proves that the construction is not at all grammaticalized in Latin. The following subsections
provide some examples.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 11
3.2.1 Finite subordinate clause
The matrix verb is a full verb one of whose semantic components is causation. They form a
lexical field properly including the verbs of T4. As shown there, all of them may govern a
complement clause in the subjunctive, optionally introduced by ut, as already illustrated by
E9.
E14. fac cogites [in quanta calamitate sis] (Sall. Cat. 44, 5)
L
AT
‘consider in what a dangerous situation you are’
There is a semantic variant of this construction, illustrated by E14, which is remarkable in that
it is formally a mediate agency construction, but semantically the agency cannot but be
immediate. This is evidence for the incipient grammaticalization of facio as a function verb in
causative constructions.
8
3.2.2 Non-finite subordinate clause
Alternatively, the cause verb may govern a non-finite construction, mostly an accusativus
cum infinitivo, but also a gerundive. E15f illustrate the a.c.i. construction.
E15. [mel inferuere] facito (Colum. 12, 38, 5)
L
AT
‘boil honey’
E16. qui [nati coram me cernere letum] fecisti (Verg. Aen. 2, 538f)
L
AT
‘you made me watch my son’s death’
The a.c.i. governed by a cause predicate iconically represents the fact that the causee is not
only the central participant of the base situation, but also directly controlled by the cause
predicate. Some cause verbs, esp. suadeo and iubeo, alternatively take the causee as an
indirect object of the cause verb, no matter whether the subordinate clause is finite. The fact
that the causee becomes a direct dependent of the matrix cause verb may be viewed as causee
ascension; but at the same time, it goes into an oblique case, which is a kind of demotion if
compared with its status as the central participant of the base situation. This reflects its
ambivalent status in a causative situation. The variation possible at the lexical-syntactic level
allows for a finely tuned reflection of the constellation designated.
Whether the base situation is expressed by a finite or a non-finite subordinate clause, the
base verb may be in the passive, thus rendering the suppression of its agent and, hence, of the
causee possible. Such constructions of mediate agency are frequent, especially with a
dependent infinitive. The accusativus cum infinitivo then reduces to a pure infinitival. E17 is
an example.
E17. puberes interfici iubet (Curt. Alex. 7, 9, 22, 5)
L
AT
‘he has the young men killed’
Since none of the more strongly morphologized strategies is fully productive, Latin has to
resort, in many cases, to the lexical-syntactic strategy. This is not particularly elegant, as
becomes clear in sentences like E9, E15 and E16, where the causing situation (C\B) reduces
8
On this construction and its grammaticalization, cf. Bodelot 2007. It is, however, not restricted to facio as
function verb; cf. cura ut Romae sis ‘try to be in Rome’ (Cic. Att. 1, 2, 2, 5).
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 12
to its constitutive components, but still a full matrix sentence is required.
9
The periphrastic
strategy emerging from the infinitival strategy is not yet grammaticalized in Classical Latin.
3.3 Causative verb
The next subset of constructions is based on a verb in which the cause predicate and the base
predicate are combined in one verb stem, so that the two situations are merged in one clause,
with one set of verbal dependents. The strategies differ in the way the cause predicate is
coded.
3.3.1 Compound causative verb
A compound causative verb has the form X-Y, where X is the base and Y is some cause verb.
In Latin, there are two basic variants of this formation (s. Brucale & Mocciaro 2013, §2). In
the first, Y is simply the verb facio ‘do, make’ and X is an adverb, adjective or verb stem. The
deadverbial formations may be foregone here, as they are not causative.
10
The deadjectival
formations are factitive verbs. An example would be uacuefacio ‘empty out’ (Cic. Cat. 1, 16,
12). These run into the competition with factitive verbs derived by transferring the stem into
the a-conjugation, as uacuo ‘empty out’, which is highly productive and survives into
Romance.
E18 illustrates the compounding pattern with a verbal base (cf. Fruyt 2001).
E18. a. fores patent
L
AT
‘the door is open’
b. patefeci fores (Pl. Most. 1046)
‘I opened the door’
The lexicon contains a few dozens of verb stems that provide the base for this construction.
With few exceptions (s. Brucale & Mocciaro 2013, §2.1.3), they are stative verbs of the e-
conjugation. To the extent that the formal process does apply to dynamic bases, it does not
causativize them. Thus, in terms of the hierarchy of S3, this process only renders elementary
service.
The bases of the relatively productive variety of this process are consequently intransitive,
although not all of them are monovalent. However, the ablative, dative or prepositional
complement of such bivalent base verbs as assuesco ‘get used to’ remains totally unaffected
by the derivation of assuefacio ‘accustom to’.
The alternative compounding strategy uses the bound variant -fico as a cause verb on
adjectives as bases, as in amplifico ‘amplify’, magnifico ‘magnify’.
11
These formations
therefore render the same service as the type uacuefacio, although they are overall more
productive than the latter (Brucale & Mocciaro 2013, §2.2.2). The compounding strategy is,
thus, constrained in every respect. As is well known, compounding is, in general,
underdeveloped in Latin. This strategy is, thus, not in consonance with the linguistic type.
12
9
Cf. the Turkish version of E9: güneş her şehir geliş-tir-iyor (sun every thing grow-CAUS-HAB).
10
There are only four such verbs, including the frequent verbs benefacio ‘do well’, malefacio ‘do evil’ and
satisfacio ‘satisfy’.
11
There are also desubstantival formations such as significo ‘signify’.
12
It would be interesting to see whether the Romance derivational suffixes evolving from it, e.g. French -fier as
in fortifier ‘fortify’, play a systematic role in the overall system of causativization of those languages.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 13
3.3.2 Derived causative verb
T5 contains a rather comprehensive list of agentive verbs that share the thematic vowel –e:
T5. Agentive verbs of the -e conjugation
verb meaning base
representative
meaning
compleo fill plenus full
deleo destroy -
moneo admonish memini remember
noceo damage nex killing
spondeo vow -
terreo frighten terror fear
tondeo shear -
torreo roast terra land
Diachronically, this thematic vowel is a causative suffix. The right hand side of T5 shows the
closest Latin reflex of the erstwhile base. At the synchronic level, the formation is fossilized,
its products are fully lexicalized. This means that -e- is not a causative suffix in Latin, and
these Latin verbs are not causative verbs proper.
The e-conjugation class also includes a larger subset of verbs that are stative in meaning,
like taceo ‘keep silent’, habeo ‘have’, and that provide the bases for the causative compounds
seen in section 3.3.1. Even this stative formation is not fully productive in Latin; but its
existence may contribute to blocking the causative function of the -e- suffix.
3.3.3 Lexical causative alternation
Latin has quite a number of pairs of verbs that are in a causative semantic relationship but are
either in no derivational relationship at all or at least in no regular derivational relationship.
T6 contains a selection of them.
T6. Intransitive – agentive-transitive lexical pairs
intransitive
meaning agentive-
transitive meaning
1 fio become facio make
2 accidit happen efficio bring about
3 intereo perish interficio kill
4 pereo perish perdo mar
5 veneo be sold vendo sell
6 transeo cross traduco lead across
7 cado fall caedo fell
8 occido fall down occido make fall down
9 disco learn doceo teach
10 vapulo get a thrashing verbero thrash
A few fragmentary patterns are observable in this set. Facio again serves causativization in a
few cases, as does do ‘put’ in two other cases. However, there is no regularity here, as a stem
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 14
based on eo ‘go’ may have both of the latter verbs as its causative counterpart and in addition
duco ‘lead’, as in traduco.
The paradigmatic lexical relationship of these pairs is corroborated by regular
paradigmatic syntactic relationships between variant causative constructions, for instance of
the kind of S4 (cf. García-Hernández 1989).
S4. Transformational relationships between semantically causative pairs
causer adjunct construction
semantically causative construction
a x
N
OM
(ab y
A
BL
) V
intr
y
N
OM
x
A
CC
V
tr
b x
N
OM
(ab y
A
BL
) z
A
CC
V
tr
y
N
OM
x
A
CC
z
A
CC
V
ditr
S4.a holds at least for the pairs 1, 5, 7, 8, 10; S4.b holds for pairs 6 and 9. The relationship is
exemplified for pair 10 in E19.
E19. a. rogatus an ab reo fustibus uapulasset (Quintil. 9, 2, 12)
L
AT
‘asked whether he had been thrashed by the accused with clubs’
b. decemuiros Bruttiani uerberauere (Cato orat. 58, 2)
‘the Bruttians thrashed the decemvirs’
As we shall see in section 3.4, the causer adjunct is indistinguishable from the agent phrase of
a passive construction. In fact, this kind of transformational relationship is so tight that it can
be a passive-active relationship just as well as a base-causative relationship.
In S4.b, it is clearly the valency of the semantically passive verb that is conserved in the
semantically causative verb. This is so with traduco and doceo, which both take a double
accusative, one for the subject and one for the object of the base. Still, the sheer existence of
the double accusative and the fact that there are only two such cases is sufficient witness to
the fact that the language system did not have a syntactic pattern for a causative construction.
At the end of this section, two results should be stressed: 1) During the entire documented
history of Latin, there is no regular causative derivation. In this, Latin is probably in a
minority among the world’s languages. 2) Lexical pairs that are in a syntactically regular
paradigmatic relationship largely do the same service. However, the important difference
from a derivation process is that the latter is oriented, while lexical pairs are in an equipollent
opposition.
3.4 Nominal strategies
In the nominal strategies, the cause predicate is coded as a specific case relator. Starting from
a base clause, either the causer or the causee may be added in an NP equipped with such a
relator. In the former case, the base subject is interpreted as causee; in the latter case, it is
interpreted as causer. The verb is not touched in either case.
A causative situation may be expressed by the combination of a clause based on an
intransitive inactive predicate with a causer NP governed by the preposition ab,
13
as in E20
E23 (Kühner & Stegmann 1962, 1:99f). This will be called the causer adjunct strategy.
13
Because of the multifunctionality of the ab-phrase in Latin, this construction may be ambiguous. Thus, a tanto
cecidisse viro (Ov. M. 5, 192) means ‘that he fell from the hands of such a man’, but cecidere ab Romanis
ducenti (Liv. 42, 60, 1) means ‘200 of the Romans fell’.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 15
E20. pereat uitreo miles ab hoste tuus (Ov. A.A. 2, 208)
L
AT
‘your soldier may die from a canting enemy’
E21. occidit a forti ... Achille (Ov. Met. 13, 597)
L
AT
‘he died from the hand of strong Achilles’
E22. (mare) nunc qua a sole conlucet albescit (Cic. Luc. 105, 16)
L
AT
‘the sea now becomes white where the sun makes it glisten’
This strategy entails no rearrangement of the base situation. The locus of the causer adjunct
strategy is in intransitive base verbs, as in E20 – E22. Observe, however, that it is not
excluded from transitive base verbs, as in E23:
E23. a Polyphemo plurimos sociorum amiserit (Dict. Cretens. 6, 5)
L
AT
‘he lost most of his companions through Polyphemus / Polyphemus made him loose
most of his companions’
Again, the causee may be introduced into a clause based on an active verb by an appropriate
preposition, normally per. This will be called the causee adjunct strategy, illustrated by E24
– E26:
E24. (Caesar) suos per Antonium cohortatus (Caes. BG 3, 46, 4)
L
AT
‘Caesar had Anthony cheer his soldiers’
E25. recede de medio; per alium transigam (Cic. S. Rosc. Am. 112)
L
AT
‘get out of the way; I will achieve it by somebody else / I will have somebody else
achieve it’
E26. Labienus ... Caesarem per nuntios facit certiorem quid faciendum existimet (Caes.
BG 7, 87)
L
AT
‘Labienus informs Caesar by messengers / has messengers inform Caesar of what
he thinks should be done’
Structurally, the causee adjunct strategy is a variant of the base verb strategy seen in section
3.1, the difference being that mediate agency is here coded by making explicit the
intervention of the direct agent, the causee.
14
The sense of the verb in the case relator constructions is causative, as has been brought
out in the translations. The causee adjunct triggers the causative interpretation of the base verb
which was merely due to (a knowledge-based) inference in the base-verb strategy of section
3.1. The causer adjunct triggers an interpretation of a verb as if it were a passive of its own
causative. For instance, in E21 occidit ‘fell’ is interpreted as ‘was made fall’. This is probably
facilitated if the lexicon does contain such a causative counterpart to the base verb (occīdit
‘felled’ in this case). This would explain why the majority of the corpus examples of the
causer adjunct construction contain a base verb that is in such a paradigmatic relationship. Cf.
S4 in section 3.3.3.
Typological studies have not so far subsumed the nominal strategies under the notion of
causativity because the cause predicate does not take the form of a verb or a verbal operator.
They are, however, functional equivalents to verbal causative constructions; and they do bear
14
As indicated before, the Mandarin construction illustrated by E11.b has commonalities with both the causer
adjunct and the causee adjunct constructions: Just as in the former, the causer plus the relational formative is
added to a base construction containing the causee. Just as in the latter, the relational formative governs the
causee.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 16
structural relations to the latter. If the preposition introducing the causer adjunct is compared
to an ergative case marker, it may be seen that the causer adjunct itself is like the subject of an
ergative construction as already exemplified in E8.
15
If the preposition introducing the causee
adjunct is compared with the instrumental relator marking the causee in the French causative
sentence E27, it may be seen that the causee adjunct itself is like the demoted causee of a
causative construction.
E27. je ferais réparer ces chaussures par un bon cordonnier
F
R
‘I will have these shoes repaired by a good shoe-maker’
In both cases, the essential difference boils down to the fact that the verb of those Latin
constructions is not causativized. This fits well a dependent-marking type that relies very little
on verb valency and much more on the coding of semantic functions by case relators.
3.5 Transitivity alternations
At the end of section 2.4, we saw that agentivization is converse to deagentivization, and
consequently lack of causativization in a language system may be partly compensated by
detransitivization operations such as reflexivization, passivization and the like. This idea has
been pursued, for a number of languages, by starting from a list of thirty verbal concepts that
involve an optional agent, translating them into a couple of target languages and analyzing the
distribution of transitive and intransitive base verbs and of the kinds of derivational processes
in the paradigmatic relations between the two versions.
16
Responsible application of the
method to Latin would require a study of its own. T7 only illustrates the procedure for some
selected concepts, sorting them by their morphological relationship in Latin.
T7. Transitivity alternations
strategy meaning
intransitive
transitive
1. transitivization
causative compounding boil ferueo feruefacio
open pateo patefacio
preverbation hang pendo suspendo
2. detransitivization
passivization break frangor frango
dry siccor sicco
turn uertor uerto
roll uoluor uoluo
plunge mergor mergo
fill compleor compleo
reflexivization change me muto muto
turn me uerto uerto
3. symmetric
derivation/compounding wake up expergiscor
expergefacio
lexical causative alternation
burn ardeo uro
valency conversion turn uerto uerto
begin incipio incipio
15
It may here be recalled that the ergative case was called ‘causative case’ up to the 20
th
century.
16
Cf. Nedjalkov 1990, Haspelmath 1993, Nichols et al. 2004 and Comrie 2005.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 17
The quantitative proportion of the derivational processes in question in T7 is representative of
the behavior of the 30 concepts in Latin. To what extent it may characterize the language
system remains to be investigated. At any rate, it strikes the eye that while transitivization
plays no great role, detransitivization and symmetric strategies prevail.
4 Conclusion
Like other Indo-European languages of the ancient type, Latin may be characterized, with
respect to strategies of rearrangement of participant structure, by relying more on its
obligatory case marking than on valency operations. This means that if a certain participant is
needed, it is added in a suitable case, and when it is not needed, it is not expressed. None of
this affects verbal valency in any way. Given that any verb may have as many dependents as
makes sense, the essential remaining task is to mark that kind of constellation where a certain
participant which would be compatible with the meaning of the verb is definitely absent. This
is achieved to some extent by lexical causative alternations, but mostly by passivization. In
this, these ancient Indo-European languages are quite unusual in cross-linguistic comparison
(Nichols 1993).
In the written standard of the Latin language, there was no established grammaticalized
causative construction. It was only in Proto-Romance that the complex sentence based on
facio plus a.c.i. was grammaticalized as a dedicated causative construction.
Abbreviations in interlinear glosses
1, 2, 3 first, second, third person
ABS absolutive
ACC accusative
BEN benefactive
CAUS causative
DAT dative
ERG ergative
IMPFV imperfective
M masculine
PL plural
PST past
SG singular
TEL telic
References
Biville, Frédérique 1995, ‘Énoncés factitifs latin: syntaxe et sémantique.’ Longrée, Dominique (ed.),
De usu. Études de syntaxe latine offertes en hommage à Marius Lavency. Louvain-la-Neuve:
Peeters (Bibliothèque des Cahiers de l'Institut de Linguistique de Louvain, 70); 31-44.
Bodelot, Colette 2007, ‘Un exemple de grammaticalisation: Les aboutissants de facere + proposition
subordonnée / juxtaposée.’ Longrée, Dominique & Lenoble, Muriel (eds.), Actes du 13 colloque
international de linguistique latine. Bruxelles, 9 – 13 avril 2005. Louvain & Paris: Peeters
(Collection BEC); 000-000.
Brucale, Luisa & Mocciaro, Egle 2013, "Verbal compounding in Latin: the case of -
MAKE
verbs.
Contribution to the 17th International Colloquium on Latin Linguistics, Rome, May 20th - 25th,
2013".
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 18
Comrie, Bernard 1985, ‘Causative verb formation and other verb-deriving morphology.’ Shopen,
Timothy (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. III: Grammatical categories
and the lexicon. Cambridge etc.: Cambridge University Press; 309-348.
Comrie, Bernard 2005, "Transitivity pairs, markedness, and diachronic stability." Linguistics
44(2):303-318.
Dik, Simon C. 1985, ‘Formal and semantic adjustment of derived constructions.’ Bolkestein, A.
Machtelt et al. (eds.), Predicates and terms in functional grammar. Dordrecht & Cinnaminson:
Foris (Functional Grammar Series, 2); 1-28.
Dixon, Robert M.W. 2000, ‘A typology of causatives: form, syntax and meaning”. Dixon, Robert
M.W. & Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (eds.), Changing valency. Case studies in transitivity.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 30-83.
Fruyt, Michèle 2001, ‘Réflexions sur la notion de mot en latin: les verbes du type calefacio.’ Moussy,
Claude (ed.), De lingua Latina novae quaestiones. Actes du Xe Colloque International de
Linguistique Latine, Paris-Sèvres, 19-23 avril 1999. Louvain etc.: Peeters; 81-94.
García-Hernández, Benjamin 1989, ‘Complementarité lexicale et voix verbale’. Calboli, Gualtiero
(ed.), Subordination and other topics in Latin. Proceedings of the Third Colloquium on Latin
Linguistics, Bologna 1-5 April 1985. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: J. Benjamins (SLCS, 17); 289-
309.
Haspelmath, Martin 1993, ‘More on the typology of inchoative/causative verb alternations. Comrie,
Bernard et al. (eds.), Causatives and transitivity. Festschrift for Vladimir P. Nedyalkov on his 75th
birthday. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins; 87-120.
Hoffmann, Roland 2007, ‘On causativity in Latin.Longrée, Dominique & Lenoble, Muriel (eds.),
Actes du 13 colloque international de linguistique latine. Bruxelles, 9 – 13 avril 2005. Louvain &
Paris: Peeters (Collection BEC); 000-000.
Kittilä, Seppo 2002, Transitivity: towards a comprehensive typology. Turku: University of Turku
(Publications in General Linguistics, 5).
Kühner, Raphael & Stegmann, Carl 1962, Ausführliche Grammatik der lateinischen Sprache. Teil 2:
Satzlehre; 2 Bde. Leverkusen: Gottschalk (4. Aufl.; reimpr. Darmstadt: WBG, 1971).
Lehmann, Christian 2002, ‘Latin valency in typological perspective.’ Bolkestein, A. Machtelt &
Kroon, Caroline H.M. & Pinkster, Harm & Remmelink, H. Wim & Risselada, Rodie (eds.),
Theory and description in Latin linguistics. Selected papers from the XIth International
Colloquium on Latin Linguistics, Amsterdam June 24-29, 2001. Amsterdam: J.C. Gieben; 183-
203.
Lehmann, Christian 2006, ‘Les rôles sémantiques comme prédicats’. BSL 101/1:67-88.
Lehmann, Christian & Shin, Yong-Min & Verhoeven, Elisabeth 2004, Direkte und indirekte
Partizipation. Zur Typologie der sprachlichen Repräsentation konzeptueller Relationen. Erfurt:
Seminar für Sprachwissenschaft der Universität (ASSidUE, 13). 2nd rev. ed.
Nedjalkov, Vladimir P. 1990, Das Verhältnis zwischen semantischen und formalen Oppositionen in
verbaler Derivation. [Handout]. Leningrad: ANSSSR.
Nichols, Johanna 1993, "Transitive and causative in the Slavic lexicon: Evidence from Russian." Comrie,
Bernard & Polinsky, Maria (eds.), Causatives and transitivity. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: J.
Benjamins (Studies in Language Companion Series, 23); 69-86.
Nichols, Johanna & Peterson, David A. & Barnes, Jonathan 2004, 'Transitivizing and detransitivizing
languages.' Linguistic Typology 8:149-211.
Shibatani, Masayoshi 1996, ‘Applicatives and benefactives: A cognitive account.’ Shibatani,
Masayoshi & Thompson, Sandra A. (eds.), Grammatical constructions. Their form and meaning.
Oxford: Clarendon Press; 157-194.
Shibatani, Masayoshi 2006, "On the conceptual framework for voice phenomena." Linguistics
44(2):217-269.
Simone, Raffaele & Cerbasi, Donato 2001, ‘Types and diachronic evolution of Romance causative
constructions.’ Romanische Forschungen 113(3):441-473.
Christian Lehmann, Latin causativization in typological perspective 19
Talmy, Leonard 1976, "Semantic causative types." Shibatani, Masayoshi (ed.), The grammar of
causative constructions. New York etc.: Academic Press (Syntax and Semantics, 6); 43-116.
... Following Nichols, Peterson and Barnes (2004), I do not take into consideration analytic causative constructions, unless a causative periphrasis is the only way to express the induced member of a given verb pair (see Section 5.2.1). This means that analytic causative constructions, which are numerous and of different types in Latin, fall out of the scope of this study and will not be systematically discussed (see Simone and Cerbasi 2001;Brucale and Mocciaro 2016;Hoffman 2016: 45-60;Lehmann 2016). ...
... From the survey conducted in Section 5.2, it is remarkable that, with the exception of the limited use of facio-compounds, Latin lacks a productive pattern of formation of morphologically causative verbs (cf. Hoffmann 2016: 35;Lehmann 2016). Again, this fact is particularly striking if compared to the abundance of der-ivational verbal causative morphology attested in other ancient IE languages and that can be reconstructed for the protolanguage (cf. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This paper focuses on basic valency orientation in Latin, based on the typology laid out by Nichols, Peterson and Barnes (2004). The data analyzed shows that Latin does not feature a strong orientation in its basic valency, due to a widespread use of suppletion. Only with inanimate verbs can one detect a certain tendency for intransitivization via either verbal voice or the use of the reflexive pronoun se. Other more marginal strategies include the use of causative verbal compounds with -facio ‘make’ and lability. In this respect, the Latin data sharply contrasts with current reconstructions of Proto-Indo-European (PIE) as a transitivizing language, as well as with modern Romance languages, which make extensive use of intransitivization and lability. Such an intermediate position of Latin is historically explained as reflecting the convergence of different factors, chiefly the loss of the PIE causative morphology and the functional extension of the inherited mediopassive voice. Once put in a diachronic perspective, the Latin data provides unique insights on the dynamics, the direction, and the timing of the drift from transitivization to intransitivization that notoriously characterizes the Indo-European languages of Europe (Comrie 2006).
... 18 As is well known, doceo is the causative counterpart of disco 'to learn'. According to Lehmann (2016), the fact that they form a lexical causative alternation explains why doceo takes a double accusative: the semantically causative form retains the valency proper to the semantically "passive" verb, taking one accusative for the subject (the person learning) and one accusative for the object of derived form edoceo; celo 'to hide'; verbs of asking and demanding, which share the semantics of entreating and/or begging urgently, i.e. to quote the most frequent of them, flagito 'to demand (fiercely or violently), to entreat', oro 'to pray, to beg, to entreat', exoro 'to gain or obtain by entreaty', posco 'to ask for urgently, to beg, to demand', reposco 'to demand back, to ask for again', rogo 'to ask, to question, to interrogate', rogito 'to ask or inquire with eagerness'. In defining the syntactic behavior of verbs belonging to this group, one could say that they have neutral alignment, giving rise to a double object construction. ...
Article
This paper aims to describe the behavior of Latin verbs with three arguments basically encoding the transfer of a Theme from an Agent to a Recipient : these verbs, labelled as ditransitives within the framework of linguistic typology, will be analyzed here on the basis of this theoretical approach. Across languages, the object arguments of ditransitives may be marked as the Patient of monotransitives or differently from it, giving rise to various types of alignment : the most frequent are the indirective , the secundative and the neutral alignment . In Latin, these three basic types of alignment are attested with ditransitives in the active and passive voice, although they are differently distributed across the lexicon. Moreover, some verbs allow more than one type of encoding of the relevant arguments, showing alternating alignments. I shall focus on the reasons of the distribution and alternation of the three types, with the goal of providing a point of departure for a deeper understanding of ditransitives in the Latin language.
Article
The present study gives a comprehensive portrait of the accusatival-passival type of the composite-predicate (or support-verb) construction in the history of Latin. Taking stock of properties shows them to only minimally serve as a touchstone distinguishing between Latin composite predicates and syntagmatically identical verb + direct object constructs. The intricate issue of idiomaticity and the prospects of establishing its parameters are discussed, concluding that although characteristics such as figurativity of the verb or direct-object incorporation indicate this status, the closed corpus of Latin securely allows only for a quantitative parameter, or familiarity, for this kind of a complex predicate. We are looking from the diachronic perspective at individual support verbs, primarily capere and habere , in the evolvement of their function and status—grammatical or lexical-phraseological—as well as at other sets of verbs, figurative ones and verbs otherwise departing from their basic meaning. The support verb emerges as the cardinal constituent of the construct, determining the function of the complex, and as considerably autonomous, being capable of representing, in anaphoric contexts, the entire construction. A reconsideration of the opposition between composite predicates and their monolexematic counterparts brings up again their added value as to exhaustiveness, clarity, perspective, and focus of the message conveyed through a composite predicate.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, I formulate and explain a number of universal generalizations about the formation of causative verbs (overtly marked verbs with causal meaning) and anticausative verbs (overtly marked verbs with noncausal meaning). Given the “spontaneity scale” of basic verb meanings (transitive > unergative > automatic unaccusative > costly unaccusative > agentful), we can say that verb pairs with a noncausal verb higher on the scale tend to be causative pairs, and verb pairs with a noncausal verb lower on the scale tend to be anticausative pairs. I propose that these generalizations can be subsumed under form-frequency correspondence: That transitive base verbs tend to form causatives (often analytic causatives) is because they rarely occur in causal contexts, and the fact that unaccusative verbs tend to be coded as anticausatives is because they frequently occur in causal contexts, and special marking is required for the rarer and less expected situation.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper our propose is to analyze the Latin collocation poena afficere , ‘to inflict punishment’, a type of collocation having both syntactic and semantic specificities that distinguish it from the prototypical ones: the predicative noun functions as third argument of the support verb, a syntactic scheme that is ideal, as we shall attempt to demonstrate, for the expression of causative predicates. From the examples of poena afficere documented in a large text corpus, we will intend to highlight the main features of this type of collocation. For its description and formalization, we will use the theoretical framework proposed by the Meaning-Text Theory.
Article
Full-text available
Cet article se donne pour objectif de contribuer à l’explicitation de la notion de rôle sémantique. Comme il est bien connu, les relateurs casuels peuvent naître par la grammaticalisation de coverbes. Ces constructions nous font entendre mieux les procédés sémantiques. On distinguera deux niveaux de rôles sémantiques : • celui des classes de situations et de prédicats qui leur correspondent – comme mouvement, transmission, communication etc. –, dans lesquelles les partici­pants reçoivent des rôles génériques tels qu’actor, undergoer, but etc. • celui d’un prédicat individuel comme fendre à partir duquel il est attribué aux participants des rôles spécifiques, ainsi de fendeur et fendu. Ces rôles sont spécifiés par le contenu lexical du verbe. Dans une situation complexe comme je fends le bois en utilisant une hache, il y a deux prédicats, un prédicat central (fendre) et un prédicat latéral (utiliser). Si ce dernier se grammaticalise, son contenu lexical se réduit, il devient un prédicat élémentaire et, en dernière analyse, un relateur casuel. Ce faisant, ses rôles sémantiques spécifiques deviennent à leur tour génériques. Il en résulte qu’un rôle sémantique périphérique d’un prédicat central, ici l’instrument de fendre, est le rôle sémantique spécifique d’un prédicat élémentaire qui lui sert de prédicat latéral. Cette conception sera démontrée à partir d’exemples empruntés au japonais, au chinois et à d’autres langues.
Article
Full-text available
This article attempts to lay the conceptual foundations of voice phenomena, ranging from the familiar active/passive contrast to the ergative/antipassive opposition, as well as voice functions of split case-marking in both transitive and intransitive constructions. We advance the claim that major voice phenomena have conceptual bases rooted in the human cognition of actions, which have evolutionary properties pertaining to their origin, development, and termination. The notion of transitivity is integral to the study of voice as evident from the fact that the so-called transitivity parameters identified by Hopper and Thompson (1980) and others are in the main concerned with these evolutionary properties of an action, and also from the fact that the phenomena dealt with in these studies are mostly voice phenomena. A number of claims made in past studies of voice and in some widely-received definitions of voice are shown to be false. In particular, voice oppositions are typically based on conceptual - as opposed to pragmatic - meanings, may not alter argument alignment patterns, may not change verbal valency, and may not even trigger verbal marking. There are also voice oppositions more basic and wide-spread than the active/passive system, upon which popular definitions of voice are typically based.
Article
Full-text available
Si descrive l'evoluzione delle strutture sintattiche causative dal latino alle lingue romanze, mostrando che il latino già conteneva virtualmente tutte le soluzioni che le lingue romanze hanno poi elaborato per rispondere alla necessità di esprimere le strutture di eventi di tipo causativo. In base a criteri sia sintattici che semantici, si propone una scala a tre livelli delle strutture causative romanze, secondo il modo in cui le lingue romanze trattano il grado di forza dei causativi ed il modo in cui pongono i due predicati nell'enunciato.
Article
Since causativity is either scarcely or not at all marked in Latin, most grammars and textbooks do not mention this verbal category. Nevertheless, from a functional point of view, there are many different means of causative constructions (CCs) in Latin, especially upon examination of analytical forms. After some methodological remarks in
Article
Semantically basic and almost universally intertranslatable verbs such as 'sit', 'fear', 'laugh', 'break', and 'fall' are not always formally underived, and languages differ systematically in the formal treatment they give to these verbs vis-à-vis their transitive counterparts (respectively 'seat', 'scare', 'amuse', 'break', 'drop'). A set of 18 basic verb glosses and their semantic causative counterparts is used here as a convenient probe to test whether a language tends to treat intransitives as basic and transitives as derived, vice versa, both, or neither. Accordingly, in their overall lexical cast languages fall into four major types: transitivizing, detransitivizing, neutral, and indeterminate. The general typological parameter is (lexical or basic) VALENCE ORIENTATION. In a genealogically based sample of 80 languages, basic valence orientation is shown to interact with alignment but is independent of most other standard typological metrics, and serves as a useful building block for characterizing the lexical and morphosyntactic signatures of languages, language areas, and language families.
Article
Utilizing the classification of formal relations in causative-inchoative verb pairs developed by Haspelmath (1993), this article examines two further ramifications. First, the 30 verbs in his list are divided into two groups of fifteen, one where crosslinguistically a causative pairing is most likely and the other where the anticausative pairing is most likely. Individual languages are then tested to see whether their verbs conform to this ordering, with overall very positive results. Second, the diachronic stability of languages with predominance of one or other of the individual types within the classification, in particular of the causative and anticausative types, is examined, with some results suggesting stability at least over millennia. Thus, Classical Arabic and Maltese have very similar profiles, even down to individual lexical concepts (even where the two languages have noncognate lexical items). Uralic languages, despite their close proximity to Indo-European languages with predominance of the anticausative type, still show predominance either of the causative type (Finnish, Udmurt), or of the equipollent type (Hungarian), the latter perhaps reflecting a compromise between the putative predominance of the causative type in Proto-Uralic and contact with Indo-European.
Études de syntaxe latine offertes en hommage à Marius Lavency
  • Frédérique Biville
Biville, Frédérique 1995, 'Énoncés factitifs latin: syntaxe et sémantique.' Longrée, Dominique (ed.), De usu. Études de syntaxe latine offertes en hommage à Marius Lavency. Louvain-la-Neuve: Peeters (Bibliothèque des Cahiers de l'Institut de Linguistique de Louvain, 70); 31-44.