Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E Open Access
An examination of Vygotsky’s socio-cultural
theory in second language acquisition: the
role of higher order thinking enhancing
techniques and the EFL learners’use of
private speech in the construction of
reasoning
Sepideh Mirzaee and Parviz Maftoon
*
* Correspondence:
pmaftoon@gmail.com
Department of English Language,
Science and Research Branch,
Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran
Abstract
The present study investigates the impact of higher order thinking enhancing
techniques as two post-reading strategies on the EFL students’reasoning power as
determined by their private speech production. Also, the relationship between the
learners’private speech production and their reasoning power is also investigated. In
so doing, the study utilizes a quasi-experimental design with 30 participants in each
control and experimental groups. The results of the pretest administration indicate
that the participants of the two groups are homogenous regarding their language
proficiency level as determined by the Babel Test, and their reasoning power as
determined by the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal (W-GCTA). The participants
in the experimental group are instructed in a way to enhance their reasoning power
basedon6readingcomprehensiontexts.WGCTA’s two subtests of deduction and
inference making which contain reasoning-gap tasks are utilized as the posttest. While
the participants are engaged in carrying out such tasks, their private speech productions
are recorded. Data analysis and transcription indicate that private speech which is
categorized into 4 classes in this study has a positive and significant influence on the
learners’reasoning power. Higher order thinking enhancing techniques are also found
to have impact on the experimental group in enhancing their private speech production
and subsequently improving their reasoning. The detailed results, discussion, and
conclusions of the research are further presented.
Keywords: Logical reasoning, Private speech, Higher order thinking enhancing
techniques, Reasoning-gap tasks, Quasi-experimental design, Watson-Glaser critical
thinking appraisal
Introduction
The human’s cognitive development, in general, and higher order abilities, in
particular, have long been under debate. Studies on these issues started by carrying out
a bulk of research over children and after that on adults. Gradually, linguists became
among those who were interested in working on the relationship between human’s
Asian-Pacific Journal of Second
and Foreign Language Education
© The Author(s). 2016 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and
indicate if changes were made.
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and
Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18
DOI 10.1186/s40862-016-0022-7
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
cognitive development and language learning. When Vygotsky (1986) proposed socio-
cultural theory (SCT), a new perspective was presented regarding the relationship be-
tween human beings’cognitive development and language learning. However, the most
outstanding studies in this domain were limited to the child’s cognitive development
and less were related to adults’.
In fact, Vygotsky (1986) maintains that SCT is regarded as a theory of human cogni-
tive development and higher mental functions. He states that the human cognitive de-
velopment and higher mental functions initiate from social communications; and while
people participate in social activities, they are involved in mental and communicative
functions. As a matter of fact, SCT considers mental functioning as a mediated process
in which there should be some artifacts in order to mediate between the human’s
psychological and social worlds.
Therefore, the present study compatible with SCT, in general, and private speech, as
well as higher order thinking, in particular, aimed at analyzing the private speech use of
participants in order to determine their power of logical reasoning while doing
reasoning-gap tasks to see whether there are any significant differences between those
who are treated using specific higher order thinking enhancing techniques in reading
comprehension (RC) and those who are not.
Hereafter, to achieve the very primary goal, the present researchers hold that by
studying the specific instances of learners’private speech in which it can be categorized
into a variety of types, we are able to further understand the relationship among higher
order thinking enhancing techniques, private speech, and learners’reasoning. In other
words, this effort is meaningful since it helps us have a better understanding of the na-
ture of private speech, and it entails the exploration of its different types and its rela-
tion to learners’power of logical reasoning.
Relevant literature
On private speech, reasoning, and higher order thinking enhancing techniques
As to Berk (1992), private speech is “a critical intermediate stage in the transition
from external social communication to internal self-direction and as the corner-
stone of all higher cognitive processes, including selective attention, voluntary
memory, planning, concept formation and self- reflection”(cited in Fernyhough &
Lloyd, 1999, p. 34). Accordingly, for Vygotsky (1978), the advent of private speech
at about 3 years of age initiates in primary socialized speech, which steadily divides
into two functionally particular types: speech applied in order to communicate with
others and speech only directed at the self. As to Stanley (2011), “since private
speech branches off from social speech, it becomes thought spoken out loud”
(p.16) and “an externalized self-monitoring system, that plans, directs and controls
behavior”(Bivens & Berk, 1990, p. 444).
Until the 1980s, Vygotsky’s ideas about the private speech role as a mediational tool
in child development was not extended to second language (L2) acquisition. Lantolf
and Frawley were among the first who started working on private speech in the domain
of adults’second language acquisition (SLA). The studies conducted by Lantolf and
Frawley (1984), as well as Frawley and Lantolf (1985) showed that picture narration
tasks in these studies explored probable parallels between Vygotsky’s conclusions for
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 2 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
children and the private speech used by L2 learners. Regarding the production of pri-
vate speech, Frawley and Lantolf (1985) found that adult English as a second language
(ESL) learners and native English-speaking children showed parallel behavior. They
were engaged in the picture narration task and they produced a considerable amount
of private speech in order to maintain or regain self-regulation in the task but both
groups had trouble completing these tasks. Though, the sources of the difficulty were
not the same (i.e., children could not organize their narration, whereas the ESL
learners’problems were in their lack of skill in the language).
In a related study, the likelihood of some parallels between mastery of the com-
municative L2 use and a reduction in the use of private speech were investigated
by Lantolf and Frawley (1984). However, since private speech is extremely situated,
they specified that this issue did not suggest that the learner with native-like profi-
ciency would be self-regulated all the time and not require generating any private
speech. In a similar vein, the relationship between the L2 proficiency and the use
of private speech which was a similar picture narration study was carried out by
McCafferty (1994). His findings seem to “support the hypothesis that with in-
creased proficiency, learners’use of private speech diminishes”(p. 131). However,
McCafferty concludes that “a number of other factors have been shown to influ-
ence private speech production”(p.133). The type of the task, task difficulty
(Appel, 1986), the goal of the task, the number of participants involved, task mo-
dality, the performance mode of the task, the amount of concern a participant feels
about the consequence of the task, and the cultural experience of the participants
are among these (McCafferty, 1992). Similarly, private speech was considered as
the main focus of Saville-Troike’s (1988) research of children operating L2 forms in
their private speech that were used in their social speech later.
To direct their own activity, as to Donato (1994), learners talk aloud to themselves while
they are ignoring what their partners are saying, or whisper to themselves when they are
trying to do problem-solving tasks. It is observed that students whisper self-addressed di-
rectives (‘wait’), self-addressed negation (‘no’), self-addressed questions with hidden refer-
ents such as ‘how do you say that?,’‘how would you say that?,’‘how do I say that?’. In fact,
the referent of that is being recognized merely to the speaker. Learners repeat new or dif-
ficult L2 forms in a form of whispering and translate those forms into their first language
for themselves. In addition, they whisper emotional expressions (‘oh’), and opening and
closing words such as ‘Let’ssee,’‘OK.’‘Now what?’. In such studies, private speech is in-
spired by the social context; it is realized that it has a self-regulatory function and pro-
motes “linguistic development and the internalization of collective knowledge”(p. 48).
In ‘what is called thinking’Heidegger (cited in Peters, 2008) mentions a variety of
thinking that has been perceived by philosophers within the Western tradition. There-
fore, based on his thoughts, thinking can be considered as:
✓Doxa: forming an opinion or having an idea (opining)
✓Vorstellen: representing a state of affairs (representing)
✓Ratiocination: developing a chain of premises leading to a valid conclusion
(reasoning)
✓Problem-solving: scientific thinking (problem-solving)
✓Beriff (Hegel): conceptual or systematic thinking (conceiving)
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 3 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Or possibly it is not mistaken if one contemplates reasoning as one of the subdivi-
sions of critical thinking skills because for instance, Watson and Glaser (2002), the cre-
ators of the world’s frequently used measure of critical thinking, linked it to the
following abilities: Inferences drawn from factual statements; recognition of assump-
tions in a series of statements; interpreting whether conclusions are warranted or not;
determine if conclusions follow from information in given statements, and evaluating
arguments as being strong and relevant or weak and irrelevant (pp. 21–22).
In fact, part one of this appraisal which is called ‘inference’requires one to distin-
guish among degrees of truth or falsity of inference drawn from given data. Further,
part three of this appraisal which is called ‘deduction’requires one to decide whether
definite conclusions inevitably follow from information in given statements or premises.
If these two parts’definitions are compared with the Heidegger’s concept of reasoning
which was mentioned at the beginning of this part, it can be understood that they are
mostly accorded. Another evidence that we can use in order to validate that reasoning
can be considered as a subcategory of the huge concept of critical thinking is the Scha-
fersman’s (1991) classification of diverse subparts of critical thinking in which he pin-
points ‘reasonable thinking’which is described by a dependence on reason to search
for and determine reliable knowledge.
There are some studies indicating the relationship between reasoning power and pri-
vate speech use of the individuals. As to Winsler et al. (2007), self-talk serves to con-
nect words, actions, and ideas and helps planning and critical thinking. Accordingly,
Munro (2009) claims that the ability to deliberately attend to the way we think is often
designated as reasoning.
Moreover, the development of private speech is influenced by a particular emphasis
placed on the knowledge of the language which is regarded as a platform for self-talk.
Likewise, language permits the storing and recovery of information that is known cog-
nition, as well as reasoning about what an individual does or does not know which is
called metacognition (Winsler et al. 2007).
Smith (2007) observed both the function and the consequences of private speech dur-
ing a group activity which may result in the activation of one’s own mind. He also con-
cluded that the act of uttering a word or phrase can simplify problem solving and
attention to a particular aspect of a problem as well.
The study performed by Salmon (2008) took place in two Reggio-inspired schools
where contributing teachers considered children’s work as part of their teaching. He
used some methods in order to make children’s thinking observable as they installed
the culture and language of thinking in their classrooms through the use of thinking
routines. The results showed that thinking routines build up positive views about think-
ing and learning. In fact, children established metacognitive and critical thinking skills
which made them more attentive to situations that call for thinking.
According to Salmon, private speech engages our critical thinking, helps us consider
alternatives and consequences, and make reasonable decisions. Private speech is used
to control impulses and to think before acting. Reading skills simplify the progress of
private speech.
Likewise, Centeno-Cortes and Jimenez Jimenez (2004) have reviewed the literature
on private speech, formulating a category exclusively for the function of reasoning dur-
ing problem-solving tasks, which they called private verbal thinking (PVT). The second
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 4 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
aspect of their study focused on discovering how PVT occurs for three different groups
of Spanish speakers: native speakers with a high degree of proficiency in English and
two groups of American university students, one at advanced language proficiency level
and one at intermediate language proficiency level. The participants had to answer a
number of challenging questions of a general nature in Spanish. Findings showed nu-
merous forms of self-regulatory PVT. Moreover, PVT was found to follow particular
patterns during reasoning. Additionally, the language of thought for PVT proved to be
exclusively Spanish for all. Both the advanced and intermediate L2 speakers of Spanish
used both Spanish and English when solving the problems, but the advanced speakers
tried explicitly to maintain Spanish for thinking. Overall, the researchers suggest, fol-
lowing Ushakova (1994) that it is unlikely that PVT changes completely from the L1 to
the L2 even at advanced levels of proficiency. Also, the authors make a plea for teachers
to respect the need for students to use their L1 to think through problems. Another
contribution of the study concerns the explication of how participants at the different
levels of proficiency deployed different forms of PVT in relation to L2 learning.
Concept maps as the first technique of enhancing students’reasoning power are
regarded as visual representations of knowledge which allow us to recognize the con-
nection between ideas by generating a graphic map of the relations. They consist of
concepts, encircled in boxes, and linking lines demonstrating the relationships between
concepts or propositions (Canas, 2003). In constructing concept maps, concept words
or phrases are referred to as nodes and are placed in boxes or circles. The connecting
structures between the nodes are termed links and are signified by labeled link. The
concepts are commonly rank ordered from the most general, most comprehensive con-
cept to the most detailed, least common concept. According to Canas et al. (2003), for
concept map construction, a standard process includes defining the topic for focus
question, recognizing, and listing the most significant concepts that are linked with that
topic, ordering the concepts from highest to lowest, and adding and tagging linking
phrases.
Furthermore, Angelo and Cross (1993) designate that concept maps develop student
capabilities in definite critical areas. Among these are:
✓The ability to draw reasonable inferences from observations.
✓The ability to synthesize and integrate information and ideas.
✓The ability to learn concepts and theories in the subject area.
In a study, Carrell et al. (1989) investigated the usefulness of text mapping techniques
in improving second language reading as a good alternative to traditional prereading
and postreading activities. In so doing, they used semantic mapping as a tool in order
to present the important vocabulary of the passage to be read and to provide the
teacher with a tool to assess the students’prior knowledge of the topic. Furthermore,
as the result of incorporating concept mapping technique into reading or postreading
phase, the learners learned to display interrelationships prevailing in the passage by rec-
ognizing the main ideas and putting them in circles or boxes, as well as forming the
connection between ideas and putting them in circles or boxes (Ghanizadeh, 2007).
Besides, as to Pintrich and De Groot (1990), learners’self-regulated learning skill is
regarded as one of the powerful factors amongst cognitive ability to raise meaningful
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 5 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
understanding. As Corno and Mandinach (1983) hold, those who have weak self-
regulated learning skill are less competent to guide their own learning. Consequently,
they take less advantage of their learning environment. Therefore, it can be concluded
that using concept map strategy to develop learners’metacognitive skill can be consid-
ered as an influential means to stimulate self-regulation learning skills. It will support
learners to be engaged in the meaningful learning in the long run.
Similarly, Talebinezhad and Negari (2007) investigated the influence of concept map-
ping as a learning strategy on EFL learners’self-regulation in writing classes. Their find-
ings established that concept mapping enhances students’self-regulation.
The researchers of the study assume the effectiveness of concept mapping on enhan-
cing learners’reasoning, based on Khodadady and Ghanizadrh (2010) who contend that
concept mapping is regarded as organizational tools such that concept mapping scaf-
folds a variety types of reasoning about content, make students think critically about
the content of whatever they study. They maintain that the application of this tech-
nique to increase students’reasoning ability while reading requires “assessing argu-
ments and statements, deducing conclusions and hypotheses, detecting inconsistencies
and common mistakes in reasoning, and making inferences”(p. 52). In fact, as to
Khodadady and Ghanizadeh, it can be asserted that there is a range of properties inher-
ent in concept mapping which may help to the improvement of learners’higher order
ability. All concept map qualities fall effectively within higher order abilities related
with making meaning, deduction, argument evaluation, and interpretation, all of which
are revelations of reasoning ability.
The second technique of enhancing higher order thinking used in this study was ar-
gumentation. To give an argument means to try to persuade someone by giving worthy
reasons (Ghanizadeh & Mirzaee, 2012).
As to Bowell and Kemp (2005), an argument has the following structure:
➢a set of assumptions or premises;
➢a method of reasoning or deduction;
➢a conclusion or point.
The premises can be considered as any statement that we have to create which are
not themselves confirmed at the moment. Premises may be undeniable pieces of know-
ledge; or they may be propositions that we make for the sake of argument. In fact, they
are the preliminary ideas of an argument. Some premises are definitions; some others
are factual declarations about things. For instance, ‘X is a book’is regarded as a factual
declaration about a thing. It is depending on whether it is true or false. Some factual
assertions are very straightforward (such as ‘X is a book’); others are more noticeably
inductive in character for example, ‘most children are allergic to peanuts’. The conclu-
sion is whatever proposition the argument is trying to show. Thus, the conclusion can
be a premise in a later argument. Regarding argumentation, Bowell and Kemp believe
that we are often faced with arguments that endeavor to convince us, to influence our
beliefs and actions, by giving us some reasons to believe this or that, or to act in this
way or that. The essential thing to understand here is that an attempt to persuade by
argument is “an attempt to provide you with reasons for believing a claim, desiring
something or doing something”(p. 5).
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 6 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Because of the important role of socio-cultural theory in language acquisition, it
seems necessary to investigate the private speech role in the reasoning process of Eng-
lish as a foreign language (EFL) learners in dealing with reasoning-gap tasks. In the
current study, higher order thinking enhancing techniques and private speech use were
hypothesized to positively influence EFL learners’reasoning ability.
Therefore, the following research questions were investigated in this study:
Q1. Is EFL learners’use of private speech correlated with reasoning?
Q1.1. Do parts of private speech–repetition, translation, question, and filler–enhance
advanced EFL learners’reasoning?
Q2. Do higher order thinking enhancing techniques make any statistically significant
difference between the reasoning power of the control and experimental groups as
determined by their private speech?
Q2.1.Do higher order thinking enhancing techniques make any statistically significant
difference between the inductive reasoning of the control and experimental groups
as determined by their private speech?
Q2.2. Do higher order thinking enhancing techniques make any statistically significant
difference between the deductive reasoning of the control and experimental groups
as determined by their private speech?
Method
The present study is regarded as a quasi-experimental one which includes treatment
and pretest/posttest phases.
Participants and setting
The participants of the current study were chosen based on intact group design (Hatch
& Lazaraton, 1991). They were from both genders, males and females. All of them were
attending English RC course, at English Language Teaching major at a University in
Iran. It seemed that using the techniques of enhancing higher order thinking ability
was more in accordance with the expository texts used to be taught during this course.
Students ranged in age from 20 to 32 years old. The data were gathered during their
5th semester of their Bachelor’s program. Students belonged to two university classes,
one of them was regarded as the control and another was the experimental group. Be-
sides, for the experimental group, two types of higher order thinking enhancing tech-
niques for their RC class were taught. By discarding the students who were outliers, the
researchers selected 30 participants for each group. All of them were non-native
speakers of English in Iran whose first language was Persian. English students in Iran
should pass four RC courses during the four years of Bachelor of Art (B. A.) graduation
studies.
In order to be certain of students’homogeneity at the outset of the study, it was re-
quired to have the tests to check whether they were homogeneous. Participants needed
to be checked for homogeneity in two domains, one was their language proficiency
level using the Babel Test, and another was their homogeneity in their power of logical
reasoning using the W-GCTA as pretests. Results of the tests showed that participants
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 7 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
of both groups were homogeneous in their language proficiency, as well as their rea-
soning power.
Instrumentation
The Babel English Language Placement Test was the first instrument used throughout
the current study. The test was used in order to check the participants’general lan-
guage proficiency level. Kuder- Richardson (KR-21) Formula was used to measure the
reliability index of the current test which was reported as .75.
The second instrument was the W-GCTA used for both pretest and posttest. There
are five different sections included in the WGCTA. They are specially designed in order
to check every learner’s ability to think analytically and logically. The five sections are
as follows (Watson & Glaser, 2010):
✓to make correct inferences
✓to recognize assumptions
✓to make deductions
✓to come to conclusions
✓to interpret and evaluate arguments
The two subsections of making inference and deduction were chosen for the purpose
of this study including appropriate reasoning-gap tasks. Students were required to do
the tasks in parts one (number 1 to 16) and three (number 33 to 48) only (because
these two parts were in accordance with the purpose of the study to measure learners’
power of logical reasoning in order to check their deductive and inductive reasoning
after the treatment). Therefore, tasks were arranged from number one up to number
32 in which from number one up to number 16 inference making tasks were put. De-
duction part started from the task 17 up to the task 32. Attached to each test paper
was an answer sheet. For the tasks one to 16 answers were in the multiple choice for-
mat in which there were five alternatives. Alternatives for these tasks started with True,
Probably True, Insufficient Data, Probably False, and False. Each student was provided
with a copy of an answer sheet and s/he was told to choose the best options on the an-
swer sheet. The instruction for each part of the questionnaire was written on the ques-
tion paper. The students were also told about the instruction for each part of the
questionnaire. Moreover, for the first section of the questionnaire, the students were
asked to read three reading passages. For the first two passages, there were 10 tasks,
five for each. The last passages included six tasks. The students were required to decide
which option could be best suited based on the content of the reading text.
In the second part, each task included some premises followed by several proposed
conclusions. In order to do such tests, students should consider the statements in each
exercise as true without exception. After reading the conclusion below the statement, if
the students think that it necessarily follows from the statements given, they can make
a black mark under conclusion follows in the appropriate place on the answer sheet. If
the students think it is not anecessary conclusion from the statements given, they
should put a black mark under conclusion doesn’t follow, albeit they may believe it to
be true based on their general knowledge.
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 8 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Besides, the test required to be validated again. To determine the validity of the two
subscales of W-GCTA that is, inference making and deduction, a confirmatory factor ana-
lysis (CFA) utilizing the LISREL 8.50 statistical package was performed. Each module went
through CFA separately. A number of fit indices were examined to evaluate the model fit.
It can be concluded that the model had a moderate fit regarding these two subtests.
Figures 1 and 2 present the results of making the two subsections of W-GCTA valid.
In this study, the total reliability of this test was measured by Cronbach’s alpha to ver-
ify the reliability of the questions used which was found to be 0.77.
Procedure
The present study was carried out in 12 sessions during an academic semester. Tables 1
and 2 present the summaries of treatment for the experimental group during these ses-
sions. As mentioned before, concept mapping and argumentation are regarded as two
higher order thinking enhancing techniques.
The texts were chosen for the two groups. The tests conducted and the numbers of
sessions were identical for both groups. Six reading texts were selected to teach, three
of them were chosen from among their course book (i.e., Inside Reading IV by
Richmond, 2009). And three others were IELSTS expository texts selected by the re-
searchers. Having worked on the first three texts based on the instructions provided by
the teacher, students were asked to fill in the blanks of the concept maps provided for
them in order to be more familiar with concept mapping. For the next three reading
texts, students were required to draw their own concept maps based on the instruc-
tions and their previous experiences about concept mapping. Having finished working
on concept mapping, the students were getting familiar with the argumentation tech-
nique and how they can determine premises and conclusions in the texts in line with
the recognition of argument structures by Bowell and Kemp (2005).
Finally, in the twelfth session which was regarded as the last session of carrying out
the process of data collection, the participants were asked to answer the 32 questions
of the W-GCTA. The reason for selecting this type of questionnaire for the participants
Fig. 1 The schematic representation of inference-making and the corresponding items
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 9 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
of this study was that these types of questions were recommended, since according to
McCafferty (1994), if the tasks are difficult, the probability of producing private speech
will be higher. In general, as to Duncan and Cheyne (2002), the same has been found
in children’production of private speech in which children produced more private
speech when working on difficult tasks compared to easier tasks, a pattern that sup-
ports that private speech is regarded as a form of thinking and a means of problem-
solving. The present study investigated this effect with an adult sample. Therefore, the
W-GCTA was regarded as possessing the appropriate tasks in this regard.
At the time of the exam, the participants were provided with a pen and a piece of
paper, and they were told to use them in the case of need. Students were aware of the
fact that they could use their pen and paper but they were also told that using pen and
paper would subtract a point from them. Since pen and paper could serve as media-
tional artifacts that could facilitate their thinking process (Centeno-Cortes & Jimenez
Jimenez 2004). So, the students were implicitly discouraged from using this external aid
instead of private speech. It was believed that this tactic would foster the production of
private speech. The questionnaire consisted of 80 questions divided into five parts: (1)
inference making, (2) recognition of assumptions, (3) deduction, (4) interpretation, and
(5) evaluation of arguments. The students were required to do the tasks in parts one
(number one to 16) and three (number 33 to 48) only (because these two parts were in
accordance with the purpose of the study to measure learners’power of logical reason-
ing). Extra questions in the W-GCTA (i.e., part two: recognition of assumptions, part
Table 1 A Summary of Treatment Sessions for Concept Mapping
Session 1 Pretest for checking students’language proficiency
Session 2 Pretest for checking students’power of logical reasoning
Session 3 Introducing main and supporting ideas for single paragraphs
Session 4 Introducing main and supporting ideas of multi paragraph passages
Session 5 Drawing concept map
Session 6 Drawing concept maps based on step by step procedure they were provided with
Fig. 2 The schematic representation of deduction and the corresponding items
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 10 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
four: interpretation and part five: evaluation of arguments) were omitted and two other
sections (i.e., inference making and deduction) were kept.
In order to consider the ethical value of the study, the participants were aware that
the results of the study will be used in a language research. The process of scoring for
them was that they obtained one point for their correct answer to each question, they
got no points for those answers which were incomplete or wrong, and one point was
deduced if they had used paper and pen to discover the answer. Consequently, although
the participants were told they were free to use pen and paper during problem-solving,
since as to Centeno-Cortes and Jimenez Jimenez (2004), these tools were regarded as
mediational artifacts (object regulation) to make their thinking process during solving
tasks easy, by deducting a point for using them the research was implicitly discouraging
them from using these tools instead of private speech.
The tasks students did, were considered as posttest tasks. Approximately,
60 min’time lasted for taking the test. The participants were comprehensively
briefed on what exactly they were supposed to do in this session. The participants’
private speech were audiotaped in an almost quiet lab with few distractions,
through a set of personal computers and a headset for every participant individu-
ally while they were engaging in solving the reasoning-gap tasks in two sessions
for the two groups of control and experimental, separately. During this process, all
participants were audio-taped. A stereo headset was adjusted to the participants’
heads and connected to the audio port of the computer cases. It is worthy of note
that students did not hear anything through the headset but it was assumed that
when participants’ears were covered, in order to hear their private speech to
analyze the tasks, they had to produce louder voices. Additionally, the recording
level and volume were attuned so that the software could detect even the lowest
sounds. In this way, high-quality digital recordings were obtained.
Data analysis
In analyzing data obtained from audio taped materials, the researchers, in line with
Ohta (2001) and Sonmez (2011), divided private speech of the second language learners
into four main types of repetition, translation, filler, and question. Additionally, the
protocol produced by learners in the posttest session were coded and analyzed. As to
Diaz and Berk (1992), appropriate procedures for classification of private speech should
Table 2 A Summary of Treatment Sessions for Argumentation
Sessions 7 & 8 Introducing concept of argumentation and its introductory aspects
Sessions 9 & 10 Indicating premises and conclusions more skillfully by providing them with more examples
Session 11 Checking and discussing the students’understanding of premises and conclusions
throughout the whole class
Table 3 Students’Descriptive Statistics to Check Homogeneity of Proficiency
Groups n M SD SE(M)
Babel Control 30 86.53 7.20 1.31
Experimental 30 82.60 13.05 2.38
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 11 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
depend on some specific research questions being explored and coding systems should
be specifically designed concerning the hypotheses being tested.
Each participant’s recorded private speech while performing the tasks were listened
carefully by the researchers, two times. In the first time, they were listened in order to
check whether any one of them had produced private speech or not. In the second
time, those who had produced private speech were listened one more time in order to
analyze their speech and categorize their voices into appropriate categories, (i.e.,
whether they were of repetition, translation, filler, or question). Having coded the data,
the researchers counted the frequency of each type of private speech produced, and,
did some related and essential analysis. Also, before putting data in the statistical pack-
age for the social sciences (SPSS) software in order to analyze them statistically, the re-
searchers wanted two more raters who were their students to kindly consider the data
again in order to be sure of the identification of various types of private speech and the
sound analysis.
The present researchers dealt with the recorded data by transcribing them com-
pletely. The transcription conventions are listed below.
As to Diaz et al. (1992) and Feigenbaum (1992), the utterance is the unit of analysis
in most private speech research. It is defined as a complete sentence, a sentence frag-
ment, a clause with deliberate markers of closing, a conversational turn, or any se-
quence of speech which is detached from another by at least 2 s, temporally. Diaz et al.
hold that no temporal or semantic discontinuities were included in an utterance. A
temporal discontinuity contains a pause of at least 2 s. A semantic discontinuity com-
prises any significant change of content. Moreover,many other researchers including
Appel and Lantolf (1994), Lantolf and Frawley (1984, 1985), and McCafferty (1992,
1994a) in the socio-cultural framework applied utterances as the unit of analysis.
Therefore, in this study utterances were used as the unit of analysis as well.
In fact, the impetus of employing private speech coding and categorization in this
study was in line with Cohen and Upton’s (2006) ideas that “the act of reading a text to
understand it and that of taking a test about the text involve cognitive processes that
are not for the most part observable, nor are the strategies deployed by the reader/test
taker for the purpose of accomplishing these activities”(p.11). As to Cohen and Upton,
in order to get the best result conceivable of what readers do while they are reading test
prompts and answering to test items, verbal protocols are typically regarded as an in-
strument of choice.
Table 4 Independent Samples T-Test to Check Students’Homogeneity of Proficiency
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Babel Equal variances assumed 15.27 .00 1.44 58 .15 3.93 2.72 1.51 9.38
Equal variances not assumed 1.44 45.14 .15 3.93 2.72 1.54 9.41
Table 5 Students’Descriptive Statistics to Check Students’Homogeneity of Reasoning
Pretest control experimental nM SDSE(M)
Pretest 1.00 15 12.83 2.24 .649
2.00 15 14.00 3.86 1.11
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 12 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Therefore, having discarded those participants who did not produce private speech as
it was revealed in the first listening to the recorded data, other audio-recordings from
each group were selected, transcribed, and analyzed by the researchers with the help of
two raters. The following coding system was considered for the different events in-
cluded in their private speech:
Private speech: repetition (R), translation (T), filler (F), question (Q).
First language use (L)
Incomprehensible or inaudible word (IC)
As Green (1998) asserts, protocols should be transcribed exactly as they are. It means
that they should not be modified by adding or substituting words.
Transcription instances
➢The following is a sample transcription of the inference and deduction parts of
W-GCTA. Different parts were extracted from different learners.
S10: Mr. Brown, who lives in the town of Salem, was brought before the Salem
municipal court for the sixth time in the past month on a charge of keeping his pool
hall open after 1a.m [R] [IC]. He again admitted his guilt and was fined the maximum,
$500, as in each earlier instance [R] Uhh [F].
S2: Rice and celery [R] Umm [F] must have a good deal of moisture in order to
grow well, but rye and cotton grow best [R] Kodamha behtar roshd mikonanad? [Q]
where it is relatively dry [R] [T].
Results
Independent samples t-tests, Pearson product moment correlation, and one-way re-
peated measures (ANOVA) were run using SPSS version 22 for obtaining the results of
the study. Alpha level was set at .05.
Table 7 Results of Correlation between Reasoning power and Private Speech
RPS
R Pearson Correlation 1 .47
**
Sig. (2-tailed) .00
N6060
PS Pearson Correlation .47
**
1
Sig. (2-tailed) .00
N6060
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
Table 6 Independent Samples T-Test to Check Students’Homogeneity of Reasoning
Levene’sTest t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Pretest Equal variances assumed 1.85 .18 .90 22 .37 1.16 1.29 3.84 1.50
Equal variances not assumed .90 17.69 .37 1.16 1.29 3.88 1.54
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 13 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Results of the pretests
In order to achieve a more reliable data, those students who were 3 standard deviations
below or above the mean were first omitted from the study. Then, the t-tests for homo-
geneity were run to compare the Babel Test scores for the experimental and control
groups in order to check the students’language proficiency levels. The results showed
that there was not a significant difference in scores for the experimental group (M=
82.60, SD = 13.05) and the control group, (M= 86.53, SD = 7.20); t(45.147) = 1.44, p
= .000 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differences in the means was (mean differ-
ence = 3.93, 95% Cl [1.54 to 9.415]). Therefore, it can be concluded that the students in
the control and experimental groups had no significant difference regarding their
language proficiency (Tables 3 and 4).
In order to check that students’homogeneity of their reasoning power, they were
given the W-GCTA as pretest. The results indicated that there was not a significant dif-
ference in scores for the experimental group (M=14.00, SD = 3.86) and the control
group, (M=12.83, SD = 2.24); t(17.696) = 0.37, p= .000 (two · tailed). The magnitude of
the differences in the means was (mean difference = 1.16, 95% Cl [1.54 to 3.88]). Re-
sults indicated that the students in the control and experimental groups had no signifi-
cant difference regarding their power of logical reasoning (Tables 5 and 6).
According to the first research question stating ‘Is EFL learners’use of private speech
correlated with reasoning?’the relationship between the private speech use and the rea-
soning power of the EFL students was investigated using Pearson product–moment
correlation coefficient. Preliminary analyses were performed to ensure no violation of
the assumptions of normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity. The results revealed that
there was a strong, positive correlation between the two variables, r= .47, n= 60, P
< .05, with high levels of private speech use associated with high levels of reasoning
power (Table 7).
Considering the first sub-research question, ‘do parts of private speech–repetition,
translation, question, and filler–enhance advanced EFL learners’reasoning?,’according
to Ohta (2001) and Sonmez’s (2011) categorizations of private speech, the following
private speech categorization based on Table 8 was used to analyze data.
Table 9 illustrates the frequencies of occurrences of different types of private speech
in the control and experimental groups in order to make a comparison and find the re-
lationship between private speech type frequencies to see whether its different
categorization makes any statistically significant difference in EFL learners’reasoning.
The frequency of private speech used by different participants in the two groups was
different in the way that totally, the amount of private speech produced by the EFL
learners who belonged to the experimental group (n= 1097) was significantly higher
than the private speech produced by those of the control group (n= 620). Of course,
the amount of private speech produced was not the same among all individuals of the
Table 8 Private Speech Study and Its Accompanying Categorization
Citation Private speech categorization Task Setting
Ohta (2001) 1. Repetition Reasoning-gap tasks Lab setting
2. Translation
Sonmez (2011) 3. Filler
4. Question
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 14 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
same group. In the other words, some participants in both groups did not produce pri-
vate speech at all. In fact, as to Berk (1992), this point is in accordance with the find-
ings of the experimental studies on children private speech, in which it is usual for only
about half of the children in a sample to truly produce private speech.
Furthermore, as it was depicted in Table 9, the frequency of occurrences of different
categorizations of private speech was different among individuals in the control and ex-
perimental groups. The frequency of repetition as the first category of private speech in
the control group was (n= 347) and in the experimental group was (n= 654). Besides,
the frequency of occurrences of the second category (i.e., translation) was (n= 126) in
the control group and it was (n= 209) in the experimental group. Regarding filler as
the third category, EFL learners in the control group produced (n= 82) and in the ex-
perimental group they produced (n= 126). The last category was question which had
the least frequency. The frequency of this part was (n= 65) in the control and (n= 108)
in the experimental group. Additionally, since the amount of private speech produced
by the experimental group was generally higher than private speech in the control
group, it seems that higher order thinking enhancing techniques made them create
more private speech in order to enhance their reasoning. The amount of private speech
produced by every individual across different groups varied. It means that different in-
dividuals performed differently regarding private speech production when they dealt
with reasoning-gap tasks. For example, student 6 in the experimental group produced
the most amount of private speech (n= 51) and student 21 of the control group pro-
duced the most amount of private speech in the tasks (n= 46). Student 27 in the con-
trol group produced the least amount of private speech (n= 3) and student 21 of the
experimental group produced the least amount of private speech for the tasks (n= 11).
The use of private speech is an indication of difficulty with the task (Frauenglass &
Diaz 1985). In other words, participants who struggled with an experimental task
tended to self-verbalize, whereas participants who had no difficulty tended not to.
Tables 10 and 11 illustrate the frequency of occurrences of the participants’private
speech relating to different groups, as well as its descriptive statistics.
Table 12 presents the result of ANOVA conducted to compare scores on the produc-
tion of private speech types and its impact on the EFL learners’reasoning scores. As
Table 9 Comparison of the Types of Private Speech and Reasoning Power in Control and
Experimental Groups
Private speech type frequencies Control group Experimental group
Private speech 620 1097
Repetition 347 654
Translation 126 209
Filler 82 126
Question 65 108
Table 10 The Most and the Least Frequent Private Speech in accordance with Groups
Participants of
experimental group
Experimental group private
speech frequency
Participants of
control group
Control group private
speech frequency
S6 51 S21 46
S21 11 S27 3
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 15 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
indicated in Table 12, there was a statistically significant difference for private speech
use among the participants in accordance with their reasoning power, Wilks’Lambda
= .21, F(3.00) = 70.99, P< .05, multivariate partial eta squared = .78. It means that differ-
ent categories of private speech made significant changes in the students’reasoning
power.
In order to check whether ‘repetition makes any statistically significant difference be-
tween the reasoning of control and experimental groups’both groups were checked to
see whether there is any statistically significant difference between them regarding their
reasoning as determined by the first category of private speech (i.e., repetition). An in-
dependent samples t-test was run. Table 13 shows the t-test results for this analysis
subsequently.
The results indicated that there was a significant difference in scores for the control
group (M=11.56, SD = 4.98) and the experimental group, (M= 21.70, SD = 6.11); t(58)
= 7.03, p= .000 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differences in the means was (mean
difference = 10.13, 95% Cl [13.01 to 7.25]). Therefore, the results specified that the stu-
dents in the control and experimental groups had a significant difference regarding
their reasoning as determined by repetition.
In order to check whether ‘translation makes any statistically significant difference
between the reasoning of control and experimental groups’both groups were analyzed
to regarding their reasoning as determined by the second category of private speech
(i.e., translation); an independent samples t-test was run. Table 14 shows the t-test re-
sults for this analysis.
The results indicated that there was a significant difference in scores for the control
group (M=4.20, SD = 3.01) and the experimental group, (M= 6.96, SD = 4.52); t(50.43)
= 2.78, p= .00 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differences in the means was (mean
difference = 2.76, 95% Cl [4.76 to 0.77]). Therefore, the results indicated that the stu-
dents in the control and experimental groups had significant difference regarding their
power of reasoning as determined by translation.
Table 11 Descriptive Statistics of Four Categories of Private Speech
Private speech MSDn
Repetition 16.63 7.52 60
Translation 5.58 4.06 60
Filler 3.46 2.65 60
Question 2.88 2.82 60
Table 12 One-Way Repeated Measures ANOVA to Check the Relationship between Private Speech
Categorization and Reasoning Power
Multivariate Tests
a
Effect Value FHypothesis df Error df Sig. Partial Eta Squared
Privatespeech Pillai’s Trace .78 70.99
b
3.00 57.00 .00 .78
Wilks’Lambda .21 70.99
b
3.00 57.00 .00 .78
Hotelling’s Trace 3.73 70.99
b
3.00 57.00 .00 .78
Roy’s Largest Root 3.73 70.99
b
3.00 57.00 .00 .78
a
Design: Intercept Within Subjects Design: privatespeech
b
Exact statistic
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 16 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
In order to check whether ‘filler makes any statistically significant difference between
the reasoning of control and experimental groups’both groups were analyzed to see
the probable difference between them regarding their reasoning as determined by the
third category of private speech (i.e., filler); an independent samples t-test was per-
formed. Table 15 displays the t-test results for this analysis.
The results designated that there was a statistically significant difference in scores for
the control group (M=2.73, SD = 2.75) and the experimental group, (M= 4.20, SD =
2.38); t(58) = 2.20, p= .03 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differences in the means
was (mean difference = 1.46, 95% Cl [2.79, .13]). Therefore, the results showed that the
students in the control and experimental groups had a statistically significant difference
regarding their power of reasoning as determined by fillers.
Finally, in order to check whether ‘question makes any statistically significant differ-
ence between the reasoning of control and experimental groups’both groups were ex-
amined to see any significant difference between them regarding their reasoning as
determined by the last category of private speech (i.e., question); an independent sam-
ples t-test was run. Table 16 shows the t-test results for this analysis.
The results indicated that there was a statistically significant difference in scores for
the control group (M=2.16, SD = 2.61) and the experimental group, (M= 3.60, SD =
2.89); t(58) = 2.01, p= .04 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differences in the means
was (mean difference = 1.43, 95% Cl [2.85, .00]). Therefore, the results showed that the
students in the control and experimental groups had a statistically significant difference
regarding their power of reasoning as determined by question.
Additionally, in order to check which of the categories of private speech had more ef-
fect on learners’reasoning; their t-values were compared. With regards to the t-value,
since repetition had the highest t-value (t= 7.03), it can be concluded that, it had the
highest impact on the learners’reasoning. T-value of translation (t= 2.78) indicates that
after repetition, translation had the highest impact on EFL learners’reasoning. After-
wards, filler and question with the t-value of (t= 2.20) and the t-value of 2.01 (t= 2.01)
respectively, had the least effect.
Table 13 Independent Samples T-Test to Check the Effect of Repetition on Reasoning in Control
and Experimental Groups
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Repetition Equal variances assumed .75 .38 7.03 58 .00 10.13 1.43 13.01 7.25
Equal variances not assumed 7.03 55.71 .00 10.13 1.43 13.01 7.24
Table 14 Independent Samples T-Test to Check the Effect of Translation on Reasoning in Control
and Experimental Groups
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Translation Equal variances assumed 6.44 .014 2.78 58 .00 2.76 .99 4.75 .77
Equal variances not assumed 2.78 50.43 .00 2.76 .99 4.76 .77
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 17 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
With Regards to the second research question stating that ‘do higher order thinking
enhancing techniques make any statistically significant difference between the reason-
ing power of the control and experimental groups as determined by their private
speech?’two groups of the EFL learners were analyzed to check their probable differ-
ences regarding their reasoning power. Tables 17 and 18 show the descriptive statistics
and t-test results of the control and experimental groups when they took part in the
posttest session, in order to check whether there was any statistically significant differ-
ence between these groups regarding their power of reasoning.
An independent samples t-test was conducted. Table 18 illustrates, there was a sig-
nificant difference in scores for the experimental group (M= 20.84, SD = 3.57) and the
control group, (M= 12.60, SD = 2.32); t(54) = 10.35, p= .000 (two · tailed). The magni-
tude of the differences in the means was (mean difference = 8.24, 95% Cl [9.84, 6.65]).
With regards to the first sub-research question stating that ‘do higher order thinking
enhancing techniques make any statistically significant difference between the inductive
reasoning of the control and experimental groups as determined by their private
speech?,’the control and experimental groups’power of inductive reasoning was con-
sidered by their performance on inductive part of reasoning-gap tasks. Tables 19 and
20 depict the results.
An independent samples t-test was done in order see if there is any statistically sig-
nificant difference between the control and experimental groups regarding their induct-
ive reasoning. Findings are shown in Table 20. According to the results, there was a
significant difference in scores for the experimental group (M= 9.15, SD = 1.76) and the
control group, (M= 4.00, SD = 1.76); t(54) = 9.5, p= .000 (two · tailed). The magnitude
of the differences in the means was (mean difference = 5.15, 95% Cl [6.24, 4.06]).
Regarding the second sub-research question stating that ‘do higher order thinking en-
hancing techniques make any statistically significant difference between the deductive
reasoning of the control and experimental groups as determined by their private
speech?,’the control and experimental groups’performance on deduction part of
reasoning-gap tasks were examined. Therefore, in order to see whether participants of
these groups in posttest have any statistically significant difference in the deductive
Table 15 Independent Samples T-Test to Check the Effect of Filler on Reasoning in Control and
Experimental Groups
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Fillers Equal variances assumed .09 .75 2.20 58 .03 1.46 .66 2.79 .13
Equal variances not assumed 2.20 56.83. .03 1.46 .66 2.79 .13
Table 16 Independent Samples T-Test to Check the Effect of Question on Reasoning in Control
and Experimental Groups
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Question Equal variances assumed .26 .60 2.01 58 .04 1.43 .71 2.85 .00
Equal variances not assumed 2.01 57.40 .04 1.43 .71 2.85 .00
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 18 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
reasoning, another independent samples t-test was run. Tables 21 and 22 describe the
results.
According to the results depicted in Tables 21 and 22, there was a significant differ-
ence in scores for the experimental group (M= 11.69, SD = 2.70) and the control group,
(M= 8.60, SD = 2.38); t(54) = 4.54, p= .00 (two · tailed). The magnitude of the differ-
ences in the means was (mean difference = 3.09, 95% Cl [4.45, 1.72]).
Discussion
The results of the data analysis to check the probable impacts of different categories of
private speech on the EFL learners’reasoning power showed significant differences
these categories made on the students’reasoning. As to English (1997), private speech
produced by the most and the least proficient reasoners was rather similar in quantity.
It means that, both groups of the reasoners involved in nearly the same amount of pri-
vate speech during working on tasks. Nevertheless, regarding the quality of the private
speech production, English believes; however, conspicuous variances between the most
proficient and the least proficient reasoners were recorded, with regards to the quality
of that private speech. For instance, the most expert problem solvers produced private
speech to help them solving tasks showed their attention to the task. English holds pri-
vate speech utterances produced by the skillful problem solvers referenced the nature
of the reasoning tasks and echoed their capability to verbalize conceptual relationships.
On the other hand, the private speech produced by less proficient reasoners designated
that they did not attend well to the several dimensions of the tasks. Neither these types
of private speech seem to assist the problem-solving process.
In a research carried out Duncan and Cheyne (2002), the authors assert that “private
speech appears to be more than a stage or phase of ontogenetic development”(p. 901).
The findings indicated that “overt self-verbalization continues to play a mediational role
in problem-solving and self-regulatory processes during early adulthood”(p. 901).
On the whole, the findings of a variety of studies which indicate that private speech
use is greater on difficult tasks than on easier tasks, reveal that private speech is
regarded as a form of thinking and a means of problem-solving. The replication of
studies based on children’s private speech production on the young adults shows the
experimental evidence which indicates that the relationship between private speech
production and task difficulty continues beyond childhood. In fact, adults as the same
Table 17 Descriptive Statistics of Students to Check Their Reasoning Power
Posttestce NM SD SE(M)
Posttest Control 30 12.60 2.32 .42
Experimental 30 20.84 3.57 .70
Table 18 Independent Samples T-Test to Check Students’Power of Reasoning
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Posttest Equal variances assumed 2.58 .11 10.35 54 .00 8.24 .79 9.84 6.65
Equal variances not assumed 10.05 41.88 .00 8.24 .81 9.90 6.59
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 19 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
as children, have the greater cognitive demands of more difficult tasks. Therefore, they
elicit more self-directed speech (Duncan & Cheyne, 2002).
In another study, El’konin (cited in Diaz & Berk, 1992) performed a private speech
research in which he supports the reasoning process in learners as determined by pri-
vate speech. He presents analyses of the private speech produced during drawing activ-
ity and during solving practical problems.
As the results of the present study indicated, the amount of the private speech pro-
duction was higher in the experimental than the control group. It seems that the ex-
perimental group who received higher order thinking enhancing techniques was more
successful in being self-regulated and production of private speech. Consequently, they
obtained higher scores in carrying out their reasoning-gap activities.
The amount of private speech production, as well as its categories in this study, is in
line with some similar studies. According to Ohta (2001), repetition was the most com-
mon form where the learners privately repeated the utterances in the tasks. This was
common with the most difficult sentences or those structures they could not under-
stand them. Additionally, Sonmez (2011) claims that private speech can emerge in the
form of repetition when “the participants have self-discoveries about some part of their
own task, performance, or knowledge”(p. 109). Furthermore, Sonmez asserts, by repe-
tition, learners can make sense of the new information and recover prevailing know-
ledge from their memory. Moreover, he mentions repetition of the new language forms
may contribute participants to establish their thoughts and understand the gaps exist in
their performance or knowledge while internalizing new information. Besides, as to
Centeno-Cortes and Jimenez Jimenez (2004), “by repeating pieces of discourse, speakers
concentrate on the information that seems to be problematic or crucial”(p. 18). Add-
itionally, Frawley (1997) claims that by using repetition, speakers help themselves to
concentrate on the problem. Also, Anani and Gordani (2014) contend that “repetition
mainly signals the participants’ongoing thought process and is an attempt to take it
under control”. In fact, “the purpose here is to focus and direct the thought towards
the item”(p. 8).
Using translation by the participants during performing tasks can indicate the im-
portant role that L1 may play here. Based on Centeno-Cortes and Jimenez Jimenez
(2004), having used translation, the learners may realize that the line of reasoning de-
ployed was futile, abandoned it and endeavored to find an alternative path. A break-
down could also be accompanied by a switch in the language used (from L2 to L1).
Table 19 Descriptive Statistics of Students to Check Their Power of Induction
Induction control experimental NM SD SE(M)
Induction Control 30 4.00 1.76 .32
Experimental 30 9.15 2.29 .44
Table 20 Independent Samples T-Test to Check Students’Power of Induction
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. T df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Induction Equal variances assumed .77 .38 9.50 54 .00 5.15 .54 6.24 4.06
Equal variances not assumed 9.32 46.61 .00 5.15 .55 6.26 4.04
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 20 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Sonmez (2011) holds that fillers can be regarded as indicators of the thought process
and they help learners avoid distraction. He maintains that the participants use fillers
“to signal the ongoing thought process and sometimes to organize the thought process”
(p. 121). As Centeno-Cortes and Jimenez Jimenez (2004) hold, fillers can help learners
“avoid getting distracted”(p. 18). Anani and Gordani (2014), also, assert that using
fillers can give learners abundant time in order to think about the next action they
would like to do. Moreover, regarding using question during carrying out reasoning-
gap tasks Sonmez (2011) contends that asking questions can help the participants “to
orient and re-orient themselves to the task”(p. 126).
The results also indicated that the two techniques of concept mapping and argumen-
tation positively and significantly influenced the power of logical reasoning as it was
measured by the two subscales of W-GCTA. In other words, the integration of such
techniques to the EFL reading classes tended to foster the learners’power of reasoning.
Accordingly, the results revealed that the power of logical reasoning was increased
through using higher order thinking enhancing techniques. It was proved through run-
ning independent samples t-test, and as a result, the frequency of reasoning, in general,
and inductive, deductive reasoning, in particular, was increased in the experimental
group more than the control group who received no treatment.
The researchers of the present study set out to examine the differences between the
control and experimental groups based on the current models and theories of learning
suggesting that critical thinking enhancing techniques such as concept mapping and ar-
gumentation can augment the EFL learners’reasoning power which is regarded as
higher order thinking. In other words, the findings of the current study lend enough
support to the claim that reasoning as a subpart of the general concept of critical learn-
ing (W-GCTA, 2010) could be enhanced by means of critical thinking enhancing tech-
niques such as concept mapping and argumentation (Ghanizadeh & Mirzaee, 2012).
Regarding argumentation, for instance, Kelly and Bazerman (2003) endeavored to
understand the ways students engaged in the scientific reasoning associate general the-
oretical claims to complete data in making evidence. Besides, as to Hillocks (2010), ar-
guments are regarded as the single most important component in the critical thinking.
We produce an argument when for thinking that a claim is true, we give a reason.
What is more, according to Hillocks, “formal argument is a line of reasoning that at-
tempts to prove by logic”(p. 305). In fact, argument is at the heart of critical thinking
and academic discourse. Likewise, as to Dabaghi et al. (2012), language tasks which
Table 21 Descriptive Statistics of Students to Check their Power of Deduction
Deduction control experimental NM SD SE(M)
Deduction Control 30 8.60 2.38 .43
Experimental 30 11.69 2.70 .53
Table 22 Independent Samples T-Test to Check Students’Power of Deduction
Levene’s Test t-test for Equality of Means
F Sig. t df Sig. M SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
Deduction Equal variances assumed .00 1.00 .54 54 .00 3.09 .68 4.45 1.72
Equal variances not assumed 4.50 50.31 .00 3.09 .68 4.47 1.71
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 21 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
necessitate greater criticality on the part of learners might request more critical think-
ing; therefore, one aspect of the critical thinking simply refers to people’s reasoning in-
ductively and deductively, and to infer sound conclusions from ambiguous statements
(Freeley & Steinberg, 2000). What is more, as Dabaghi et al. hold, one of the domains
in which critical thinking might open up a related line of examination is the argumen-
tative genre because argumentation is cognitively-demanding.
Regarding the probable impact of concept mapping on the EFL learners’power
of reasoning, as to Ghanizadeh and Mirzaee (2012), organizational tools including
concept mapping scaffold various forms of reasoning about content and engage
students in thinking critically about the content of whatever they study. Jonassen
et al. (1998) claim that applying critical thinking while reading requires evaluating
arguments and statements, deducing conclusions and hypotheses, noticing incon-
sistencies, and common mistakes in reasoning, and making inferences. Indeed,
there are numerous properties intrinsic in concept mapping which may help the
improvement of learners’critical thinking ability. Ghanizadeh and Mirzaee (2012)
believe that “extracting the main ideas or concepts of the passage and enclosing
them in squares or boxes may help them conceptualize and organize text ideas
and information into a coherent whole”(pp. 57–58). In fact, by labeling the arrows
in the concept map the learners can recognize the rhetorical functions like cause-
effect, compare-contrast, arguments, etc., as well as deciding upon deductively and
inductively valid arguments. This, in turn, can help learners to discover main ideas,
to differentiate between salient and subsidiary points or arguments, and to detect
the text structure. To Ghanizadeh and Mirzaee, “all the above-mentioned qual-
ities fall adequately within higher-order abilities associated with making meaning
from text and reading between lines, such as inference-making, deduction, argu-
ment evaluation, and interpretation, all of which are manifestations of critical
thinking ability”(p. 58).
Besides, the results were in line with Wheeler and Collins’s (2003) findings that con-
cept mapping is effective in helping nursing students improve critical thinking skills.
The results were also consistent with previous research by Ghanizadeh (2007) on con-
cept mapping in which she considers concept mapping as an assessment tool of EFL
reading comprehension which investigated test-taking strategies of concept mapping
via think-aloud technique. The results of her study revealed that among the strategies
related to higher-order abilities, proposition synthesizing, and inference-making were
the most regular ones. Furthermore, various properties intrinsic in concept mapping
may have added to the improvement of learners’critical thinking ability.
Conclusion
The present study investigated the role of the advanced EFL learners’private speech
production characterized as being the externalization of the process of reasoning dur-
ing carrying out reasoning-gap tasks. It was observed that while performing the
reasoning-gap tasks applied in this study, the EFL learners produced four types of pri-
vate speech, repetition, translation, filler, and question, in both their L1 and L2. The re-
sults, also, indicated that all these four types of private speech enhanced learners’
reasoning. Besides, this study scrutinized the probable differences between the two
groups of control and experimental in order to check their private speech types and
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 22 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
reasoning power. The results revealed that private speech production was mostly pro-
duced by students in the experimental group rather than the control group. Moreover,
results showed that the reasoning of the experimental group was more powerful than
that of the control group. With regards to the effect of L1 on learners’reasoning, ana-
lyzing data showed that the learners employed the L2 mainly while reading and for rep-
etitions of parts of the questions, proper names, etc., while they were performing
reasoning tasks. The data collected from the participants’private speech indicated a
shift to the L1 in the participants when the problem became difficult.
The results of the data analysis to check the probable impacts of private speech on
the EFL learners’reasoning power showed significant differences private speech types
made on students’reasoning. Furthermore, in the current study, private speech pro-
duced by the experimental group and their corresponding reasoning power indicated
that using higher order thinking enhancing techniques could improve the learners’
quality of reasoning, as well as their power of self-regulation. In fact, such learners who
were treated by the techniques of enhancing reasoning paid more attention to the task,
and they were more self-regulated. On the other hand, those learners who did not re-
ceive treatment, paid less attention to the reasoning-gap tasks, being less self-regulated,
and consequently, produced less amount of private speech. In this regard, Duncan and
Cheyne (2002) remark private speech plays a mediational role in problem-solving and
self-regulatory processes. Therefore, private speech can be regarded as a form of think-
ing and a means of problem-solving. In fact, since critical thinking is an essential skill
for education and even life, the techniques of enhancing higher order thinking can help
learners to achieve greater academic and life success. As to Wright (2002), the com-
plexity of modern life requires increasing and deploying reasoning abilities. Simple
thinking cannot be of help in solving the problems of complicated situations, such situ-
ations need higher order intelligent capabilities including multifaceted critical thinking.
These results, as well as the results of the previous studies on the role of private
speech, have shown the contribution of higher order thinking enhancing techniques in
promoting the EFL learners’power of reasoning and the role that private speech has in
the process of L2 acquisition.
Authors’contributions
This work was carried out in collaboration between the two authors. Author PM supervised the work, decided upon
and provided the sources, and revised and edited the drafts. Author SM collected the data and wrote the drafts of
the manuscript, performed the statistical analyses, and implemented the revisions. Both authors read and approved
the final manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Received: 11 August 2016 Accepted: 8 November 2016
References
Anani sarab, M. R., & Gordani, Y. (2014). The role of L2 private speech in cognitive regulation of adult foreign language
learners. Journal of English Language Teaching and Learning, 14,1–18.
Angelo, T. A., & Cross, P. K. (1993). Classroom assessment techniques: A handbook for college teachers. San Francisco:
Jossey Bass.
Appel, G. (1986). L1 and L2 narrative and expository discourse production: A Vygotskian analysis. Newark: Ph.D.
dissertation, University of Delaware.
Appel, G., & Lantolf, J. P. (1994). Speaking as mediation: A study of L1 and L2 text recall tasks. Modern Language Journal,
78, 437–452. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1994.tb02062.x.
Berk, L. (1992). Children’s private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In R. Diaz & L. Berk (Eds.),
Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 23 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of elementary school children’s private speech. Merrill Palmer
Quarterly, 36(4), 443–463.
Bowell, T., & Kemp, G. (2005). Critical thinking, a concise guide (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.
Canas, A. J. (2003). A summary of literature pertaining to the use of concept mapping techniques and technologies for
education and performance support (The institute for human and machine cognition).
Canas, A. J., Hill, G., Carff, R., & Suri, N. (2003). Cmap Tools: A knowledge modeling and sharing toolkit, (technical Reportt
IHMC CmapTools, 93–01) (Institute for human and machine cognition).
Carrell, P. L., Pharis, B. G., & Liberto, J. C. (1989). Metacognitive strategy training for ESL reading. TESOL Quarterly, 23(4),
647–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3587536.
Centeno-Cortes, B., & Jimenez Jimenez, A. F. (2004). Problem-solving tasks in a foreign language: the importance of the
L1 in private verbal thinking. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14(1), 7–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1473-
4192.2004.00052.x.
Cohen, A. D., & Upton, T. A. (2006). Strategies in responding to the new TOEFL reading tasks. TOEFL Monograph Series,
33. doi:10.1002/j.2333-8504.2006.tb02012.x.
Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. (1983). The role of cognitive engagement in classroom learning and motivation. Educational
Psychologist, 18,88–100.
Dabaghi, A., Zabihi, R., & Rezazadeh, M. (2012). Argumentative and narrative written task performance: Differential
effects of critical thinking. International Journal of Research studies in language learning, 1(2), 1–12.
Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (Eds.). (1992). Private Speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Diaz, R., Winsler, A., Atencio, D., & Harbers, K. (1992). Mediation of self-regulation through the use of private speech.
International Journal of Cognitive Education and Mediated Learning, 2, 155–167.
Donato, R. (1994). Collective scaffolding in second language learning. In J. P. Lantolf & G. Appel (Eds.), Vygotskian
approaches to second language research (pp. 33–56). Norwood: Ablex.
Duncan, R. M., & Cheyne, J. A. (2002). Private speech in young adults, task difficulty, self-regulation and a psychological
predication. Cognitive Development, 16, 889–906.
English, L. D. (1997). Mathematical reasoning: Analogies, metaphors, and images. New York: Routledge.
Feigenbaum, P. (1992). Development of the syntactic and discourse structures of private speech. In R. M. Diaz & L. E.
Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 181–198). Hiisdale: Erlbaum.
Fernyhough, C., & Lloyd, P. (1999). Lev Vygtosky: Critical assessments. New York: Routledge.
Frauenglass, M., & Diaz, R. (1985). Self-regulatory functions of children’s private speech: a critical analysis of recent
challenges to Vygotsky’s theory. Developmental Psychology, 21, 357–364.
Frawley, W. (1997). Vygotsky and cognitive science: Language and the unification of the social and computational mind.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Frawley, W., & Lantolf, J. P. (1985). Second language discourse: a vygotskyan perspective. Applied Linguistics, 6,19–44.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/applin/6.1.19.
Freeley, A. J., & Steinberg, D. L. (2000). Argumentation and debate: Critical thinking for reasoned decision-making.
Stamford: Wadsworth.
Ghanizadeh, A. (2007). On the impact of concept mapping on EFL learners’reading comprehension. A paper presented at
the fourth conference on issues in language teaching in Iran, Tehran University.
Ghanizadeh, A., & Mirzaee, S. (2012). Critical thinking: How to enhance it in language classes. Germany: Lambert
Academic Publishing.
Green, A. (1998). Verbal protocol analysis in language testing research. Cambridge: CUP.
Hatch, E., & Lazaraton, A. (1991). The research manual: Design and statistics for applied linguistics. Boston: Heinle & Heinle
Publishers.
Hillocks, G. (2010). Teaching argument for critical thinking and writing: An introduction. English Journal, 99(6), 24–32.
Jonassen, D. H., Carr, C., & Yueh, H. P. (1998). Computers as Mindtools for engaging learners in critical thinking. Tech
Trends, 43(2), 24–32.
Kelly, G. J., & Bazerman, C. (2003). How students argue scientific claims: A rheorical-semantic analysis. Applied Linguistics,
24(1), 28–55.
Khodadady, E., & Ghanizadrh, A. (2010). The impact of concept mapping on EFL learners’critical thinking ability. English
Language Teaching, 4(4), 49–60. 105532/elt.v4n4p49.
Lantolf, J. P., & Frawley, W. (1984). Second language performance and Vygotskyan psycholinguistics: Implications for L2
instruction. In A. Manning, P. Martin, & K. McCalla (Eds.), The tenth LACUS forum (pp. 425–440). Columbia: Hornbeam
Press.
McCafferty, S. G. (1992). The use of private speech by adult second language learners: a cross- cultural study. Modern
Language Journal, 76, 179–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1992.tb01098.x.
McCafferty, S. G. (1994). The use of private speech by adult ESL learners at different levels of proficiency. In J. P. Lantolf
& G. Appel (Eds.), Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 135–56). Norwood: Ablex.
Munro, J. K. (2009). Oral language learning: The primary years (Lecture notes).
Ohta, A. S. (2001). Second language acquisition process in the classroom. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Peters, A. P. (2008). Kinds of thinking, styles of reasoning. In M. Mason (Ed.), Critical thinking and learning. Oxford:
Blackwell Publishing.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of a classroom academic
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33–40.
Richmond, K. (2009). Inside Reading 4. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Salmon, A. K. (2008). Promoting a culture of thinking in the young child. Early Childhood Education Journal, 35, 457–461.
Saville-Troike, M. (1988). Private speech: evidence for second language learning strategies during the ‘silent’period.
Journal of Child Language, 15, 567–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012575.
Schafersman, S. D. (1991). An introduction to critical thinking.Retrieved from http://www.freeinquiry.com/criticalthinking.
html
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 24 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Smith, H. J. (2007). The social and private worlds of speech: speech for inter- and intramental activity. The Modern
Language Journal, 91, 341–356.
Sonmez, S. S. (2011). A study of private speech and cognitive regulation in native non-native speaker interactions (Ph.D.
dissertation, The Florida State University, The United States). Retrieved from http://www.diginole.lib.fsu.edu.
Stanley, F. (2011). Vygotsky-from public to private: Learning from personal speech in T. Waller, J. Whitmarsh, & K. Clarke
(Eds.), Making sense of theory and practice in early childhood (pp. 11–25). England: Open University Press.
Talebinezhad, M. R., & Negari, G. M. (2007). The effect of explicit teaching of concept mapping in expository writing on
EFL students’self-regulation. The Linguistics Journal, 2(1), 69–90.
Ushakova, T. N. (1994). Inner speech and second language acquisition: An experimental-theoretical approach. In J. P.
Lantolf & G. Appel (Eds.), Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 135–56). Norwood: Ablex.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University
Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Watson, G. B., & Glaser, E. M. (2002). Watson-Glaser critical thinking appraisal. London: The Psychological Corporation.
Watson, G. B., & Glaser, E. M. (2010). Watson-Glaser critical thinking appraisal: Technical manual and users’guide. The
United States of America: Pearson Education.
Wheeler, L., & Collins, S. (2003). The influence of concept mapping on critical thinking skills in baccalaureate nursing
students. Journal of professional Nursing, 19(6), 339–346.
Winsler, A., Abar, B., Feder, M. A., Schunn, C. D., & Alarcon Rubio, D. (2007). Private speech and executive functioning
among high-functioning children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 37,
1617–1635.
Wright, I. (2002). Is that right? Critical thinking and the social world of the young learner. Toronto: Pippin Publishing.
Submit your manuscript to a
journal and benefi t from:
7 Convenient online submission
7 Rigorous peer review
7 Immediate publication on acceptance
7 Open access: articles freely available online
7 High visibility within the fi eld
7 Retaining the copyright to your article
Submit your next manuscript at 7 springeropen.com
Mirzaee and Maftoon Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2016) 1:18 Page 25 of 25
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.