ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Microplastics are ubiquitous in the marine environment. Their small size makes them bioavailable to a range of organisms and studies have reported ingestion across the food chain. Few studies have demonstrated physical transfer of microplastics between organisms, and no research has assessed the ecological impacts of transferred microplastics and contaminants over different trophic levels. Contaminants associated with plastics can alter animal behaviour; thus, exploring changes in behaviour may be fundamental in understanding ecosystem effects of microplastics. This study explored the effects of microplastics and associated contaminants through the food chain in the marine intertidal zone. We exposed beach hoppers, Platorchestia smithi, to environmentally relevant concentrations of microplastics and then fed them to Krefft's frillgobies, Bathygobius krefftii, ray-finned fish that inhabit shallow coastal ecosystems. We tested fish personality to see whether there were any changes that could be attributed to trophic transfer of microplastics, as even subtle changes in behaviour can have cascading effects on other organisms and the wider ecosystem. Exploring behavioural changes in response to contaminant exposure is a developing area in ecotoxicology due to its increased sensitivity compared with the traditional LD50 approach. While gobies readily ingested contaminated beach hoppers, we detected no effect of microplastic trophic transfer on fish personality relative to control groups. While chronic exposure studies assessing a suite of behaviours are required, it is possible that the transfer of microplastics via trophic interactions does not provide an additional exposure pathway for contaminants through the food web.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Trophic transfer of microplastics does not affect sh personality
Louise Tosetto
*
, Jane E. Williamson, Culum Brown
Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, North Ryde, NSW, Australia
article info
Article history:
Received 25 May 2016
Initial acceptance 5 August 2016
Final acceptance 6 October 2016
MS. number: 16-00465R
Keywords:
Bathygobius krefftii
beach hopper
behaviour
food web
microplastic
plastic pollution
Microplastics are ubiquitous in the marine environment. Their small size makes them bioavailable to a
range of organisms and studies have reported ingestion across the food chain. Few studies have
demonstrated physical transfer of microplastics between organisms, and no research has assessed the
ecological impacts of transferred microplastics and contaminants over different trophic levels. Con-
taminants associated with plastics can alter animal behaviour; thus, exploring changes in behaviour may
be fundamental in understanding ecosystem effects of microplastics. This study explored the effects of
microplastics and associated contaminants through the food chain in the marine intertidal zone. We
exposed beach hoppers, Platorchestia smithi, to environmentally relevant concentrations of microplastics
and then fed them to Krefft's frillgobies, Bathygobius krefftii,ray-nned sh that inhabit shallow coastal
ecosystems. We tested sh personality to see whether there were any changes that could be attributed to
trophic transfer of microplastics, as even subtle changes in behaviour can have cascading effects on other
organisms and the wider ecosystem. Exploring behavioural changes in response to contaminant expo-
sure is a developing area in ecotoxicology due to its increased sensitivity compared with the traditional
LD50 approach. While gobies readily ingested contaminated beach hoppers, we detected no effect of
microplastic trophic transfer on sh personality relative to control groups. While chronic exposure
studies assessing a suite of behaviours are required, it is possible that the transfer of microplastics via
trophic interactions does not provide an additional exposure pathway for contaminants through the food
web.
©2016 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Food webs and trophic interactions are essential for the transfer
of energy and nutrients throughout ecosystems. Urbanization,
development and industry have inuenced pollutants in aquatic
systems, with chemicals, metals, pesticides and pharmaceuticals all
presenting risks to ecosystems and associated biota (von Glasow
et al., 2013; Islam &Tanaka, 2004). Contaminants can accumulate
and concentrate in biota (bioaccumulation) and then subsequently
transfer through the food web (Borgå, Fisk, Hoekstra, &Muir, 2004;
Nfon, Cousins, &Broman, 2008). These pollutants can impact ani-
mal physiology (Hontela, 1998) and can also alter behaviours
important for foraging, reproduction, social interactions and anti-
predator behaviour (Clotfelter, Bell, &Levering, 2004; S
offker &
Tyler, 2012).
Plastic is a contemporary pollutant in our marine environment.
While the deleterious effects of large plastic debris on marine life
have received much publicity (Barnes, Galgani, Thompson, &
Barlaz, 2009), microplastics are of increasing concern (Cole,
Lindeque, Halsband, &Galloway, 2011). Their small size and vari-
able densities mean they occur throughout marine (Law &
Thompson, 2014) and freshwater (Eerkes-Medrano, Thompson, &
Aldridge, 2015) environments and are bioavailable to a broad
range of organisms (Thompson et al., 2004). Microplastics present a
physical and chemical risk to organisms. (Rochman, 2013). Poten-
tially toxic additives such as phthalates, bisphenol A (BPA) and
ame-retardants are added to many plastics at manufacture to
increase functionality and extend their life (Browne, Galloway, &
Thompson, 2008; Rochman, 2013). Furthermore, plastics are
porous and accumulate and concentrate contaminants including
polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), pesticides and fertilizers at high
intensities from the surrounding sea water (Mato et al., 2001).
Many of the additives and the absorbed contaminants are known
endocrine disruptors, carcinogens and mutagens (Lithner,
Damberg, Dave, &Larsson, 2009) and it is thought that contami-
nants may transfer from plastics to organisms following ingestion
(Teuten, Rowland, Galloway, &Thompson, 2007; Teuten et al.,
2009).
Many of the contaminants associated with plastics such as BPA
and PCBs can affect foraging efciency, alter schooling behaviour or
*Correspondence: L. Tosetto, Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie
University, North Ryde, NSW 2109, Australia.
E-mail address: louise.tosetto@mq.edu.au (L. Tosetto).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Animal Behaviour
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.035
0003-3472/©2016 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167
increase agonistic encounters in sh (Clotfelter et al., 2004; Sloman
&Wilson, 2006). Complex behaviours such as foraging, predator
avoidance and social interactions are all fundamental to individual
tness and an animal's functioning in an ecosystem (Wolf &
Weissing, 2012). Changes in behaviour may take effect even with
small quantities of pollutants (Bae &Park, 2014), and while changes
in animal health may not be apparent, these behavioural alterations
may affect how the animal performs in an ecological context (Scott
&Sloman, 2004). Behavioural responses provide useful markers of
pollution effects on individuals, potentially performing as reliable
and economical indicators of sublethal effects of pollutants (Weis,
2014). Accordingly, there is growing emphasis in ecotoxicology on
examining changes in behaviour in response to exposure to con-
taminants (Oulton, Taylor, Hose, &Brown, 2014).
Microplastics can be ingested by organisms across the marine
environment (Fig. 1). A range of studies have assessed effects of
plastic consumption at the individual organism level but few have
assessed the capacity for microplastics and associated contami-
nants to move through the food chain. Our understanding of how
pollutants can bioaccumulate (Borgå et al., 2004) suggest micro-
plastics provide an additional exposure pathway for contaminants
to transfer through the food web. The physical transfer of micro-
plastic fragments between organisms has been demonstrated from
mussels, Mytilus edulis, to crabs, Carcinus maenas (Farrell &Nelson,
2013) and mesozooplankton to macrozooplankton (Set
al
a,
Fleming-Lehtinen, &Lehtiniemi, 2014;Fig. 1). Recently, the trans-
fer of microplastic fragments as well as contaminants was
demonstrated in an articial food chain from Artemia nauplii to the
intestinal tract of laboratory-raised zebrash, Danio rerio. The study
used high concentrations of microplastics spiked with large
amounts of benzo(a)pyrene (BaP) and there was no assessment of
biological consequences for either Artemia or sh (Batel, Linti,
Scherer, Erdinger, &Braunbeck, 2016). No studies have assessed
the biological effects of a microplastic-contaminated diet on higher
trophic levels in an ecologically relevant setting. Exposing animals
to more environmentally relevant concentrations and evaluating
behaviours in relation to an animal's habitat is important in
providing realistic insights to the risks our marine ecosystems face.
We established a model food web to assess the effects of
contaminated microplastics via trophic transfer. As microplastics
accumulate on shorelines (Set
al
a, Norkko, &Lehtiniemi, 2016),
coastal biota are exposed to them. Coastal ecosystems are often
dominated by small crustaceans such as talitrid amphipods. In this
study, coastal talitrids (beach hoppers, Platorchestia smithi), pri-
mary consumers that inhabit the sediment, were exposed to
environmentally relevant concentrations of naturally contaminated
microplastics and then fed to Krefft's frillgobies, Bathygobius krefftii,
teleost sh that inhabit shallow coastal ecosystems. Beach hoppers
Seabirds
(Derraik, 2002,
Hammer et al., 2012)
Marine mammals
(Derraik, 2002, Eriksson & Burton, 2003)
Amphipods
(Chua et al., 2004,
Ugolini, 2013)
Estuarine fish
(Possatto et al., 2002)
Turtles
(Hammer et al., 2012,
Lazar & Gracan, 2011)
Sharks & Rays
(Hammer et al., 2012)
Crustaceans
(Farrell & Nelson, 2013,
Murray & Cowie, 2011)
Piscivorous fish
(Jantz et al., 2013)
Zooplankton
(Setala et al., 2014,
Cole et al., 2015)
Marine larvae
(Kaposi et al., 2014)
Mussels
(Farrell & Nelson, 2013,
Browne et al., 2008) Microplankton
(Setala et al., 2014)
Planktivorous fish
(Boerger et al., 2010)
Corals
(Hall et al., 2015)
Bryozoans
(Ward & Shumway 2004)
Holothurians
(Graham & Thompson
2009)
Lugworms
(Thompson et al., 2004)
Isopods
(Hamer et al., 2004)
Artificial food chain (Artemia to Zebrafish)
(Batel et al., 2016)
Figure 1. The range of marine biota that have been shown to ingest microplastics. The green arrows indicate where physical transfer of microplastic fragments has been
demonstrated. The box in the dotted lines illustrates a recent study where transfer of microplastics and associated contaminants was demonstrated via BaP spiked microplastics in
an articial food chain study Hall et al. 2015.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167160
are an important food source for birds (Dugan, Hubbard, McCrary, &
Pierson, 2003), insects (Poore &Gallagher, 2013) and sh (Fanini &
Lowry, 2014) and therefore important in the transfer of energy
between the different trophic levels (Grifths, Stenton-Dozey, &
Koop, 1983). Gobies are intermediate predators that include am-
phipods in their diet (Souza, Dias, Marques, Antunes, &Martins,
2014), thus making the model suitable to assess trophic transfer
of microplastics. This study used wild-caught sh rather than
laboratory-raised animals to recreate a more realistic setting in
assessing impacts of contamination. Fish came from an urban bay
and so it is likely that some background levels of contaminants
were present in them.
We assessed how transfer of a microplastic-contaminated diet
affected sh personality. We used personality tests as a sensitive
assay for assessing the impact of a microplastic-contaminated diet
on sh behaviour. Personality is a major driver in population dy-
namics that can inuence foraging success, reproduction and
predator interactions (Budaev &Brown, 2011). Variation in per-
sonality traits such as activity or boldness can affect food acqui-
sition and social encounters (Stamps, 2007), with bolder
individuals beneting in a foraging and social context (Smith,
Miner, Wiegmann, &Newman, 2009). Some studies have found
alterations in personality traits following exposure to pollutants.
These studies include modications to boldness in three-spined
sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus, in response to increased car-
bon dioxide (Jutfelt, de Souza, Vuylsteke, &Sturve, 2013),
increased activity and decreased sociality in European perch, Perca
uviatilis, following exposure to pharmaceuticals (Brodin, Fick,
Jonsson, &Klaminder, 2013), changes to boldness and activity
levels in female Siamese ghting sh, Betta splendens, after expo-
sure to the hormone 17
a
-ethinylestradiol (Dzieweczynski,
Campbell, Marks, &Logan, 2014) and changes in key male be-
haviours in Siamese ghting sh following treatment with the
contraceptive Ruta graveolens (Forsatkar, Nematollahi, &Brown,
2016).A shift in the personality distribution within a population
can have consequences for the local community; thus, changes to
personality on an individual level may be an indicator of wider
ecosystem effects (Weis, Smith, Zhou, Santiago-Bass, &Weis,
2001). Moreover, it is well established that hormone levels can
inuence boldness in sh (Chang, Li, Earley, &Hsu, 2012; King,
Fürtbauer, Mamuneas, James, &Manica, 2013), and contaminants
can alter endocrine systems, with many having a feminizing effect
(Vos et al., 2000); thus, we predicted a shift in the personality of
affected sh in response to ingestion of a microplastic-
contaminated diet.
METHODS
Preparation of Microplastics
Commercial polyethylene (PE) microspheres (Cospheric
UVPMS-BG, 1.004 g/ml density, nominal 38e45
m
m diameter,
colour green, Cospheric, Santa Barbara, CA, U.S.A.) were used as
proxies for microplastics in marine environments (Kaposi, Mos,
Kelaher, &Dworjanyn, 2014). To replicate microplastics in the
marine environment, the microplastics were deployed in an urban
bay in Port Jackson, Australia (33
50
0
24
00
,151
15
0
13
00
) for 2 months.
Microplastics were assessed post deployment in Port Jackson for
polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs) as PAHs are one of the
most widespread organic pollutants in the aquatic environment
(Gonçalves, Scholze, Ferreira, Martins, &Correia, 2008). Following
exposure in sea water, the microplastics contained 0.007
m
g/g of
PAHs, suggesting that the microplastics had absorbed a small
amount of contaminants (Tosetto, Brown, &Williamson, 2016).
Study Animals
Platorchestia smithi were collected from March to July 2015 from
the supralittoral zone of Forrester's Beach, NSW (33
24
0
37.06
00
,
151
28
0
05.39
00
). Details of transport and husbandry are provided in
Tosetto et al. (2016). Beach hoppers in treatment groups were
exposed to sediment that had an addition of contaminated PE
microplastics at 3.8% (dry weight) while the beach hoppers in
control groups were exposed to sediment only. Previous studies
reporting negative effects on biota have used microplastics of over
5% wet weight (Besseling, Wegner, Foekema, van den Heuvel-
Greve, &Koelmans, 2013; Graham &Thompson, 2009; Wright,
Rowe, Thompson, &Galloway, 2013). The average distribution of
microplastics on polluted beaches in Hawaii has been recorded as
3.3% by weight (Carson, Colbert, Kaylor, &McDermid, 2011). As
microplastics continue to accumulate in coastal environments
(Turra et al., 2014), the concentration of 3.8% microplastics in the
current study was deemed an environmentally relevant concen-
tration. Beach hoppers were exposed to treatments for 72 h
(exposure period based on previous studies with amphipods (Chua,
Shimeta, Nugegoda, Morrison, &Clarke, 2014; H
amer, Gutow,
K
ohler &Saborowski, 2014)). They readily ingested microplastics,
which were found to accumulate in the beach hoppers. Following
72 h exposure there was a 30% increase in PAHs in the treatment
group (3.09
m
g/g) compared with the control group (2.34
m
g/g;
Tosetto et al., 2016).
Krefft's frillgobies are small, ray-nned marine sh that grow to
a maximum length of 90 mm (Kuiter, 1993). Fish were caught using
hand nets from Chowder Bay, Sydney, Australia (33
50
0
24
00
,
151
15
0
13
00
) in March and April 2015. A total of 33 sh were
collected and acclimated in the laboratory for 2 months prior to
testing. Individuals were housed in groups of eight in four ow-
through sea water aquaria (640 420 mm and 260 mm deep: 70
litres), held in the owthrough sea water facility at the Sydney
Institute of Marine Science (SIMS), Australia. All aquaria were
maintained at the same sea water ow rate (1 litre/min), temper-
ature (16e22
C), with a gravel substrate and aeration from an air
stone. Fish were fed live black worms, Lumbriculus variegatus,
during the 1-week settling period and then a mixture of frozen
commercial Hikari Bio-PureMysis shrimp, brine shrimp and black
worms every other day as per White and Brown (2014). Tanks were
physically enriched with terracotta pots and were lit from overhead
uorescent tubes for 12 h/day. Four weeks before testing, sh were
lightly anaesthetized using a solution of 50 mg of MS222 buffered
with sodium bicarbonate (sh were placed in a bucket containing
1.5 litres of solution until subdued). While anaesthetized their total
length was measured and individuals were tagged using visible
implant uorescent elastomer tags (Northwest Marine Technology,
Inc., Shaw Island, WA, U.S.A.) to assist identication. Gobies recover
from tagging almost immediately and no long-term effects on
behaviour are apparent (White &Brown, 2013). Tanks were cleaned
weekly and all excess food removed after feeding.
Fish Behaviour Assays
Fish behaviour was tested using combined emergence (Brown &
Braithwaite, 2004) and open-eld trial tests (Archard &
Braithwaite, 2011). These assays quantify a range of boldness and
exploration variables that are used as proxies for overall personality
in individual sh (Budaev &Brown, 2011). To maintain novelty
across behavioural trials two slightly different arenas were estab-
lished in 70-litre aquariums identical in size to the housing tanks
for the boldness-exploration tests: (1) the bottom of the aquarium
was covered with 5 cm of sand from a nearby beach and the sides
lined with sand colour shade cloth (Coolaroo 1.83 m Sandstone 70%
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167 161
UV Shade Cloth, Gale Pacic Australia, Braeside, VIC, Australia); and
(2) shell grit (3 cm) covered the bottom of the tank and the sides
lined with green turf (1.8 m wide Natural Synthetic Turf, Tuff Turf,
Mentone, VIC, Australia). Fish were tested individually in both
arenas. The aquarium was lled with 150 mm sea water. A start box
was placed at one end of the arena. The start box (140 140 mm
and 190 mm high) was made of dark plastic, had a lid to cover the
top and a doorway (30 mm 60 mm) on one side that could be
covered with a sliding piece of black plastic. An air stone was placed
behind the start box for aeration. After capture with a hand net
from the housing tank, the test sh was placed into the start box,
and the lid placed on top. The sh was left for a 2 min settling
period after which a remote pulley lifted the door, allowing the
individual to emerge from the doorway and explore the test arena.
To eliminate impacts of human observers on sh behaviour, ob-
servers stood behind a screen and sh were observed remotely via
a computer monitor connected by rewire (Belkin FireWire cable
IEEE 1394) to a digital camera mounted over the test arena. At the
end of the test period (600 s) sh were netted and returned to their
housing tank.
Experimental Treatments
Six individuals died prior to microplastic exposure and so were
excluded from the treatment groups; 28 sh remained for analysis
of personality. All sh were put through behavioural trials before
exposure to beach hopper treatments. The test was repeated three
times prior to microplastic exposure: trials 1 and 3 were in arena 1
(sandy) and trial 2 in arena 2 (turf). There was at least a 7-day in-
terval between each trial. Following initial tests, gobies were
randomly assigned to two treatment groups and housed accord-
ingly. We placed around 25 beach hoppers in each tank every 2
days (twoethree beach hoppers per sh). We fed the sh with a
pipette so that we could ensure that they all ate the beach hoppers.
Tanks were observed until all sh had fed. The second round of
testing commenced at the end of the rst week of the beach hopper
diet. Three trials were undertaken after microplastic exposure.
Trials 4 and 6 were again undertaken in arena 1 and trial 5 in arena
2. There was at least 7 days between trials 4, 5 and 6 (Fig. 2).
Scoring Behaviour
Videos were scored for emergence, exploration and activity
levels using Etholog (Ottoni, 2000). Emergence time was recorded
when the entire sh (i.e. the last of the caudal n) had emerged out
of the start box. Fish that failed to emerge were given the maximum
score of 600 s. To quantify exploration, i.e. the amount of time sh
spent in various parts of the arena, a grid was placed over the tank
that divided the arena into 10 sections (Fig. 3). The various be-
haviours scored are outlined in Table 1. The start box and cover
were considered safe areas for the sh, the top and middle rows
were identied as risky, and the bottom row and centre were
deemed dangerous as they were furthest from any cover. The total
time spent in each quadrat was recorded, and a sh was not
deemed to be in a new quadrat until the entire sh had moved into
the area. The number of movements, each time the sh swam
forwards or turned left or right, made by the sh over the test
period was a proxy for activity.
Statistical Analysis
To test for the presence of personality, incorporating boldness
and exploration, individual traits were collapsed into principal
component scores using principal components analysis (PCA). PCA
was undertaken using the princomp function in R (R Core Team,
2013). Eigenvectors with a value greater than one were used to
describe the variance in the data set. Each retained principal
component was then used as a composite behavioural variable in
future analyses.
Agreement repeatability (R; the intraclass correlation coef-
cient) of component scores was calculated between trials using the
within- and between-variance components in a linear mixed-
effects model (LMM). The restricted maximum likelihood method
(REML) was used in the package rptR, with individual sh identity
as the grouping factor. Adjusted repeatability (R
A
) was assessed
Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3
Days
0 8
24
Trial 4 Trial 5 Trial 6
78 86
93
Before exposure After exposure
Figure 2. Schematic diagram of behavioural trials. The dashed line indicates where diet changed from commercial sh food to beach hoppers. Trials with solid lines indicate arena 1
(Sand) and dashed lines indicate arena 2 (Turf).
SB G0 G0
G5
G8
G1
G2
G3
G4
G6
G7
G9
CVR
Figure 3. The test arena used for scoring sh boldness and exploration.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167162
across trials for each treatment group before and after exposure
where size and days between trial were included as xed effects
and individual sh identity as the grouping factor. Condence in-
tervals and standard errors for both Rand R
A
were calculated from
parametric bootstraps that created the distributions of likelihood
ratios (1000 times). Pvalues for repeatability estimates were
derived from permutation tests (Nakagawa &Schielzeth, 2010). See
the Supplementary Material for the R code for Rand R
A
.
We analysed data in line with their longitudinal nature
(repeated measures over time) using linear mixed models (LMM;
see the Supplementary Material for the R code). The PC1 scores
were not normally distributed but because both positive and
negative values were present in the data set the negative signs were
removed before performing a log transformation on the PC1 values.
The negative sign was replaced following transformation. As the
PC2 scores were normally distributed, no transformation was car-
ried out.
In the analysis, we rst assessed whether there was a trend over
time within each exposure (before and after exposure to the beach
hopper diet) for PC1 and PC2. We built a model using days since the
rst observation as the principal variable of interest to quantify any
changes in behaviour over time within each of the exposure pe-
riods. The variables treatment (control/treatment) and sh size
were also included as xed effects. To account for individual sh
variability we included individual sh identity as a random effect.
There was no trend over time before exposure to the contaminated
diet or either PC1 (P¼0.727) or PC2 (P¼0.621). There was also no
effect of sh size for either PC1 (P¼0.738) or PC2 (P¼0.141).
Similarly, when we assessed any effects after exposure to a
contaminated diet there was no effect of time for PC1 (P¼0.812) or
PC2 (P¼0.359), nor was there an effect of sh size in either PC1
(P¼0.999) or PC2 (P¼0.479; Fig. 4).
As there was no effect of days or size within each exposure
period we ran a nal model that assessed changes in PC scores
before and after exposure. Fixed effects were treatment and
exposure with an interaction term between them. Random effects
were individual sh identity.
Ethical Note
Our experimental methods conformed to the standards set by
Macquarie University Animal Ethics committee (ARA 2014/003-7).
Fish and beach hopper collections were conducted under NSW
Fisheries Scientic Collection Permit numbers P08/0010-4.2 and
P14/0032-1.1, respectively. Fish were observed at least every second
day to ensure their health and wellbeing and environmental
housing conditions (water temperature, salinity and pH) were also
regularly monitored. At completion of the study the sh were
released to their original environment.
RESULTS
PCA condensed the variables to just two components that
accounted for 56% of the total variance. Component score co-
efcients showed that boldness variables, such as emergence time,
total start box time and time spent in more risky parts of the arena,
Table 1
The 10 variables used in the sh personality testing
Code Variable Description
FE Emergence The time for sh to emerge in full from the start box, recorded when the caudal n was out of the box
SB Start box The total time (initial time þreturn time) is indicated here. The total time spent in the start box. The start box was a safe
area and sh would return to the start box after emergence
TR Top row Total time sh spent in grids 1, 2 and 3
MR Middle row Total time sh spent in grids 4, 5 and 6
G5 Grid 5 Although related to MR, the centre of the arena was deemed one of the most dangerous areas for the sh to move into
CV Cover The total time spent exploring the area around the start box. Cover was dened as 2 cm around the start box
BR Bottom row Total time sh spent exploring the bottom of the arena, a combination of time spent in grids 7, 8 and 9
G0 Grid 0 Total time spent the top area of the arena once outside the cover of the start box
G8 Grid 8 Related to BR but like G5, a little riskier given it was in the centre of the arena
AC Activity The total movements made by the sh over the course of the test
–2.5
–2.5
0
2.5
5
123
PC1
456
Treatment
Control
Treatment
0
2.5
5
123
PC2
456
Trial
(a)
(b)
Figure 4. Effect of microplastic exposure on PC scores of gobies monitored in the six
behavioural trials. The dotted line between trials 3 and 4 indicates where the diet
changed to beach hoppers. (a) PC1 (boldness) scores. (b) PC2 (exploration) scores.
White triangles indicate control individuals and black circles indicate treatment in-
dividuals. The solid lines indicate the population mean with the solid line representing
control sh and broken lines representing treatment sh.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167 163
were the largest contributors to PC1. Exploratory variables, such as
time spent exploring around the start box (cover) and the more
exposed and furthest areas from the start box (bottom row),
contributed most to PC2. Boldness contributed to almost half of the
variance compared with exploratory variables that explained a
relatively small proportion of the variance, suggesting the experi-
ment was largely a boldness assay (Table 2).
Both repeatability (R) and adjusted repeatability (R
A
) results are
provided in Table 3.Assh size and number of days were not
included in the nal mixed model we used the repeatability (R)
values in all further results. All trials wererepeatable for PC1 before
exposure (R¼0.476 (0.116),95% CI 0.220 to 0.664, P<0.001) and
after exposure (R¼0.684 (0.085),95% CI 0.474 to 0.816, P<0.001)
suggesting that the personality traits were robust. Only trials after
exposure were repeatable for PC2 (R¼0.401 (0.119),95% CI 0.129 to
0.603, P<0.001). When repeatability was assessed for treatment
and control groups separately, there was no signicant difference in
repeatability values for PC1 before or after exposure to treatments
suggesting that consumption of prey items contaminated with
microplastics did not affect repeatability of boldness traits. There
was, however, an increase in repeatability values for PC2 in both
treatments following exposure. This implies an effect of familiar-
ization to the test arena rather than the exposure to a contaminated
diet (Table 3).
Exposure to a microplastic-contaminated diet did not affect
boldness or exploration in sh. No signicant interactions occurred
between exposure (before and after exposure to beach hoppers)
and treatment (contaminated or not) for either PC1 (boldness:
t
137
¼0.035, P¼0.891) or PC2 (exploration: t
137
¼0.197,
P¼0.557). There was a signicant effect of exposure to beach
hoppers on the PC1 scores (t
27
¼0.418, P<0.05) with sh in both
treatment and control groups decreasing in boldness following
exposure to the beach hopper diet (Fig. 4). In scores indicating
exploratory behaviour (PC2), there was a trend for sh in the
control group to show greater exploratory tendencies across both
exposures; however, this was not signicant (t
27
¼0.594,
P¼0.058; Fig. 4).
DISCUSSION
This is the rst study to assess possible behavioural conse-
quences of trophic transfer of microplastics and associated con-
taminants, in this case through consumption of contaminated prey
by a sh. As toxic contaminants found in plastics have been asso-
ciated with endocrine disruption (S
offker &Tyler, 2012), we ex-
pected a shift in the behaviour and personality of affected sh. Here
we have shown that, contrary to expectations, transfer of
contaminated microplastics from beach hoppers to sh did not
affect sh personality. There was, however, an overall shift in
boldness with sh becoming shyer after exposure to the beach
hopper diet irrespective of treatment. This was perhaps due to sh
habituating to the experimental tasks, or possibly the alteration in
diet. Exploration remained relatively constant before and after
exposure possibly due to the variation in individuals between
treatments, with control sh being more exploratory throughout.
Repeatability values for boldness were not signicantly different
before or after exposure to treatments. Exploration repeatability
values increased across both control and treatment groups after
exposure suggesting acclimatization with the test arena.
Concern surrounding microplastics is largely due to the associ-
ated contaminants and their capacity to desorb into animals (Bakir,
Rowland, &Thompson, 2014), and subsequently accumulate
through the food web in different trophic levels (Teuten et al.,
2009). The assumption that pollutants biomagnify at all levels of
the marine food chain has been disputed (Gray, 2002) but the
biomagnication of organic pollutants from lower trophic levels to
sh has been demonstrated (Kelly, Ikonomou, Blair, Morin, &
Gobas, 2007; Nfon et al., 2008). Whether microplastics are a
viable pathway for contaminant exposure is currently under
debate. Conceptual models assessing the rates of desorption in
organisms suggest that microplastics do not provide a relevant
exposure pathway (Gouin, 2011; Koelmans, 2013, 2014). However,
positive relationships between microplastic consumption and
increased contamination concentration in animal tissue have been
demonstrated in seabirds (Ryan, Connell, &Gardner, 1988), am-
phipods (Chua et al., 2014) and bivalves (Avio et al., 2015).
Furthermore, recent studies suggest that desorption rates of con-
taminants in gut surfactants are 30 times faster than in sea water
for some contaminants, with pH and body temperature also inu-
encing the rate (Bakir et al., 2014). Polyethylene microplastics
deployed in the marine environment have the capacity to absorb
contaminants, which can accumulate in beach hoppers following
consumption, increasing the contaminant load of the organism
(Tosetto et al., 2016). Given our current understanding of micro-
plastics and contaminants we expected to see a change in sh
personality following consumption of contaminated beach
hoppers.
Using personality to assess anthropogenic impacts on in-
dividuals is a relatively new area of research. Personality is a major
driver in population dynamics that can inuence competitive in-
teractions and how animals deal with changes in their environment
(Montiglio &Royaut
e, 2014), particularly those attributed to
anthropogenic impacts such as environmental contamination
(Wong &Candolin, 2015). Assessing how contaminants alter per-
sonality traits provides a far subtler way of establishing effects in
comparison to the standard LD50 approach commonly used in
ecotoxicology (Oulton et al., 2014). Recent studies have observed
sublethal effects of pollutants on behaviour (Brodin et al., 2013,
2014; Dzieweczynski et al., 2014; Forsatkar et al., 2016; Jutfelt
et al., 2013); however, when assessing the effects of hormone
17
a
-ethinylestradiol on female Siamese ghting sh
(Dzieweczynski et al., 2014) and increased carbon dioxide on three-
spined sticklebacks (Jutfelt et al., 2013) some habituation to bold-
ness assays over time was reported.
The current study did not observe signicant effects of treat-
ment on sh personality or the repeatability of these behaviours.
There was, however, an alteration in sh behaviour following
exposure to the beach hopper diet with all sh becoming shyer. The
longer sh are exposed to a similar task the more inexible
behaviour comes, with less motivation to emerge and subsequently
less time given to exploring an area (Kieffer &Colgan, 1992).
Moreover, sh will decrease activity as they become familiar with a
Table 2
PCA loadings for each of the behaviour variables in the personality tests
Behaviour Code Boldness (PC1) Exploration (PC2)
Full emergence FE ¡0.372 0.188
Start box SB ¡0.423 0.161
Cover CV 0.280 ¡0.514
Bottom row BR 0.246 0.513
Middle row MR 0.379 0.260
Top row TR 0.365 0.146
Grids adjacent to start box G0 0.213 0.119
Centre grid G5 0.285 0.091
Bottom centre grid G8 0.287 0.523
Activity AC 0.241 0.164
Eigenvalue 4.99 1.44
Percentage variance explained 49.4 6.4
The corresponding code for each behaviour is provided. Variables in bold contribute
the highest amounts to each behavioural variable.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167164
tank environment (Brown, 2001; Sharma, Coombs, Patton, &de
Perera, 2009). By varying the test arenas in trials 2 and 5 we
attempted to reduce habituation to the behavioural assays. While
some variation in boldness traits was observed in the trials before
exposure, we did not see corresponding variation after exposure,
suggesting sh had become familiarized to the task in the later
trials. It is possible that the change in behaviour could be due to
increased fear (Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw, &Heinsohn,
2013) but this is unlikely as the open-eld trial was designed to
mirror the ecological setting of B. krefftii which seek shelter in rock
pools and crevices (White &Brown, 2015), and there was no novel
object or risk of predation included in the assay. Alternatively, the
alteration in diet may have affected the behaviour of the sh after
exposure. Hunger, or perceived hunger, has been shown to increase
activity and emergence times in sh (Brown &Braithwaite, 2004;
Thomson, Watts, Pottinger, &Sneddon, 2012); thus, it is possible
the gobies became satiated on beach hoppers in this study. Note
that while there was a shift in sh boldness after exposure to the
beach hopper diet, there was no difference across the treatment
and control groups that could indicate an effect of our treatments
on these processes.
It is possible that a longer exposure timeframe is required to
observe an effect of microplastic exposure. When directly exposed
to marine contaminated microplastics, Japanese meduka, Oryzias
latipes, suffered hepatic distress but this was not obvious until 2
months after initial exposure, suggesting that a certain contami-
nation threshold may be needed before a physiological effect is
observed (Rochman, Hoh, Kurobe, &Teh, 2013). It is possible that
the short duration of this study may not have been long enough to
reach the contamination threshold for either P. smithi or B. kreftii.As
these animals live up to 8 months and several years, respectively, it
is possible they are exposed to far greater amounts of plastics and
associated pollutants in their lifetime.
Alternatively, it is possible that background contamination
levels in the wild-caught sh masked any environmental effects of
microplastic exposure. Studies assessing contaminant transfer
from microplastics to sh have fed microplastics directly to
laboratory-raised sh that have not had prior exposure to envi-
ronmental pollutants (Rochman et al., 2013). Moreover, a recent
laboratory-based study assessing the capacity for microplastics
and associated contaminants to transfer through an articial food
chain used concentrations of microplastics and contaminants at
much higher levels than environmentally relevant concentrations
and used animals raised in the laboratory environment (Batel
et al., 2016). Theoretical models predict that the consumption of
microplastics does not increase the burden of pollutants in sh
given the background levels already present in the environment
(Gouin, 2011). Recently Koelmans, Bakir, Burton, and Janssen
(2016), following an assessment of the scientic literature,
determined that the uptake of contaminants through natural
pathways possibly exceeds accumulation via microplastics in most
habitats. This implies that ingestion of microplastics is unlikely to
increase an animal's exposure to contaminants in the marine
environment. Using wild-caught sh is more realistic than using
laboratory-raised animals in assessing ecological impacts of pol-
lutants. As we used sh from an urbanized area in Sydney,
Australia and fed a diet of commercial frozen food during accli-
mation, it is plausible that the experimental effects of plastic
consumption were not discernible from the background levels in
our wild-caught sh.
Gobies collected from highly polluted areas have shown differ-
ences in behavioural responses to stress than counterparts
collected from less contaminated sites (Marentette, Tong, &
Balshine, 2013). It is possible that sh with lower levels of back-
ground pollution may elicit a different response when exposed to
contaminated microplastics. As we expect the distribution of
microplastics to increase and expand through our marine ecosys-
tems (I~
niguez, Conesa, &Fullana, 2016), the effects of microplastic
exposure to wildlife from areas of low contamination should be
explored. Studies have demonstrated that assessment of complex
behaviours provides us with effective tools to assess sublethal
levels of pollutants; however, future studies should also incorpo-
rate analysis of animal digestive tracts and tissues to establish
whether microplastics are changing the contaminant load of ani-
mals through the food web.
Overall, this study found that short-term trophic transfer of
microplastics via prey does not affect the personality of sh.
(Brodin et al., 2014; Dzieweczynski et al., 2014; Jutfelt et al., 2013).
We did observe a shift in boldness after exposure to the beach
hoppers that may be attributed to familiarization, or a change in
diet; however, there was no signicant effect between the treat-
ment groups after exposure. The problem of animals learning and
habituating to an experiment will persist across any longitudinal
study. To understand the broad impacts of plastic exposure we
need to take an all-inclusive approach to behavioural experiments
that assess the impacts of microplastics on behaviours applicable to
foraging, sexual activity, sociality and learning. Future long-term,
ecologically relevant studies should directly examine, via tissue
analysis, whether microplastics and contaminants are accumu-
lating through the food web. A suite of personalities and behav-
iours should also be assessed to get a more comprehensive picture
of changes in behaviour in response to microplastics through
different trophic levels. It is possible, however, given the back-
ground levels of contamination already present in coastal envi-
ronments, that trophic transfer of microplastics does not provide
an additional exposure pathway for contaminants to move through
the food web.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by the Department of Biological Sci-
ences at Macquarie University. We thank members and interns of
Table 3
Repeatability estimates across trials for both Control and Treatment groups
Treatment Exposure PC R(SE) CI PR
A
(SE) CI P
Control Before PC1 0.462 (0.163) [0.089, 0.729] 0.005 0.493 (0.162) [0.124, 0.735] 0.003
PC2 0.072 (0.128) [0.000, 0.047] 0.294 0.155 (0.146) [0.000, 0.495] 0.184
After PC1 0.690 (0.126) [0.361, 0.860] 0.001 0.686 (0.133) [0.353, 0.855] 0.001
PC2 0.328 (0.171) [0.000, 0.642] 0.017 0.343 (0.175) [0.000, 0.649] 0.024
Treatment Before PC1 0.498 (0.161) [0.138, 0.746] 0.004 0.620 (0.145) [0.254, 0.818] 0.002
PC2 0.116 (0.131) [0.000, 0.430] 0.210 0.293 (0.165) [0.000, 0.590] 0.041
After PC1 0.688 (0.129) [0.344, 0.853] 0.001 0.691 (0.146) [0.282, 0.856] 0.001
PC2 0.488 (0.166) [0.096, 0.732] 0.001 0.470 (0.171) [0.066, 0.731] 0.008
The table shows repeatability (R) and adjusted repeatability (R
A
), standard errors (SE), the 95% condence interval (CI) and associated Pvalue of each estimate (N¼14 in-
dividual sh per treatment). Signicant repeatability estimates are shown in bold.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167 165
the MEG and BEEF labs for eld and laboratory assistance, Sarah
Houlahan for assistance with GCMS and analysis of beach hopper
contamination, Petra Graham for guidance on statistical analysis,
and Alistair Poore for advice on beach hopper husbandry. This
paper is contribution number 193 from the Sydney Institute of
Marine Science.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material associated with this article is available,
in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.
10.035.
References
Archard, G. A., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2011). Variation in aggressive behaviour in the
poeciliid sh Brachyrhaphis episcopi: Population and sex differences. Behav-
ioural Processes, 86(1), 52e57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09.002.
Avio, C. G., Gorbi, S., Milan, M., Benedetti, M., Fattorini, D., d'Errico, G., et al. (2015).
Pollutants bioavailability and toxicological risk from microplastics to marine
mussels. Environmental Pollution, 198,211e222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.envpol.2014.12.021.
Bae, M. J., & Park, Y.-S. (2014). Biological early warning system based on the responses
of aquatic organisms to disturbances: A review. Science of the Total Environment,
466e467,635e649. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2013.07.075.
Bakir, A., Rowland, S. J., & Thompson, R. C. (2014). Enhanced desorption of persistent
organic pollutants from microplastics under simulated physiological conditions.
Environmental Pollution, 185,16e23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2013.10.007.
Barnes, D. K., Galgani, F., Thompson, R. C., & Barlaz, M. (2009). Accumulation and
fragmentation of plastic debris in global environments. Philosophical Trans-
actions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1526), 1985e1998. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0205.
Batel, A., Linti, F., Scherer, M., Erdinger, L., & Braunbeck, T. (2016). The transfer of
benzo [a] pyrene from microplastics to Artemia nauplii and further to zebrash
via a trophic food web experimenteCYP1A induction and visual tracking of
persistent organic pollutants. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry.http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/etc.3361/full.
Besseling, E., Wegner, A., Foekema, E. M., van den Heuvel-Greve, M. J., &
Koelmans, A. A. (2013). Effects of microplastic on tness and PCB bio-
accumulation by the Lugworm Arenicola marina (L.). Environmental Science &
Technology, 47(1), 593e600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es302763x.
Borgå, K., Fisk, A. T., Hoekstra, P. F., & Muir, D. C. (2004). Biological and chemical
factors of importance in the bioaccumulation and trophic transfer of persistent
organochlorine contaminants in arctic marine food webs. Environmental Toxi-
cology and Chemistry, 23(10), 2367e2385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1897/03-518/full.
Brodin, T., Fick, J., Jonsson, M., & Klaminder, J. (2013). Dilute concentrations of a
psychiatric drug alter behavior of sh from natural populations. Science,
339(6121), 814e815. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1226850.
Brodin, T., Piovano, S., Fick, J., Klaminder, J., Heynen, M., & Jonsson, M. (2014).
Ecological effects of pharmaceuticals in aquatic systemsdimpacts through
behavioural alterations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London.
Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1656), 20130580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/
rstb.2013.0580.
Brown, C. (2001). Familiarity with the test environment improves escape responses
in the crimson spotted rainbowsh, Melanotaenia duboulayi.Animal Cognition,
4(2), 109e113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710100105.
Brown, C., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2004). Size matters: A test of boldness in eight
populations of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi.Animal Behaviour, 68(6),
1325e1329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.004.
Browne, M. A., Galloway, T., & Thompson, R. (2008). Microplasticdan emerging
contaminant of potential concern? Integrated Environmental Assessment and
Management, 3(4), 559e561.
Budaev, S., & Brown, C. (2011). Personality traits and behaviour. In C. Brown,
K. Laland, & J. Krause (Eds.), Fish cognition and behaviour (pp. 135e165). Oxford,
U.K.: Wiley-Blackwell.
Carson, H. S., Colbert, S. L., Kaylor, M. J., & McDermid, K. J. (2011). Small plastic debris
changes water movement and heat transfer through beach sediments. Marine
Pollution Bulletin, 62(8), 1708e1713. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.marpolbul.2011.05.032.
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G., & Heinsohn, R. (2013).
Animal personality: What are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biological
Reviews, 88(2), 465e475. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/brv.12007.
Chang, C., Li, C.-Y., Earley, R. L., & Hsu, Y. (2012). Aggression and related behavioral
traits: The impact of winning and losing and the role of hormones. Integrative
and Comparative Biology, 52(6), 801e813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es405717z.
Chua, E., Shimeta, J., Nugegoda, D., Morrison, P. D., & Clarke, B. O. (2014). Assimi-
lation of polybrominated diphenyl ethers from microplastics by the marine
amphipod, Allorchestes Compressa.Environmental Science &Technology, 48(14),
8127e8134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es405717z.
Clotfelter, E. D., Bell, A. M., & Levering, K. R. (2004). The role of animal behaviour in
the study of endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Animal Behaviour, 68(4), 665e676.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.004.
Cole, M., Lindeque, P., Halsband, C., & Galloway, T. S. (2011). Microplastics as con-
taminants in the marine environment: A review. Marine Pollution Bulletin,
62(12), 2588e2597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.marpolbul.2011.09.025.
Dugan, J. E., Hubbard, D. M., McCrary, M. D., & Pierson, M. O. (2003). The response
of macrofauna communities and shorebirds to macrophyte wrack subsidies
on exposed sandy beaches of southern California. Estuar ine, Coastal an d Shelf
Science, 58(Suppl. 0), 25e40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7714(03)
00045-3.
Dzieweczynski, T. L., Campbell, B. A., Marks, J. M., & Logan, B. (2014). Acute exposure
to 17
a
-ethinylestradiol alters boldness behavioral syndrome in female Siamese
ghting sh. Hormones and Behavior, 66(4), 577e584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.yhbeh.2014.08.005.
Eerkes-Medrano, D., Thompson, R. C., & Aldridge, D. C. (2015). Microplastics in
freshwater systems: A review of the emerging threats, identication of
knowledge gaps and prioritisation of research needs. Water Research, 75,
63e82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.watres.2015.02.012.
Fanini, L., & Lowry, J. (2014). Coastal talitrids and connectivity between beaches: A
behavioural test. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 457,
120e127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2014.04.010.
Farrell, P., & Nelson, K. (2013). Trophic level transfer of microplastic: Mytilus edulis
to Carcinus maenas.Environmental Pollution, 177(0), 1e3. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.envpol.2013.01.046.
Forsatkar, M. N., Nematollahi, M. A., & Brown, C. (2016). The toxicological effect of
Ruta graveolens extract in Siamese ghting sh: A behavioral and histopatho-
logical approach. Ecotoxicology, 25(4), 824e834. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10646-016-1639-5.
von Glasow, R., Jickells, T. D., Baklanov, A., Carmichael, G. R., Church, T. M.,
Gallardo, L., et al. (2013). Megacities and large urban agglomerations in the
coastal zone: Interactions between atmosphere, land, and marine ecosystems.
Ambio, 42(1), 13e28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13280-012-0343-9.
Gonçalves, R., Scholze, M., Ferreira, A. M., Martins, M., & Correia, A. D. (2008). The
joint effect of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons on sh behavior. Environmental
Research, 108(2), 205e213.
Gouin, T. (2011). A thermodynamic approach for assessing the environmental
exposure of chemicals absorbed to microplastic. Environmental Science &
Technology, 45(4), 1466e1472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es1032025.
Graham, E. R., & Thompson, J. T. (2009). Deposit- and suspension-feeding sea cu-
cumbers (Echinodermata) ingest plastic fragments. Journal of Experimental Marine
Biology and Ecology, 368(1), 22e29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2008.09.007.
Gray, J. S. (2002). Biomagnication in marine systems: The perspective of an
ecologist. Marine Pollution Bulletin, 45(1e12), 46e52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0025-326X(01)00323-X.
Grifths, C. L., Stenton-Dozey, J. M. E., & Koop, K. (1983). Kelp wrack and the ow of
energy through a Sandy Beach ecosystem. In A. McLachlan, & T. Erasmus (Eds.),
Sandy Beaches as Ecosystems: Based on the Proceedings of the First International
Symposium on Sandy Beaches, held in Port Elizabeth, South Africa, 17e21 January
1983 (pp. 547e556). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.
Hall, N., Berry, K., Rintoul, L., & Hoogenboom, M. (2015). Microplastic ingestion by
scleractinian corals. Marine Biology, 162(3), 725e732. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s00227-015-2619-7.
H
amer, J., Gutow, L., K
ohler, A., & Saborowski, R. (2014). Fate of microplastics in the
marine Isopod Idotea emarginata.Environmental Science &Technology, 48(22),
13451e13458.
Hontela, A. (1998). Interrenal dysfunction in sh from contaminated sites: In vivo
and in vitro assessment. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry, 17(1), 44e48.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620170107/full.
I~
niguez, M. E., Conesa, J. A., & Fullana, A. (2016). Marine debris occurrence and
treatment: A review. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews, 64, 394e402.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rser.2016.06.031.
Islam, S., & Tanaka, M. (2004). Impacts of pollution on coastal and marine ecosys-
tems including coastal and marine sheries and approach for management: A
review and synthesis. Marine Pollution Bulletin, 48(7), 624e649. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.marpolbul.2003.12.004.
Jutfelt, F., de Souza, K. B., Vuylsteke, A., & Sturve, J. (2013). Behavioural disturbances
in a temperate sh exposed to sustained high CO
2
levels. PLoS One, 8(6), e65825.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065825.
Kaposi, K. L., Mos, B., Kelaher, B. P., & Dworjanyn, S. A. (2014). Ingestion of micro-
plastic has limited impact on a marine larva. Environmental Science &Technol-
ogy, 48(3), 1638e1645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es404295e.
Kelly, B. C., Ikonomou, M. G., Blair, J., Morin, A. E., & Gobas, F. A. P. C. (2007). Food
webeSpecic biomagnication of persistent organic pollutants. Science,
317(5835), 236e239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1138275.
Kieffer, J. D., & Colgan, P. W. (1992). The role of learning in sh behaviour. Reviews in
Fish Biology and Fisheries, 2(2), 125e143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00042881.
King, A. J., Fürtbauer, I., Mamuneas, D., James, C., & Manica, A. (2013). Sex-differ-
ences and temporal consistency in stickleback sh boldness. PLoS One, 8(12),
e81116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081116.
Koelmans, A. A. (2013). Plastic as a carrier of POPs to aquatic organisms: A model
analysis. Environmental Science &Technology, 47(14), 7812e7820. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1021/es401169n.
Koelmans, A. A. (2014). Leaching of plastic additives to marine organisms. Envi-
ronmental Pollution, 187,49e54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2013.12.013.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167166
Koelmans, A. A., Bakir, A., Burton, G. A., & Janssen, C. R. (2016). Microplastic as a
vector for chemicals in the aquatic environment: Critical review and model-
supported reinterpretation of empirical studies. Environmental Science and
Technology, 50(7), 3315e3326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/acs.est.5b06069.
Kuiter, R. H. (1993). Coastal shes of south-eastern Australia. Bathurst, Australia:
Crawford House Press.
Law, K. L., & Thompson, R. C. (2014). Microplastics in the seas. Science, 345(144), 2.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1254065.
Lithner, D., Damberg, J., Dave, G., & Larsson, Å. (2009). Leachates from plastic
consumer productsescreening for toxicity with Daphnia magna.Chemosphere,
74(9), 1195e1200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2008.11.022.
Marentette, J. R., Tong, S., & Balshine, S. (2013). The cortisol stress response in male
round goby (Neogobius melanostomus): Effects of living in polluted environ-
ments? Environmental Biology of Fishes, 96(6), 723e733. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10641-012-0064-8.
Mato, Y., Isobe, T., Takada, H., Kanehiro, H., Ohtake, C., & Kaminuma, T. (2001).
Plastic resin pellets as a transport medium for toxic chemicals in the marine
environment. Environmental Science &Technology, 35(2), 318e324. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1021/es0010498.
Montiglio, P.-O., & Royaut
e, R. (2014). Contaminants as a neglected source of
behavioural variation. Animal Behaviour, 88,29e35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.anbehav.2013.11.018.
Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2010). Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian
data: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews, 85(4), 935e956. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00141.x.
Nfon,E.,Cousins,I.T.,&Broman,D.(2008).Biomagnication of organic pollutants in
benthic and pelagic marine food chains from the Baltic Sea. Science of the Total
Environment, 397(1e3), 190e204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2008.02.029.
Ottoni, E. B. (2000). Etholog 2.2eA tool for the transcription and timing of behavior
observation sessions. Behaviour Research Methods, Instruments &Computers,
32(3), 4.
Oulton, L. J., Taylor, M. P., Hose, G. C., & Brown, C. (2014). Sublethal toxicity of un-
treated and treated stormwater Zn concentrations on the foraging behaviour of
Paratya australiensis (Decapoda: Atyidae). Ecotoxicology, 23(6), 1022e1029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10646-014-1246-2.
Poore, A. G., & Gallagher, K. M. (2013). Strong consequences of diet choice ina talitrid
amphipod consuming seagrass and algal wrack. Hydrobiologia, 701(1), 117e127.
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna,
Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/.
Rochman, C. M. (2013). Plastics and priority pollutants: A multiple stressor in
aquatic habitats. Environmental Science &Technology, 47(6), 2439e2440. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1021/es400748b.
Rochman, C. M., Hoh, E., Kurobe, T., & Teh, S. J. (2013). Ingested plastic transfers
hazardous chemicals to sh and induces hepatic stress. Scientic Reports, 3,
3263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep03263.
Ryan, P., Connell, A., & Gardner, B. (1988). Plastic ingestion and PCBs in seabirds: Is
there a relationship? Marine Pollution Bulletin, 19(4), 174e176. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/0025-326X(88)90674-1.
Scott, G. R., & Sloman, K. A. (2004). The effects of environmental pollutants on
complex sh behaviour: Integrating behavioural and physiological indicators of
toxicity. Aquatic Toxicology, 68(4), 369e392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.aquatox.2004.03.016.
Set
al
a, O., Fleming-Lehtinen, V., & Lehtiniemi, M. (2014). Ingestion and transfer of
microplastics in the planktonic food web. Environmental Pollution, 185,77e83.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2013.10.013.
Set
al
a, O., Norkko, J., & Lehtiniemi, M. (2016). Feeding type affects microplastic
ingestion in a coastal invertebrate community. Marine Pollution Bulletin, 102(1),
95e101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.marpolbul.2015.11.053.
Sharma, S., Coombs, S., Patton, P., & de Perera, T. B. (2009). The function of wall-
following behaviors in the Mexican blind cavesh and a sighted relative, the
Mexican tetra (Astyanax). Journal of Comparative Physiology A , 195(3), 225e240.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00359-008-0400-9.
Sloman, K. A., & Wilson, R. W. (2006). Anthropogenic impacts upon behaviour and
physiology. In K. A. Sloman, R. W. Wilson, & S. Balshine (Eds.), Behaviour and
physiology in sh (pp. 413e468). London, U.K.: Elsevier.
Smith, K. L., Miner, J. G., Wiegmann, D. D., & Newman, S. P. (2009). Individual dif-
ferences in exploratory and antipredator behaviour in juvenile smallmouth bass
(Micropterus dolomieu). Behaviour, 146(2), 283e294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/
156853909X410784.
S
offker,M.,&Tyler,C.R.(2012).Endocrinedisruptingchemicalsandsexual
behaviors in sh-a critical review on effects and possible consequences.
Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 42(8), 653e668. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/
10408444.2012.692114.
Souza, A., Dias, E., Marques, J., Antunes, C., & Martins, I. (2014). Population structure,
production and feeding habit of the sand goby Pomatoschistus minutus (Acti-
nopterygii: Gobiidae) in the Minho estuary (NW Iberian Peninsula). Environ-
mental Biology of Fishes, 98(1), 1e14.
Stamps,J.A.(2007).Growth-mortalitytradeoffsandpersonality traitsin animals.
Ecology Letters, 10(5), 355e363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2007.01034.x/
full.
Teuten, E. L., Rowland, S. J., Galloway, T. S., & Thompson, R. C. (2007). Potential for
plastics to transport hydrophobic contaminants. Environmental Science &
Technology, 41(22), 7759e7764. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/es071737s.
Teuten, E. L., Saquing, J. M., Knappe, D. R., Barlaz, M. A., Jonsson, S., Bj
orn, A., et al.
(2009). Transport and release of chemicals from plastics to the environment and
to wildlife. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London Series B, Bio-
logical Sciences,364(1526), 2027e2045.http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0284.
Thompson, R. C., Olsen, Y., Mitchell, R. P., Davis, A., Rowland, S. J., John, A. W., et al.
(2004). Lost at sea: Where is all the plastic? Science, 304(5672), 838. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1094559.
Thomson, J. S., Watts, P. C., Pottinger, T. G., & Sneddon, L. U. (2012). Plasticity of
boldness in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss: Do hunger and predation in-
uence risk-taking behaviour? Hormones and Behavior, 61(5), 750e
757. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.03.014.
Tosetto, L., Brown, C., & Williamson, J. E. (2016). Microplastics on beaches: Ingestion
and behavioural consequences for beachhoppers. Marine Biology, 163(10),1e13.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00227-016-2973-0.
Turra, A., Manzano, A. B., Dias, R. J. S., Mahiques, M. M., Barbosa, L., Balthazar-
Silva, D., et al. (2014). Three-dimensional distribution of plastic pellets in sandy
beaches: Shifting paradigms. Scientic Reports, 4, 4435. http://www.nature.
com/articles/srep04435.
Vos, J. G., Dybing, E., Greim, H. A., Ladefoged, O., Lambr
e, C., Tarazona, J. V., et al.
(2000). Health effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals on wildlife, with
special reference to the European situation. CRC Critical Reviews in Toxicology,
30(1), 71e133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10408440091159176.
Weis, J. (2014). Physiological, developmental and behavioral effects of marine pollu-
tion. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.
Weis, J., Smith, G., Zhou, T., Santiago-Bass, C., & Weis, P. (2001). Effects of con-
taminants on behavior: Biochemical mechanisms and ecological consequences.
Bioscience, 51(3), 209e217.
White, G. E., & Brown, C. (2013). Site delity and homing behaviour in intertidal
shes. Marine Biology, 160(6), 1365e1372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00227-
013-2188-6.
White, G. E., & Brown, C. (2014). A comparison of spatial learning and memory
capabilities in intertidal gobies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,1e9. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00265-014-1747-2.
White, G. E., & Brown, C. (2015). Microhabitat use affects goby (Gobiidae) cue choice
in spatial learning task. Journal of Fish Biology, 86(4), 1305e1318. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/jfb.12638.
Wolf, M., & Weissing, F. J. (2012). Animal personalities: Consequences for ecology
and evolution. Trends in Ecology &Evolution, 27(8), 452e461. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.001.
Wong, B. B., & Candolin, U. (2015). Behavioral responses to changing environments.
Behavioral Ecology, 26(3), 665e673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru183.
Wright, S. L., Rowe, D., Thompson, R. C., & Galloway, T. S. (2013). Microplastic
ingestion decreases energy reserves in marine worms. Current Biology, 23(23),
R1031eR1033. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.068.
L. Tosetto et al. / Animal Behaviour 123 (2017) 159e167 167
... However, evidence for personality-dependent contaminant exposure remains limited, and contamination does not always cause behavioural effects that could increase vulnerability to predators. For example, in gobies (Bathygobius krefftii), trophic transfer of contaminants from microplastics had no effect on activity and exploration behaviour [44]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Animal personality differences may have evolved as alternative strategies for negotiating multiple stressor landscapes. Indeed, ecologists are increasingly recognizing that interactions among multiple stressors can transform selective landscapes and behavioural and physiological responses to stress regimes. Yet, evaluating this hypothesis poses challenges, as most studies involving relationships between personality variation and the environment consider single stressors. Here, we review the literature to explore the theory and evidence that multiple stressor environments may mediate personality variation. We consider effects on evolution of personality variation, as influenced by life-history, energetic and behavioural trade-offs, and effects on phenotypic expression of personality traits. We then explore how personality variation may modulate behavioural and physiological responses to multiple stressors, and how differential responses may be affected by personality-dependent movement ecology and cognitive strategies. Among-individual differences in responses to multiple stressors are critical to elucidate, as multi-stress interactions may transform animal behavioural and physiological responses relative to those predicted under single stressor scenarios, and because among-individual variation comprises the basis for evolutionary shifts in stress responsiveness and population resiliency to global environmental change.
... However, results of experimental studies in this area have not always been consistent. Indeed, a handful of laboratory studies with various species have reported no significant adverse effects of MNPs (e.g., Oliveira et al., 2013;Tosetto et al., 2017;Assas et al., 2020). Moreover, the limited number of studies conducted on wild animals with long-term exposure to MNPs in their natural habitats most of them with fish, have documented disparate findings; some have found adverse effects related to MP contamination (e.g., Barboza et al., 2020;Chenet et al., 2021;Cocci et al., 2022;Buwono et al., 2022;Barboza et al., 2023b), while others have reported no such relationship (e.g., da Silva et al., 2022;Mancuso et al., 2022;Patsiou et al., 2024). ...
Article
Global environmental contamination by microplastics (MPs) is a growing problem with potential One Health impacts. The presence of MPs in vital organs, such as the heart, is of particular concern, but the knowledge is still limited. The goal of the present pilot study was to investigate the potential presence of MPs in the heart of wild specimens of three commercial fish species (Merluccius merluccius, Sardina pilchardus, and Trisopterus luscus) from the North East Atlantic Ocean. Heart samples from 154 fish were analysed for MP content (one heart sample per fish). A total of 44 MPs were recovered from heart samples from the three species. MPs had varied chemical composition (5 polymers), shapes (4) and colours (5). Differences in the profile of the MPs among species was observed (p ≤ 0.05). Thirty fish (19%) had MPs in their hearts, with a total mean (±SD) concentration of 0.286 ± 0.644 MPs/fish. S. pilchardus had the highest heart contamination (p ≤ 0.05). There were no significant (p > 0.05) differences between M. merluccius and T. luscus. These findings in fish with different biological and ecological traits together with literature data suggest that heart contamination likely is a disseminated phenomenon. Therefore, further research on the presence of MPs in the cardiovascular system and its potential health effects is very much needed.
... We inspected videos from both angles of all fish feeding trials (n = 224) to document several behaviors. We used lunges as a proxy for feeding behavior [61,62] and swimming activity in response to a predator for the likelihood of predation. The number of lunges and the time code of each lunge was recorded. ...
Article
Full-text available
Plastics have become a pervasive global contaminant since the mid-20th century, causing harm to organisms at all levels. Preventative measures to reduce plastic pollution and awareness-raising campaigns about the damaging effects of plastic debris on the environment and its inhabitants are crucial; however, most plastic assessments focus on singular trophic levels. Microplastics, tiny plastic particles ranging from 25 μm to 5 mm, have emerged as a widespread form of pollution found in ecosystems worldwide. They can enter the environment directly or through the breakdown of larger plastic debris and are thought to be mistaken for food by foraging animals. This leads to microplastics circulating through ecosystems via direct and indirect consumption, ultimately impacting even higher-order predators. Here, we assess the impacts of microplastics on Chlorophyll a concentrations, algal community structure, copepod survivorship, and fish behavior in experimental trials, in addition to simulated top-predator foraging success on plastic-exposed fish. Our results indicate that microplastics have detrimental effects on algal growth and copepod survival. We also observed the trophic transmission of small plastic spheres from copepods to fish predators, highlighting a concerning pathway for microplastic pollution within aquatic ecosystems, where fish consumed plastics through direct and indirect means. Primary consumers, like copepods, face dual pressures from top-down forcing, as they are preferred over plastic particles as food sources, and bottom-up resource depletion, as algal food supplies can be limited by microplastic exposure. Our findings demonstrate the system-wide impacts that can occur when microplastics are included in food chains and underscore the urgent need for comprehensive strategies to mitigate the entry of plastic debris into aquatic ecosystems.
... The detection of MPs in the excreta of periwinkles implies a rapid expulsion rather than accumulation within the organism (Gutow et al., 2016). Additionally, the study highlights that Bathygobius krefftii, a type of ray-finned fish, consumed beach hoppers Platorchestia smithi, exposed to polyethylene micro-plastics (PE-MPs) of 38-45 µm for 72 hours, without showing any adverse effects on their behavioral traits (Tosetto et al., 2017). On another note, the benthic filter feeder, Cerastoderma edule, exhibited a 30% reduction in predation rate on the deposit feeder Limecola balthica, which had been exposed to PS-MPs of 4.8 µm for 40 min, a phenomenon primarily linked to the altered swimming behavior of zooplankton post-exposure, reducing their susceptibility to being filtered (Van Colen et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The increase of micro-and nano-plastics (MNPs) in aquatic environments has become a significant concern due to their potential toxicological effects on ecosystems, food web dynamics, and human health. These plastic particles emerge from a range of sources, such as the breakdown of larger plastic waste, consumer products, and industrial outputs. This review provides a detailed report of the transmission and dangers of MNPs in aquatic ecosystems, environmental behavior, and interactions within aquatic food webs, emphasizing their toxic impact on marine life. It explores the relationship between particle size and toxicity, their distribution in different tissues , and the process of trophic transfer through the food web. MNPs, once consumed, can be found in various organs, including the digestive system, gills, and liver. Their consumption by lower trophic level organisms facilitates their progression up the food chain, potentially leading to bioaccumulation and biomagnification, thereby posing substantial risks to the health, reproduction, and behavior of aquatic species. This work also explores how MNPs, through their persistence and bioaccumulation, pose risks to aquatic biodiversity and disrupt trophic relationships. The review also addresses the implications of MNPs for human health, particularly through the consumption of contaminated seafood, highlighting the direct and indirect pathways through which humans are exposed to these pollutants. Furthermore, the review highlights the recommendations for future research directions, emphasizing the integration of ecological, toxicological, and human health studies to inform risk assessments and develop mitigation strategies to address the global challenge of plastic pollution in aquatic environments.
... They can act as a source or transfer vector of pollutants with a high potential to adsorb and leach toxic chemicals [such as hydrophobic organic compounds (HOCs) and potentially toxic elements (PTEs)] from aqueous environments to living tissues (Verla et al. 2019). MPs-contaminated diets can also impose numerous behavioral, functional and body development impairments (Tosetto et al. 2017;da Costa Araújo and Malafaia 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Microplastics (MPs) are an emerging pollutant whose ability to adsorb potentially toxic elements (PTEs) poses a serious threat to aquatic ecosystems, including rivers. In highly developed basins, the abundance of MPs in river sediment is expected to be high, elevating the sedimentary accumulation of PTEs. This hypothesis was tested in the Zayandeh-Rood River, Central Iran, with 21 sediment sampling stations distributed along the entire river stretch. Results of sediment analysis showed significant variations in the abundance and size of MPs, with concentrations ranked as Ba (270.71 mg/kg) > Li (21.29 mg/kg) > Cs (2.50 mg/kg) > Be (1.44 mg/kg) > Sn (1.17 mg/kg) > Mo (1.06 mg/kg) > Ag (0.76 mg/kg), along with sediment physicochemical attributes such as EC, TOC, pH and grain size. MPs were identified in all sediment samples with a mean of 588 items/kg dry weight. Except for Ag, all other PTEs were classified as uncontaminated but exhibited increased enrichment downstream. According to the results of the generalized additive model (maximum R-sq of 0.766), the sedimentary concentration of the majority of PTEs is nonlinearly and positively associated with smaller and more abundant MPs. This study acknowledges that MPs might influence sediment porosity, permeability and structure, thereby directly affecting the settling dynamics of other particles, especially PTEs. Graphical Abstract
... The trophic transfer of MPs is already evident in many experimental and natural ecosystems [26,30,[98][99][100]. Although studies on the trophic transfer of MPs mainly focused on arthropods [30,99,100], an experimental study reported the transfer of MPs from tadpoles and fishes to mammals [26]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Given their pervasiveness in the environment, particularly in aquatic ecosystems, plastics are posing a growing concern worldwide. Many vertebrates and invertebrates in marine, freshwater, and terrestrial ecosystems exhibit microplastic (MP) uptake and accumulation. Some studies have indicated the fatal impacts of MPs on animals and their possible transfer through food chains. Thus, it is crucial to study MP pollution and its impacts on environment-sensitive and globally threatened animal groups, such as amphibians, which also play an important role in the energy transfer between ecosystems. Unfortunately, research in this field is lacking and sources of organized information are also scarce. Hence, we systematically reviewed published literature on MPs in amphibians to fill the existing knowledge gap. Our review revealed that most of the previous studies have focused on MP bioaccumulation in amphibians, whereas, only a few research highlighted its impacts. We found that more than 80% of the studied species exhibited MP accumulation. MPs were reported to persist in different organs for a long time and get transferred to other trophic levels. They can also exhibit cytotoxic and mutagenic effects and may have fatal impacts. Moreover, they can increase the disease susceptibility of amphibians. Our study concludes the MPs as a potential threat to amphibians and urges increasing the scope and frequency of research on MP pollution and its impacts on this vulnerable animal group. We also provide a generalized method for studying MPs in amphibians with future perspectives and research directions. Our study is significant for extending the knowledge of MPs and their impacts on amphibians and guiding prospective research.
Chapter
This handbook lays out the science behind how animals think, remember, create, calculate, and remember. It provides concise overviews on major areas of study such as animal communication and language, memory and recall, social cognition, social learning and teaching, numerical and quantitative abilities, as well as innovation and problem solving. The chapters also explore more nuanced topics in greater detail, showing how the research was conducted and how it can be used for further study. The authors range from academics working in renowned university departments to those from research institutions and practitioners in zoos. The volume encompasses a wide variety of species, ensuring the breadth of the field is explored.
Article
Full-text available
Microplastics, which have a diameter of less than 5 mm, are becoming an increasingly prevalent contaminant in terrestrial and aquatic ecosystems due to the dramatic increase in plastic production to 390.7 million tonnes in 2021. Among all the plastics produced since 1950, nearly 80% ended up in the environment or landfills and eventually reached the oceans. Currently, 82–358 trillion plastic particles, equivalent to 1.1–4.9 million tonnes by weight, are floating on the ocean's surface. The interactions between microorganisms and microplastics have led to the transportation of other associated pollutants to higher trophic levels of the food chain, where microplastics eventually reach plants, animals, and top predators. This review paper focuses on the interactions and origins of microplastics in diverse environmental compartments that involve terrestrial and aquatic food chains. The present review study also critically discusses the toxicity potential of microplastics in the food chain. This systematic review critically identified 206 publications from 2010 to 2022, specifically reported on microplastic transport and ecotoxicological impact in aquatic and terrestrial food chains. Based on the ScienceDirect database, the total number of studies with “microplastic” as the keyword in their title increased from 75 to 4813 between 2010 and 2022. Furthermore, various contaminants are discussed, including how microplastics act as a vector to reach organisms after ingestion. This review paper would provide useful perspectives in comprehending the possible effects of microplastics and associated contaminants from primary producers to the highest trophic level (i.e. human health).
Article
Full-text available
Microplastics are ubiquitous in the marine environment worldwide, and may cause a physical and chemical risk to marine organisms. Their small size makes them bioavailable to a range of organisms with evidence of ingestion at all levels of the marine ecosystem. Despite an increasing body of research into microplastics, few studies have explored how consumption changes complex behaviours such as predator avoidance and social interactions. Pollutant exposure can result in alterations in behaviour that not only leads to sub optimal conditions for individual organisms but may also serve as a warning sign for wider effects on a system. This research assessed the impacts of microplastics on the ecology of coastal biota using beachhoppers (Platorchestia smithi) as model organisms. We exposed beachhoppers to marine-contaminated microplastics to understand effects on survival and behaviour. Beachhoppers readily ingested microplastics, and there was evidence for accumulation of microplastics within the organisms. Exposure tests showed that microplastic consumption can affect beachhopper survival. Individuals also displayed reduced jump height and an increase in weight, however, there was no significant difference in time taken to relocate shelter post disturbance. Overall, these results show that short-term ingestion of microplastics have an impact on survival and behaviour of P. smithi. A reduction in the capacity for beachhoppers to survive and function may have flow on effects to their local environment and higher trophic levels.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of pharmacological waste on aquatic ecosystems are increasingly highlighted in ecotoxicology research. Many of these products are designed for human physiology but owing to the conservative nature of vertebrate evolution they also tend to have effects on aquatic organisms and fishes in particular when they find their way into aquatic systems via wastewater effluent. One area of research has focused on reproductive control and the associated hormone treatments. Many of these hormones affect the reproductive physiology of fishes and may cause feminization of male reproductive traits. Alternative medicines have also been widely used particularly in traditional cultures but few of these alternative treatments have been assessed with respect to their potential impact on aquatic ecosystems. Rue (Ruta graveolens) has been used as a male contraceptive in traditional medicines but its effects on fish behavior and reproductive anatomy have yet to be established. Here we show that treating Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens, with extract of rue has a significant effect on key aggressive/reproductive behaviors and the propensity to explore novel objects (boldness). In all cases the respective behaviors were reduced relative to controls and sham injected fish. Histological analysis of the testes revealed that rue exposure reduced the number of spermatozoa but increased the number of spermatocytes relative to controls.
Article
Full-text available
The hypothesis that 'microplastic will transfer hazardous hydrophobic organic chemicals (HOC) to marine animals' has been central to the perceived hazard and risk of plastic in the marine environment. The hypothesis is often cited and has gained momentum, turning it into paradigm status. We provide a critical evaluation of the scientific literature regarding this hypothesis. Using new calculations based on published studies, we explain the sometimes contrasting views and unify them in one interpretive framework. One explanation for the contrasting views among studies is that they test different hypotheses. When reframed in the context of the above hypothesis, the available data become consistent. We show that HOC microplastic-water partitioning can be assumed to be at equilibrium for most microplastic residing in the oceans. We calculate the fraction of total HOC sorbed by plastics to be small compared to that sorbed by other media in the ocean. We further demonstrate consistency among (a) measured HOC transfer from microplastic to organisms in the laboratory, (b) measured HOC desorption rates for polymers in artificial gut fluids (c) simulations by plastic-inclusive bioaccumulation models and (d) HOC desorption rates for polymers inferred from first principles. We conclude that overall the flux of HOCs bioaccumulated from natural prey overwhelms the flux from ingested microplastic for most habitats, which implies that microplastic ingestion is not likely to increase the exposure to and thus risks of HOCs in the marine environment.
Article
Marine debris produces a wide variety of negative environmental, economic, safety, health and cultural impacts. Most marine litter has a very low decomposition rate (as plastics, which are the most abundant type of marine debris), leading to a gradual, but significant accumulation in the coastal and marine environment. Along that time, marine debris is a significant source of chemical contaminants to the marine environment. Once extracted from the water, incineration is the method most widely used to treat marine debris. Other treatment methods have been tested, but they still need some improvement and so far have only been used in some countries. Several extraction and collection programs have been carried out. However, as marine debris keep entering the sea, these programs result insufficient and the problem of marine debris will continue its increase. The present work addresses the environmental impact and social aspects of the marine debris, with a review of the state of the art in the treatments of this kind of waste, together with an estimation of the worldwide occurrence and characteristics.
Article
The uptake of microplastic particles and the transfer of potential harmful substances along with microplastics has been studied in a variety of organisms, especially invertebrates. However, the potential accumulation of very small microplastic particles along food webs ending with vertebrate models has not been investigated so far. Therefore, a simple artificial food chain with Artemia spec. nauplii and zebrafish (Danio rerio) was established to analyze the transfer of microplastic particles and associated persistent organic pollutants (POPs) between different trophic levels. Very small (1 - 20 µm) microplastic particles accumulated in Artemia nauplii and were subsequently transferred to fish. Virgin particles not loaded with POPs did not cause any observable physical harm in the intestinal tracts of zebrafish, although part of the particles were retained within the mucus of intestinal villi and might even be taken up by epithelial cells. The transfer of associated POPs was tested with the polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon benzo[a]pyrene and an ethoxyresorufin-O-deethylase (EROD) assay for CYP1A induction in zebrafish liver as well as via fluorescence analyses. Whereas a significant induction in the EROD assay could not be shown due to high individual variation and low sensitivity regarding substance concentration, the fluorescence tracking of benzo[a]pyrene indicates that food-borne microplastic-associated POPs may actually desorb in the intestine of fish and are thus transferred to the intestinal epithelium and liver. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Book
Synthesizing decades of work, but up-to-date, this book focuses on organism-level responses to pollutants by marine animals, mainly crustaceans, molluscs, and fishes. Emphasizing effects on physiological processes (feeding/digestion, respiration, osmoregulation), life-cycle (reproduction [including endocrine disruption], embryo development, larval development, developmental processes later in life (growth, regeneration, molting, calcification, cancer), and behaviour, the book also covers bioaccumulation and detoxification of contaminants, and the development of tolerance. The major pollutants covered are metals, organic compounds (oil, pesticides, industrial chemicals), nutrients and hypoxia, contaminants of emerging concern, and ocean acidification. Some attention is also devoted to marine debris and noise pollution. © 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht. All rights are reserved.