ChapterPDF Available

Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality

Authors:
S
Sexual Orientation and Human
Sexuality
Jaroslava Varella Valentova and Marco Antonio
Correa Varella
Department of Experimental Psychology, Institute
of Psychology, University of São Paulo, Cidade
Universitária São Paulo, SP, Brazil
Synonyms
Bisexuality;Heterosexuality;Homosexuality;
Same-sex sexuality;Sexual attraction;Sexual
minorities;Sexual preference
Definition
Sexual orientation refers to a psychological mech-
anism directing ones sexuality towards individ-
uals based on their apparent sex. Individuals form
a continuum between extreme point of exclusive
heterosexuals, i.e., oriented exclusively towards
individuals of the opposite sex, and exclusive
homosexuals, i.e., oriented exclusively towards
individuals of the same sex, with bisexuals being
similarly attracted to both sexes and other individ-
uals being attracted to both sexes in various
degrees (e.g., predominantly heterosexual with
some potential to same-sex attraction). Sexual
orientation refers more to attraction or desire
than behavior or identity, because orientation is
not necessarily manifested by overt sexual behav-
ior, associated with self-identication of sexual
orientation label, or aligned with affectional bond-
ing (love).
Introduction
The majority of males and females predominantly
prefer opposite-sex sexual and/or romantic part-
ners. Such sexuality has been accepted as a stan-
dard default, possibly because it is easier to see its
biological/evolutionary relevance. Indeed, in sex-
ually reproducing species, preference for and sex-
ual activities with opposite-sex partners evolved
as a mechanism for combining genomes through
complementary gametes gaining genetic variabil-
ity and direct tness. Thus, scholars have argued
that homosexuality is an evolutionary puzzle
because it impedes the reproductive success of
their owners. However, on the phylogenetic
level, evolutionary tness is more than direct t-
ness, and on the ontogenetic level, sexuality is
more than fertile heterosexual penile-vaginal
intercourse. Although direct reproduction is the
ultimate evolutionary force behind most sexual
activities, sexuality in general gained many other
proximate functions during its evolution, e.g., as a
social and pair bonding mechanism or means of
resource acquisition. Such functions are interme-
diate goals to achieve direct reproduction, e.g., by
means of survival and alliance formation and/or
indirect reproduction by helping kin, also known
#Springer International Publishing AG 2016
T.K. Shackelford, V.A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3622-1
as inclusive tness. Therefore, many forms of
sexuality, such as oral or anal sex, masturbation,
sexual preferences for different species, prefer-
ences of same-sex individuals, or individuals out-
side of reproductive age, cannot per se offer direct
reproductive success of the individual, but still
these nonfertile forms of sexuality can offer
other adaptive sociosexual functions (e.g., pair
bonding, alliance formation, resource acquisition,
well-being, etc.) that might indirectly foster future
reproduction, regardless of sexual orientation. It is
important to note that personal motivations for
sexual activities, homosexual or heterosexual
ones, are quite different from their possible socio-
sexual functions or possible evolutionary adaptive
values. Sexual relations are mostly motivated by
pleasure, love, self-esteem/revenge, and/or
obtaining resources. Such personal motivations
triggering homo/heterosexual behavior should
not be confused with the socioecological or evo-
lutionary reasons that maintain sexual activity in
the population.
Further, sexual orientation is a psychological
mechanism that generates a continuous array of
individual variation and not a dichotomous psy-
chological trait (e.g., Kinsey et al. 1948; Petterson
et al. 2015). In this sense, exclusive homosexual-
ity and exclusive heterosexuality present the
extreme opposite poles of the continuum, while
the majority of individuals fall in between,
although closer to the heterosexual extreme. Fol-
lowing this line, homosexuality or nonhetero-
sexuality (including bisexuals) is more than
exclusive homosexuality, and sexual orientation
does not equal homosexuality. One possibility
would be to focus on the adaptive value of the
mechanism itself (i.e., sexual orientation); the
other would be to explore the possible adaptive
value of one of its manifestations (e.g., exclusive
homosexuality). The adaptiveness of the mecha-
nism itself is usually not the focus of the debate.
The debate is mostly centered on whether and
how the individual differences generated by this
mechanism are adaptive. A mistake would be to
extrapolate a possible nonadaptiveness of an
extreme point within the continuum (exclusive
homosexuality) to the other variations along the
whole continuum of sexual orientation (e.g.,
predominantly heterosexual, bisexual, predomi-
nantly homosexual, etc.). Similarly, a severe clin-
ical depression or severe forms of autism are most
probably not adaptive; they are not only neutral
but rather maladaptive. However, they both are
extremes on a continuum of adaptive variation,
such as the capacity of sadness and systemizing
abilities, respectively (for an overview about
adaptive individual differences see, Buss and
Greiling 1999). In the same line, bisexuality or
some degree of homosexual tendencies among
predominantly heterosexual individuals as well
as some heterosexual tendencies among predom-
inantly homosexual individuals could be viewed
as adaptive variation of sexual orientation. From
this perspective, even if exclusive homosexuality
does not have any possible adaptive value, it
would be a mistake to assume that the same is
true for the whole continuum of sexual
orientation.
Evolutionary Theories of Sexual
Orientation
Sexual orientation has been described as a mech-
anism navigating or directing sexuality towards
individuals of the opposite sex, same sex, or in
varying degrees to both sexes (Bailey 2009; Bai-
ley et al. 2016; Savin-Williams 2016; Vrangalova
and Savin-Williams 2012). Similar to some het-
erosexual practices (e.g., extra-pair affairs), atti-
tudes toward same-sex sexuality can vary greatly
between cultures. In some cultures, same-sex sex-
uality constitutes an integral part of the sociocul-
tural system; in others such sexuality is
criminalized and persecuted (for review see, Bai-
ley et al. 2016). For these reasons, individuals can
vary in their overt manifestation of sexual prefer-
ence, from a tendency hidden from public or even
self to open preferences and behaviors. There are
also other motives why sexual preferences, behav-
iors, and identities do not need to be aligned.
Nonpreferred sexual behavior can be manifested,
for example, under conditions without access to
preferred sexual partners; it can be done for nan-
cial motives or simply to satisfy ones partner.
Similarly, romantic affection can be largely
2 Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality
disconnected from sexual desires (Diamond
2003). For example, a person can repeatedly fall
in love only with individuals of the same sex, but
prefer and actively seek sexual partners of both
sexes. Thus, sexual orientation refers to sexual
desires, attraction, and preferences rather than to
overt sexual behavior, identity, or affective
feelings.
Sexual orientation has a genetic component
that explains approximately one third of the vari-
ation between individuals (e.g., Zietsch
et al. 2012; for review, see Bailey et al. 2016).
This suggests that sexual orientation is a complex
trait that is inuenced by genetic and environmen-
tal factors. Sex hormones are one of the environ-
mental factors that inuence the development of
sexual orientation. According to the organiza-
tional hypothesis, the neural system of homosex-
ual individuals is inuenced by sex-atypical
hormone levels during early developmental
phases. Systematic differences in neuroanatomy
between homosexual and heterosexual individ-
uals suggest that compared to their heterosexual
counterparts homosexual men were, on average,
less androgenized and homosexual women more
androgenized during early, probably prenatal,
development (for review, see Bailey et al. 2016;
LeVay 2010; Wilson and Rahman 2005). Neither
genetic nor hormonal inuences suggest that sex-
ual orientation is necessarily xed and rigid over
the lifespan. Sexual orientation develops during
ontogeny whereas sexual identity can change over
time, particularly among women (Diamond
2008). Such a uidity of sexual orientation does
not equal to a conscious choice of different sexual
desires and attractions (Diamond 2008). Thus,
sexual orientation is not a choice nor a cultural
invention; it has genetic and neurohormonal
underpinnings and is not necessarily immutable.
Furthermore, occasional same-sex behavior and
stable same-sex preferences have been
documented along a number of nonhuman animal
species (Sommer and Vasey 2006). Thus, a varia-
tion on the sexual orientation continuum is not a
human specic and has its phylogenetic roots.
Moreover, it is commonly accepted that homo-
sexuals do not have any reproductive success.
Although it has been shown that reproductive
success of transgendered male homosexuals
(males who adopt female look and behavior) is
close to zero (Vasey et al. 2014), this does not
need to apply to other forms of nonheterosexual
individuals. In one study, 32.8 % of men and
65.4 % of women reported a potential for homo-
sexual response, suggesting that a huge percent-
age of predominantly heterosexual people do have
a propensity to experience same-sex sexual attrac-
tion and/or interaction (Santtila et al. 2008). In this
study, the potential for homosexual response was
based on responses (quite impossible, very
unlikely, quite unlikely, not likely, not unlikely,
relatively likely, very likely) on the following
question: If a, in your opinion, handsome man
[to male participants]/beautiful woman [to female
participants], whom you like, suggested sexual
interaction with you, how likely would you be
able to do it (if you could dene the nature of the
interaction and nobody else would know about
it)?. In line with this nding, a large-scale study
demonstrated that almost 10 % of heterosexually
identied men reported that they have engaged in
same-sex sexual activities (Pathela et al. 2006).
This applies even more to non-Western
populations, such as Samoa, where the majority
of predominantly heterosexual men engage in sex
with both women and transgender males called
Faafane (Petterson et al. 2015). Further, 20 % of
homosexually identied white American men
reported having been married to a woman at
some point in their life, and 50 % of homosexual
men reported having produced at least one child
(Bell and Weinberg 1978). According to the more
recent demographic data (2013) of the USA, 37 %
of LGBT-identied individuals report having at
least one child. Keeping in mind these assump-
tions, we can outline some of the empirically
supported evolutionary theories explaining the
adaptiveness of human homosexuality and
nonheterosexuality.
1. Homosexuality as an adaptation: Kin selection
theory
One of the oldest and partly supported theories of
homosexuality as an adaptation was postulated by
E. O. Wilson (1978), inspired by Hamiltons kin
Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality 3
selection theory. Homosexual individuals, who do
not invest into their own exclusive reproduction,
may propagate their genes indirectly by
supporting reproduction of close relatives, with
whom he/she shares own genes. Studies
conducted in the USA, Great Britain, or Japan
did not support the theory (for review, see Bailey
et al. 2016), which can be explained by persisting
relatively negative attitudes toward homosexual
individuals in these countries. On the other hand,
systematic investigations of Faafane, who are
ofcially recognized as third genderor a trans-
gender type of male homosexuals (individuals
who are biologically males but have feminine
characteristics and adopt feminine gender roles)
among the population of Samoa, supported the
theory of kin selection. In particular, Faafane
show higher avuncular tendencies (support of
nieces and nephews) compared to men who prefer
women as sexual partners, which supposedly
increases their tness (e.g., Vasey and
VanderLaan 2010).
Samoan Faafane t a specic subgroup of
homosexual men, the transgender type of male
homosexuals that is a specic although still het-
erogeneous group. This subgroup of transgender
individuals whose sexuality is aimed at individ-
uals of the same biological sex is frequent in many
other societies, such as India (Hijra), Thailand
(Kathoey), or native North American tribes
(Berdache). The transgendered homosexuals
mostly prefer same-sex relationships, although
usually with cisgender (i.e., biological sex aligned
with gender role) men. Sexual and/or romantic
relations between partners of different gender
roles are documented in numerous populations
around the world (Crapo 1995). Furthermore,
age stratied type of same-sex relationships (i.e.,
one of the partners is much younger than the
other) has been documented across populations,
such as ancient Greece, Middle Ages Japan, or
contemporary New Guinea (Crapo 1995). Such
relationships, as opposed to gender-stratied
ones, do not need to imply one transgendered
and one cisgender partner, rather both of them
are cisgender but of different age categories,
although such interactions can be temporary and
do not need to imply predominant same-sex
preferences of none of the partners. Both
age-stratied and gender-stratied styles of
same-sex relationships exist in the modern West-
ern culture, although the most frequent style of
relationships is egalitarian between two individ-
uals who identify as homosexuals and are of sim-
ilar gender role, status, education, and age. This
latter style of same-sex relationships seems to be
specic for contemporary Western populations.
On the other hand, the other two styles of same-
sex relationships appear in populations that share
more common sociocultural characteristics with
human ancestral populations (e.g., geographical
connectedness of the kin and strong familiar
bonds), and the evolutionary strategy can thus
still be adaptive in the present as it could have
been advantageous in the ancestral environment
(VanderLaan et al. 2014). Importantly, evolution-
ary strategy of transgender homosexuals can dif-
fer from the one adopted by cisgender
homosexuals or nonheterosexuals. In other
words, some evolutionary hypotheses (e.g., kin
selection) can be supported in one specic sub-
group of homosexual individuals (e.g., in trans-
gender homosexual men) in specic society
contexts (e.g., large families, close kin bonds,
geographical proximity) but not in others (e.g.,
in cisgender homosexual men in big anonymous
cities with prevalent homophobia).
2. Homosexuality as a by-product of
sex-atypicality
Miller (2000) suggested that male homosexuality
per se is not adaptive, but rather it is a by-product
of another adaptation. He proposed that some
characteristics that are, on average, more typical
for women than men, such as lower aggressive-
ness, higher cooperation, and social skills in gen-
eral, became adaptive also in men during the
changes in social structure of relatively recent
human evolution. Futher, more feminine charac-
teristics in men can be typical for slow life history
of men who invest more in relationship commit-
ment and parental care than in mating success
(Jeffery 2015). Thus, men who have more femi-
nine and fewer masculine characteristics can
thrive better in a more complex and
4 Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality
predominantly monogamous society, including
having relatively higher reproductive success.In
this scheme, male homosexuality would thus be
an extreme nonadaptive by-product of a general
adaptive male shift to feminization. Gender non-
conformity (lower masculinity in men) has a
strong genetic component (for review, see Bailey
et al. 2016), and women nd femininity in men
attractive (Perrett et al. 1998). Furthermore,
numerous studies indicate that nonheterosexual
men are, in comparison to heterosexuals, more
feminine (for review, see Bailey et al. 2016).
Homosexual men would thus form an extreme
point on a continuum between male masculinity
and male femininity, while relatively feminine
heterosexual men would have some adaptive
advantages, at least in urban, middle class Western
groups.In line with the kin selection theory
outlined above, this theory also aims to explain
the existence and maintenance of feminine male
homosexuals in the population. This theory, none-
theless, does not apply to masculine male homo-
sexuals and other nonheterosexuals nor to female
homosexuals. Rice et al. (2013) suggested that
homosexual orientation is a by-product of
sex-atypical epigenetic marks that feminize
males and masculinize females during embryonic
development. Thus, the opposite logic of the the-
ory could also be applied to explain masculine
female homosexuals who would represent an
extreme pole of a general female masculinization.
This might have been adaptive for some women
during the evolutionary past, for example, by con-
centrating more social and physical power, lead-
ing to higher independence, better self-defense
and defense of their kin, and higher resource
acquisition. Such characteristics might have been
advantageous in the ancestral and also recent soci-
ety, where mortality in men has been relatively
higher than in women.
3. Predominant homosexuality as an adaptation:
Sneaking and conditional sexual strategy
Following the previous line of reasoning, predom-
inant homosexuality in feminine males can be also
understood as a sneaking sexual strategy (Jeffery
2015). In humans, it has been suggested that the
most frequent strategy would be exclusive or at
least predominant heterosexuality with males of
rather masculine characteristics, while bisexuality
and predominant homosexuality linked to
increased femininity in men would represent a
sneaking sexual strategy. From this point of
view, feminine men who have some potential for
opposite-sex activities can be successful in oppor-
tunistic heterosexual activities and secure direct
reproductive success. This can happen in particu-
larly because feminine characteristics were shown
to be attractive to women (e.g., Perrett et al. 1998),
at least under certain conditions. At the same time,
more feminine bisexual or predominantly homo-
sexual men can be perceived as weak rivals to
more masculine exclusively or predominantly het-
erosexual men and can thus reduce intrasexual
male-male competition. Consequently, feminine
predominantly homosexual men can be accepted
more as friends of heterosexual mens female
partners, thus offering opportunities to sneak cop-
ulations.High numbers of predominantly homo-
sexual individuals reporting some opposite-sex
attraction, behavior, and also biological offspring
(see above) would tend to support this theory. The
theory thus does not attempt to explain exclusive
homosexuality or transgender homosexual men
who do not have any interest in sexual encounters
with opposite-sex partners. It rather shows that
various degrees of bisexuality and predominant
homosexuality can serve as an alternative and
successful reproductive strategy.This theory need
not apply only to men because bisexual and pre-
dominantly homosexual women can also show
some opposite-sex attraction and behavior, as
well as some reproductive success. On average,
lesbian women are more masculine than hetero-
sexual women (Bailey and Zucker 1995), and
masculine heterosexual women report higher
sociosexuality (tendency for uncommitted sexual
variety) than feminine women (Mikach and Bai-
ley 1999). We can thus speculate that masculine
predominantly homosexual women can secure
direct reproductive success without the necessity
of creating and maintaining long-term bonds with
men but rather with other women.Compared to
predominant female homosexuality or bisexual-
ity, a uid sexual orientation might have helped
Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality 5
ancestral women secure resources and care for
their offspring under conditions without a primary
male partner by receiving parental investment
from other women (Kuhle and Radtke 2013).
According to this theory, uidity of female sexual
orientation can be understood as a conditional
sexual strategy, meaning that variations between
opposite-sex and same-sex sexuality can be adap-
tive in different conditions, such as different age
or life phases.
4. Nonheterosexuality as an adaptation: Same-
sex alliance formation
This theory holds that various degrees of sexual
feelings and behaviors aimed toward both males
and females offer an individual advantage over an
exclusive orientation toward only one sex
(Kirkpatrick 2000). The main reason towards
nonheterosexual interactions would be formation
and maintenance of same-sex alliances that are
crucial in many other primates and in particular
in the human evolutionary past. The author shows
anthropological and primatological evidence
supporting the view that strong and emotional
same-sex alliances can offer a reproductive
advantage to the individual. More specically,
same-sex alliances reduce competition and
increase cooperation between members of the
same sex, while maintained access to partners of
the opposite sex ensures potential reproductive
success. In this perspective, heterosexual sex
among stable partners outside the fertile period
(i.e., outside the fertile window within the female
menstrual cycle and after menopause) or any sex-
ual activities that do not lead to fertilization (e.g.,
oral or anal sex) serve the same function, i.e.,
reducing disputes, increasing cooperation, and
thus maintaining the stable dyad (Bártová and
Valentová 2012).The question is whether
nonheterosexual behavior was originally selected
for alliance formation or whether alliance forma-
tion is rather a newly acquired sociosexual func-
tion of same-sex sexual behavior. In this sense,
nonheterosexuality would serve proximate socio-
sexual functions for which nonprocreative sexu-
ality in general was co-opted, such as alliance
formation, dyad maintenance, lowering stress,
peacemaking and reconciliation, or resource
gains.
5. Homosexuality as a by-product: Overdomi-
nance and sexually antagonistic gene
hypothesis
Both behavioral and molecular genetic studies
have shown that the male homosexuality is linked,
among others, to the X chromosome and is thus
inherited from the maternal lineage (Sanders
et al. 2014). Further, female relatives of male
homosexuals have more children and
grandchildren than control group of women with
no homosexual individuals among relatives (e.g.,
Ciani and Pellizzari 2012; for review, see Bailey
et al. 2016). It is thus suggested that greater fecun-
dity and fertility of female relatives of homosex-
ual men can explain the persistence of a
nonadaptive form of male sexuality through sex-
ually antagonistic selection (e.g., Ciani and
Pellizzari 2012; Zietsch et al. 2008). In other
words, sexually antagonistic genes can bring
advantages for one sex (e.g., higher fecundity in
women) but can be principally disadvantageous
for the other sex (e.g., can cause exclusive homo-
sexuality in men).However, as shown by recent
models, the frequency of only female carriers
cannot explain a stable proportion of exclusive
male homosexuals in the population but a large
proportion of both nonhomosexual female and
male carriers can (Chaladze 2016). Based on
behavioral genetic studies, a considerable propor-
tion of men carrying the homosexual genes are not
exclusively or predominantly homosexual. It was
suggested that homosexual phenotype can only be
expressed in a homozygous form of the underly-
ing alleles, while individuals with a heterozygous
form (only one allele of the gene) carry the phe-
notype but the phenotype is not expressed (for
review, see McKnight 1997). Such heterozygous
carriers are supposed to possess some advanta-
geous characteristics that can increase reproduc-
tive success of their owners.So far, this theory was
supported by a study showing that heterosexual
twins of homosexual men, who most probably
share the genetic component of homosexuality
although not homosexual orientation, report
6 Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality
higher number of female sexual partners than
heterosexual men with a heterosexual twin
(Zietsch et al. 2008). Thus, heterosexual men
who carry the homosexuality-linked genetic com-
ponent, potentially in a heterozygous form, do
have higher mating success (considered a proxy
of a reproductive success) than the control group
of heterosexual men, which can explain its rela-
tively stable prevalence in the population.
The theories of by-product thus focus more on
explanation of the extreme exclusive form of male
homosexuality, rather than the rest of the
nonheterosexual continuum. The theories can
apply to other forms of nonheterosexuals, though,
because they can also be carriers of the homosex-
ual genotype. It has been shown that a potential
for homosexual response has a stronger genetic
component than homosexual behavior or self-
labeled identication (Santtila et al. 2008). Thus,
any degree of nonheterosexual tendencies might
be more suitable for genetic analyses than exclu-
sive homosexuality with no heterosexual
potential.
Furthermore, the theories of homosexuality as
a by-product originally did not apply to women,
although in principle they can work similarly,
since genetic component has been documented
also in female sexual orientation (for review, see
Bailey et al. 2016), and the potential to homosex-
ual response is even more frequent in women than
men (Santtila et al. 2008). Thus, exclusive male
and female homosexuality indeed can be a
by-product of otherwise adaptive variation of sex-
ual orientation.
Conclusion
During the last century, human and other animal
(vertebrates and invertebrates) sexual orientation
has been extensively studied within many scien-
tic areas. Despite important ndings in the areas
of psychology, anthropology, medicine, neurosci-
ence, genetics, endocrinology, and development,
evolutionary reasoning has suffered numerous
conceptual and terminology issues. These mis-
conceptions have led to the view that homosexu-
ality can only be exclusive and thus an
evolutionary paradox. However, when essentialist
categorical thinking is discarded, it might become
clearer that the majority of variation on the con-
tinuum of sexual orientation can offer adaptive
advantages for their carriers. This view can be
supported by changing the perspective of sexual-
ity equaling only with reproduction, and by
acknowledging the fact that at least some homo-
sexual and nonheterosexual individuals within the
whole spectrum of sexual orientation do repro-
duce and raise their offspring.
Most outlined theories present adaptive rea-
sons for the evolution of nonheterosexual orienta-
tions, either by stressing indirect reproduction via
kin selection or direct reproduction via sneak cop-
ulations or same-sex alliance formation that can
increase survival and future direct reproductive
capacity. Even by-product theories offer plausible
evolutionary reasoning for the origins and main-
tenance of nonheterosexual orientations. In this
sense, nonheterosexual orientations would have
been passed on throughout generations together
with the adaptive trait of sex-atypicality, advan-
tages of an increased fertility in the other-sex kin,
or in carriers who do not express the homosexual
phenotype. These theories are not mutually exclu-
sive, and together they can explain a bigger pro-
portion of the sexual orientation continuum.
Moreover, these theories offer a stronger case
against the argument that it is impossible for any
form of homosexuality to have evolved.
Finally, it is important to realize that the
outlined theories are not just-so-stories nor naïve
panadaptationism as critics often portrait it. The
theories offer specic hypotheses with predictions
that can be empirically tested. We attempted to
provide the existing empirical support for each
theory, although we realize that more research
using a variety of sources of evidence and
methods is still needed. Only convergent evidence
of numerous empirical tests can give decisive
support and scientic credibility to a particular
adaptive hypothesis.
Sexual orientation is a complex psychological
mechanism that can prot from analysis within an
evolutionary framework, where otherwise hetero-
sexuality might be taken for granted. Future stud-
ies should pay more attention to female
Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality 7
nonheterosexuality, to sex-typical nonheter-
osexuals, bisexuals, integration and empirical
confrontation of the existing theories, and to
empirical testing of the theories in Western as
well as non-Western populations with varying
concepts of same-sex sexuality.
Cross-References
Homosexuality
Kin Selection
Mate Preferences
Sexual Strategies
References
Bailey, J. M. (2009). What is sexual orientation and do
women have one? In D. A. Hope (Ed.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation: Vol. 54. Contemporary per-
spectives on lesbian, gay, and bisexual identities
(pp. 4363). New York: Springer.
Bailey, J. M., Vasey, P. L., Diamond, L. M., Breedlove,
S. M., Vilain, E., & Epprecht, M. (2016). Sexual ori-
entation, controversy, and science. Psychological sci-
ence in the public interest: A journal of the American
Psychological Society, 17(2), 45.
Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed
behavior and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis
and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology,
31(1), 43.
Bártová, K., & Valentová, J. (2012). Evolutionary perspec-
tive of same-sex sexuality: Homosexuality and homo-
sociality revisited. Anthropologie, 50(1), 61.
Bell, A. P., & Weinberg, M. S. (1978). Homosexualities:
A study of diversity among men and women. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual
differences. Journal of Personality, 67(2), 209243.
Chaladze, G. (2016). Heterosexual male carriers could
explain persistence of homosexuality in men:
Individual-based simulations of an X-linked inheri-
tance model. Archives of Sexual Behavior.
doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0742-2.
Ciani, A. C., & Pellizzari, E. (2012). Fecundity of paternal
and maternal non-parental female relatives of homo-
sexual and heterosexual men. PloS one, 7(12), e51088.
Crapo, R. H. (1995). Factors in the cross-cultural pattern-
ing of male homosexuality: A reappraisal of the litera-
ture. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 178202.
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation ori-
ent? A biobehavioral model distinguishing romantic love
and sexual desire. Psychological review, 110(1), 173.
Diamond, L. M. (2008). Sexual uidity: Understanding
womens love and desire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Jeffery, A. J. (2015). Two behavioral hypotheses for the
evolution of male homosexuality in humans. In The
evolution of sexuality (pp. 207219). Cham, CH:
Springer International Publishing.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948).
Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia:
W. B. Saunders.
Kirkpatrick, R. C. (2000). The evolution of human
homosexual behavior. Current anthropology, 41(3),
385413.
Kuhle, B. X., & Radtke, S. (2013). Born both ways:
The alloparenting hypothesis for sexual uidity in
women. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(2). doi:1474704
91301100202.
LeVay, S. (2010). Gay, straight, and the reason why: The
science of sexual orientation. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
McKnight, J. (1997). Straight science?: Homosexuality,
evolution and adaptation. Routledge, London.
Mikach, S. M., & Bailey, J. M. (1999). What distinguishes
women with unusually high numbers of sex partners?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(3), 141150.
Miller, E. M. (2000). Homosexuality, birth order, and evo-
lution: Toward an equilibrium reproductive economics
of homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29(1),
134.
Pathela, P., Hajat, A., Schillinger, J., Blank, S., Sell, R., &
Mostashari, F. (2006). Discordance between sexual
behavior and self-reported sexual identity: A -
population-based survey of New York City men.
Annals of Internal Medicine, 145(6), 416425.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D.,
Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of
sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature,
394(6696), 884887.
Petterson, L. J., Dixson, B. J., Little, A. C., & Vasey, P. L.
(2015). Viewing time measures of sexual orientation in
Samoan cisgender men who engage in sexual interac-
tions with Faafane. PloS one, 10(2), e0116529.
Rice, W. R., Friberg, U., & Gavrilets, S. (2013). Homosex-
uality via canalized sexual development: A testing pro-
tocol for a new epigenetic model. Bioessays, 35(9),
764770.
Sanders, A. R., Martin, E. R., Beecham, G. W., Guo, S.,
Dawood, K., Rieger, G., et al. (2014). Genome-wide
scan demonstrates signicant linkage for male sexual
orientation. Psychological Medicine, 45, 13791388.
Santtila, P., Sandnabba, N. K., Harlaar, N., Varjonen, M.,
Alanko, K., & von der Pahlen, B. (2008). Potential for
homosexual response is prevalent and
genetic. Biological Psychology, 77(1), 102105.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2016). Sexual orientation: Catego-
ries or continuum? commentary In Bailey et al. (Eds),
Psychological science in the public interest: A Journal
of the American Psychological Society,17(2), 37.
8 Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality
Sommer, V., & Vasey, P. L. (2006). Homosexual behaviour
in animals: An evolutionary perspective. New York,
NY: Cambridge University Press.
VanderLaan, D. P., Ren, Z., & Vasey, P. L. (2014). Male
androphilia in the ancestral environment: An ethnolog-
ical analysis. Human Nature, 24, 375401.
Vasey, P. L., Parker, J. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2014).
Comparative reproductive output of androphilic and
gynephilic males in Samoa. Archives of Sexual Behav-
ior, 43(2), 363367.
Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010). Avuncular ten-
dencies and the evolution of male androphilia in
Samoan Faafane. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
39(4), 821830.
Vrangalova, Z., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (2012). Mostly
heterosexual and mostly gay/lesbian: Evidence for new
sexual orientation identities. Archives of sexual behav-
ior, 41(1), 85101.
Wilson, G. D., & Rahman, Q. (2005). Born gay? The
psychobiology of sex orientation. London: Peter Owen.
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Chicago: Harvard
University Press.
Zietsch, B. P., Morley, K. I., Shekar, S. N., Verweij, K. J.,
Keller, M. C., Macgregor, S., et al. (2008). Genetic
factors predisposing to homosexuality may increase
mating success in heterosexuals. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 29(6), 424433.
Zietsch, B. P., Verweij, K. J., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A.,
Martin, N. G., Nelson, E. C., & Lynskey, M. T. (2012).
Do shared etiological factors contribute to the relation-
ship between sexual orientation and depression? Psy-
chological Medicine, 42, 521532.
Sexual Orientation and Human Sexuality 9
... Lesbian and bisexual women show preferences for feminized stimuli including voice pitch than masculinized voices, which mirror preferences of heterosexual men (Zhang, 2022). These findings may inform future studies designed to test evolutionary (Varella Valentova & Varella, 2016) and developmental hypotheses for nonheterosexual mating. ...
... As has already been stated elsewhere, "the psychoneuroendocrine theories based on the concept of psychosexual differentiation, if they are validated at all, may apply only to the two thirds of homosexuals who have a history of atypical gender-role behavior" (Meyer-Bahlburg, 1993). Future investigations should also take into account possible evolutionary mechanisms which gave rise to same-sex attraction (Valentova & Varella, 2016b). Under the term same-sex attraction/ behavior, homosexuality, or nonheterosexuality can be hidden different phenomena with different biodevelopmental pathways. ...
Article
Full-text available
Male sexual functioning is a prerequisite for conception and consequently for reproduction and is thus a crucial mechanism from an evolutionary and social perspective. Previous studies reported better sexual functioning in coupled compared to single individuals. However, it is not clear whether sexual functioning increases or decreases with a short-term casual sexual strategy, which is another possibility, along with a long-term strategy. Furthermore, sexual orientation may represent yet another mating strategy that may influence sexual functioning. Here, we aimed to test the possible associations between male sexual functioning and sexual strategies, as measured through sociosexuality, relationship status, and sexual orientation. We hypothesize that due to its relationship with the evolved motivation of mate acquisition and mate retention, both stable relationships and sociosexuality would be positively associated with male sexual functioning. We did not expect significant differences in sexual functioning among men with different sexual orientations. We sampled 427 healthy men of different sexual orientations (203 heterosexuals, 77 bisexuals, and 147 gay men) who completed the Male Sexual Function Index (MSFI) and its subscales of desire, arousal, erection, orgasm, and satisfaction; the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised; and sociodemographic questions. A multivariate general linear model showed that, compared to single individuals, partnered individuals reported greater MSFI-arousal, MSFI-erection, MSFI-orgasm, and MSFI-satisfaction. Sociosexuality and sexual orientation only weakly predicted MSFI domains. Male sexual functioning thus seems to be moderately associated with a long-term mating strategy and can thus serve for relationship maintenance rather than for acquiring new partners.
Chapter
The chapter explains how the concepts of physical and sexual attraction differ from each other. The materials of the chapter describe the ideas and research on beauty and physical attractiveness. The chapter demonstrates how attractive physical appearance influences overall interpersonal attraction. Evolutionary, ecological, social, cultural, and psychological perspectives show that the values and notions of physical attractiveness depend on many of these contextual factors. The studies reviewed in the chapter have demonstrated variability in preferences for physical appearances across cultures. The chapter presents research findings that reveal the effects of familiarity, imprinting, and exposure on the impressions of how physically attractive a person looks. The features of physical attractiveness are sexually polymorphic and differ between genders. The sections of this chapter provide an overview of the qualities of physical appearance that make a person attractive, as well as multisensory qualities of attractive appearance, including visual, auditory, tactile-kinesthetic, olfactory, and gustatory modalities of sensation and perception. The chapter describes in detail how people experience and express physical and sexual attraction. The last section of the chapter presents varieties of sexual attraction in love that depend on sexual identities, sexual orientations, and factors influencing diversity in sexual attraction.
Chapter
Evolutionary social science is having a renaissance. This volume showcases the empirical and theoretical advancements produced by the evolutionary study of romantic relationships. The editors assembled an international collection of contributors to trace how evolved psychological mechanisms shape strategic computation and behavior across the life span of a romantic partnership. Each chapter provides an overview of historic and contemporary research on the psychological mechanisms and processes underlying the initiation, maintenance, and dissolution of romantic relationships. Contributors discuss popular and cutting-edge methods for data analysis and theory development, critically analyze the state of evolutionary relationship science, and provide discerning recommendations for future research. The handbook integrates a broad range of topics (e.g., partner preference and selection, competition and conflict, jealousy and mate guarding, parenting, partner loss and divorce, and post-relationship affiliation) that are discussed alongside major sources of strategic variation in mating behavior, such as sex and gender diversity, developmental life history, neuroendocrine processes, technological advancement, and culture. Its content promises to enrich students’ and established researchers’ views on the current state of the discipline and should challenge a diverse cross-section of relationship scholars and clinicians to incorporate evolutionary theorizing into their professional work.
Chapter
The Oxford Handbook of Human Mating covers the contributions and up-to-date theories and empirical evidence from scientists regarding human mating strategies. The scientific studies of human mating have only recently risen, revealing fresh discoveries about mate attraction, mate choice, marital satisfaction, and other topics. Darwin’s sexual selection theory primarily guides most of the research in the scientific study of mating strategies. Indeed, research on the complexities of human mate competition and mate choice has centred around Darwin’s classic book. This book discusses theories of human mating; mate selection and mate attraction; mate competition; sexual conflict in mating; human pair bonding; the endocrinology of mating; and mating in the modern world.
Article
Compared to the body of literature on male homosexuality, the continuum of bisexual orientations between the exclusively homosexual and heterosexual poles has been largely overlooked in the scientific and evolutionary literature. Possibly, male bisexuality is not as hard a puzzle to evolutionary thinking because it does not reduce individual direct reproductive success as much as exclusive male homosexuality. Or, bisexual men are expected to fall in between the exclusive poles of sexual orientation, and they thus would not differ from them in the studied characteristics. Moreover, the existence of bisexual men has sometimes been doubted or denied in scientific and lay literature. Despite recent Western biphobia (and homophobia) aimed specifically at men, we show that different forms of male sexuality aimed at both men and women are common among different human populations and non-human species, making it a viable candidate for evolutionary analysis. We first outline the concept and measurements of male bisexuality, its prevalence, and after reviewing the proximate socio-biological factors associated with male bisexuality, we outline evolutionary hypotheses on male bisexuality. We show that several hypotheses originally designed to explain exclusive homosexuality apply also to bisexuality, although most of them deal with the more feminine form of male non-heterosexuals. Finally, we outline the importance of studies on bisexuality for evolutionary psychological science.
Preprint
Human sexual orientation is an intriguing phenomenon which is still poorly understood and has important evolutionary implications. Evolutionary based studies mostly focus on heterosexual individuals and relationships, probably because non-heterosexuality concerns a minority of the population and decreases individual direct reproductive success. To better understand human nature, it is important to analyse whether the mating psychology of minorities exhibit specific evolved sexual/reproductive strategies. Here we review studies on partner preferences, mate choice, and flirting in non-heterosexual populations, to identify which patterns are similar to or different from heterosexuals. The general pattern supports the notion that sex differences are larger than within sex variation among people of different sexual orientations. However, although some mating strategies among non-heterosexuals resemble heterosexuals of the same sex, others resemble heterosexuals of the opposite sex, and yet in others, the pattern is different than among either heterosexual men or women. We point to limitations of the current state of this research, and we suggest possible future directions in the study of non-heterosexual relationship initiation.
Preprint
Human sexual orientation is an intriguing phenomenon which is still poorly understood and has important evolutionary implications. Evolutionary based studies mostly focus on heterosexual individuals and relationships, probably because non-heterosexuality concerns a minority of the population and decreases individual direct reproductive success. To better understand human nature, it is important to analyse whether the mating psychology of minorities exhibit specific evolved sexual/reproductive strategies. Here we review studies on partner preferences, mate choice, and flirting in non-heterosexual populations, to identify which patterns are similar to or different from heterosexuals. The general pattern supports the notion that sex differences are larger than within sex variation among people of different sexual orientations. However, although some mating strategies among non-heterosexuals resemble heterosexuals of the same sex, others resemble heterosexuals of the opposite sex, and yet in others, the pattern is different than among either heterosexual men or women. We point to limitations of the current state of this research, and we suggest possible future directions in the study of non-heterosexual relationship initiation.
Article
Full-text available
Although it is typically presumed that heterosexual individuals only fall in love with other-gender partners and gay-lesbian individuals only fall in love with same-gender partners, this is not always so. The author develops a biobehavioral model of love and desire to explain why. The model specifies that (a) the evolved processes underlying sexual desire and affectional bonding are functionally independent; (b) the processes underlying affectional bonding are not intrinsically oriented toward other-gender or same-gender partners; (c) the biobehavioral links between love and desire are bidirectional, particularly among women. These claims are supported by social-psychological, historical, and cross-cultural research on human love and sexuality as well as by evidence regarding the evolved biobehavioral mechanisms underlying mammalian mating and social bonding.
Article
Full-text available
Ongoing political controversies around the world exemplify a long-standing and widespread preoccupation with the acceptability of homosexuality. Nonheterosexual people have seen dramatic surges both in their rights and in positive public opinion in many Western countries. In contrast, in much of Africa, the Middle East, the Caribbean, Oceania, and parts of Asia, homosexual behavior remains illegal and severely punishable, with some countries retaining the death penalty for it. Political controversies about sexual orientation have often overlapped with scientific controversies. That is, participants on both sides of the sociopolitical debates have tended to believe that scientific findings—and scientific truths—about sexual orientation matter a great deal in making political decisions. The most contentious scientific issues have concerned the causes of sexual orientation—that is, why are some people heterosexual, others bisexual, and others homosexual? The actual relevance of these issues to social, political, and ethical decisions is often poorly justified, however.
Article
Full-text available
Homosexuality has been documented throughout history and is found in almost all human cultures. Twin studies suggest that homosexuality is to some extent heritable. However, from an evolutionary perspective, this poses a problem: Male homosexuals tend to have on average five times fewer children than heterosexual males, so how can a phenomenon associated with low reproductive success be maintained at relatively stable frequencies? Recent findings of increased maternal fecundity of male homosexuals suggest that the genes responsible for homosexuality in males increase fecundity in the females who carry them. Can an increase in maternal fecundity compensate for the fecundity reduction in homosexual men and produce a stable polymorphism? In the current study, this problem was addressed with an individual-based modeling (IBM) approach. IBM suggests that male homosexuality can be maintained in a population at low and stable frequencies if roughly more than half of the females and half of the males are carriers of genes that predispose the male to homosexuality.
Article
Full-text available
Androphilia refers to attraction to adult males, whereas gynephilia refers to attraction to adult females. The current study employed self-report and viewing time (response time latency) measures of sexual attraction to determine the sexual orientation of Samoan cisgender men (i.e., males whose gender presentation and identity is concordant with their biological sex) who engage in sexual interactions with transgender male androphiles (known locally as fa’afafine) compared to: (1) Samoan cisgender men who only engage in sexual interactions with women, and (2) fa’afafine. As expected, both measures indicated that cisgender men who only engaged in sexual interactions with women exhibited a gynephilic pattern of sexual attraction, whereas fa’afafine exhibited an androphilic one. In contrast, both measures indicated that cisgender men who engaged in sexual interactions with fa’afafine demonstrated a bisexual pattern of sexual attraction. Most of the cisgender men who exhibited bisexual viewing times did not engage in sexual activity with both men and women indicating that the manner in which bisexual patterns of sexual attraction manifest behaviorally vary from one culture to the next.
Article
Bailey et al. (2016) have provided an excellent, state-of-the-art overview that is a major contribution to our understanding of sexual orientation. However, whereas Bailey and his coauthors have examined the physiological, behavioral, and self-report data of sexual orientation and see categories, I see a sexual and romantic continuum. After noting several objections concerning the limitations of the review and methodological shortcomings characteristic of sexual-orientation research in general, I present evidence from research investigating in-between sexualities to support an alternative, continuum-based perspective regarding the nature of sexual orientation for both women and men. A continuum conceptualization has potential implications for investigating the prevalence of nonheterosexuals, sexual-orientation differences in gender nonconformity, causes of sexual orientation, and political issues.
Chapter
All else equal, men who are less interested in having reproductive sex will produce fewer offspring and thus selection will eliminate the genes that contribute to their sexual orientation. However, all else is not equal, as sexual orientation corresponds with a constellation of traits that may inform reproductive success. In this chapter, I present two hypotheses regarding the adaptive logic of reduced interest in the opposite sex. The first hypothesis addresses the tradeoff between offspring quantity and quality, proposing that men who abstain from sex with women make more effective parents. The second hypothesis invokes sperm competition and suggests that sneak copulating men can benefit from reduced arousal towards women. The question of exclusive homosexuality is addressed in the final section. I contend that self-identification as an exclusive homosexual is the product of a culture that promotes exclusive sexualities to isolate and remove nonheterosexuals from the reproductive arena. In other words, coming out of the closet as a homosexual man serves to distinguish oneself as a noncompetitor to local sexual rivals, alleviating the severity of one’s victimization at their hands. Throughout the chapter, I argue that the modern notion of sexual identity has corrupted our understanding of sexuality as a fluid and functional product of evolved cognitive mechanisms. Reliance on categorical sexual archetypes subverts our ability to characterize sexual variance, not only by limiting the depth of our measures, but also by limiting the depth of our theoretical thinking.
Article
Background: Persons reporting sexual identity that is discordant with their sexual behavior may engage in riskier sexual behaviors than those with concordant identity and behavior. The former group could play an important role in the spread of sexually transmitted diseases. Objective: To describe discordance between self-described sexual identity and behavior among men who have sex with men and associations between identity-behavior and risk behaviors. Design: Cross-sectional, random digit-dialed telephone survey of health status and risk behaviors. Setting: New York City. Participants: Population-based sample of 4193 men. Measurements: Concurrent measures of sexual identity and sexual behaviors, including number and sex of sex partners, condom use during last sexual encounter, and recent testing for HIV infection. Sex partner information was ascertained in a separate section from sexual identity; all participants were asked about the number of male sex partners and then were asked about the number of female sex partners in the past year. Results: Of New York City men reporting a sexual identity, 12% reported sex with other men. Men who had sex with men exclusively but self-identified as heterosexual were more likely than their gay-identified counterparts to belong to minority racial or ethnic groups, be foreign-born, have lower education and income levels, and be married. These men were more likely than gay-identified men who have sex with men to report having only 1 sexual partner in the previous year. However, they were less likely to have been tested for HIV infection during that time (adjusted prevalence ratio, 0.6 [95% Cl, 0.4 to 0.9]) and less likely to have used condoms during their last sexual encounter (adjusted prevalence ratio, 0.5 [Cl, 0.3 to 1.0]). Limitations: The survey did not sample groups that cannot be reached by using residential telephone services. Conclusions: Many New York City men who have sex with men do not identify as gay. Medical providers cannot rely on patients' self-reported identities to appropriately assess risk for HIV infection and sexually transmitted diseases; they must inquire about behavior. Public health prevention messages should target risky sexual activities rather than a person's sexual identity.
Article
Homosexuality presents a paradox for evolutionists who explore the adaptedness of human behavior. If adaptedness is measured by reproductive success and if homosexual behavior is nonreproductive, how has it come about? Three adaptationist hypotheses are reviewed here and compared with the anthropological literature. There is little evidence that lineages gain reproductive advantage through offspring care provided by homosexual members. Therefore, there is little support for the hypothesis that homosexuality evolved by kin selection. Parents at times control children’s reproductive decisions and at times encourage children in homosexual behavior. There is therefore more support for the hypothesis of parental manipulation. Support is strongest, however, for the hypothesis that homosexual behavior comes from individual selection for reciprocal altruism. Same‐sex alliances have reproductive advantages, and sexual behavior at times maintains these alliances. Nonhuman primates, including the apes, use homosexual behavior in same‐sex alliances, and such alliances appear to have been key in the expanded distribution of human ancestors during the Pleistocene. Homosexual emotion and behavior are, in part, emergent qualities of the human propensity for same‐sex affiliation. Adaptationist explanations do not fully explain sexual behavior in humans, however; social and historical factors also play strong roles.