ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

In the current climate of curriculum reform, the traditional lecture has come under fire for its perceived lack of effectiveness. Indeed, several institutions have reduced their lectures to 15 min in length based upon the “common knowledge” and “consensus” that there is a decline in students’ attention 10–15 min into lectures. A review of the literature on this topic reveals many discussions referring to prior studies but scant few primary investigations. Alarmingly, the most often cited source for a rapid decline in student attention during a lecture barely discusses student attention at all. Of the studies that do attempt to measure attention, many suffer from methodological flaws and subjectivity in data collection. Thus, the available primary data do not support the concept of a 10- to 15-min attention limit. Interestingly, the most consistent finding from a literature review is that the greatest variability in student attention arises from differences between teachers and not from the teaching format itself. Certainly, even the most interesting material can be presented in a dull and dry fashion, and it is the job of the instructor to enhance their teaching skills to provide not only rich content but also a satisfying lecture experience for the students.
A Personal View
Attention span during lectures: 8 seconds, 10 minutes, or more?
Neil A. Bradbury
Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Chicago Medical School, Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science,
North Chicago, Illinois
Submitted 12 July 2016; accepted in final form 19 October 2016
Bradbury NA. Attention span during lectures: 8 seconds, 10
minutes, or more? Adv Physiol Educ 40: 509 –513, 2016; doi:10.1152/
advan.00109.2016.—In the current climate of curriculum reform, the
traditional lecture has come under fire for its perceived lack of
effectiveness. Indeed, several institutions have reduced their lectures
to 15 min in length based upon the “common knowledge” and
“consensus” that there is a decline in students’ attention 10 –15 min
into lectures. A review of the literature on this topic reveals many
discussions referring to prior studies but scant few primary investi-
gations. Alarmingly, the most often cited source for a rapid decline in
student attention during a lecture barely discusses student attention at
all. Of the studies that do attempt to measure attention, many suffer
from methodological flaws and subjectivity in data collection. Thus,
the available primary data do not support the concept of a 10- to
15-min attention limit. Interestingly, the most consistent finding from
a literature review is that the greatest variability in student attention
arises from differences between teachers and not from the teaching
format itself. Certainly, even the most interesting material can be
presented in a dull and dry fashion, and it is the job of the instructor
to enhance their teaching skills to provide not only rich content but
also a satisfying lecture experience for the students.
lecture; attention; note taking; clickers
TED and the Goldfish
THE ENORMOUSLY POPULAR TED talks are a series of talks in
which speakers present their ideas on a wide range of topics
from technology to biomedical research to culture. One key
stipulation given to all speakers is that they have a maximum
of 18 min to present their material. The rule dictating 18 min
is based on the notion that 18 min is long enough to have a
“serious” presentation but short enough to hold a person’s
attention. With the broad spectrum of physiology teaching
from undergraduate courses to health professional and graduate
level courses, should a “TED” approach be widely imple-
mented in physiology curricula? Since the founding of Western
universities in the middle of the 11th century, the lecture has
been the traditional means of passing on knowledge. Indeed,
the 50-min lecture still holds sway at many institutions. De-
spite nearly a millennium of usage, the established lecture
format has come under more and more scrutiny. It is criticized
as being too long to hold a student’s attention based on several
authors’ claims that a student’s attention span declines precip-
itously after 10 –15 min. Such observations would support the
TED approach of an 18-min limitation. If, as is contended, a
student can mentally focus only in 15-min increments, it would
seem not only unreasonable but also grossly inefficient to
subject students to a 50-min lecture. Thus many authors would
make the case that a lecture session should last no more than
10 –15 min to accommodate the biological set point of a
student’s attention span. In 2015, a study commissioned by
Microsoft and discussed in Time magazine found that the
average attention span was in fact only 8 s. If indeed this is the
case, then even participating in a 15-min lecture would be
positively heroic. To place this in perspective, it was reported
in the same Time article, that goldfish, of the piscine rather than
snack variety, have an attention span of 9 s, one whole second
greater than humans! It is perhaps rather premature to opt for
an 8-s lecture format, as there are many caveats to the Time
article, not the least of which is that no one knows how to
actually measure a goldfish’s attention span. What has been
measured is goldfish memory, which, according to researchers
in the School of Psychology at the University of Plymouth, is
actually quite good (7). Similarly the 8-s attention span for
humans actually reflects the average time a person will spend
on a web page before looking somewhere else. So thankfully,
we can dispense with the 8-s lecture limit, but we are still left
with the idea that a physiology curriculum should only enter-
tain the concept of a 15-min attention span. However, despite
the perceived and agreed upon wisdom of this time point, are
there actually any data that would support such a thesis?
Genesis of the 10-Min Attention Span
The academic literature is replete with articles and books
supporting and propagating the conclusion that lectures should
adhere to the 10- to 15-min attention span that is characteristic
of modern students. In the book Tools for Teaching, Davis (5)
states that “...student attention during lectures tends to wane
after approximately 10 –15 minutes.” Similarly, Wankat (20)
argues that “Although student attention is high at the start of a
lecture, it has reached a low point after 10 –15 minutes.” In
essays honoring the psychologist Wilbert J. McKeachie, Ben-
jamin (1) asserts that “When the lecture begins, most students
are paying attention, and for most students that attention lasts
for about 10 minutes.” Indeed, McKeachie (13), in Teaching
Tips (8th Ed.), has maintained that “Attention typically in-
creases from the beginning of the lecture to 10 minutes into the
lecture and decreases after that point,” a sentiment still echoed
by this author more than 20 yr later in the 14th edition of the
book (19). Several points are noteworthy regarding these
publications. First, all of them agree on a quantitatively precise
10 –15 min time course for a variable (attention) that is nebu-
lous and never quantitatively defined. Second, evidentiary
discussion for such a precise time span is negligible. Third, all
of the above reports do not provide any primary data on
attention but are content to all cite the same single initial report
as the basis for the 10- to 15-min attention span assertion. For
example, McKeachie (13) states that “Hartley and Davies’
Address for reprint request and other correspondence: N. A. Bradbury, Dept.
of Physiology and Biophysics, Member Master Teacher Guild, Chicago
Medical School, 3333 Green Bay Rd., North Chicago, IL 60064 (e-mail:
neil.bradbury@rosalindfranklin.edu).
Adv Physiol Educ 40: 509–513, 2016;
doi:10.1152/advan.00109.2016.
5091043-4046/16 Copyright © 2016 The American Physiological Society
by 10.220.33.1 on December 24, 2016http://advan.physiology.org/Downloaded from
review of research on attention of students during lecture
reports that attention typically increases from the beginning of
the lecture to 10 minutes into the lecture and decreases after
that point.” Thus, the propagated concept of a 10- to 15-min
attention span ultimately appears to rely on a single key
manuscript published in 1978 (10) describing the waning of
attention during a lecture.
Note Taking
If all of the citations for a 10- to 15-min attention span
originate with a 1978 article by Hartley and Davies (10), then
a thorough examination of this article is clearly warranted.
What is remarkable regarding this publication is that attention
span is not actually the subject of the article; rather, the subject
of the manuscript is in fact “note taking.” This article itself is
also not a primary data source, but it reviews the literature up
to that point regarding the taking of notes in class by students.
Although this publication concerns note taking and not atten-
tion span, perhaps note taking is a reasonable surrogate marker
for attention. Indeed, the review by Hartley and Davies (10)
contends that the amount of notes taken declines over the
course of a lecture, consistent with “attention” decline after the
first 10 –15 min of a lecture. Certainly, a review of prior
literature, even work by Hartley and Cameron (9) and Maddox
and Hoole (12), argued for a connection between a 10-min time
point and a decline in note taking. Unfortunately the decline in
note taking was observed during the last 10 min of the lecture
and not the first (9, 12). Moreover, the decline in note taking at
the end the lecture was not caused by a lack of attention or
mental exhaustion on the part of the student but rather reflected
a drop in lecture content during the waning few minutes of the
presentation (12). In fact, the rate of note taking appears to be
relatively constant throughout the course of a lecture, and
changes in note taking appear to reflect whether the lecturer is
making key points rather than student fatigue. However, the
question still remains: is note taking a good surrogate for
attention? The answer appears to be no. Hartley and Davies
(10) concede that there is a waxing and waning in attention
span during a lecture but that measuring note taking was not an
indicator of attention. Indeed, citing the author’s own work (9)
and that of Maddox and Hoole (12), Hartley points out that
note taking is not necessarily indicative of attention at all. So,
note taking is not a good proxy for attention whatsoever, and
even if it were, it does not support a 10- to 15-min limit on
student engagement.
Personal Assessment
As discussed above, observations of students note taking has
been imputed as a surrogate for student attention. Yet as we
have seen, this is unreliable and not even supported by authors
who were studying note taking. If note taking is not a useful
metric, then what other approaches can be used to study
student attention spans? A study by Stuart and Rutherford (18)
attempted to discern attention of British medical students by
asking the students themselves what their attention level was
during a lecture. Every 5 min during a lecture, a buzzer would
go off and the students would record their own perceived
attention level ona1to9scale. The results showed that
attention rose rapidly during the first 10 –20 min and then
slowly and steadily declined until the end of the 50-min
lecture. Attention span did decline after 20 min, but the decline
was slow and never went below the initial attention level
observed in the first few minutes of the lecture. Interestingly,
although the attention level at the beginning of a lecture was
not different between second- and fifth-year medical students,
the attention level observed at the end of the lecture was
significantly greater for fifth-year students compared with sec-
ond-year students. Had senior medical students somehow
learned to pay more attention in class? Were senior students
acquiring new skills and techniques to enable them to be alert
for longer periods of time than their junior colleagues? In
reality, a review of the data shows that the entire difference
between second- and fifth-year students was all due to the fact
that one class for fifth-year students was taught by an experi-
enced lecturer who did not present to second-year students.
Thus, differences in attention appeared to be related to indi-
vidual lecturers’ teaching styles and not related to the student’s
individual abilities to remain attentive. A finding like this
should be axiomatic. We have all experienced lectures where
the lecture has been so awful and jejune that 10 min of lecture
has been 10 min too long, yet for other lecturers 1 h seems
wholly inadequate.
Direct Observation
Johnstone and Percival (11) attempted to evaluate attention
span not by utilizing the student’s own evaluations but by
employing two outside observers, whose job it was to watch
the class and record the times of perceived attention drift. Out
of 90 lectures given, the lectures “...were attended by at least
one of us observers,” state the authors. Although gratifying that
at least one of the authors usually turned up to observe the
class, in fact observation by both authors occurred only 13% of
the time, with 87% of all subjective data collected by a single
observer. Johnstone and Percival (11) reported that attention
dropped during the first 5 min of class, with another attention
lapse 10 –18 min into the class, a finding seemingly consistent
with prior observations. However, there are several problems
with the methodology that raise concerns about the validity of
the findings. Although it is stated that when both authors
observed the same lecture they were in agreement as to when
attention lapses occurred, there was no definition as to what
was an attention lapse. A person looking away from the teacher
may be reflecting on the material and integrating it with prior
work. A student fixedly staring at the teacher may be thinking
about last night’s dinner.
Clickers and Attention
Recently, Bunce et al. (4), attempted to address the question
of student attention using “clickers.” Nonchemistry majors
taking chemistry classes were asked to self-report using three
possibilities: with students asked to press button no. 1 for
attention lapses of 1 min, pressing button no. 2 for attention
lapses of 2–3 min duration, or pressing button no. 3 for
attention lapses 5 min. The authors noted that students did
not engage in consistent levels of attention but rather went
between phases of attention and inattention throughout the
entire lecture. Interestingly the examples the authors give for
attention lapses argue not for deficits in attention but rather for
attention directed toward things other than the lecture. As
examples of the kinds of things that were classified as attention
A Personal View
510 STUDENT ATTENTION SPAN
Advances in Physiology Education doi:10.1152/advan.00109.2016 http://advan.physiology.org
by 10.220.33.1 on December 24, 2016http://advan.physiology.org/Downloaded from
lapses, the authors reported that button no. 1 was pressed by
students glancing at and paying attention to the clock, button
no. 2 was pressed by students paying attention to and respond-
ing to text messages, and button no. 3 was pressed by students
paying attention to homework from another class. All of these
so-called attention lapses are extrinsic to whether a student is
losing attention in the present lecture due to mental fatigue.
Recall and Retention
One of the arguments in favor of a 10- to 15-min lecture is
that material covered in the lecture is not retained during
lectures lasting 15 min. Data in support of this contention
were presented by Johnstone and Percival (11), who attempted
to test knowledge of material covered during an apparent
“attention lapse” during subsequent tests. The authors report
that students performed poorly on recall of material covered
during an “attention lapse,” a finding that was “....statistically
highly significant.” Yet what statistical analyses were per-
formed, what method of data collection was utilized, and what
the criterion for significance was were not elaborated upon in
the article. McLeish’s report (15) of the work by Trenaman
argued that there was an inverse relationship between the
length of the lecture and the retention of the material covered
in that lecture. In these studies, students listened to a recorded
lecture (what would now be called a podcast) and were eval-
uated immediately following the lecture on their retention of
the material. Trenaman found that students listening to only 15
min of lecture had immediate retention of almost 41% of the
material compared with students listening to 40 min of mate-
rial, who only retained 20% of the material. Although this
study might seem to highlight the concept of diminishing
returns, there are several flaws in the analysis that preclude any
hard interpretation of the data. Clearly, more material is cov-
ered in a 40-min session compared with a 15-min session, so it
is not surprising that retention percentage is reduced, and this
can be fully accounted for by the number of testable items
covered in the two sessions. Even if the outcome from this
study does define an inverse relation between lecture length
and material retention immediately following the lecture, is
this in any way a meaningful analysis? No physiology course
director would expect students to immediately take examina-
tions on the material just covered in a lecture. No one can
imagine that immediate testing following exposure to material
would in any way be a reasonable assessment of learning and
comprehension. Indeed, to do so would likely incite a minor
revolt, or at least a robust complaint to the Dean. Attempts to
replicate Trenaman’s data have not been satisfactory. McLeish
(15), utilizing a live lecture format, rather than Trenaman’s
recorded format, found that there was no difference nor decline
in retention rates of material between live lectures of 25, 40, or
50 min. It is ironic that in today’s student preference for
listening to recorded rather than live lectures, the most parsi-
monious solution to the difference between Trenaman and
McLeish’s studies would argue that recorded material is infe-
rior to live lecture in content retention.
This article started by looking at note taking and has mean-
dered to material retention. Therefore, it would seem fitting, to
ask whether there is a correlation between note taking and
content recall. Fortunately, such an analysis was performed by
Scerbo et al. (16) in 1992. Consistent with previous reports,
there was an observed decline in note taking during the length
of a lecture. However, the decline in note taking had no impact
on the retention of covered material during subsequent evalu-
ation. That is not to say that note taking is not an important
skill for students to acquire, but as would be expected, it is the
quality of the notes being taken, not their quantity, that is
important. One critical point made by Scerbo et al. (16), which
should be taken into account by teachers, is that written cues
are recorded more frequently and better retained than state-
ments preceeded by spoken cues. Whether using chalk boards
or PowerPoint, the conclusion is clear, that key points should
always be highlighted in writing as well as spoken.
Objective Assessment of Attention Span
As discussed, the concept of attention is somewhat nebulous
and the working description for student attention ill-defined.
Given the subjective nature of many publications on attention
assessment, there appears to be clear need for an objective
determination of student attention, but what physiological
variables are amenable to easy and minimally invasive data
collection? At least two factors can be associated with atten-
tion: arousal and motivation. Arousal refers to a general level
of activity and a measure of nonspecific stimulation of a
student’s cerebral cortex. Heart rate is one indication of arousal
that is fairly easy to collect. Bligh (2) monitored 16 students’
heart rates every 5 s during a lecture using a pulsemeter. Heart
rates steadily declined by ~14% from the start to end of the
lecture, with a modest rise toward the end of the lecture.
Initially, such data would seem to confirm the notion of a
steady decline in attention during a lecture. However, Bligh (2)
found the same decline in heart rate when the teaching format
was a discussion class, and not a lecture. If heart rate is indeed
a relevant measure of attention, these studies would imply that
a drop in attention during so-called passive learning sessions,
such as a lecture, is no worse than the drop in attention during
active learning sessions, such as group discussions. In addition,
Bligh (2) was unable to find any overall correlation between
declining heart rate and retention of covered material. What
little correlation did exist showed that student retention of
material (as measured by immediate testing) was highest dur-
ing the last 20 min of the lecture, when arousal (at least as
determined by heart rates) was supposedly at its lowest. The
observation that material covered during the latter half of a
lecture is more readily retained is a finding previously noted by
Giles et al. (8). In a study of medical student retention, Giles et
al. (8) found that information presented between the 15- and
30-min time segments was recalled best, whereas material
presented during the first 15 min had the worst retention.
Interestingly, the seating position of the student in the lecture
hall had as much impact on material retention as the placement
of material within the lecture. Students sitting at the front,
middle, and back of the lecture hall scored 80, 71.6, and 68.1%,
respectively, on tests given immediately following the lecture.
However, these findings likely reflect motivational factors
(another component of attention) that determine where a stu-
dent sits, rather than the seating position by itself.
Factors Affecting Attention during Lectures
In the book What’s the Use of Lectures?, Bligh (2) outlines
several factors affecting attention. The author argues that there
A Personal View
511STUDENT ATTENTION SPAN
Advances in Physiology Education doi:10.1152/advan.00109.2016 http://advan.physiology.org
by 10.220.33.1 on December 24, 2016http://advan.physiology.org/Downloaded from
is no reason that lectures should be “solo performances,” with
paired faculty dialogues or group presentations being equally
valid. As part of the physiology curriculum in gastrointestinal
physiology at our medical school, one section is devoted to
pathophysiology. This section is taught in clinical vignettes by
four faculty all in attendance during the lecture session, each
presenting a clinical case in rotation. This seems to be appre-
ciated by the students and also serves as role modeling for how
faculty interacts with each other in a professional manner.
Similarly, there is no reason why students attending a lecture
cannot be part of the lecture. This is probably best exemplified
by the use of “clickers” that allow the students to answer
questions posed by the teacher. This can give a lecturer
immediate feedback on how well the students are comprehend-
ing the material. Of course, such two-way dialogue does not
have to rely on electronic devices but can be achieved by
verbal responses from the students. Stimulation can also come
from auditory and visual cues. One eminently useful aspect of
the internet is access to incredible graphics and videos. Teach-
ers are no longer consigned to devising their own crayon or
pencil drawings of tissues or organs but can avail themselves of
great images (subject to appropriate copyright usage) to illus-
trate dynamic physiological processes. Despite it being an old
adage, it is nonetheless still true that a picture is worth a
thousand words. Students quickly pick up on the idea that if
faculty are unwilling to take the time to provide the best
possible graphics for their lectures, then students are unwilling
to devote attention and time to that lecture; and who can blame
them? Attention by students can also be seriously hampered by
teachers merely reading long tracts of projected text. To the
extent that lectures are a performance in front of an audience,
teachers should actively take measures to alter rates of speech,
cadence, and style. In this regard, watching videos of motiva-
tional speakers can be very illustrative. Equally if not more
illuminating is watching videos of one’s own lectures. This
allows a teacher to see the lecture from a student’s viewpoint
and can be a great guide for improvements. Although all of the
above aspects of attention and how teachers can impact that
attention are discussed by Bligh (2), it is curious that Bligh (2)
omits mentioning student motivation as a critical aspect of
attention.
Has the Sun Set on the Day of the Lecture?
Many arguments have been raised against the utility of the
lecture format, although many seem to be straw men, rather
than reasoning based on findings. Clearly, the notion that
lectures should be dismissed because students have only a 10-
to 15-min attention span is erroneous and has little if any data
to support it. The lecture format has been deemed a “passive”
learning experience, with the current trend in “active” learning
being focused on activities such as “flipped teaching” and
“small problem-solving groups. A further argument against the
lecture approach is that the lecture represents a form of edu-
cational Luddism, which is not student-centered and focusses
on the “sage-on-the-stage”. It is informative to look at the
definition of a sage. Merriam Webster’s dictionary defines sage
as “wise through reflection and experience, or characterized by
wisdom, prudence and good judgement.” It is surprising that
such qualities are deemed unworthy or passé in a good teacher.
In fact, recent studies show that the lecture can be an effective
way to help students acquire new knowledge. Perhaps this
comes to the heart of the issue and requires an answer to the
fundamental question of what is the purpose of a lecture?
Certainly, one aspect is the conveyance of information, for
which the lecture is an effective method (2). There are mani-
fold online sources for information from which the student can
collect; however, determining which sources are reliable or just
plain wrong or discerning what information is salient vs.
minutiae still requires a sage. The quoted dichotomy of “sage-
on-the-stage” vs. “guide-by-the-side” is often used to disparage
the lecture format. In reality, this contrast is nothing more than
a sophomoric debating sleight, displaying the fallacy of the
false choice. The construct implies that these are the only two
options available, yet a minimal amount of thought reveals that
this is clearly not the case.
Is this article meant to imply that the 50-min lecture is the
preeminent or only means of conveying information? Certainly
not. Multiple approaches can and should be used to help
students not only to remember material but also to have a deep
comprehension of physiological processes and mechanisms
and be able to utilize such knowledge in various applications.
However, part of an overall balanced portfolio of instructional
tools can include the lecture. Indeed, recent studies show that
the lecture can be an effective way to help students acquire new
knowledge and may have benefits over flipped or small group
learning (17). In the book Teaching Naked, Bowen (3) articu-
lates aspects of teaching suited particularly for lectures. Lec-
tures are useful for introducing students to content. A quick
search on Google for “gastrointestinal physiology,” for exam-
ple, generates 1,400,000 hits. Which of these sites provides
accurate information, which of them are relevant to undergrad-
uate students with a minor in physiology, and which of them
are relevant to medical students? It is unlikely that a student
can adequately assess these options for themselves (at least
initially). Lectures provide a good entry into a topic; they
provide context and the level of detail and comprehension that
are required for a particular class. These aspects are things that
rely on a teacher’s accumulated experience and wisdom or,
dare one say it, on the teacher being a sage.
If a student can get the identical learning experience viewing
a YouTube video in bed just as they can attending a lecture in
person, why is this assumption not evident in other aspects of
life? We all have access to virtually unlimited recordings of
music; we are able to watch shows on television when it is
convenient for our schedule. If a virtual experience is indeed
identical to a real experience, then no one need go to a live
music concert, no one need to go to watch a live play or
musical, no one need go to hear a distinguished speaker give a
talk, and no one need attend a football or baseball game at a
stadium. Yet such venues are often quickly sold out. What is
different between a live and recorded event is the emotional
buy-in. Certainly books, or even videos, can be excellent media
for conveying content, but a live teacher can inspire a student
to think more about a subject and delve deeper into content
than can be achieved by passive media alone. Motivational
speakers know this very well, and many make a remarkably
good living by giving live presentations. Certainly charisma
helps in generating excitement about a subject in students, but
probably the biggest aspect of inspiring students is passion for
the subject on the part of the teacher. Lectures are one place
where a teacher can model intellectual, personal, and moral
A Personal View
512 STUDENT ATTENTION SPAN
Advances in Physiology Education doi:10.1152/advan.00109.2016 http://advan.physiology.org
by 10.220.33.1 on December 24, 2016http://advan.physiology.org/Downloaded from
values. They are usually one of the few places where students
meet faculty in person, allowing students to interact with
teachers and pose questions; it is critical that faculty acknowl-
edge when they don’t know the answers. In this way, faculty
can provide a broader education in terms of modeling respect
and care for students. Indeed, Dr. Martin Luther King stated
that “...intelligence plus character that is the goal of true
education.” Lectures can also be used as a platform to allow
questions to be raised that can focus attention on areas of
physiology that are not well worked out. Textbooks by their
very nature are considerably out of date by the time they are
published, and although many things remain true (e.g., the
heart pumps blood, the pancreas secretes hormones and en-
zymes), the mechanistic understanding of these processes is
continuously evolving. Thus, a lecture can be a good launching
point for discussions on what is not known about physiological
processes or areas in which there is considerable disagreement
about mechanism. We tend to think of lectures as only passing
on fixed content, but they can be a valuable tool in showing
uncertainty and developing the intellectual processes for han-
dling disagreements. Fortunately, there are now many sources
to help teachers improve the quality of lectures and move the
lecture beyond merely conveying information but also to in-
spire, motivate, and open doors to new ways of thinking. In this
regard, there are many books that help faculty encourage
critical thinking by their students, even within a lecture format
(3, 6, 14).
Conclusion
As scientists and physiologists, we are called on to provide
evidence for our research and data backing up our assertions.
Yet when it comes to attention span, an unsubstantiated mantra
of 15 min is chanted, with no support other than “That’s what
I’ve been told.” With the current educational trends of “life-
long learning” and “evidence-based teaching,” if we insist on
dogmatically applying a 10- to 15-min limit on lectures, we are
implying that we really don’t care about evidence. Beyond that,
it still behooves teachers in physiology classes to do as much
as possible to increase student motivation by showing the
relevance of material and providing a context for what is taught
as well as eagerly displaying a passion for the subject. Physi-
ology is a wonderful science, and students should expect
nothing less than having physiology taught with passion and
enthusiasm. What could be better for the future of the disci-
pline of physiology than to have students wanting to be in class
and eager to spend as much time as they possibly can in the
subject?
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I thank Dr. Ann Snyder, Associate Professor and Vice Chair of Cellular and
Molecular Pharmacology at the Chicago Medical School, for many helpful
discussions and encouragement.
GRANTS
N. Bradbury is a member of the Master Teacher Guild at the Rosalind
Franklin University of Medicine and Science. Dr. Bradbury’s research on
Cystic Fibrosis and Prostate Cancer is supported by a grant from the National
Institutes of Health (1-R01-HL-102208).
DISCLOSURES
N. Bradbury receives funding from Abbvie Pharmaceuticals.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
N.A.B. edited and revised manuscript; N.A.B. approved final version of
manuscript.
REFERENCES
1. Benjamin LT, Jr. Lecturing. In: The Teaching of Psychology: Essays in
Honor of Wilbert J. McKeachie and Charles L. Brewer, edited by Davis
SF and Buskist W. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2002, p.
57– 67.
2. Bligh DA. What’s the Use of Lectures? San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass,
2000.
3. Bowen JA. Teaching Naked: How Moving Technology Out of the Class-
room Will Improve Student Learning. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass,
2012, p. 327.
4. Bunce DM, Flens EA, Neiles KY. How long can students pay attention
in class? A study of student attention decline using clickers. J Chem Educ
87: 1438 –1443, 2010. 10.1021/ed100409p.
5. Davis BG. Tools for Teaching. San Franciso, CA: Jossey-Bass, 1993.
6. Forsyth DR. The Professor’s Guide to Teaching: Psychological Princi-
ples and Practices. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
tion, 2003.
7. Gee P, Stephenson D, Wright DE. Temporal discrimination learning of
operant feeding in goldfish (Carassius auratus). J Exp Anal Behav 62:
1–13, 1994.
8. Giles RM, Johnson MR, Knight KE, Zammett S, Weinman J. Recall
of lecture information: a question of what, when and where. Med Educ 16:
264 –268, 1982. 10.1111/j.1365-2923.1982.tb01262.x.
9. Hartley J, Cameron A. Some observations on the efficiency of lecturing.
Educ Rev 20: 30 –37, 1967. 10.1080/0013191670200103.
10. Hartley J, Davies IK. Note taking: A critical review. Program Learn
Educ Tech 15: 207–224, 1978.
11. Johnstone AH, Percival F. Attention breaks in lectures. Educ Chem 13:
49 –50, 1976.
12. Maddox H, Hoole E. Performance decrement in the lecture. Educ Rev 28:
17–30, 1975. 10.1080/0013191750280102.
13. McKeachie WJ. Teaching Tips: Strategies, Research and Theory for
College and University Teachers. Lexington, MA: Heath, 1986.
14. McKeachie WJ, Sviinicki M. McKeachie’s teaching tips: Strategies,
research, and theory for college and university teachers. Boston: Houg-
ton-Mifflin, 2006.
15. McLeish J. The Lecture Method. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Institute of
Education, 1968.
16. Scerbo MW, Warm JS, Dember WN, Grasha AF. The role of time and
cuing in a college lecture. Contemp Educ Psychol 17: 312–328, 1992.
10.1016/0361-476X(92)90070-F.
17. Schwerdt G, Wuppermann AC. Is traditional teaching really all that
bad? A within-student between-student approach. Econ Educ Rev 30:
365–379, 2010. 10.1016/j.econedurev.2010.11.005.
18. Stuart J, Rutherford RJ. Medical student concentration during lectures.
Lancet 312: 514 –516, 1978. 10.1016/S0140-6736(78)92233-X.
19. Svinicki MD, McKeachie WJ. McKeachie’s Teaching Tips: Strategies,
Research and Theory for College and University Teachers. Boston, MA:
Houghton-Mifflin, 2013.
20. Wankat PC. The Effective Efficient Professor: Scholarship and Service.
Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 2002.
A Personal View
513STUDENT ATTENTION SPAN
Advances in Physiology Education doi:10.1152/advan.00109.2016 http://advan.physiology.org
by 10.220.33.1 on December 24, 2016http://advan.physiology.org/Downloaded from
... All four students successfully completed the eye-tracking calibration and the full measurement procedure, which involved solving nine visual identification tasks and one training task for explanation. The overall measurement time, including the explanation task, was 329,88 seconds (about 5,5 minutes), which is well within the typical attention span for lower-secondary students and potentially could be easily extended with additional tasks [13]. This short duration helped maintain concentration and comfort throughout the session. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Modern education increasingly emphasizes the development of scientific skills to equip students for contemporary challenges. Developing such skills is essential for scientific literacy. In biology, species distinguishing and following determination are key skills. In educational practice, through textbooks and the use of determination keys, specific species of organisms are typically described on the basis of selected determination morphological features. However, the first step of species identification is to distinguish that the organisms differ. This peculiar step is often neglected in education, and it is not mapped whether students are able to recognize distinct species in order to proceed to search for specific features. To reflect this authentic aspect of species identification in biology, a set of tasks has been developed that require students to visually distinguish between orga nisms without additional information or feature specification. The aim of this pilot study was to develop such organisms' species distinguishing tasks enabling the obtaining of eye-tracking data about students' skills. The focus was on evaluating technical feasibility, estimating average completion time, identifying problematic aspects of the tasks that may require revision, and collecting student feedback for further development. Additionally, the pilot aimed to describe students' problem-solving processes qualitatively. Both successful and unsuccessful as a foundation for a follow-up study. The pilot was conducted with four lower secondary school students. The average solution time ranged from 16 to 33 seconds per task. None of the tasks were solved correctly by all students, suggesting that the tasks possess the potential to identify students' misleading strategies. Three of the tasks were not solved correctly by any participant. In two of these cases, the difficulty appeared to stem from intraspecific variability, shifting the challenge from recognizing fixed diagnostic traits to evaluating the degree of similarity among observed features. This phenomenon seems to require more attention and shows the direction for the following research.
... Scientists and physiologists draw on research and data that suggest the average attention span for effective learning is around 15 minutes. With this understanding, we have structured our exercises to last for 15 minutes, optimizing our learning potential during each session [12]. Movement science research indicates that strategically structuring work-rest cycles, with longer rest periods than work periods, enhances motor skill learning and performance [13]. ...
... Involving students in the effort to construct their knowledge requires effective student participation in every learning activity [13]. Efforts are needed to engage and motivate students in order to achieve effective student participation in the learning process [14] [15], so that learning is not limited to the transfer of knowledge, but presents learning activities that are fun and challenging for students [16], [17]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objectives of this community service are: 1) to assist biology teachers in designing a 'Mangrove Project', a high school biology learning project that facilitates mangrove campaigns and conservation; 2) to determine the response of biology teachers to the design of the 'Mangrove Project'. This community service is carried out through the stages of curriculum analysis, teacher interviews, focus group discussions followed by measurement of teacher responses to the community service. The resulting mangrove project design has four components: lesson planning, place of learning, expected competence and parties involved. Teachers responded positively to the implementation of community service and hoped.
... Passive learning is effective when complex analysis is unnecessary, such as when recognition and recall are sufficient to measure knowledge acquisition (Michel et al., 2009). Passive learning methods require minimal active participation from students, leading to relatively low student engagement, attention lapses, and poorer learning outcomes (Bradbury, 2016). Taking lecture notes is a typical example of passive learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Application of knowledge is a primary education goal and often used as a foundational measurement of teaching effectiveness (McKeough et al., 2013). Although confluent findings advocate the importance of teaching for transfer, little is known about precisely what knowledge students gain and how that knowledge is applied outside the classroom when taking specific graduate courses in psychology. As such, a phenomenological, qualitative methodology employing open, axial, and selective coding was used to determine what 58 graduate students gained, applied, and transferred when taking a graduate-level motivation course. Results analysis revealed that the motivation course experience generated greater self-awareness, cultivated acceptance or revision of self-beliefs, resulted in agentic engagement, and facilitated openness toward changing unproductive learning strategies, in addition to providing fundamental knowledge of motivation science concepts. Suggestions for course design that cultivates value and enhances transfer during instruction are discussed.
... Readers' attention spans can be as short as eight seconds; additionally, only around a quarter of the information on a webpage is actually read in most cases [4,21]. Patients are likely to read only the headlines and key points, which is why it is important to use a tool that gives a clear indication of reading ease, such as the FRES. ...
... [11] The advent of Generation Z, characterized by a short attention span and a preference for interactive learning, further highlights the need for effective and engaging teaching methods that resonate with their technological adeptness while maintaining an authentic learning environment. [12,13] The cognitive-constructivist paradigm emphasizes student engagement, participation, and ownership of learning, with teachers acting as facilitators to optimize the process. [14] Despite the autonomy of INI exploring innovative strategies, there is a lack of empirical research on how the learning styles of medical students in emerging premier institutes align with this pedagogical shift. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Medical education in India has adopted Competency-Based Medical Education, which focuses on learner-centered approaches to develop competent healthcare professionals. The knowledge of students’ learning styles helps develop pedagogical strategies. This study aims to find out the preferred learning style of second-year MBBS students using the VARK model. Methodology A descriptive cross-sectional study was conducted in May 2024 among 68 second-year MBBS students at All India Institute of Medical Sciences, Kalyani. After obtaining ethical approval, the validated VARK Questionnaire (Version 8.01) was administered physically. Students anonymously responded to the questionnaire, which allowed multiple responses. Data were entered into MS Office 2021, Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, Washington, United States and analyzed using descriptive statistics to determine learning style distributions and modal preferences. Results All respondents filled out the questionnaire. Kinesthetic was the unimodal technique with the highest preference rate (37.71%), followed by auditory (29.24%), then visual (22.69%), and finally read/write (10.33%). Bimodal preferences prevailed with 51%, such as auditory-kinesthetic (38%) and visual-kinesthetic (26%). Trimodal preferences had 9.4%, especially Visual-auditory-kinesthetic (45%). Quad Model preference was observed in only 1.1%. Conclusion Kinesthetic learning is the most dominant among medical students, whereas multimodal preferences indicate the need for dynamic, experience-based teaching methods. Curriculum designers must incorporate interactive and practice-based strategies to improve academic engagement and performance. Combining diverse teaching approaches could better realize positive outcomes. Such an adaptive and inclusive learning environment helps them prepare as future healthcare professionals.
... Research indicates that professors limit lecture duration to 15 min due to a concerning decline in students' attention spans [34]. The renowned TED restricts the duration of presenters' lectures to 18 min since this is the optimal amount for maintaining a listener's attention. ...
Article
Full-text available
Brain-computer interface (BCI) technology has been utilized in research for several decades. Using BCI, a person interacts with their environment by controlling it using their brain signals. Brain signals can also help determine the level of confusion among students. In this study, we discuss using BCI to measure students' concentration levels. Using student data obtained from Kaggle, a publicly accessible data source, we deployed a variety of deep learning and machine learning algorithms that include support vector machines, logistic regression, decision trees, random forests, multi-layer perceptron, deep neural networks, and bidirectional long short-term memory were used in our comparative analysis. We proposed 1D CNN and DNN models in this research study. Although LSTM performs better when time stamps are present in the data, our analysis of the findings revealed that deep neural networks performed better in this case, with an accuracy of 99%. Another 1D CNN model is proposed to compare its performance with the DNN model. We found that the 1DCNN model is more stable than the DNN model but has slightly lower accuracy, i.e., 95%. Additionally, deep learning algorithms typically employ the rectified linear unit (ReLU) to mitigate vanishing gradient issues, resulting in a well-performing model. In contrast, 'RELU' has reversed the findings of BiSTM in this investigation, reducing accuracy from 82 to 67%. We have also conducted numerous experiments to identify the optimal parameter values for deep neural networks (DNNs) that yield the highest accuracy, specifically 99.9%. Therefore, DNN can be utilized in a real-time setting for the development of a confusion detection system, and BCI can be incorporated into the educational paradigm to reap the benefits of technological progress.
... Graduate students may appear to be making much effort to participate in the discussion, but this is exhausting, especially if they attend classes after working hours (Aristeidou et al., 2024). Talks should be condensed and integrated with real-world assessments (Bradbury, 2016). Graduate students are always eager to relate personal experiences from their theoretical talks, and most are employed (Mahoney & Retallick, 2015;Adeniyi et al., 2024). ...
Article
Technology is changing higher education, but the greatest value of a physical university will remain its face-to-face (naked) interaction between faculty and students. The most important benefits to using technology occur outside of the classroom. New technology can increase student preparation and engagement between classes and create more time for the in-class dialogue that makes the campus experience worth the extra money it will always cost to deliver. Students already use online content but need better ways to interact with material before every class. By using online quizzes and games, rethinking our assignments and course design, we can create more class time for the activities and interactions that most spark the critical thinking and change of mental models we seek.
Article
This paper aims (1) to determine why students take notes, (2) to survey the research findings available in the current literature, (3) to identify inadequacies and omissions in this literature in order to suggest more profitable lines of research for future study, and (4) to suggest guide‐lines for both teachers and students which will exploit more effectively the potential of note‐taking.
Article
The lecture process was studied systematically with the following objectives: to reexamine the effects of time upon note-taking and immediate retention, to compare the relative effectiveness of spoken and written cues, and to investigate cuing schedules. Students were asked to view one of a series of videotaped lectures in which certain statements were highlighted by either spoken or written cues. The contents of students' notes and assessments of immediate recall and recognition provided the dependent measures. Students recorded increasingly less information in their notes over the course of the lecture, but retention of material from different portions of the lecture was essentially the same. Written-cued statements were recorded more frequently and retained better than statements preceded by spoken cues. Finally, the different schedules of cuing were shown to have subtle effects upon note-taking and recall.
Article
Contrary to much published opinion, little evidence was found of decrement in the performance of students in a University lecture. Decrement was measured by the percentage of “ideal” notes recorded by students in the course of an expository lecture. Nevertheless the students’ notes although taken under the best of conditions, were so imperfect as to provide further condemnatory evidence against the descriptive lecture.