ChapterPDF Available

Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making

Authors:
  • Fresenius University of Applied Sciences, Berlin, Germany

Abstract and Figures

Teenagers are typically described as impulsive and risk taking. Yet recent research shows that this observation does not hold in all contexts. Rather, adolescents show higher impulsivity and risk taking than children or adults in affective contexts. Motivational and affective processes are therefore of particular interest when trying to understand typical adolescent behavior. Additionally, pubertal hormones are hypothesized to play a special role in adolescents' motivated decision making. However, evidence for the mechanisms underlying this relationship is sparse. In this chapter, we aim to integrate findings from human and animal studies in order to elucidate the specific impact of pubertal hormones on motivational processes in adolescence. Against this background, we critically discuss and reinterpret recent findings in psychology and neuroscience, speculate about underlying mechanisms, and suggest new approaches for future studies of adolescent behavior. © 2017 by Emerald Group Publishing Limited All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Recent Developments in Neuroscience Research on
Human Motivation
Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Corinna Laube Wouter van den Bos
Article information:
To cite this document: Corinna Laube Wouter van den Bos . "Hormones and Affect
in Adolescent Decision Making" In Recent Developments in Neuroscience Research on
Human Motivation. Published online: 22 Nov 2016; 259-281.
Permanent link to this document:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/S0749-742320160000019013
Downloaded on: 30 November 2016, At: 01:26 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 0 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by Ms
Corinna Laube
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please
use our Emerald for Authors service information about how to choose which
publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please visit
www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society.
The company manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books
and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online products
and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner
of the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the
LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation.
*Related content and download information correct at time of download.
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
HORMONES AND AFFECT IN
ADOLESCENT DECISION MAKING
Corinna Laube and Wouter van den Bos
ABSTRACT
Teenagers are typically described as impulsive and risk taking. Yet
recent research shows that this observation does not hold in all contexts.
Rather, adolescents show higher impulsivity and risk taking than children
or adults in affective contexts. Motivational and affective processes are
therefore of particular interest when trying to understand typical adoles-
cent behavior. Additionally, pubertal hormones are hypothesized to play
a special role in adolescents’ motivated decision making. However,
evidence for the mechanisms underlying this relationship is sparse. In this
chapter, we aim to integrate findings from human and animal studies in
order to elucidate the specific impact of pubertal hormones on motiva-
tional processes in adolescence. Against this background, we critically
discuss and reinterpret recent findings in psychology and neuroscience,
speculate about underlying mechanisms, and suggest new approaches for
future studies of adolescent behavior.
Keywords: Adolescence; puberty; testosterone; risk taking;
impulsivity; affect
Recent Developments in Neuroscience Research on Human Motivation
Advances in Motivation and Achievement, Volume 19, 259281
Copyright r2017 by Emerald Group Publishing Limited
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved
ISSN: 0749-7423/doi:10.1108/S0749-742320160000019013
259
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
ADOLESCENCE AS A PERIOD OF
MOTIVATED BEHAVIOR
“Adolescence is practically synonymous in our culture with risk taking, emotional
drama and all forms of outlandish behavior.” (Friedman, 2014)
Adolescence describes the developmental phase between childhood and
adulthood. It is characterized by a transition from dependence to indepen-
dence, as well as by sexual maturation. The path to independence is asso-
ciated with a process of major social re-orientation, in which the influence
of parents becomes less pronounced and that of peers increases (Nelson,
Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005;van den Bos, 2013). The start of adoles-
cence is often defined by the onset of puberty, whereas its end is culturally
defined: whenever an individual is considered to be an adult. Puberty cov-
ers a shorter time interval; it is the process of hormonal and physiological
changes by which individuals reach sexual maturity and is found in most
mammals (Spear, 2010).
One salient characteristic of adolescence is an increase in risky and
impulsive behavior (Braams, van Duijvenvoorde, Peper, & Crone, 2015;
O’Brien, Albert, Chein, & Steinberg, 2011;Steinberg et al., 2009). Social
problems in adolescence such as drunk driving, suicide, and teenage preg-
nancy are often attributed to this increased risk taking and impulsivity
(Dahl, 2004). Furthermore, risky behavior results in a significant increase
in adolescents’ visits to the emergency room (Steinberg, 2007) and to a
200% increase in the mortality rate among teenagers (Dahl, 2004).
Adolescence as a time of turbulence and excess is not a creation of mod-
ern society. Historical accounts of adolescent impetuosity go back as far as
ancient Greece (300600 BC), with typical teenage behaviors such as rash-
ness, sexual excess, frivolity, drunkenness, and lack of self-control being
portrayed in ancient Greek art (Harlow & Laurence, 2002). “Irrational”
adolescent behavior has traditionally been attributed to “raging hormones”
(Buchanan, Eccles, & Becker, 1992); more recently, neuroscientific methods
have shown that teenagers’ prefrontal cortex, an area involved in decision
making and rationality, is not yet fully developed (Yurgelun-Todd, 2007).
However, the latest scientific discoveries suggest that typical adolescent
behavior has multiple interactive causes and may often be adaptive rather
than irrational (Crone & Dahl, 2012;Dahl, 2004;Pfeifer & Allen, 2012).
Besides the development of cognitive control, motivational processes also
seem to play an important role in adolescent behavior (Crone & Dahl,
2012;Ernst, 2014;Somerville & Casey, 2010;Steinberg, 2008). It has been
260 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
argued that these adolescent-specific changes in motivational processes
afford both risks and opportunities (Blakemore & Robbins, 2012;Crone &
Dahl, 2012).
The shift in attention to motivational processes has sparked renewed
interest in the role of pubertal hormones in adolescent behavior. It is
hypothesized that these hormones specifically impact motivational pro-
cesses during this developmental phase, which in turn leads to increased
risk taking and impulsive behavior, but also supports positive develop-
ment in adolescence (Crone & Dahl, 2012). However, little is known
about the mechanisms that mediate the relationship between hormones
and changes in motivational processes during adolescence or how these
changes in motivational processes may heighten adolescent risk taking
and impulsivity.
In this chapter, we aim to integrate findings elucidating the specific
impact of pubertal hormones on motivational processes in adolescence.
First, we review findings from self-report and behavioral studies of adoles-
cent decision making in order to better understand the changes that occur
in motivational processes during this developmental period and how those
changes relate to a specific subset of behaviors: risk taking and impulsiv-
ity. Next, we consider the role of hormones in pubertal development and
how they relate to adolescent risk taking and impulsivity. Together, these
findings suggest that pubertal hormones indeed have a sizeable influence
on risky and impulsive behavior in adolescence, and that this effect is
accompanied by context-dependent changes in motivational processing.
To better understand the possible mechanisms by which pubertal
hormones impact behavior, we turn to recent findings from neuroscience.
Here, we focus on adolescent-specific changes in motivational brain
systems and the potential impact of hormones on these systems. We con-
clude by deriving general educational implications and identifying direc-
tions for future research.
Note that we constrain adolescent behavior to risk taking and impulsiv-
ity given the fact that we currently find the most available evidence
in these two domains. Finally, although we recognize that increased ado-
lescent risk taking in the real world can be partly attributed to decreased
parental control (Defoe, Dubas, Figner, & van Aken, 2015;Romer,
2010) and to developmental changes in cognitive skills, such as reasoning
skills (Reyna & Brainerd, 2012), addressing these issues would go beyond
the scope of this chapter. On an endocrinological level, we focus on
testosterone, which is known to be associated with risky and impul-
sive behavior.
261Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
THE ROLE OF MOTIVATIONAL PROCESSES IN
ADOLESCENT DECISION MAKING: FINDINGS FROM
SELF-REPORT AND BEHAVIORAL STUDIES
Self-report measures indicate that impulsivity, sensation seeking, and con-
sequently risk taking are elevated during adolescence. A common definition
of impulsivity is the tendency to act without planning or considering poten-
tial consequences (e.g., “I like to stop and think things over before I do
them” [reversed item]). Sensation seeking can be defined as the tendency to
seek out experiences and situations that are novel, exciting, or rewarding;
typical items are “I quite enjoy taking risks” or “Life with no danger in it
would be too dull for me” (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011). Findings from
behavioral and self-report studies indicate that impulsivity tends to show a
linear decline as a function of age (Quinn & Harden, 2013;Steinberg, 2008;
Steinberg et al., 2009;Vaidya, Latzman, Markon, & Watson, 2010;
van den Bos, Rodriguez, Schweitzer, & McClure, 2015), whereas sensation
seeking follows an inverted U-shape curve (Cauffman et al., 2010;Harden &
Tucker-Drob, 2011;Romer & Hennessy, 2007;Steinberg, 2008). Harden
and Tucker-Drob (2011) have replicated these developmental patterns of
age trends in a longitudinal study with a sample of 7,640 adolescents.
Impulsivity and sensation seeking can thus be regarded as distinct traits that
are influenced by qualitatively different developmental processes in adoles-
cence. Specifically, impulsivity is strongly associated with lack of cognitive
control, whereas sensation seeking is thought to stem from sensitivity to
motivational cues (Steinberg, 2007, 2008). Yet although the two constructs
are distinct, they are both associated with developmental changes in risky
behavior. For instance, a longitudinal study by Quinn and Harden (2013)
showed that age-related changes in both impulsivity and, to a lesser extent,
sensation seeking accounted for variability in substance use change.
Specifically, individuals who showed slower declines in impulsivity and
sensation seeking showed faster increases in substance use. This pattern of
results suggests that adolescent behavior is the outcome of an interaction
between distinct motivational forces and (limited) cognitive control, all of
which are still under development.
Laboratory experiments have provided further insights into (1) the
cognitive/affective processes involved in adolescent behavior and (2) the
conditions under which this behavior tends to occur. It is important to
note, however, that a recent meta-analysis of behavioral risk studies by
Defoe et al. (2015) revealed a discrepancy between patterns of risk taking
262 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
in the real world and adolescent behavior as studied in the laboratory. In
contrast to many reports about real-world risk taking (Dahl, 2004;
Willoughby, Good, Adachi, Hamza, & Tavernier, 2013), laboratory stu-
dies have not generally found risk-taking behavior to peak in adoles-
cence: Although adolescents take more risks than adults, and early
adolescents take more risks than mid-late adolescents, lab studies typi-
cally show no difference between early adolescents and children in terms
of risk taking. One possible reason for this discrepancy is that the struc-
ture of the tasks used in laboratory studies does not match that of the
real-world environment.
For instance, a common way to study risky decision making is to present
simple gambles (wheels of fortune) that link outcomes of different magni-
tudes with different probabilities (see Ernst et al., 2004). In a study by Van
Leijenhorst et al. (2010), participants were presented with choices between
options with a high probability of a small reward (low-risk/low-reward)
and options with a low probability of a large reward (high-risk/high-
reward). Such tasks differ from the real world in several crucial ways. First,
in the real world, the true probabilities are often unknown (Hertwig &
Erev, 2009). Second, real-world adolescent risk taking is thought to occur
mainly in arousing contexts. For example, the risk of fatal injury for a
16- or 17-year-old driver increases with the number of passengers (Chen,
Baker, Braver, & Li, 2000). In the following, we review evidence from
laboratory studies suggesting that adolescents indeed show more risk-
taking behavior than either adults or children in experimental paradigms
that involve arousing conditions, to which they are more sensitive.
Figner, Mackinlay, Wilkening, and Weber (2009) designed a dynamic
risk-taking task, the Columbia Card Task, to study age differences in risk
taking and underlying information use. In this task, participants are shown
32 cards face down on a computer screen. Each card indicates a gain
(which is added to the trial payoff) or a loss. Players can turn over as many
cards as they like until they encounter a loss card, which terminates the
trial. The aim of the game is to score as many points as possible. Figner
et al. (2009) implemented two conditions: one “hot” and one “cold” condi-
tion. In the hot condition, participants received immediate feedback and
were allowed to make stepwise incremental decisions about turning over
another card. In the cold condition, they had to make a single decision on
the total number of cards to be turned over in each trial and received out-
come feedback only at the very end of the game. Figner et al. (2009) argued
that both receiving immediate feedback and deciding which card to turn
over next trigger affective processes. They found that adolescents took
263Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
more risks than children and adults in the hot condition, whereas risk
taking was similar across all ages in the cold condition.
The Iowa gambling task is another dynamic risk-taking task that is
frequently used to assess risk taking in the laboratory. It approximates real-
life decision making under uncertainty (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, &
Anderson, 1994)that is, the probabilities and outcomes are not described
upfront but have to be learned during the task. Specifically, participants are
presented with four decks of cards, each containing cards that reward or
punish the player by adding or subtracting points or amounts of money
from his or her account. Two of the decks lead to net increases over the
course of repeated play (the advantageous decks); the other two lead to net
losses (the disadvantageous decks). Cauffman et al. (2010) used a modified
version of the Iowa Gambling Task to measure affective risk taking in a
diverse sample of 901 individuals aged between 10 and 30 years.
Adolescents increasingly played from the advantageous decks; this learned
preference for the advantageous decks was interpreted as approach beha-
vior, and displayed an inverted U-shape relation to age, peaking in mid- to
late adolescence.
In the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002), another
“hot” laboratory risk-taking task, participants have to inflate a computer-
ized balloon one pump at a time. They earn a monetary reward each time
they pump the balloon but lose the entire reward if the balloon bursts.
Consequently, larger rewards can be earned by taking more risks. Several
recent studies using the BART have found a peak in risk-taking behavior
in adolescents, relative to children and adults (Braams et al., 2015;van
Duijvenvoorde et al., 2015); others have found no such peak in adolescence
(see Defoe et al., 2015).
Finally, several studies have shown that the mere presence of peers
increases adolescents’ risk taking relative to adults’ (Gardner & Steinberg,
2005;O’Brien et al., 2011). For instance, Gardner and Steinberg (2005)
found that the presence of peers more than doubled the number of risks
teenagers took in a video driving game. However, it had no effect at all on
adults. Similarly, in a study with a delay discounting task, O’Brien et al.
(2011) found that adolescents preferred more immediate rewards to later
ones when in the presence of their peers. In this study, participants had to
choose between a smaller sooner reward (e.g., $200 today) and a larger
later reward (e.g., $1,000 in one year). Adolescents were more likely to
choose the smaller sooner reward in the presence of peers than when alone.
The presence of peers is hypothesized to be a highly arousing context for
adolescents and this increased arousal is thought to change their behavior
264 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
in a similar fashion as the arousing context provided by “hot” gambling
games. It is also possible that adolescents in peer social contexts take risks
in order to send social signals (Milchunas, Sala, & Lauenroth, 1988).
In conclusion, there is substantial evidence that adolescents show
increased risk taking and impulsivity in laboratory studies under arousing
conditions. Given that the cognitive components of the tasks administered
are assumed to be stable across conditions, these findings suggest that there
is something specific about how adolescents process the affective motiva-
tional components of the tasks. However, behavioral studies can provide
only limited insights into the underlying mechanisms. In the next section,
we argue that hormones play an important role in modulating the affective
motivational processes involved in adolescent risk taking and impulsivity.
THE ROLE OF PUBERTAL HORMONES
The idea that the particularly pronounced changes in mood and behavior
observed in adolescence result from biological factors such as a rapid
change in hormone levels goes back to at least the early 1900s (Hall, 1904).
This change in pubertal hormones is not specific to humans but is a cross-
species phenomenon, and a variety of species show puberty-typical beha-
viors such as novelty seeking and increased peer interactions (Spear, 2000).
Yet it remains unclear how pubertal hormones relate to increased risk
taking and impulsivity in adolescence. Previous research has focused on
age, rather than pubertal maturation, as a predictor of developmental
changes in risk behavior. However, a handful of studies suggest a link
between pubertal hormones and developmental changes in arousal, motiva-
tion, and emotion (Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, 2010;Crone & Dahl,
2012;Forbes & Dahl, 2010;Peper & Dahl, 2013;Steinberg, 2005). Before
we review the relationship between hormones, specifically testosterone, and
adolescent behavior, we briefly outline the mechanisms behind the hormo-
nal changes seen in puberty.
Pubertal Development
Pubertal development is associated with a rapid rise in gonadal hormone
release initiating development of secondary sexual characteristics, such as
breast development in girls and pubertal hair growth, as well as other
265Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
physiological changes, such as physical growth. The sex hormones regulating
bodily changes are testosterone, oestradiol, and dehydroepiandrosterone
(DHEA). The hormonal cascade initiating the release of these hormones
from the gonads begins in the brain, and a feedback mechanism known as
the hypothalamicpituitarygonadal (HPG) axis regulates endocrine
function. Pubertal onset is associated with the activation of neurons
located in the hypothalamus that secrete gonadotropin-releasing hor-
mone (GnRH). GnRH then travels to the pituitary gland, where it
regulates the synthesis and secretion of two pituitary gonadotropins:
luteinizing hormone (LH) and follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) (see
Fig. 1). Released into the bloodstream, LH and FSH then jointly act to
stimulate the production of gonadal steroid hormones, completing the
process of egg and sperm development. The appearance of secondary sex-
ual characteristics in peripheral tissues (e.g., breast development in girls
and facial hair in boys) in turn leadstoelevatedlevelsofestrogenand
progesterone.
How does the brain know when to activate the HPG axis and trigger
puberty? Both internal and external cues provide information on the avail-
ability of necessary resources for successful reproduction. Internal cues
Fig. 1. Dopaminergic and Hormonal Pathways in the Brain.
266 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
include biological prerequisites for pregnancy, such as metabolic levels of
insulin, glucose, and leptin that indicate somatic growth and metabolic fuel
availability; lower body fat has been associated with delayed pubertal
onset (Frisch, 1984). External cues include information on the availabil-
ity of a suitable mate and food. Furthermore, females typically enter and
move through adolescence more quickly than males (Savin-Williams &
Weisfeld, 1989), suggesting biological sex as an important factor in pub-
ertal timing. To conclude, the timing of pubertal onset should be viewed
as a neurological rather than a gonadal event (Sisk & Zehr, 2005). That
is, it is the nervous system that integrates different cues from the body
and the environment and, as such, determines pubertal onset (Sisk &
Zehr, 2005).
Pubertal Maturation Measures
Different methods are used to assess pubertal development. The indirect
method of Tanner staging (Tanner, 1962) categorizes individuals along
an ordinal puberty scale from 1 (no development) to 5 (adult develop-
ment). These standardized categories capture visible secondary sexual
characteristics such as breast or genital development and pubic hair
growth. Tanner stages can be determined by physical exam conducted by
a trained clinician or by self-report. Another common way of measuring
external pubertal status is the Pubertal Developmental Scale (PDS)
(Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, 1988). Like the Tanner scale, the
PDS asks adolescents about hair growth, skin changes, and growth
spurts, with sex-specific items, such as menarche and breast development
in females and genital growth and facial hair in males. The resulting com-
posite puberty score reflects the effects of adrenal and gonadal hormones
as well as growth hormones.
Shirtcliff, Dahl, and Pollak (2009) examined the interrelations of multi-
ple indices of puberty and found that PDS scores are related to levels of
basal hormones, such as testosterone in boys. However, even the best mea-
sure of external pubertal status captures less than half of the variability in
basal hormones (Shirtcliff et al., 2009). Measuring hormone levels in saliva
or blood can thus give important insights into pubertal maturation that are
not available from overt physical measures alone. Given the variety of
changes accompanying puberty, the best pubertal status measurement will
depend on the issues being investigated.
267Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
The Influence of Testosterone on Adolescent Behavior
Vermeersch, T’Sjoen, Kaufman, and Vincke (2008) found a positive rela-
tionship between testosterone and risk taking in a sample of 301 adoles-
cent boys, independent of age and pubertal development as measured by
the Tanner scale. Interestingly, this effect was partly mediated by peer
influence: boys with high levels of testosterone also had peers who were
more involved in risky behaviors, which in turn influenced their own risk
taking. This finding is in line with substantial evidence indicating that
individual levels of testosterone also predict differences in risky behavior
in adults (Booth, Johnson, & Granger, 1999;Campbell et al., 2010;
Goudriaan et al., 2010;Rosenblitt, Soler, Johnson, & Quadagno, 2001;
Stanton, Liening, & Schultheiss, 2011;Stenstrom, Saad, Nepomuceno, &
Mendenhall, 2011).
Furthermore, testosterone has been described as a “social hormone”
and is often related to behaviors such as status seeking and social domi-
nance (Eisenegger, Haushofer, & Fehr, 2011). Single time-point measure-
ments of testosterone correlate positively with high dominance in
both adolescents (Rowe, Maughan, Worthman, Costello, & Angold,
2004;Vermeersch, T’Sjoen, Kaufman, Vincke, & Van Houtte, 2010)and
adults (Carre
´, Putnam, & McCormick, 2009;Grant & France, 2001;
van den Bos, Golka, Effelsberg, & McClure, 2013). However, according
to Wingfield’s challenge hypothesis, testosterone predicts social behavior
only when status is threatened or challenged (Josephs, Newman,
Brown, & Beer, 2003;Wingfield, Ball, Dufty, Hegner & Ramenofsky,
2013). For instance, a review by Sapolsky (1991) showed that testosterone
predicts social rank in primates only when hierarchy in the group is
unstable. Consequently, the effects of testosterone on behavior are highly
context dependent. There is an interesting parallel here with behavioral
studies on risk taking and impulsive behavior, which have found adoles-
cents to show more risky behavior in arousing and social contexts. These
findings may in turn be related to the context-specific effects of testoster-
one. Whatever form the underlying relationships prove to take, it is clear
that the context needs to be taken into account when considering effects
of pubertal hormones on adolescent behavior.
In sum, risky and impulsive behavior in adolescence may be attributed
to (1) developmental change in motivational processing, with risky beha-
vior increasing specifically in arousing contexts; (2) rapid change in gona-
dal hormone levels at pubertal onset. Importantly, these findings suggest
a link between changes in motivational processing and changes in
268 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
pubertal hormone levels. However, the mechanisms behind these effects
remain unclear. Neuroscience can be a helpful tool in identifying
these mechanisms.
THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF MOTIVATED
ADOLESCENT BEHAVIOR
Over the past decades, numerous studies have investigated adolescent brain
development (for a review, see Crone & Dahl, 2012), and several closely
related theoretical models have emerged. The most influential are the dual-
system models (Casey, Getz, & Galvan, 2008;Steinberg, 2008) based on
the two-system model of willpower by Metcalfe and Mischel (1999), which
postulates a cool, cognitive “know” system and a hot, emotional “go”
system. The cool system enables self-regulation and self-control by being
cognitive, emotionally neutral, flexible, slow, and strategic. The hot system
is the origin of emotionality, fears and passion, and is driven by impulsive
and reflexive forces. These two systems are in continuous interaction; their
balance varies as a function of stress, developmental level, and individual
self-control. Neural dual-system models propose that increased risk taking
in adolescence is the result of an interaction between distinct brain
networks: a motivational system associated within subcortical limbic struc-
tures, including the striatum, and a control system associated with the
prefrontal cortex.
The triadic model by Ernst et al. (2014) further distinguishes between
approach and avoidance behavior. In this model, the limbic system is
divided into two subsystems: the ventral striatum, which is particularly sen-
sitive to rewards and is thus associated with approach, and the amygdala,
which is particularly responsive to aversive or fearful stimuli and is thus
associated with avoidance.
Importantly, neurodevelopmental models suggest that changes in adoles-
cent behavior are specifically due to the differential developmental patterns
of these distinct systems. Moreover, all models emphasize the special role
of the motivational system in early adolescence. Specifically, it is hypothe-
sized that a rapid increase in dopaminergic activity within the motivational
system leads to increased reward seeking during early adolescence. At the
same time, the slower maturation of cognitive control systems results in
unregulated or risky behavior, “like driving a car with a sensitive gas pedal
and bad brakes” (Steinberg, 2014). Consequently, unregulated reward
269Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
seeking may result in heightened vulnerability to risk taking during
adolescence.
Although all models predict that the motivational brain system develops
early in adolescence, evidence has been mixed. In line with the models’ pre-
dictions, some studies have found that the ventral striatum, an important
region in the brain’s reward circuitry, shows peak activity in adolescence
(Braams, Peters, Peper, Gu
¨roˇ
glu, & Crone, 2014;Galvan et al., 2006);
others have found no such evidence (e.g., Bjork, 2004;Bjork, Smith,
Chen, & Hommer, 2010;Richards, Plate, & Ernst, 2013). Richards et al.
(2013) systematically reviewed the fMRI reward paradigms used in studies
with adolescents versus adults and found task design to have a sizable
impact as one source of variability across findings. However, they high-
lighted that the mixed results cannot be attributed solely to the type of
task and that it remains unclear how other variables, such as pubertal
status and environmental context, influence the neural systems underlying
reward-related behavior.
One possible reason for the mixed findings is that most studies focused
on age rather than using direct measures of pubertal status. Yet chronolo-
gical age is not a reliable predictor of pubertal status. For instance, data
from a five-year longitudinal study in the 1970s showed that puberty may
begin from age 8.0 to 14.9 years in females and from age 9.7 to 14.1 years
in males and is complete by age 12.4 to 16.8 years in females and by age
13.7 to 17.9 years in males (Lee, 1980). Given the considerable degree of
individual difference in pubertal onset, it seems likely that many studies
with a focus on chronological age have failed to detect pubertal changes in
motivational behavior and related brain activity.
Indeed, a handful of studies suggest that pubertal status is a good pre-
dictor of reward-related activity. For example, a recent longitudinal study
by Braams et al. (2015), using a large sample (N= 299), found that the
average developmental trajectory of nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activation,
an important region in the brain’s reward circuitry (see Fig. 1), showed an
inverted U-shape pattern. Moreover, change in NAcc activation was posi-
tively correlated with change in pubertal testosterone. This finding is in line
with previous research reporting a relationship between pubertal testoster-
one and ventral striatum activity (Forbes et al., 2010;Op De Macks et al.,
2011). For instance, Forbes et al. (2010) found that testosterone was posi-
tively associated with striatal activity during reward anticipation in boys,
but negatively correlated with outcome-related caudate reactivity. Their
finding suggests that the effects of testosterone on reward processing may
be variable and depend on the decision phase.
270 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
What is still missing is a clear understanding of how hormones may lead
to changes in striatal activation. Animal studies may be informative here,
as puberty is not specific to humans but is a cross-species phenomenon
(Spear, 2004). Indeed, single-unit recordings and lesion studies in animals
have provided numerous insights into the mechanisms that underlie brain
and behavioral changes during adolescence (Spear, 2004; for a review, see
Casey, Duhoux, & Cohen, 2010). For instance, it is well established that
dopamine plays an important role in goal-directed behavior, reward, and
motivation (see Schultz, 2015). There are two major pathways of dopamine
projection (see Fig. 1). First, the meso-corticolimbic dopamine system is
associated with reward, learning, and motivation. Central regions of this
system include the ventral tegmental area (VTA), the NAcc, and the pre-
frontal cortex. Second, the nigrostriatal dopamine system (Ilango et al.,
2014;Rossi, Sukharnikova, Hayrapetyan, Yang, & Yin, 2013), which
was originally associated with motor function (Wise, 2004), originates in
the substantia nigra, where dopamine neurons primarily project to the
dorsal striatum.
Animal studies have found evidence of adolescence-specific changes in
both dopamine pathways. Focusing on the nigrostriatal pathway in peria-
dolescent rodents, for instance, Stamford (1989) found reduced basal dopa-
mine levels in adolescence. Laviola, Macrı
`, Morley-Fletcher, and Adriani
(2003) interpreted these findings as a reason for lower basal levels of moti-
vation and general underarousal, possibly resulting in typical human ado-
lescent characteristics such as boredom and dissatisfaction. Interestingly,
general underarousal is thought to lead to sensation seeking (Herpertz &
Sass, 2000). Note, however, that adolescent rodents do not generally have
less dopamine per se than adults; in fact, they have a larger dopamine sto-
rage pool (Stamford, 1989). In rewarding contexts, such as novel environ-
ments that can be explored, dopamine release is higher in adolescent than
in adult rodents (Laviola, Pascucci, & Pieretti, 2001). Consequently, the
potential amount of dopamine release is higher in adolescence than in
adulthood, but this potential can be reached only when a stimulating con-
text is given.
Sex steroids are known to modulate dopamine signaling and may thus
play an important role in regulating adolescent developmental changes in
the dopamine system (Sinclair, Purves-Tyson, Allen, & Weickert, 2014).
For instance, several studies with adolescent rodents have indicated a regu-
lating effect of testosterone on dopamine neurotransmission. Purves-Tyson
et al. (2012) investigated whether gonadectomy (the surgical removal of
testes or ovaries) or experimental augmentation of testosterone influences
271Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
dopamine levels. On the one hand, they found testosterone to be positively
associated with increases in enzymes that play an important role in dopa-
mine metabolism (e.g., catechol-O-methyltransferase [COMT], monoamine
oxidase A [MAOA], and monoamine oxidase B [MAOB]). Specifically,
these enzymes break down dopamine into its essential parts and are
involved in its inactivation. Testosterone may thus lead to inactivation of
dopamine. On the other hand, Purves-Tyson et al. (2012) also found testo-
sterone to be associated with increases in the enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase
(TH), which is an integral part of the dopamine synthesis process. Thus,
testosterone may increase local dopamine production in the substantia
nigra. Taken together, these results suggest that testosterone has multi-
faceted effects on dopamine functioning, with various behavioral conse-
quences. Specifically, the findings are in line with the idea that adolescents
generally feel underaroused due to lower dopamine levels (Laviola et al.,
2003). However, when they find themselves in novel, rewarding environ-
ments, more dopamine may be released from their larger storage pool,
making them more risk taking and impulsive.
In a second study, Purves-Tyson et al. (2014) looked at the proteins
responsible for dopamine packaging and reuptake (e.g., vesicular mono-
amine transporter [VMAT] and dopamine transporter [DAT]), as well as
dopamine receptors (DRD1D5) and investigated whether they were chan-
ged by testosterone in the nigrostriatal pathway of adolescent male rats.
Their results suggested that increased testosterone at adolescence may
change the dopamine responsivity of the nigrostriatal pathway by modulat-
ing the capacity of neurons to transport and respond to dopamine. For
instance, pubertal testosterone led to an increase in excitatory dopamine
receptors in the substantia nigra and the striatum, possibly reflecting
greater sensitivity to dopamine via testosterone exposure. Furthermore, the
general increase in dopamine receptors in the substantia nigra in response
to adolescent testosterone may mirror changes in the available dopamine.
Consequently, testosterone may lead to more midbrain dopamine signaling
during puberty, presumably modulating reward-related decision making,
such as risk taking and impulsive behavior in adolescence.
Interestingly, Purves-Tyson et al. (2014) also found differential effects of
testosterone on dopamine transportation and synthesis in the substantia
nigra versus the dorsal striatum: Whereas dopamine activity in the dorsal
striatum was increased after gonadectomy and weakened by testosterone
replacement, the opposite was true in the substantia nigra. Consequently,
these results suggest that the regulation of dopamine-related molecular
parameters by testosterone is greater in the substantia nigra than in the
272 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
dorsal striatum. This interpretation is in line with the findings of a study
by Matthews, Bondi, Torres, and Moghaddam (2013) showing that early-
adolescent male rats have decreased dopamine release and synthesis in the
dorsal striatum.
In conclusion, studies in adolescent male rodents have shown that testo-
sterone has differential effects across dopaminergic pathways, increasing
dopamine neurotransmission in the substantia nigra and decreasing dopa-
mine levels in the dorsal striatum. In order to disentangle the site-specific
effects of testosterone, it is crucial to investigate the functional outcomes of
these molecular changes. Specifically, researchers need to understand the
context dependency of the effect of testosterone, which may be due to inter-
actions with other neuromodulators and/or hormones. Another question to
be addressed is how developmental changes on a molecular level are related
to brain activity as measured by the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD)
signal. Studies with adolescent male rats give reason to believe that testos-
terone impacts the motivational system. However, this interaction is likely
to be complex and many unknowns remain, including how testosterone-
induced molecular changes in the VTA, the substantia nigra, and the dorsal
and ventral striatum influence behavior.
IMPLICATIONS
The risk taking behavior typical of adolescence results from an interaction
of biological and environmental influences. As noted throughout this
chapter, adolescents show unique developmental brain characteristics that
are associated with particular behavioral patterns. Clearly, the best way to
reduce maladaptive behavior in teenagers is not to try to change their
nature by teaching them to think differently. Telling teenagers about the
risks of smoking marijuana and drunk driving or instructing them to con-
sider the potential consequences of their actions is unlikely to change their
behavior (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). Instead, the focus should be on the
surrounding context and on ensuring that it matches teenagers’ biological
developmental state and capabilities.
For instance, pubertal hormones have shown to influence the circadian
rhythm, leading the typical 12-year-old to stay up later and sleep in later
on weekends. In combination with the use of devices like TV or computers,
this small biological change can result in sleep deprivation, which can in
turn lead to behavioral problems. Specifically, the blue light spectrum has
273Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
strong effects on the human circadian system, leading to late and restless
sleep (see Peper & Dahl, 2013). Limiting teenagers’ access to devices
absorbing blue light at night may reduce the risk of pubertal effects on the
circadian system resulting in health problems such as sleep deprivation.
Another idea would be to delay the start of the school day to 10 am, thus
ensuring that teenagers get enough sleep.
More generally, policy and regulations need to be modified to limit teen-
agers’ opportunities to engage in risky behaviors. Steinberg (2015) offers
concrete policy recommendations that focus primarily on restricting the
time that adolescents are left unsupervised.
SUMMARY AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
In this chapter, we have sought to integrate findings elucidating the specific
impact of pubertal hormones on motivational processes in adolescence.
A rapid change in gonadal hormone levels at pubertal onset has been
linked to motivational changes that may prompt increased risk taking and
impulsivity during adolescence. Mechanisms that mediate this relationship
are associated with a neuronal motivation system, in which dopamine is of
special interest. Neurobiological models of adolescent brain development
highlight the impact of pubertal hormones on reward-related regions,
resulting in strong reward-approach behavior, which is in turn hypothe-
sized to account for increased risk taking and impulsivity in adolescence.
Although the current literature does not offer unreserved support for the
hypothesis that reward-sensitive brain regions are more responsive during
adolescence, rodent studies have highlighted the impact of testosterone on
dopamine neurotransmission in the substantia nigra. However, there is a
lack of research that specifically (1) tests the relationship between pubertal
hormone levels and risky and impulsive behavior in adolescence; (2) investi-
gates the impact of pubertal hormone levels on brain regions associated
with motivational processing; (3) describes how the relationship between
pubertal hormones levels and brain regions changes over time; and
(4) explains how this change is linked to developmental changes in risk
taking and impulsivity.
To address these questions, we propose the following directions for
future research into pubertal effects on behavior and brain functioning:
First, studies should apply measures of pubertal age, ideally hormonal, as
well as assessing chronological age. Second, the integration of different
274 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
levels of analysis is essential. Studies that combine measures of behavior,
its neural correlates, and how they interact with pubertal hormones such as
testosterone in specific contexts are needed. Third, and most importantly,
assessments at multiple time points are needed to capture intraindividual
change over time. Longitudinal studies of development are imperative and
can provide insights into interactions early versus late in puberty. Finally,
gender differences must be considered, and hormones other than testoster-
one that undergo rapid changes during adolescence should also be investi-
gated (e.g., estradiol and oxytocin).
In summary, investigating developmental changes on a hormonal level
and focusing on neurotransmitters such as dopamine in the human adoles-
cent brain is a promising avenue for gaining further insights into the speci-
fic neurobiological mechanisms underlying risky and impulsive behavior in
adolescence.
REFERENCES
Albert, D., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Judgment and decision making in adolescence. Journal of
Research on Adolescence,21(1), 211224. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00724.x
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future
consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition,50(13), 715.
doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
Bjork, J. M. (2004). Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: Similarities and differ-
ences from young adults. Journal of Neuroscience,24(8), 17931802. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004
Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2010). Adolescents, adults and
rewards: Comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS One,
5(7), e11440. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011440
Blakemore, S.-J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing ado-
lescent brain. Human Brain Mapping,31(6), 926933. doi:10.1002/hbm.21052
Blakemore, S.-J., & Robbins, T. W. (2012). Decision-making in the adolescent brain. Nature
Neuroscience,15(9), 11841191. doi:10.1038/nn.3177
Booth, A., Johnson, D. R., & Granger, D. A. (1999). Testosterone and men’s health. Journal
of Behavioral Medicine,22(1), 119. doi:10.1023/A:1018705001117
Braams,B.R.,Peters,S.,Peper,J.S.,Gu
¨roˇ
glu, B., & Crone, E. A. (2014). Gambling for self,
friends, and antagonists: Differential contributions of affective and social brain regions on
adolescent reward processing. NeuroImage,100,281289. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2014.06.020
Braams, B. R., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peper, J. S., & Crone, E. A. (2015). Longitudinal
changes in adolescent risk-taking: A comprehensive study of neural responses to
rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience,
35(18), 72267238. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4764-14.2015
275Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
Buchanan,C.M.,Eccles,J.S.,&Becker,J.B.(1992).Areadolescentsthevictimsof
raging hormones: Evidence for activational effects of hormones on moods and
behavior at adolescence. Psychological Bulletin,111(1), 62107. doi:10.1037/
0033-2909.111.1.62
Campbell, B. C., Dreber, A., Apicella, C. L., Eisenberg, D. T. A., Gray, P. B., Little, A. C.,
Lum, J. K. (2010). Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in
young men. Physiology & Behavior,99(4), 451456. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011
Carre
´, J. M., Putnam, S. K., & McCormick, C. M. (2009). Testosterone responses to competition
predict future aggressive behaviour at a cost to reward in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
34(4), 561570. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.018
Casey, B. J., Duhoux, S., & Cohen, M. M. (2010). Adolescence: What do transmission, transi-
tion, and translation have to do with it? Neuron,67(5), 749760. doi:10.1016/j.
neuron.2010.08.033
Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review,
28(1), 6277. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003
Cauffman,E.,Shulman,E.P.,Steinberg,L.,Claus,E.,Banich,M.T.,Graham,S.,&
Woolard, J. (2010). Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by per-
formance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology,46(1), 193207.
doi:10.1037/a0016128
Chen, L.-H., Baker, S. P., Braver, E. R., & Li, G. (2000). Carrying passengers as a risk factor
for crashes fatal to 16- and 17-year-old drivers. JAMA,283(12), 15781582.
doi:10.1001/jama.283.12.1578
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective
engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,13(9), 636650.
doi:10.1038/nrn3313
Dahl, R. E. (2004). Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportu-
nities. Keynote address. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1021,122.
doi:10.1196/annals.1308.001
Defoe, I. N., Dubas, J. S., Figner, B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2015). A meta-analysis on age
differences in risky decision making: Adolescents versus children and adults.
Psychological Bulletin,141(1), 4884. doi:10.1037/a0038088
Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2011). The role of testosterone in social interaction.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences,15(6), 263271. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008
Ernst, M. (2014). The triadic model perspective for the study of adolescent motivated beha-
vior. Brain and Cognition,89, 104111. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.006
Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Munson, S., Eshel, N., Pine, D. S.
(2004). Choice selection and reward anticipation: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia,
42(12), 15851597. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.011
Figner, B., Mackinlay, R. J., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Affective and deliberative
processes in risky choice: Age differences in risk taking in the Columbia card task.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,35(3), 709730.
doi:10.1037/a0014983
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). Pubertal development and behavior: Hormonal activation
of social and motivational tendencies. Brain and Cognition,72(1), 6672. doi:10.1016/
j.bandc.2009.10.007
Forbes, E. E., Ryan, N. D., Phillips, M. L., Manuck, S. B., Worthman, C. M., Moyles, D. L.,
Dahl, R. E. (2010). Healthy adolescents’ neural response to reward: Associations with
276 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,49(2), 162165. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.006
Friedman, R. A. (2014). Why teenagers act crazy. The New York Times, June 28. Retrieved
from http://www.nytimes.com
Frisch, R. E. (1984). Body fat, puberty and fertility. Biological Reviews,59, 161188.
Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2006).
Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie
risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience,26(25), 68856892.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky
decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental
Psychology,41(4), 625635. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625
Goudriaan, A. E., Lapauw, B., Ruige, J., Feyen, E., Kaufman, J.-M., Brand, M., &
Vingerhoets, G. (2010). The influence of high-normal testosterone levels on risk-taking
in healthy males in a 1-week letrozole administration study. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
35(9), 14161421. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.005
Grant, V. J., & France, J. T. (2001). Dominance and testosterone in women. Biological
Psychology,58(1), 4147. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00100-4
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: In psychology and its relation to physiology, anthropology,
sociology, sex, crime, religion, and education (Vols. 1 and 2). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Individual differences in the development of sen-
sation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: Further evidence for a dual systems
model. Developmental Psychology,47(3), 739746. doi:10.1037/a0023279
Harlow, M., & Laurence, R. (2002). Growing up and growing old in ancient Rome: A life course
approach. London: Routledge.
Herpertz, S. C., & Sass, H. (2000). Emotional deficiency and psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences
and the Law,18(5), 567580. doi:10.1002/1099-0798(200010)18:5 <567::aid-bsl410 >
3.0.CO;2-8
Hertwig, R., & Erev, I. (2009). The descriptionexperience gap in risky choice. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences,13(12), 517523. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004
Ilango, A., Kesner, A. J., Keller, K. L., Stuber, G. D., Bonci, A., & Ikemoto, S. (2014).
Similar roles of substantia nigra and ventral tegmental dopamine neurons in reward
and aversion. Journal of Neuroscience,34(3), 817822. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
1703-13.2014
Josephs, R. A., Newman, M. L., Brown, R. P., & Beer, J. M. (2003). Status, testosterone, and
human intellectual performance: Stereotype threat as status concern. Psychological
Science,14(2), 158163. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01435
Laviola, G., Macrı
`, S., Morley-Fletcher, S., & Adriani, W. (2003). Risk-taking behavior in
adolescent mice: Psychobiological determinants and early epigenetic influence.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,27(12), 1931. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(03)
00006-X
Laviola, G., Pascucci, T., & Pieretti, S. (2001). Striatal dopamine sensitization to D-ampheta-
mine in periadolescent but not in adult rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior,
68(1), 115124. doi:10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00430-5
Lee, P. A. (1980). Normal ages of pubertal events among American males and females.
Journal of Adolescent Health Care,1(1), 2629. doi:10.1016/S0197-0070(80)80005-2
277Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L.,
Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon
Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,8(2),
7584. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.8.2.75
Matthews, M., Bondi, C., Torres, G., & Moghaddam, B. (2013). Reduced presynaptic dopa-
mine activity in adolescent dorsal striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology,38(7),
13441351. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.32
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics
of willpower. Psychological Review,106(1), 319. doi:10.1037/0033- 295X.106.1.3
Milchunas, D. G., Sala, O. E., & Lauenroth, W. K. (1988). A generalized model of the effects
of grazing by large herbivores on grassland community structure. The American
Naturalist,132(1), 87106. doi:10.1086/284839
Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation
of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psycho-
pathology. Psychological Medicine,35(2), 163174. doi:10.1017/S0033291704003915
O’Brien, L., Albert, D., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Adolescents prefer more immediate
rewards when in the presence of their peers. Journal of Research on Adolescence,21(4),
747753. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2011.00738.x
Op De Macks, Z. A., Moor, B. G., Overgaauw, S., Gu
¨rolu, B., Dahl, R. E., & Crone, E. A.
(2011). Testosterone levels correspond with increased ventral striatum activation in
response to monetary rewards in adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,
1(4), 506516. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.003
Peper, J. S., & Dahl, R. E. (2013). The teenage brain: Surging hormones—Brainbehavior
interactions during puberty. Current Directions in Psychological Science,22(2),
134139. doi:10.1177/0963721412473755
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pub-
ertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
17(2), 117133. doi:10.1007/BF01537962
Pfeifer, J. H., & Allen, N. B. (2012). Arrested development? Reconsidering dual-systems mod-
els of brain function in adolescence and disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,16(6),
322329. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.011
Purves-Tyson, T. D., Handelsman, D. J., Double, K. L., Owens, S. J., Bustamante, S., &
Weickert, C. (2012). Testosterone regulation of sex steroid-related mRNAs and dopa-
mine-related mRNAs in adolescent male rat substantia nigra. BMC Neuroscience,13,
95. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-13-95
Purves-Tyson, T. D., Owens, S. J., Double, K. L., Desai, R., Handelsman, D. J., & Weickert,
C. S. (2014). Testosterone induces molecular changes in dopamine signaling pathway
molecules in the adolescent male rat nigrostriatal pathway. PLoS One,9(3), e91151.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091151
Quinn, P. D., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in impulsivity and sensation seek-
ing and the escalation of substance use from adolescence to early adulthood.
Development and Psychopathology,25(1), 223239. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000284
Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2012). Dual processes in decision making and developmental
neuroscience: A fuzzy-trace model. Developmental Review,31(12), 180206.
doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.004
Richards, J. M., Plate, R. C., & Ernst, M. (2013). A systematic review of fMRI reward para-
digms used in studies of adolescents vs. adults: The impact of task design and
278 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
implications for understanding neurodevelopment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews,37(5), 976991. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.004
Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: Implications
for prevention. Developmental Psychobiology,52(3), 263276. doi:10.1002/dev.20442
Romer, D., & Hennessy, M. (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking:
The role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use.
Prevention Science,8(2), 89101. doi:10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7
Rosenblitt, J. C., Soler, H., Johnson, S. E., & Quadagno, D. M. (2001). Sensation seeking and
hormones in men and women: Exploring the link. Hormones and Behavior,40(3),
396402. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1704
Rossi, M. A., Sukharnikova, T., Hayrapetyan, V. Y., Yang, L., & Yin, H. H. (2013). Operant
self-stimulation of dopamine neurons in the substantia nigra. PLoS One,8(6), e65799.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065799
Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Worthman, C. M., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2004).
Testosterone, antisocial behavior, and social dominance in boys: Pubertal development
and biosocial interaction. Biological Psychiatry,55(5), 546552. doi:10.1016/
j.biopsych.2003.10.010
Sapolsky, R. M. (1991). Testicular function, social rank and personality among wild baboons.
Psychoneuroendocrinology,16(4), 281293. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(91)90015-L
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Weisfeld, G. E. (1989). An ethological perspective on adolescence. In
G. R. Adams, R. Montemayor, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Biology of adolescent behavior
and development (pp. 4974). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: From theories to data.
Physiological Reviews,95(3), 853951. doi:10.1152/physrev.00023.2014
Shirtcliff, E. A., Dahl, R. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2009). Pubertal development: Correspondence
between hormonal and physical development. Child Development,80(2), 327337.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.x
Sinclair, D., Purves-Tyson, T. D., Allen, K. M., & Weickert, C. S. (2014). Impacts of stress
and sex hormones on dopamine neurotransmission in the adolescent brain.
Psychopharmacology,231(8), 15811599. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3415-z
Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and beha-
vior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology,26(34), 163174. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003
Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. J. (2010). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and
motivational systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,20(2), 271277. doi:10.1016/
j.conb.2010.01.006
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,24(4), 417463. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)
00014-2
Spear, L. P. (2004). Adolescent brain development and animal models. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences,1021(1), 2326. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.002
Spear, L. P. (2010). The behavioral neuroscience of adolescence. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Stamford, J. A. (1989). Development and ageing of the rat nigrostriatal dopamine system stu-
died with fast cyclic voltammetry. Journal of Neurochemistry,52(5), 15821589.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.tb09212.x
Stanton, S. J., Liening, S. H., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2011). Testosterone is positively associated
with risk taking in the Iowa Gambling Task. Hormones and Behavior,59(2), 252256.
doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.12.003
279Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences,9(2), 6974. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral
science. Current Directions in Psychological Science,16(2), 5559. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8721.2007.00475.x
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking.
Developmental Review,28(1), 78106. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002
Steinberg, L. (2014). Age of opportunity: Lessons from the new science of adolescence.
New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Steinberg, L. (2015). How to improve the health of American adolescents. Perspectives on
Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science,10(6),
711715. doi:10.1177/1745691615598510
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age
differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development,80(1),
2844. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x
Stenstrom, E., Saad, G., Nepomuceno, M. V., & Mendenhall, Z. (2011). Testosterone and
domain-specific risk: Digit ratios (2D:4D and rel2) as predictors of recreational, finan-
cial, and social risk-taking behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences,51(4),
412416. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.003
Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at adolescence: With a general consideration of the effects of her-
editary and environmental factors upon growth and maturation from birth to maturity.
Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Vaidya, J. G., Latzman, R. D., Markon, K. E., & Watson, D. (2010). Age differences on mea-
sures of disinhibition during young adulthood. Personality and Individual Differences,
48(7), 815820. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.002
van den Bos, W. (2013). Neural mechanisms of social reorientation across adolescence. The
Journal of Neuroscience,33(34), 1358113582. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2667-13.2013
van Den Bos, W., Golka, P. J. M., Effelsberg, D., & McClure, S. M. (2013). Pyrrhic victories:
The need for social status drives costly competitive behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience,
7(7), 111. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00189
van den Bos, W., Rodriguez, C. A., Schweitzer, J. B., & McClure, S. M. (2015). Adolescent
impatience decreases with increased frontostriatal connectivity. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,112(29), E3765E3774.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1423095112
van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Huizenga, H. M., Somerville, L. H., Delgado, M. R., Powers, A.,
Weeda, W. D., Figner, B. (2015). Neural correlates of expected risks and returns in
risky choice across development. Journal of Neuroscience,35(4), 15491560. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.1924-14.2015
Van Leijenhorst, L., Moor, B. G., Op de Macks, Z. A., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Westenberg,
P. M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive devel-
opment of reward and control regions. NeuroImage,51(1), 345355. doi:10.1016/
j.neuroimage.2010.02.038
Vermeersch, H., T’Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., & Vincke, J. (2008). The role of testosterone in
aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent boys. Hormones and Behavior,
53(3), 463471. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.11.021
Vermeersch, H., T’Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., Vincke, J., & Van Houtte, M. (2010). Gender
ideology, same-sex peer group affiliation and the relationship between testosterone and
280 CORINNA LAUBE AND WOUTER VAN DEN BOS
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
dominance in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Biosocial Science,42(04), 463475.
doi:10.1017/S0021932010000106
Willoughby, T., Good, M., Adachi, P. J. C., Hamza, C., & Tavernier, R. (2013). Examining the
link between adolescent brain development and risk taking from a social-developmental
perspective. Brain and Cognition,83(3), 315324. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.008
Wingfield, J. C., Ball, G. F., Dufty, A. J., Hegner, R. E., & Ramenofsky, M. (2013).
Testosterone and aggression in birds. In P. W. Sherman, J. Alcock, P. W. Sherman, &
J. Alcock (Eds.), Exploring animal behavior: Readings from the American Scientist (6th
ed., pp. 268274). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,5(6),
483494. doi:10.1038/nrn1406
Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2007). Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence. Current
Opinion in Neurobiology,17(2), 251257. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.009
281Hormones and Affect in Adolescent Decision Making
Downloaded by MAX PLANCK INSTITUT FUER GESELLSCHAFTSFORSCHUNG, Ms Corinna Laube At 01:26 30 November 2016 (PT)
Not to be Redistributed
... However, the developmental mechanisms driving impulsivity are still poorly understood. Recent research has emphasized puberty as a key maturational process involved in impulsive behavior (Crone and Dahl, 2012;Forbes and Dahl, 2010;Laube et al., 2017; for a review see also Laube and van den Bos, 2016). Puberty is defined as the onset of adolescence and is characterized by a surge in pubertal hormones, including testosterone. ...
... It is well known that the ventral striatum plays a key role in representing subjective value in decision making (Bartra et al., 2013;Haber and Knutson, 2009;Moreira et al., 2016) and has also been shown to be selectively involved in immediate choices (McClure et al., 2004). More importantly, there have been several imaging studies in humans that also point towards a positive relationship between pubertal testosterone and the ventral striatum (for a review see Laube and van den Bos, 2016). For example, a recent longitudinal study by Braams et al. (2015) with a large sample (N ¼ 299) found that the developmental trajectory of nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activity related to receiving probabilistic rewards showed a peak in activation during adolescence. ...
... Second, human and animal studies have identified the dorsal striatum as a key target of pubertal hormones (Matthews et al., 2013;Sinclair et al., 2014;Laube and van den Bos, 2016). More specifically, animal studies have suggested that there is a decrease in dopamine function in the dorsal striatum due to a rise in pubertal testosterone in male adolescents (Matthews et al., 2013;Purves-Tyson et al., 2014;Stamford, 1989). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent self-report and behavioral studies have demonstrated that pubertal testosterone is related to an increase in risky and impulsive behavior. Yet, the mechanisms underlying such a relationship are poorly understood. Findings from both human and rodent studies point towards distinct striatal pathways including the ventral and dorsal striatum as key target regions for pubertal hormones. In this study we investigated task-related impatience of boys between 10 and 15 years of age (N = 75), using an intertemporal choice task combined with measures of functional magnetic resonance imaging and hormonal assessment. Increased levels of testosterone were associated with a greater response bias towards choosing the smaller sooner option. Furthermore, our results show that testosterone specifically modulates the dorsal, not ventral, striatal pathway. These results provide novel insights into our understanding of adolescent impulsive and risky behaviors and how pubertal hormones are related to neural processes.
... Throughout this context, the ovaries and testicles (female and male sex organs, respectively) emit substances known as sex hormones, which control 35 organs and functions in the body. They produce sex hormones such as estrogen (female sex hormone) and testosterone (male sex hormone), which are important for egg and sperm maturation as well as the development of main and secondary sex characteristics in adolescence [18]. The nervous system is a collection of organs and structures that send and receive information throughout the body while also monitoring and regulating its activity [19]. ...
... The nervous system is a collection of organs and structures that send and receive information throughout the body while also monitoring and regulating its activity [19]. Although sex hormones are primarily generated in the gonads, control over their production is governed by a number of essential structures inside the brain, a critical component of the central nervous system [18]. For example, the activities of hormones called gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) (in response to low levels of sex hormones) and inhibin (in response to high levels of sex hormones) produced by the hypothalamus (a part of the brain) and sent through a capillary network to the pituitary gland, another part of the brain, regulate the levels of sex hormones produced. ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to the growing number of sexually active adolescents worldwide, adolescent sexual development is critical. The purpose of this study was to look at the factors that influence sexual development in adolescents at Niger Delta University's Faculty of Nursing Sciences in Amassoma, Bayelsa State. Researchers used a descriptive survey design. Two research objectives were established to guide the study's course. The study's sample size was 160 adolescents chosen using a random sampling technique. The study included a self-structured questionnaire. The instrument's face and content validity were assessed. The test-retest approach was employed to determine the reliability of the questionnaire. According to the findings of this survey, adolescents have adequate awareness of sexual development because the majority of the respondents (152%) had heard about it. Friends 68(42.5%), social media 40(25%), school 32(20%), and parents 28(17.5%) were the sources of knowledge about sexuality development. Sex hormones were discovered to impact sexuality development in adolescents 130(81.25%), and estrogens and testosterones were revealed to be responsible for primary and secondary sexual characteristics in adolescents 90(56.25%). Furthermore, peer pressure impacts teenage sexuality development through counseling from peers 110(68.75%), dating relationships 120(75%), drinking and clubbing 116(72.5%), and sexual language usage 100(62.5%). However, psychosocial factor influencing sexuality development in adolescents includes; anxiety/depression 90(56.25%) self-esteem (low/high self-esteem) 83(51.88%), peer pressure 140(87.5%), parental influence 102(63.75%), alcoholism 104(65%), drug addiction 99(61.88%), pornography/masturbation 83(51.88%), religiosity 92(57.5%), culture 100(62.5%), mass media 121(75.63%) and nutrition 82(51.25%). In conclusion, the effects of hormones and psychosocial variables were identified as predictors of sexuality development in adolescents. It is Friends 68(42.5%), social media 40(25%), school 32(20%), and parents 28(17.5%) were the sources of knowledge about sexuality development. Sex hormones were discovered to impact sexuality development in adolescents 130(81.25%), and estrogens and testosterones were revealed to be responsible for primary and secondary sexual characteristics in adolescents 90(56.25%). Furthermore, peer pressure other factors have impacts on sexual development. However, it is recommended that Institutions should employ and retrain counselors through in-service training programs, capacity development seminars, and refresher courses on sexual behavior counseling among adolescents.
... Zihinsel refah bir bireyin yaşam kalitesine etki etmekle kalmayıp tüm topluma olumlu yönde etki eder ayrıca psikolojik ve fiziksel sağlık gibi önemli yaşam olaylarıyla başa çıkma şeklini etkilediği düşünüldüğü için çocuklar ve gençler için özel bir öneme sahiptir (11,13). Çocukluktan yetişkinliğe evrilme olarak bilinen ergenlik dönemi, psikoanalitik görüşe göre diğer dönemlere nazaran daha sorunlu ve zorlu bir dönem olarak bilinmektedir (14). Bu dönemde ergenler impulsif ve riskli davranışlar sergileyen bireyler olarak görülür ve bu irrasyonel ergen davranışları hormonal değişime bağlanabilmektedir (14). ...
... Çocukluktan yetişkinliğe evrilme olarak bilinen ergenlik dönemi, psikoanalitik görüşe göre diğer dönemlere nazaran daha sorunlu ve zorlu bir dönem olarak bilinmektedir (14). Bu dönemde ergenler impulsif ve riskli davranışlar sergileyen bireyler olarak görülür ve bu irrasyonel ergen davranışları hormonal değişime bağlanabilmektedir (14). Bir türbülans olarak tanımlanan bu dönemde depresyon ve kaygı bozuklukları, madde kullanım bozuklukları ve en önemlisi intihar ve ölümler görülmektedir (15,16). ...
Article
Full-text available
Amaç: Bu çalışmanın amacı psikososyal ve bedensel etkileriyle özel bir süreç olan ergenlik dönemindeki iyilik hali, mutlu olma, kendini ifade etme ile sosyal medya bağımlılığının ilişkisini araştırmaktır. Gereç ve yöntem: Tanımlayıcı, kesitsel araştırmada, kartopu tekniğiyle ulaşılan 11-18 yaş arası 384 ergen, gönüllülük temelinde demografik bilgi formu, Engagement, Perseverance, Optimism, Connectedness, Happiness (EPOCH) Ölçeği, Duyguları İfade Etme Ölçeği (DİEÖ), Oxford Mutluluk Ölçeği-Kısa Formu (OMÖ-KF) ve Sosyal Medya Bağımlılığı Ölçeği-Kısa Formunu (SMBÖ-KF) yanıtlamıştır. Bulgular: Katılımcıların ortalama yaşları 14,6±2,2 yıl, %47,9’u erkek (n=184) ve %61,5’i (n=236) özel okul öğrencisiydi. EPOCH, DİEÖ, OMÖ-KF, SMBÖ-KF ölçeklerinin Cronbach alfa değerleri sırasıyla 0,818, 0,648, 0,770 ve 0,810 bulundu. Kendisini başarısız görenlerin (%24,7; n=95) EPOCH, OMÖ-KF, DİEÖ puanları (76,2±16,9; 21,7±5,9; 67,7±12,9) başarılı görenlerden (87,9±14,2; 25,5±5,2; 71,1±11,2) düşüktü (p=0,001; p=0,001; p=0,008). DİEÖ puanı kızlarda (72,1±11,2) erkeklerden (68,2±11,9) yüksek (p=0,002), özel okulda okuyanların OMÖ-KF puanı (25,5±5,4) devlet okulunda okuyanlardan (22,9±5,6) farklıydı (p=0,001). SMBÖ-KF puanı devlet okulunda okuyanlarda (3,2±2,9) özel okulda okuyanlardan (2,5±2,3) (p=0,033), kendini başarısız görenlerde (3,8±2,8) başarılı görenlerden (2,4±2,5) (p=0,001) ve spor yapmayanlarda (3,0±2,7) spor yapanlardan (2,4±2,5) fazlaydı (p=0,035). EPOCH puanı spor yapanlarda (87,2±15,8) spor yapmayanlardan (83,9±15,6) yüksekti (p=0,038). Katılımcıların EPOCH ile SMBÖ-KF puanı arasında negatif yönde zayıf (r=-0,199, p=0,001), OMÖ ile SMBÖ-KF puanı arasında ise negatif yönde orta düzeyde korelasyon saptandı (r=-0,260, p=0,001). Sonuç: Özel okulda okuyan ergenlerin devlet okulunda okuyanlara göre daha mutlu ve daha az sosyal medya bağımlısı olması, kendini başarılı görenlerin kendini daha iyi ifade etmesi, daha mutlu olması, kendini başarısız görenlerde, devlet okullarında okuyanlarda ve spor yapmayanlarda sosyal medya bağımlılığının daha yüksek olması dikkat çekicidir.
... In rodents, there are pronounced increases in dopaminergic innervation of PFC over the course of adolescence (Naneix et al., 2012). Pubertal hormones have been shown to positively impact dopamine synthesis and signaling in a region-specific manner (Laube and van den Bos, 2016), resulting in increased reward-motivated behaviors and facilitated learning. The timely co-occurrence of dopamine increases with pubertal onset may be crucial for adolescents' exploratory behavior (Kuhn et al., 2010), which in turn may promote experiences that initiate a sensitive period for learning during adolescence (Larsen and Luna, 2018). ...
... For instance, Braams et al. (2015) showed that increased activation in the nucleus accumbens for wins over losses in a heads-or-tails gambling task scaled linearly with testosterone in a longitudinal sample of individuals between 8 and 27 years of age. As the nucleus accumbens is implicated in reward processing (McClure et al., 2004;Schultz et al., 1992), this suggests (although from a reversed-inference perspective) that training benefits, for example, may be especially moderated by rewards after pubertal onset, as well as by emotional context (see Laube and van den Bos, 2016). Similarly, Spielberg et al. (2014) found that increases in testosterone levels over two years starting at 11-12 years in girls and 12-13 years in boys predicted increased responses in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens to fearful faces two years later. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence may mark a sensitive period for the development of higher-order cognition through enhanced plasticity of cortical circuits. At the same time, animal research indicates that pubertal hormones may represent one key mechanism for closing sensitive periods in the associative neocortex, thereby resulting in decreased plasticity of cortical circuits in adolescence. In the present review, we set out to solve some of the existing ambiguity and examine how hormonal changes associated with pubertal onset may modulate plasticity in higher-order cognition during adolescence. We build on existing age-comparative cognitive training studies to explore how the potential for change in neural resources and behavioral repertoire differs across age groups. We review animal and human brain imaging studies, which demonstrate a link between brain development, neurochemical mechanisms of plasticity, and pubertal hormones. Overall, the existent literature indicates that pubertal hormones play a pivotal role in regulating the mechanisms of experience-dependent plasticity during adolescence. However, the extent to which hormonal changes associated with pubertal onset increase or decrease brain plasticity may depend on the specific cognitive domain, the sex, and associated brain networks. We discuss implications for future research and suggest that systematical longitudinal assessments of pubertal change together with cognitive training interventions may be a fruitful way toward a better understanding of adolescent plasticity. As the age of pubertal onset is decreasing across developed societies, this may also have important educational and clinical implications, especially with respect to the effects that earlier puberty has on learning.
... Peper, and Crone (2015) showed that increased activation in the nucleus accumbens for wins over losses in a heads-or-tails gambling task scaled linearly with testosterone in a longitudinal sample of individuals between 8 and 27 years of age. As the nucleus accumbens is implicated in reward processing (McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein, & Cohen, 2004;Schultz, Apicella, Scarnati, & Ljungberg, 1992), this suggests (although from a reversed-inference perspective) that training benefits, for example, may be especially moderated by, or even dependent on rewards after pubertal onset, as well as on emotional context (see Laube & van den Bos, 2016). Similarly, Spielberg, Olino, Forbes, and Dahl (2014) found that increases in testosterone levels over two years starting at 11-12 years in girls and 12-13 years in boys predicted increased responses in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens to fearful faces two years later. ...
... In rodents, there are pronounced increases in dopaminergic innervation of PFC over the course of adolescence(Naneix, Marchand, Di Scala, Pape, & Coutureau, 2012). Pubertal hormones have been shown to positively impact dopamine synthesis and signaling in a region-specific manner(Laube & van den Bos, 2016), resulting in increased reward-motivated behaviors and facilitated learning. The timely co-occurrence of dopamine increases with pubertal onset may be crucial for adolescents' exploratory behavior(Kuhn et al., 2010), which in turn may promote experiences that initiate a sensitive period for learning during adolescence(Larsen & Luna, 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Adolescence may mark a sensitive period for the development of higher-order cognition through enhanced plasticity of cortical circuits. At the same time, animal research indicates that pubertal hormones may represent one key mechanism for closing sensitive periods in the associative neocortex, thereby resulting in decreased plasticity of cortical circuits in adolescence. In the present review, we set out to solve some of the existing ambiguity and examine how hormonal changes associated with pubertal onset may modulate plasticity in higher-order cognition during adolescence. We build on existing age-comparative cognitive training studies to explore how the potential for change in neural resources and behavioral repertoire differs across age groups. We review animal and human brain imaging studies, which demonstrate a link between brain development, neurochemical mechanisms of plasticity, and pubertal hormones. Overall, the existent literature indicates that pubertal hormones play a pivotal role in regulating the mechanisms of experience-dependent plasticity during adolescence. However, the extent to which hormonal changes associated with pubertal onset increase or decrease brain plasticity may depend on the specific cognitive domain, the sex, and associated brain networks. We discuss implications for future research and suggest that systematical longitudinal assessments of pubertal change together with cognitive training interventions may be a fruitful way toward a better understanding of adolescent plasticity. As the age of pubertal onset is decreasing across developed societies, this may also have important educational and clinical implications, especially with respect to the effects that earlier puberty has on learning.
... Some of them are due to exogenous environment and some others due to endogenous personal status. Motivating and influencing parameters can be external or/internal [70,71]. Environmental factors (exogenous) such sounds, colors etc. are able to influence the decision process [72,73]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Decision-making process (DMP), our everyday most frequent action, has attracted the attention of a wide range of disciplines aiming to identify and analyze its determinants, encompassing specific steps. This paper intends to investigate whether nutrition and habits of managers/employees–via hormone levels–might statistically influence DMP in the business field. Some groups of food could encourage the secretion of specific hormones, which in turn influence the brain’s function that may in turn affect humans’ behavior and emotional status, and hence, their decision. To explore the set hypothesis, fieldwork was undertaken to an extensive random sample, from Greek companies/organizations, using appropriately designed questionnaire to select and statistically analyze related quantitative and qualitative information. The questionnaire was distributed to the employees/managers (n=242) of Greek companies. The findings confirm this hypothesis (statistical significance, p<0.05) and indicate that DMP is influenced by nutrition and habits in interaction with body mass index.
... Age-related differences in aggression have been demonstrated (36), indicating a dynamic interplay of genetic and environmental factors, particularly during perinatal and adolescent periods, which are sensitive developmental stages, and these factors establish and continuously shape the structural and functional properties of aggressive behavior within the brain network (75). Several studies have shown that during adolescence, changes in motivation occur, leading to rapid fluctuations in gonadotropin levels, which may contribute to increased risk-taking and impulsive behaviors (76). Furthermore, before reaching maturity, the sex hormone levels in adolescents significantly differ from those of adults, potentially leading to differences in decision-making behaviors, including aggression (77). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Although several studies have examined the association between estradiol and human aggression, a consistent understanding of their correlation has yet to be established. This study aimed to investigate this relationship comprehensively. Methods We systematically searched five English databases (PubMed, Web of Science, EMBASE, Cochrane Library, and CINAHL) from their inception to June 5, 2023. Two authors independently screened publications and extracted data based on predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria. Statistical analyses were performed using Review Manager 5.4, and a random-effects model was employed to pool the data. Results We identified 14 eligible studies comprising data from 1,820 participants that met the inclusion criteria. This meta-analysis indicated a positive correlation between estradiol and human aggression, albeit a weak one. The pooled Fisher's z value was 0.16 (95% CI: 0.05-0.26; I ² = 73%, P<0.00001). Furthermore, we found that participants' sex and age, the measures of aggression, and the literature quality might be sources of heterogeneity. Conclusions Human aggression exhibited a weak positive correlation with estradiol concentration, while this relationship was influenced by participants' sex and age, the measure of aggression employed, and the quality assessment of the literature. Gaining a better understanding of the association between estradiol and aggression could aid in the identification of populations prone to aggression.
... In Western societies, adolescence approximately spans the period between ages 10-24 years (including an age range sometimes referred to as emerging adulthood; Arnett, 2000;Sawyer et al., 2018;Jaworska & Mac-Queen, 2015). Puberty is characterized by a rapid rise in gonadal hormones, including testosterone and estradiol, which have a large influence on bodily characteristics, brain development, and behavior (Laube & van den Bos, 2016;Schulz & Sisk, 2016). Although the exact role of these hormones is unknown, conceptual models have hypothesized that pubertal hormones trigger the limbic brain system to flexibly recruit cortical control regions and potentially boost development of higher cognitive and self-regulatory functions important for learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many of our decisions take place under uncertainty. To successfully navigate the environment, individuals need to estimate the degree of uncertainty and adapt their behaviors accordingly by learning from experiences. However, uncertainty is a broad construct and distinct types of uncertainty may differentially influence our learning. We provide a semi-systematic review to illustrate cognitive and neurobiological processes involved in learning under two types of uncertainty: learning in environments with stochastic outcomes, and with volatile outcomes. We specifically reviewed studies ( N = 26 studies) that included an adolescent population, because adolescence is a period in life characterized by heightened exploration and learning, as well as heightened uncertainty due to experiencing many new, often social, environments. Until now, reviews have not comprehensively compared learning under distinct types of uncertainties in this age range. Our main findings show that although the overall developmental patterns were mixed, most studies indicate that learning from stochastic outcomes, as indicated by increased accuracy in performance, improved with age. We also found that adolescents tended to have an advantage compared with adults and children when learning from volatile outcomes. We discuss potential mechanisms explaining these age-related differences and conclude by outlining future research directions.
... As briefly mentioned affect is another important modulator of adolescent risk-taking. In affectively arousing (i.e., "hot") contexts, adolescents make risky decisions more often than in less arousing (i.e., "cold") contexts (Figner et al., 2009;Defoe et al., 2015;Laube and van den Bos, 2016;Rosenbaum et al., 2018). In fact, social facilitation theory as well as reward sensitivity and distraction models all imply that social behavior is influenced by arousal, which itself is often understood as affectively hot. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is a period of life during which peers play a pivotal role in decision-making. The narrative of social influence during adolescence often revolves around risky and maladaptive decisions, like driving under the influence, and using illegal substances (Steinberg, 2005). However, research has also shown that social influence can lead to increased prosocial behaviors (Van Hoorn et al., 2017) and a reduction in risk-taking (Braams et al., 2019). While many studies support the notion that adolescents are more sensitive to peer influence than children or adults, the developmental processes that underlie this sensitivity remain poorly understood. We argue that one important reason for this lack of understanding is the absence of precisely formulated models. To make a first step toward formal models of social influence during adolescence, we first identify three prominent verbal models of social influence in the literature: (1) social motivation, (2) reward sensitivity, and (3) distraction. We then illustrate how these can be translated into formal models, and how such formal models can inform experimental design and help identify developmental processes. Finally, by applying our formal models to existing datasets, we demonstrate the usefulness of formalization by synthesizing different studies with seemingly disparate results. We conclude with a discussion on how formal modeling can be utilized to better investigate the development of peer influence in adolescence.
Article
Full-text available
Significance Compared with children and adults, teens and young adults often exhibit greater impulsivity and corresponding increases in emergency room visits, accidents from drug or alcohol use, and increased mortality risk. However, it remains poorly understood how increased impulsivity during adolescence may be explained in terms of brain and cognitive development. We focused on impatience, a central component of impulsiveness. We relate impatient behavior on a decision-making task to changes in connectivity within the brain’s frontostriatal circuitry. Our results suggest that relative future orientation, not sensitivity to immediate rewards, determines adolescent impatience. These findings may help to design interventions to prevent the detrimental effects of adolescent impulsiveness and serve as a template for understanding neurodevelopmental disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Prior studies have highlighted adolescence as a period of increased risk-taking, which is postulated to result from an overactive reward system in the brain. Longitudinal studies are pivotal for testing these brain-behavior relations because individual slopes are more sensitive for detecting change. The aim of the current study was twofold: (1) to test patterns of age-related change (i.e., linear, quadratic, and cubic) in activity in the nucleus accumbens, a key reward region in the brain, in relation to change in puberty (self-report and testosterone levels), laboratory risk-taking and self-reported risk-taking tendency; and (2) to test whether individual differences in pubertal development and risk-taking behavior were contributors to longitudinal change in nucleus accumbens activity. We included 299 human participants at the first time point and 254 participants at the second time point, ranging between ages 8-27 years, time points were separated by a 2 year interval. Neural responses to rewards, pubertal development (self-report and testosterone levels), laboratory risk-taking (balloon analog risk task; BART), and self-reported risk-taking tendency (Behavior Inhibition System/Behavior Activation System questionnaire) were collected at both time points. The longitudinal analyses confirmed the quadratic age pattern for nucleus accumbens activity to rewards (peaking in adolescence), and the same quadratic pattern was found for laboratory risk-taking (BART). Nucleus accumbens activity change was further related to change in testosterone and self-reported reward-sensitivity (BAS Drive). Thus, this longitudinal analysis provides new insight in risk-taking and reward sensitivity in adolescence: (1) confirming an adolescent peak in nucleus accumbens activity, and (2) underlining a critical role for pubertal hormones and individual differences in risk-taking tendency. Copyright © 2015 the authors 0270-6474/15/357226-13$15.00/0.
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is often described as a period of increased risk taking relative to both childhood and adulthood. This inflection in risky choice behavior has been attributed to a neurobiological imbalance between earlier developing motivational systems and later developing top-down control regions. Yet few studies have decomposed risky choice to investigate the underlying mechanisms or tracked their differential developmental trajectory. The current study uses a risk-return decomposition to more precisely assess the development of processes underlying risky choice and to link them more directly to specific neural mechanisms. This decomposition specifies the influence of changing risks (outcome variability) and changing returns (expected value) on the choices of children, adolescents, and adults in a dynamic risky choice task, the Columbia Card Task. Behaviorally, risk aversion increased across age groups, with adults uniformly risk averse and adolescents showing substantial individual differences in risk sensitivity, ranging from risk seeking to risk averse. Neurally, we observed an adolescent peak in risk-related activation in the anterior insula and dorsal medial PFC. Return sensitivity, on the other hand, increased monotonically across age groups and was associated with increased activation in the ventral medial PFC and posterior cingulate cortex with age. Our results implicate adolescence as a developmental phase of increased neural risk sensitivity. Importantly, this work shows that using a behaviorally validated decision-making framework allows a precise operationalization of key constructs underlying risky choice that inform the interpretation of results. Copyright © 2015 the authors 0270-6474/15/351549-12$15.00/0.
Article
The major threats to the health of American teenagers are behavioral-risky and reckless things adolescents do that threaten their well-being and that of others. The primary approach to preventing adolescent risk taking has been classroom-based health education. Yet, most systematic research indicates that even the best programs are successful mainly at changing adolescents' knowledge but not in altering their behavior. Research on adolescent brain development has revolutionized our understanding of this stage of life, but our approach to deterring adolescent risk taking remains grounded in old, antiquated, and erroneous views of the period. Classroom-based health education is an uphill battle against evolution and endocrinology, and it is not a fight we are likely to win. Instead of trying to change teenagers into something they are not, we should try to reduce the risks they are exposed to. We should spend less money and effort trying to influence how adolescents think, and focus more on limiting opportunities for their inherently immature judgment to hurt themselves or others. Although there is evidence that some programs aimed at strengthening adolescents' self-regulation may also deter risky behavior, our public health policies should emphasize changing the context in which adolescents live, rather than solely attempting to change adolescents themselves.
Article
Sexual differentiation of the nervous system and behavior occurs through organizational effects of gonadal hormones acting during early neural development and again during puberty. In rodents, a transient elevation in testosterone around the time of birth masculinizes and defeminizes the male brain, creating structural sexual dimorphisms and programming sex-typical responses to gonadal hormones in adulthood. A second wave of sexual differentiation occurs when levels of gonadal hormones are elevated at the time of puberty. At this time, both testicular and ovarian hormones further masculinize and feminize the male and female brain, respectively, fine-tuning sex differences in adult behavior. To test the hypothesis that the peripubertal period is a sensitive period for hormone-dependent sexual differentiation that is separate and distinct from the perinatal period, exposure to testosterone was experimentally manipulated in male Syrian hamsters to occur either prepubertally, during puberty, or in young adulthood. This experiment revealed that the perinatal and peripubertal periods of masculinization of male sexual behavior are not two separate critical periods of sensitivity to organizing effects of testosterone. Instead, the two periods of masculinization are driven by the two naturally occurring elevations in gonadal hormones. To explore possible neural mechanisms of peripubertal organizational effects of gonadal hormones, cell birthdating experiments in male and female rats revealed sex differences in the addition of new cells, including both neurons and glia, to sexually dimorphic cell groups in the hypothalamus and medial amygdala. These sex differences in cell addition were positively correlated with sex differences in the volume of these cell groups. Prepubertal gonadectomy abolished sex differences in the pubertal addition of new cells. These experiments provide evidence that gonadal hormone-dependent sex differences in pubertal cytogenesis contribute to the establishment or maintenance of sexual dimorphisms in the adult brain. The transition from childhood to adulthood begins with the onset of puberty and the ensuing rise in sex steroid hormones, and it is completed by the end of adolescence. This period of development comprises extraordinary gain of function: individuals acquire the capacity to procreate, function independently within their social realm, and provide for themselves and their offspring. The metamorphosis of behavior is the product of a metamorphosis of underlying neural circuits, which necessarily occurs along different trajectories in females and males. In fact, adolescence can be regarded as a period of further sexual differentiation of brain and behavior, which is mediated in part by the actions of sex steroid hormones in the brain. This paper highlights research from my laboratory that has uncovered roles for gonadal hormones in shaping sex-specific behavioral and brain development during puberty and adolescence. I will first review experiments that establish that testicular hormones, acting during puberty, organize neural circuits underlying male social behaviors. Next I will present evidence that hormonal regulation of pubertal neuro- and gliogenesis is a potential mechanism for the establishment or maintenance of structural sex differences in the brain during adolescence.
Article
Rewards are crucial objects that induce learning, approach behavior, choices, and emotions. Whereas emotions are difficult to investigate in animals, the learning function is mediated by neuronal reward prediction error signals which implement basic constructs of reinforcement learning theory. These signals are found in dopamine neurons, which emit a global reward signal to striatum and frontal cortex, and in specific neurons in striatum, amygdala, and frontal cortex projecting to select neuronal populations. The approach and choice functions involve subjective value, which is objectively assessed by behavioral choices eliciting internal, subjective reward preferences. Utility is the formal mathematical characterization of subjective value and a prime decision variable in economic choice theory. It is coded as utility prediction error by phasic dopamine responses. Utility can incorporate various influences, including risk, delay, effort, and social interaction. Appropriate for formal decision mechanisms, rewards are coded as object value, action value, difference value, and chosen value by specific neurons. Although all reward, reinforcement, and decision variables are theoretical constructs, their neuronal signals constitute measurable physical implementations and as such confirm the validity of these concepts. The neuronal reward signals provide guidance for behavior while constraining the free will to act. Copyright © 2015 the American Physiological Society.