ArticlePDF Available

Applications of lucid dreams and their effects on the mood upon awakening

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In lucid dreams the dreamers are aware that they are dreaming and can use this state for a variety of different purposes. In an online survey, 528 respondents, of whom 386 were lucid dreamers, were asked how often have they used different applications of lucid dreams lately and how did this influence their mood upon awakening. According to the reports, wish fulfilment was the most frequent application, followed by solving waking problems, overcoming fears/ nightmares, spiritual experiences, physical/mental healing, and training motor skills, with meditation being the least popular application. Younger participants, as well as men, were more likely to engage in wish fulfilment, whereas older and more experienced lucid dreamers more used their lucid dreams for inner work (solving waking problems, physical/ mental healing, meditation). Women were more likely to use their lucid dreams for overcoming fears/nightmares and healing. All applications influenced mood upon awakening positively to neutrally, with the most positive moods being after wish fulfilment, which helps to elucidate why it is the most popular application of lucid dreams. Future longitudinal studies should examine long-term effects of different lucid dream applications.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Applications of lucid dreams
International Journal of Dream Research Volume 9, No. 2 (2016)146
D
IJoR
1. Introduction
A lucid dream is a dream in which the dreamer is aware that
he or she is dreaming and often can inuence the dream
content (LaBerge, 1985). Although lucid dreaming is con-
sidered to be a rare ability, a recent meta-analysis of lucid
dream prevalence and frequency shows that 55% of popu-
lation have experienced lucid dreaming at least once in their
lifetime and 23% experience it regularly (once a month or
more frequently) (Saunders, Roe, Smith, & Clegg, 2016). It is
a learnable ability (cf. LaBerge, 1980) and a variety of differ-
ent techniques have been suggested for lucid dream induc-
tion (Stumbrys, Erlacher, Schädlich, & Schredl, 2012).
While lucid dreamers in their lucid dreams most often seek
pleasurable activities such as ying or sex (Stumbrys, Er-
lacher, Johnson, & Schredl, 2014), a number of practical ap-
plications for lucid dreams have been suggested (LaBerge
& Rheingold, 1990). According to several case reports and
a pilot study, lucid dreams can be successfully applied for
nightmare treatment (e.g. Brylowski, 1990; Spoormaker &
van den Bout, 2006; Spoormaker, van den Bout, & Meijer,
2003; Zadra & Pihl, 1997), helping to reduce nightmare fre-
quency and intensity. Lucid dreams can also be used for
rehearsing motor skills and research supports that such
practice is effective in improving subsequent performance
in wakefulness (Erlacher & Schredl, 2010; Stumbrys, Erlach-
er, & Schredl, 2016). Further, some studies indicate that lu-
cid dreams can be successfully applied for creative problem
solving (Stumbrys & Daniels, 2010) or seeking spiritual ex-
periences (Bogzaran, 1990; Esser, 2014). Moreover, accord-
ing to some anecdotal and tentative evidence, lucid dream-
ing may be applied for physical and mental healing (e.g.
Kellogg, 1989; LaBerge & Rheingold, 1990; Tholey, 1988;
Waggoner, 2009; Zappaterra, Jim, & Pangarkar, 2014). Fi-
nally, parallels between lucid dreaming and meditation have
been observed (Gackenbach & Bosveld, 1990; Hunt & Ogil-
vie, 1988) and the lucid dream state has been used as a
tool for deepening meditation practice in traditions such as
Tibetan dream yoga (Norbu, 1992; Wangyal, 1998).
However, the extent to which lucid dreamers use their
lucid dreams for various practical purposes has not been
extensively studied. A survey of German athletes showed
that only 9% of athletes with lucid dream experience used
such dreams for their sports practice (Erlacher, Stumbrys, &
Schredl, 2011-2012). In another survey of 301 lucid dream-
ers (Schädlich & Erlacher, 2012), 81% of the respondents
indicated that they have used their lucid dreams at least
once for having fun, 64% used lucid dreams for chang-
ing nightmares, while other applications – problem solving
(30%), creativity (28%) and practicing skills (21%) were less
frequent. Yet other applications such as healing or seeking
spiritual experiences were not explored and it remains un-
certain how often the different applications are used.
The aim of the present study was to investigate how often
lucid dreamers use their lucid dreams for different purposes
and what is the effect of different applications on the mood
upon awakening, as well as to examine possible underlying
factors (age, gender, lucid dream frequency).
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Five hundred twenty eight participants (290 men and
238 women) completed an online questionnaire. Their
ages ranged from 11 to 67 years, with the mean age of
Applications of lucid dreams and their effects on
the mood upon awakening
Tadas Stumbrys1, & Daniel Erlacher2
1Vilnius University, Lithuania
2University of Bern, Switzerland
Corresponding address:
Dr. Tadas Stumbrys, Vilnius University, Department of General
Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, Universiteto str. 9/1,
01513 Vilnius, Lithuania.
Email: tadas.stumbrys@gmail.com
Submitted for publication: September 2016
Accepted for publication: September 2016
Summary. In lucid dreams the dreamers are aware that they are dreaming and can use this state for a variety of different
purposes. In an online survey, 528 respondents, of whom 386 were lucid dreamers, were asked how often have they
used different applications of lucid dreams lately and how did this inuence their mood upon awakening. According to
the reports, wish fullment was the most frequent application, followed by solving waking problems, overcoming fears/
nightmares, spiritual experiences, physical/mental healing, and training motor skills, with meditation being the least
popular application. Younger participants, as well as men, were more likely to engage in wish fullment, whereas older
and more experienced lucid dreamers more used their lucid dreams for inner work (solving waking problems, physical/
mental healing, meditation). Women were more likely to use their lucid dreams for overcoming fears/nightmares and
healing. All applications inuenced mood upon awakening positively to neutrally, with the most positive moods being
after wish fullment, which helps to elucidate why it is the most popular application of lucid dreams. Future longitudinal
studies should examine long-term effects of different lucid dream applications.
Keywords: Lucid dreaming; applications; wish fullment; nightmare treatment; motor learning; problem solving; spiritual-
ity; meditation; healing
This article has been updated!
Updated on Oct 28, 2018.
Table 2 was replaced.
International Journal of Dream Research Volume 9, No. 2 (2016) 147
D
IJoR
Applications of lucid dreams
26.4 ± 10.6 years. There were 161 working professionals,
152 students, 125 schoolchildren, 34 in vocational training,
8 housewives/-husbands, 4 retired, 20 unemployed and 5
at military or civilian service (18 participants marked “other
occupation” and 1 participant did not provide information).
2.2. Materials
In addition to biographical data (age, gender, occupation),
the questionnaire included items about lucid dreams, their
applications and effects on the waking mood. The partici-
pants were asked to estimate their lucid dream frequency
on an eight-point scale (0 – never; 1 - less than once a year;
2 - about once a year; 3 - about 2 to 4 times a year; 4 - about
once a month; 5 - about 2 to 3 times a month; 6 - about
once a week; 7 - several times a week). Re-test reliability for
the scale was found to be high (r=.89; p<.001; N=93; Stum-
brys, Erlacher, & Schredl, 2013). In order to obtain units in
frequency per month, the scale was recoded using the class
means: 0 0, 1 0.042, 2 0.083, 3 0.25, 4 1.0,
5 2.5, 6 4.0, 7 18.0. To ensure a clear understand-
ing of lucid dreaming, a short denition was provided: “In a
lucid dream, one is aware that one is dreaming during the
dream. Thus it is possible to wake up deliberately, or to in-
uence the action of the dream actively, or to observe the
course of the dream passively”.
Further, those participants who had lucid dreams were
asked to indicate (in percentages, summing up to 100%) for
what purpose have they used their lucid dreams recently.
Seven categories were provided (as well as one additional
open-ended where other applications could be specied):
(1) wish fullment (e.g. ying, dancing, laughing, having
sex); (2) training motor skills (e.g. practicing sports or play-
ing a musical instrument; (3) solving waking problems (e.g.
asking dream characters for advice); (4) overcoming fears
or nightmares (e.g. confronting frightening dream charac-
ters); (5) for spiritual experiences (e.g. feeling at one with the
environment); (6) meditating (e.g. applying certain medita-
tion techniques); (7) alleviating or healing physical or mental
problems (e.g. relieving physical pain, handling grief). For
all the categories, the participants were asked to specify
how did that particular application inuence their mood
upon awakening from the lucid dream on a ve-point scale
(1 – very positive; 2 – positive; 3 – neutral; 4 – negative;
5 – very negative).
2.3. Procedure
The study was conducted in German. The online question-
naire was posted on the German website on lucid dream-
ing http://www.klartraum.de between August 22, 2007, and
January 8, 2008. The newsletter with an explicit reference
to the study was sent by email to approx. 1500 registered
users of the website. The survey was anonymous, however
participants were asked to provide their email address in
order to minimize the risk of multiple responses to the ques-
tionnaire. To answer the questionnaire, the participants had
as much time as they needed.
2.4. Statistical analysis
SPSS (Version 17) was used for statistical analysis. For each
lucid dream application (e.g. wish fullment) a logistic re-
gression analysis with the frequency of the lucid dream ap-
plication as the dependent variable was calculated to inves-
tigate possible inuencing factors of age, gender and lucid
dream frequency as independent variables.
3. Results
Three hundred eighty-six respondents (73.1%) reported
that they had at least one lucid dream. Two hundred sixty-
three respondents (49.8%) had at least one lucid dream per
month and following Snyder and Gackenbach (1988) can be
classied as frequent lucid dreamers. In average, the par-
ticipants estimated to recall 3.95 ± 5.94 lucid dreams per
month (whole sample: 2.93 ± 5.40, 521 responses).
Wish fullment was the most popular application of lucid
dreams, employed in over 40% of lucid dreams. The second
most popular application was problem solving, followed by
overcoming fears/nightmares and spiritual experiences,
while meditation was the least popular (Table 1). Among oth-
er applications participants most often mentioned exploring
the dream space (e.g. visiting unknown dreamscapes) and
performing experiments in the dream (e.g. tasting, smelling
things in dreams). According to the participants’ reports,
they also attempted to increase the number of lucid dreams,
to increase the level of control in their lucid dreams and to
stabilize the lucid dream. For all applications, the inuence
on mood upon awakening was positive/neutral, with most
positive moods after wish fullment and most neutral moods
after meditation (Table 1).
Regression analyses revealed several differences of lucid
dream applications in relation to age, gender and lucid dream
frequency. Younger participants were more likely to use their
lucid dreams for wish fullment, whereas older participants
more employed their lucid dreams for solving waking prob-
lems, meditation and physical/mental healing (Table 2). Men
were more likely to use lucid dreams for wish fullment and
meditation, while women for overcoming fears/nightmares
and physical/mental healing. More frequent lucid dreamers
more often used their lucid dreams for solving waking prob-
lems, meditation and physical/mental healing and less for
overcoming fears/nightmares (Table 2).
4. Discussion
This survey gathered data on lucid dream applications and
their effects on the mood upon awakening. The most fre-
quent application of lucid dreaming was wish fullment,
especially for younger participants, while older participants
and more frequent lucid dreamers were more likely to try
other applications, such as solving waking problems, heal-
ing, meditation. Men more tended to use their lucid dreams
for wish fullment and meditation, whereas women for over-
coming fears/nightmares and healing. All applications inu-
enced mood upon awakening more positively/neutrally, with
most positive moods being after wish fullment.
Before discussing the ndings, some limitations of the
present study have to be acknowledged. The majority
of the participants were lucid dreamers (73%) and half of
them (50%) were frequent lucid dreamers. These propor-
tions are much higher than in general population (55% and
23%, respectively, Saunders et al., 2016), but comparable
to other online surveys of lucid dreamers (e.g. Stumbrys et
al., 2014). Further, the participants were self-selected due to
their interest in lucid dream research. Therefore the general-
izations of these ndings should be cautious. Furthermore,
the data were collected via an online questionnaire, which
Applications of lucid dreams
International Journal of Dream Research Volume 9, No. 2 (2016)148
D
IJoR
might have had some effect on the quality of the responses,
although comparative analyses show that data gathered via
the Internet are at least as good as data gathered via tradi-
tional methods and do not appear tainted by false respons-
es (Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004).
The nding that wish fullment is the most popular ap-
plication of lucid dreams, supports the classical Freud’s
(1913) idea of dreams being a fullment of wishes, and is
in line with previous studies which showed that most lucid
dreamers use their lucid dreams for having fun (Schädlich
& Erlacher, 2012) and that ying and sex are among the
most popular actions planned for lucid dreams (Stumbrys
et al., 2014). Two next popular categories – solving wak-
ing problems and overcoming fears/nightmares – resemble
ndings by Schädlich & Erlacher (2012), where changing
nightmares and problem solving were also the second-third
applications by popularity, and their effectiveness is backed
by some preliminary research (e.g. Spoormaker & van den
Bout, 2006; Stumbrys & Daniels, 2010). Spiritual experi-
ences and physical/mental healing were not included in the
previous research and while they seem to be somewhat less
frequently used, their effects and phenomenology warrant
further research. Training motor skills appears to be rarely
used application (cf. Erlacher et al., 2011-2012), although re-
search supports the effectiveness of such training (Erlacher
& Schredl, 2010; Stumbrys et al., 2016). Therefore perhaps
more publicity is needed to ag up such potentials of lucid
dreaming, especially in specic populations where this is
most applicable (e.g. athletes, musicians). Meditation, while
being the least popular application of lucid dreams, is nev-
ertheles another interesting area that needs further investi-
gation. Numerous studies showed positive effects of medi-
tation on physical and psychological health and well-being
(reviews: Arias, Steinberg, Banga, & Trestman, 2006; Goyal
et al., 2014). According to Tibetan dream yoga, meditation
practice carried out in the lucid dream state is even more
effective than in the waking state (Norbu, 1992). Therefore it
would be interesting to study the effects of meditation car-
ried out while lucid dreaming and to compare their physi-
ological and phenomenological aspects.
The age differences found regarding lucid dream appli-
cations – that younger lucid dreamers were more likely to
seek wish fullment, while the older ones were more like
to engage into more serious applications (such as solving
Table 1. Applications of lucid dreams and their effects on the mood upon awakening for a total of n = 357 respondents
Application % Effect on mood*
N M SD N M SD
Wish fullment 297 42.8 33.7 290 1.8 0.8
Solving waking problems 205 14.5 19.7 200 1.9 0.8
Overcoming fears/nightmares 167 10.8 19.3 164 2.2 0.9
Spiritual experiences 153 8.1 14.8 153 2.1 0.9
Physical/mental healing 142 6.5 11.4 136 2.2 0.9
Training motor skills 110 4.2 9.7 109 2.3 0.7
Meditation 39 1.3 4.1 27 2.3 1.1
Other 94 12.0 25.4 89 1.8 0.8
Note. *1 – very positive; 2 – positive; 3 – neutral; 4 – negative; 5 – very negative
Corrected Table 2. Regression analysis for lucid dream applications with age, gender and lucid dream (LD) frequency as
independent variables.
Age Gender LD frequency
Wish fullment β=-0.25; t=-4.91*** β=-0.11; t=-2.05* β=0.03; t=0.60
Training motor skills β=0.03; t=0.49 β=-0.03; t=-0.56 β=-0.05; t=-0.84
Solving waking problems β=0.29; t=5.70*** β=0.05; t=0.95 β=0.11; t=2.13*
Overcoming fears/nightmares β=-0.02; t=-0.41 β=0.19; t=3.55*** β=-0.11; t=-2.09*
Spiritual experiences β=0.06; t=1.16 β=0.01; t=0.22 β=-0.01; t=-0.14
Meditation β=0.14; t=2.69** β=-0.16; t=-3.01** β=0.11; t=2.09*
Physical/mental healing β=0.11; t=2.00* β=0.11; t=2.12* β=0.13; t=2.43*
Other β=0.01; t=0.20 β=-0.06; t=-1.11 β=-0.10; t=-1.78
Note. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
International Journal of Dream Research Volume 9, No. 2 (2016) 149
D
IJoR
Applications of lucid dreams
waking problems, meditation, physical/mental healing)
are also in line with previous research, as well as gender
differences that men were more likely to seek wish full-
ment while women to overcome nightmares (Schädlich &
Erlacher, 2012). Generally, women have more nightmares
than men (Schredl & Reinhard, 2011) and perhaps working
with nightmares in lucid dreams may facilitate more general
self-work with mental and physical healing in lucid dreams,
in which, according to the present results, women are also
more likely to engage. The nding that men were more likely
to use their lucid dreams for meditation is somewhat unex-
pected, as generally women seem to engage in meditation
more often than men (e.g. Barnes, Bloom, & Nahin, 2008).
Similarly to older participants, more frequent lucid dream-
ers also tend to engage more into an inner work in their
lucid dreams (i.e. solving waking problems, physical/mental
healing, meditation), which is likely to reect their growing
maturity with the development of the lucid dream ability .
On the other hand, they seem to be less using their lucid
dreams for overcoming fears/nightmares, which perhaps
may be considered as preliminary “shadow” work in Jung-
ian terms (Jacobi, 1973) to be accomplished in the earlier
stages of lucid dreaming (lucid dreams quite often originate
from nightmares, see Stumbrys et al., 2014).
The effects on the waking mood for all applications were
perceived as more positive or neutral. Wish fullment ap-
pears to lead to the most positive emotions upon awaken-
ing, which may explain why it is the most popular applica-
tion of lucid dreams. Generally, lucid dreams contain more
positive emotions than non-lucid dreams (Thomas, Pollak,
& Kahan, 2015). The succesful application of a lucid dream
for a particular purpose may give a further boost of positive
emotions, which will inuence the mood after awakening (cf.
Schredl & Reinhard, 2009).
Future studies should explore long-term effects of differ-
ent lucid dream applications, for example, to see if wish ful-
llment in lucid dreams leads to increases in overall daytime
mood, whether solving waking problems or working with
nightmares and fears in lucid dreams help to cope bettter
with them while awake and to reduce related daytime dis-
tress. Further, if seeking physical/mental healing or spiritual
experiences through lucid dreams can contribute to better
physical, mental and spiritual health. To investigate this, lon-
gitudinal studies would be especially useful.
In conclusion, the present ndings support the notion
that lucid dreamers, especially the younger ones, most of-
ten use their lucid dreams for wish fullment. With the ad-
vancing age and more frequent lucid dream experience,
lucid dreamers are more starting to use their lucid dream-
ers for inner work, such as solving waking problems, physi-
cal/mental healing and meditation. Meditation and training
motor skills, however, were found to be the least frequently
used lucid dream applications. Practical applications of
lucid dreams have positive to neutral effects on the mood
upon awakening. Among them, wish fullment gives the
most positive boost on the waking mood, which elucidates
why it is the most popular application of lucid dreams. Fu-
ture longitudinal studies should examine long-term effects
of different lucid dream applications.
References
Arias, A. J., Steinberg, K., Banga, A., & Trestman, R. L. (2006).
Systematic review of the efcacy of meditation tech-
niques as treatments for medical illness. The Journal of
Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 12(8), 817–
832. http://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2006.12.817
Barnes, P. M., Bloom, B., & Nahin, R. L. (2008). Complemen-
tary and alternative medicine use among adults and
children: United States, 2007. National Health Statistics
Reports, (12), 1–23.
Bogzaran, F. (1990). Experiencing the Divine in the Lucid Dream
State. Lucidity Letter, 9(1), 169–176.
Brylowski, A. (1990). Nightmares in crisis: clinical applications
of lucid dreaming techniques. Psychiatric Journal of the
University of Ottawa, 15(2), 79–84.
Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2010). Practicing a motor task in a
lucid dream enhances subsequent performance: A pilot
study. The Sport Psychologist, 24(2), 157–167.
Erlacher, D., Stumbrys, T., & Schredl, M. (2011-2012). Frequen-
cy of lucid dreams and lucid dream practice in German
athletes. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 31(3),
237–246. http://doi.org/10.2190/IC.31.3.f
Esser, T. (2014). Kundalini and non-duality in the lucid dreaming
state. In R. Hurd & K. Bulkeley (Eds.), Lucid Dreaming:
New Perspectives on Consciousness in Sleep (Vol. 2,
pp. 233–263). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
Freud, S. (1913). The Interpretation of Dreams. (A. A. Brill,
Trans.). New York: The Macmillan Company.
Gackenbach, J., & Bosveld, J. (1990). Control Your Dreams.
New York: HarperPerennial.
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004).
Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative
analysis of six preconceptions about internet ques-
tionnaires. The American Psychologist, 59(2), 93–104.
http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.93
Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M. S., Gould, N. F., Rowland-
Seymour, A., Sharma, R., … Haythornthwaite, J. A.
(2014). Meditation programs for psychological stress
and well-being. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174(3), 357.
http://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018
Hunt, H. T., & Ogilvie, R. D. (1988). Lucid dreams in their natural
series: Phenomenological and psychological ndings
in relation to meditative states. In J. Gackenbach & S.
LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain: Per-
spectives on Lucid Dreaming (pp. 389–417). New York:
Plenum Press.
Jacobi, J. (1973). The Psychology of C. G. Jung. New Haven:
Yale University Press.
Kellogg, E. W. (1989). A personal experience in lucid dream
healing. Lucidity Letter, 8(1).
LaBerge, S. (1980). Lucid Dreaming as a Learnable Skill: A Case
Study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51(3 Pt 2), 1039–
1042.
LaBerge, S. (1985). Lucid dreaming. The power of being awake
and aware in your dreams. Los Angeles: Tarcher.
LaBerge, S., & Rheingold, H. (1990). Exploring the World of Lu-
cid Dreaming. New York: Ballantine Books.
Norbu, N. (1992). Dream Yoga and the Practice of Natural Light.
(M. Katz, Ed.). Ithaca, New York: Snow Lion Publica-
tions.
Saunders, D. T., Roe, C. A., Smith, G., & Clegg, H. (2016). Lucid
dreaming incidence: A quality effects meta-analysis of
50 years of research. Consciousness and Cognition, 43,
197–215. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.06.002
Schädlich, M., & Erlacher, D. (2012). Applications of lucid
dreams: An online study. International Journal of Dream
Research, 5(2), 134–138.
Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2009-2010). The continuity between
waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-
order effects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,
29(3), 271–282. http://doi.org/10.2190/IC.29.3.f
Applications of lucid dreams
International Journal of Dream Research Volume 9, No. 2 (2016)150
D
IJoR
Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2011). Gender differences in
nightmare frequency: A meta-analysis. Sleep Medi-
cine Reviews, 15(2), 115–21. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
smrv.2010.06.002
Snyder, T., & Gackenbach, J. (1988). Individual differences as-
sociated with lucid dreaming. In J. Gackenbach & S.
LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping Brain: Per-
spectives on Lucid Dreaming (pp. 221–259). New York:
Plenum Press.
Spoormaker, V. I., & van den Bout, J. (2006). Lucid dreaming
treatment for nightmares: a pilot study. Psychother-
apy and Psychosomatics, 75(6), 389–94. http://doi.
org/10.1159/000095446
Spoormaker, V. I., van den Bout, J., & Meijer, E. J. G. (2003).
Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: A series of
cases. Dreaming, 13(3), 181–186.
Stumbrys, T., & Daniels, M. (2010). An exploratory study of cre-
ative problem solving in lucid dreams: Preliminary nd-
ings and methodological considerations. International
Journal of Dream Research, 3(2), 121–129. http://doi.
org/10.11588/ijodr.2010.2.6167
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Johnson, M., & Schredl, M. (2014).
The phenomenology of lucid dreaming: An online sur-
vey. American Journal of Psychology, 127(2), 191–204.
http://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0191
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., & Schredl, M. (2012).
Induction of lucid dreams: A systematic review of evi-
dence. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1456–
1475. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.07.003
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2013). Reliability and
stability of lucid dream and nightmare frequency scales.
International Journal of Dream Research, 6(2), 53–56.
http://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2013.2.11137
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2016). Effectiveness
of motor practice in lucid dreams: a comparison with
physical and mental practice. Journal of Sports Sci-
ences, 34(1), 27–34. http://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.
2015.1030342
Tholey, P. (1988). A model for lucidity training as a means of
self-healing and psychological growth. In J. Gacken-
bach & S. LaBerge (Eds.), Conscious Mind, Sleeping
Brain: Perspectives on Lucid Dreaming (pp. 263–287).
New York: Plenum Press.
Thomas, S., Pollak, M., & Kahan, T. L. (2015). Subjective quali-
ties of dreams with and without awareness. Dreaming,
25(3), 173–189. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0039242
Waggoner, R. (2009). Lucid Dreaming: Gateway to the Inner
Self. Needham, MA: Moment Point Press.
Wangyal, T. (1998). The Tibetan Yogas of Dream and Sleep.
New York: Snow Lion Publications.
Zadra, A. L., & Pihl, R. O. (1997). Lucid dreaming as a treatment
for recurrent nightmares. Psychotherapy and Psycho-
somatics, 66(1), 50–55.
Zappaterra, M., Jim, L., & Pangarkar, S. (2014). Chronic pain
resolution after a lucid dream: A case for neural plas-
ticity? Medical Hypotheses, 82(3), 286–290. http://doi.
org/10.1016/j.mehy.2013.12.011
... In this sense, this ability to recognise and exert control over oneiric activity may contribute not only to a reduction in nightmare frequency but also to improvements in nightmare disorder and related sleep disturbances (de Macêdo et al., 2019;Hess et al., 2017). Moreover, lucid dreaming has also been associated with a range of other potential benefits, including problem-solving, wish fulfillment, facilitating spiritual experiences and meditation, and promoting physical and mental healing (Schädlich & Erlacher, 2012;Stumbrys & Erlacher, 2016). Also, the positive relationship between poorer sleep quality and lucid dream frequency is consistent with existing literature. ...
Article
Full-text available
The gestational period is a sensitive time marked by significant changes that can affect women's sleep and dreaming processes, with an augmented frequency and recall of dreams suggesting that dreaming represents an adaptive mechanism of emotional regulation. This study investigates the relationship between pregnancy‐related variables, alexithymia, and depressive symptoms in influencing dream characteristics in women during the first trimester of pregnancy. A total of 118 pregnant women were recruited at the Obstetric Outpatient Service of an Italian University Hospital and completed the Mannheim Dream Questionnaire, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale‐20, and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Regression analysis, t ‐test, and moderation analysis were conducted through Jamovi. Dream recall frequency was predicted by age, parity, and depressive symptoms. Nightmare frequency and lucid dream frequency were significantly predicted by depressive symptoms, while nightmare distress was predicted by an unplanned pregnancy. Alexithymia was linked to higher nightmare frequency and nightmare distress. Moderation analysis revealed that the presence of depressive symptoms predicted increased nightmare frequency only in women with higher levels of alexithymia. These findings highlight the role of emotional regulation in dreaming during pregnancy, particularly among women exhibiting alexithymic traits and depressive symptoms. Nightmare frequency may serve as an indicator of impaired emotional regulation, emphasising the need for targeted interventions to enhance emotional coping strategies in this population. Future research should examine the content of nightmares to further understand their implications for maternal mental health.
... highlighted the correlation between lucid dreaming expertise and higher scores in some dimensions of the Mysticism Scale (Hood, 1975), a psychometric scale used to assess the experience of mystical states (Stace, 1886(Stace, /1960. Previous work from Stumbrys & Erlacher (2016) has found that some people follow lucid dreaming practices to have spiritual-like experiences. Thus, we could place lucid dreaming within this dimension of Self-Revelation, situating it closer to more paradigmatic cases of "transcendental experience". ...
Article
Full-text available
I introduce a novel multidimensional framework tailored to investigate a set of phenomena that might appear intractable and render them amenable to scientific inquiry. In particular, I focus on examining altered states of consciousness that appear to the experiencing subject as “contentless” or “objectless” states in some form, either by having disrupted or reduced content of awareness, or content that appears as missing altogether. By drawing on empirical research, I propose a cluster of phenomenological dimensions aimed at enhancing our understanding of this group of experiences of minimal awareness, including Richness of the Content, Bodily-Awareness, Passage of Time, Attentional Focus, and Self-Revelation. The result is a robust framework that provides precise scientific terminology, facilitating its operationalisation and adaptability for future empirical work. I show the explanatory power of this framework through a case study of the state of clear light sleep—a state widely reported by Indo-Tibetan Buddhist traditions as an instance of pure awareness during sleep, a state of consciousness-as-such. This rare phenomenon has gained increasing attention in contemporary analytic philosophy of mind for the study of the nature of consciousness. However, we still lack consensus on its characterisation and potential identity with other associated states. I end the paper by illustrating how the state of clear light sleep can be situated in relation to other associated states and more ordinary ones as regions of a multidimensional state space.
Article
Full-text available
During most dreams, the dreamer does not realize that they are in a dream. In contrast, lucid dreaming allows to become aware of the current state of mind, often accompanied by considerable control over the ongoing dream episode. Lucid dreams can happen spontaneously or be induced through diverse behavioural, cognitive or technological strategies. Such induction techniques have spurred research into the potential therapeutic aspects of lucid dreams. In this review, we gather evidence on the link between lucid dreams and conditions like nightmare disorder, depression, anxiety, psychosis, and dissociative states, and explore the possible neurobiological basis of these associations. Furthermore, we delve into contemplative sleep practices that train lucid states during sleep, such as Dream/Sleep Yoga and Yoga Nidrâ. The potential drawbacks of lucid dreaming interventions are outlined, accompanied by an examination of the impacts of lucid dreams on individuals without clinical conditions. By shedding light on these intricate relationships, the review contributes to a deeper understanding of the therapeutic possibilities and implications of lucid dreaming.
Article
Objectives: This study aimed to investigate relationships between lucid dreaming and sleep and mental health outcomes within a representative sample of the general population. We also sought to examine how nightmares interact with the relationship between lucid dreaming, sleep, and mental health outcomes. Methods: Participants (N = 1332) completed measures of lucid dream frequency, nightmare frequency, anxiety and depressive symptoms, stress, and sleep quality. Hierarchical regression models were conducted, where step-1 examined the direct effect of lucid dreaming on all outcome variables, and step-2 added nightmares into the equation (to examine direct effects of lucid dreaming frequency and nightmares, and the indirect effect of nightmares). Results: Step-1 results demonstrated that lucid dreaming positively predicted poor sleep quality, stress, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. Step-2 results revealed that nightmares were the only significant predictor of poor sleep quality, stress, and anxiety symptoms, accounting for all of the variance of lucid dreaming. Nightmares and the interaction of nightmares and lucid dreaming positively predicted depressive symptoms. Conclusions: Our results suggest that nightmares alone explain associations between lucid dreaming and poor sleep quality, anxiety symptoms, and stress. However, both nightmares and the combination of nightmares and lucid dreaming are associated with increased depressive symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
In addition to being aware of being in a dream, dreamers in a lucid dream can sometimes also intentionally execute predetermined actions and manipulate the dream’s narrative. These features open the opportunity to use lucid dreaming (LD) as a recreational, therapeutic and skill acquisition tool. This study conducted semistructured interviews with nine adolescent lucid dreamers to gain insight into their LD experiences and the role these play in their waking lives. Reflexive thematic analysis revealed that LD provided participants with a space where they felt safe to explore movements and behaviors without the repercussions they would experience if they were awake. During LD they trained motor skills, addressed emotional conflicts, prepared for difficult waking life situations, and engaged in fun activities. These experiences promoted motor learning, self-efficacy, personal growth, and mental well-being. This study also highlighted that training motor skills in a lucid dream may at times heighten waking self-efficacy but not necessarily motor performance. LD brought participants a sense of freedom, liberation, and empowerment, alongside positive emotions, and a temporary release from the burdens of their waking lives. Advanced dream manipulation skills may not be mandatory for benefiting from LD, suggesting that LD may be a valuable tool for novice lucid dreamers and those with limited dream manipulation. Adolescence is a period marked by self-evaluations and the development of one’s own identity. LD may be a valuable tool to support these developmental processes and adolescents’ well-being.
Article
During lucid dreaming (LD), dreamers are aware of experiencing a dream and may consciously influence its content. This study used an online questionnaire to investigate the LD frequency and applications in 193 adolescent athletes and non-athletes (17.40 ± 2.09 years; 46% athletes, 54% controls). Given the critical role that sleep plays in adolescent health, development, and performance, associations of LD with various sleep parameters were also explored. LD is prominent in adolescents (67.4% experienced it at least once, 30.0% once a month or more, 12.9% at least once a week), but similar in terms of frequency and uses between athletes and non-athletes. A higher proportion of those who practiced sports/dance during LD reported improved waking self-efficacy (57.1%) over sport performance (42.9%). There was no indication that chronotype preference may influence LD nor that LD may be detrimental to adolescent sleep. Athletes and controls had similar sleep durations, daytime sleepiness, and sleep disturbances frequency, but athletes reported higher sleep quality. Despite, on average, meeting the minimum sleep recommendation guidelines for their age, a relatively large proportion of adolescents did not attain sufficient sleep, particularly on weeknights (47.4% 14–17 years; 20.0% 18–21 years), suggesting that restricted sleep remains prevalent in adolescent populations
Preprint
Full-text available
Smart home systems facilitate humans' domestic environments and enhance their quality of life. Voice control of devices is usually available right after one awakens in the morning. In this study, we tested a way to operate smart home technologies even before awakening with the help of electromyographical (EMG) signals from dreams. A software program was tuned to translate and digitally vocalize EMG spikes into commands for the voice control of devices. One advanced lucid dreaming practitioner was trained to use these commands to operate lights, electric kettle, and a radio. The goal was to achieve the same tasks in LD under polysomnographic observation in laboratory conditions. LDs was verified by pre-agreed eye movements in REM sleep. The participant in three confirmed LDs was able to control smart home units despite sleep paralysis. It was possible to control light, the electric kettle, and radio in real-time while still being in REM sleep. The experiment demonstrated a new practical application of LD-specifically, technological everyday life tasks could be performed while asleep. In the future, the same tasks could be performed directly (without voice commands) by transmitting EMG, electroencephalography, and electrooculography patterns from LDs. With development, these technologies may open new perspectives for LD application like online shopping, entertaining, education, and jobs.
Article
Full-text available
Lucid dreams (LDs) can be characterized by the presence of metacognition during dreams. LDs may have initially low perception vividness. In this study, we compare the effectiveness of eight techniques for increasing perception vividness, or deepening techniques (DTs), in LDs and the effectiveness of the general principles on which these DTs are based. In separate online experiments, volunteers were asked to perform each of the eight DTs during LDs (one DT per LD) and then evaluate how much perception vividness changed. A total of 858 reports were received. The most effective DT was palpation in LDs, both in general (88% of successful reports, p value < .001) and for achieving hyper-vivid perceptions (25% of all reports). The most ineffective DT was falling upside down (46% of successful reports, p value = .458). The most effective DT types were the suggestion (75% of successful reports, p value < .001) and sensory DT types (73% of successful reports, p value < .001). The least effective DT type was the vestibular DT type (53% of successful reports, p value = .411). A significant difference was found between the best and worst DTs. The results can be useful for LD practitioners. Further research should compare all other DTs and identify patterns among them to gain a better understanding of REM sleep and aid further LD research.
Article
Full-text available
Objective. In lucid dreams (LDs), it is possible to maintain consciousness and achieve predetermined goals while in REM sleep. Since the 1980s, scientists have been searching for LD application areas. The current study is the first to demonstrate a method for controlling a car avatar with feedback from LDs. Support for this possibility could be used for personal applications and LD studies. Method. Five experienced LD practitioners participated in the present study. Electromyographical (EMG) sensors were used to detect leg and arm muscle movements, and red diodes were put in front of participants’ eyes to indicate obstacles. Software converted EMG impulses into movements of a virtual car, which was chosen as the avatar. The study was conducted under laboratory conditions with polysomnographic LD verification (pre-agreed eye signals were recorded during REM sleep). Results. The participants spent one to four nights in the laboratory and induced 18 confirmed LDs. In 12 LDs, they drove a virtual car, successfully avoided obstacles, or both. All results were observed in real-time and recorded by the research team. Discussion. The results show an achievable way to control the functions of a computer program with feedback from LDs. In theory, many more options for controlling and feedback besides EMG sensors and diodes could be used. With development, all these technologies could help control more sophisticated objects in virtual and physical realms. The two-way communication from LDs with computers observed in this study could facilitate new studies because people could correct the settings and functions of devices by themselves from LDs. This would also foster communication between people while asleep.
Article
Full-text available
In a lucid dream the dreamer is aware of the dream state and can influence the dream content and events. The goal of this study was to investigate some applications of lucid dreaming. Our survey included 301 lucid dreamers who filled out an online questionnaire. The most frequent application (81.4%) was having fun, followed by changing a bad dream or nightmare into a pleasant one (63.8%), solving problems (29.9%), getting creative ideas or insights (27.6%) and practicing skills (21.3%). Women used lucid dreams significantly more often than men for both work on nightmares and problem solving. Our results show that lucid dreams have a great potential for improving one’s life in different ways. More research is needed to illuminate the possibilities of lucid dreaming, especially in the fields of nightmare treatment and practicing motor skills.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the subjective qualities of dreams reported as including some awareness of dreaming (DWA) with matched dreams reported as including no awareness of dreaming (Dw/oA). Twenty-eight pairs of DWA and Dw/oA were selected from a large set of dreams (N = 788) reported and rated by 144 college women during a 2-week dream journal study (Kahan & Claudatos, 2015). In that study, participants used the Subjective Experiences Rating Scale (SERS; Kahan, 1994) to rate the prevalence of particular features (sensory, affective, cognitive, structural) of dreams reported on morning awakening. Dependent groups t tests indicated that DWA included a higher prevalence of positive emotion than Dw/oA (p = < .001). Correlational analyses revealed a stronger relationship among subjective qualities for DWA than for Dw/oA. The strong positive relationship observed for DWA, but not Dw/oA, between cognition and the prevalence of negative emotion is of particular theoretical interest.
Article
Full-text available
The author was the subject in an investigation of the feasibility of learning to dream lucidly, i.e., while knowing that one is dreaming. During the 3-yr. study, the subject recorded a total of 389 lucid dreams and developed a mnemonic technique for the voluntary induction of lucid dreams (MILD). Without using any induction procedure, the subject reported less than 1 lucid dream per month. Using auto-suggestion resulted in a range of 1 to 13 lucid dreams per month (M = 5.4), with at most 2 per night. MILD yielded 18 to 26 lucid dreams per month (M = 21.5), with up to 4 per night.
Article
Full-text available
Motor practice in lucid dreams is a form of mental rehearsal where the dreamer can consciously rehearse motor skills in the dream state while being physically asleep. A previous pilot study showed that practice in lucid dreams can improve subsequent performance. This study aimed to replicate those findings with a different task (finger-tapping) and compare the effectiveness of lucid dream practice (LDP) not only to physical but also to mental practice (MP) in wakefulness. An online experiment was completed by 68 participants within four groups: LDP group, MP group, physical practice (PP) group and control (no practice) group. Pre-test was accomplished in the evening, post-test in the next morning, while the practice was done during the night. All three practice groups significantly improved their performance from pre-test to post-test, but no significant improvements were observed for the control group. Subjective sleep quality was not affected by night practice. This study thus corroborates the previous findings that practice in lucid dreams is effective in improving performance. Its effects seem to be similar to actual PP and MP in wakefulness. Future studies should establish reliable techniques for lucid dream induction and verify the effects of LDP in sleep laboratory conditions.
Article
We report a quality effects meta-analysis on studies from the period 1966–2016 measuring either (a) lucid dreaming prevalence (one or more lucid dreams in a lifetime); (b) frequent lucid dreaming (one or more lucid dreams in a month) or both. A quality effects meta-analysis allows for the minimisation of the influence of study methodological quality on overall model estimates. Following sensitivity analysis, a heterogeneous lucid dreaming prevalence data set of 34 studies yielded a mean estimate of 55%, 95% C. I. [49%, 62%] for which moderator analysis showed no systematic bias for suspected sources of variability. A heterogeneous lucid dreaming frequency data set of 25 studies yielded a mean estimate of 23%, 95% C. I. [20%, 25%], moderator analysis revealed no suspected sources of variability. These findings are consistent with earlier estimates of lucid dreaming prevalence and frequent lucid dreaming in the population but are based on more robust evidence.
Article
Lucid dreaming has been said to be within the capability of all individuals (LaBerge, 1985). Based on analyses of the incidence of this dream experience among university students and among persons with an expressed interest in dreaming, a majority have reported experiencing at least one lucid dream during their lifetime, and about 20% have reported experiencing lucid dreams with relative frequency. Our goal in this chapter is to describe and to integrate what has been learned through research about individuals who experience lucid dreams. To this end we will present data derived from the study of four separable but not unrelated functional domains for which subject differences associated with lucid dreaming, or lucidity, have been found. These functional domains are (1) oculomotor/equilibratory; (2) visual/imaginal; (3) intellectual/creative, and (4) personal/interpersonal. The extent of individual differences in lucid dreaming and the methods by which these differences have been investigated will also be discussed. Because methodology is an integral part of research into individual differences, methodological considerations will first be presented.
Chapter
James’s method of “placing things in their series”—so akin to Wittgenstein (1979) on the descriptive rendering of features along multiple lines of “family resemblance” and its recent formalizations within social science—provides the methodological context for our approach to lucid dreams, both in terms of their descriptive phenomenology and psychophysiological processes. By considering the striking interrelations and overlap among dream lucidity and control, highly bizarre dreams, mundane true-to-daily-life dreams, out-of-body experience, “near-death” epidsodes, and the range of meditative experiences and techniques, it becomes clear that prototypical lucid dreams can be seen as a species of spontaneously realized meditative state, and this in their phenomenological, physiological, and cognitive aspects.
Chapter
The theoretical principles for the psychotherapeutic use of lucid dreaming are outlined in view of my own first lucid dream studies and experiences. These are based on the field theoretical assumptions of Gestalt psychology that the personality is capable of self-healing and growth. In this way, lucid dreams have proven to be helpful. I will point out that one can discern unconscious conflicts and contribute to solving them during these dreams through appropriate behavior of the dream ego. Conciliatory interaction with threatening dream figures seems to be important in diagnosis and therapy. For instance, the way in which the dream ego should react in order to deprive a threatening figure of its dangerous character will be examined. I will argue, in accordance with the theoretical assumptions that conciliatory interchange is the most effective principle.
Article
Nocturnal dreams can be considered as a kind of simulation of the real world on a higher cognitive level. Within lucid dreams, the dreamer is able to control the ongoing dream content and is free to do what he or she wants. In this pilot study, the possibility of practicing a simple motor task in a lucid dream was studied. Forty participants were assigned to a lucid dream practice group, a physical practice group and a control group. The motor task was to toss 10-cent coins into a cup and hit as many as possible out of 20 tosses. Waking performance was measured in the evening and on the next morning by the participants at home. The 20 volunteers in the lucid dream practice group attempted to carry out the motor task in a lucid dream on a single night. Seven participants succeeded in having a lucid dream and practiced the experimental task. This group of seven showed a significant improvement in performance (from 3.7 to 5.3); the other 13 subjects showed no improvement (from 3.4 to 2.9). Comparing all four groups, the physical practice group demonstrated the highest enhancement in performance followed by the successful lucid dream practice group. Both groups had statistically significant higher improvements in contrast to the nondreaming group and the control group. Even though the experimental design is not able to explain if specific effects (motor learning) or unspecific effects (motivation) caused the improvement, the results of this study showed that rehearsing in a lucid dream enhances subsequent performance in wakefulness. To clarify the factors which increased performance after lucid dream practice and to control for confounding factors, it is suggested that sleep laboratory studies should be conducted in the future. The possibilities of lucid dream practice for professional sports will be discussed.