ArticlePDF Available

The benefits of using the UPPS model of impulsivity rather than the Big Five when assessing the relationship between personality and problem gambling

Authors:
5. Landon J., Lobstein T., Godfrey F., Johns P., Brookes C., Jernigan D.
International codes and agreements to restrict the promotion
of harmful products can hold lessons for the control of alcohol
marketing. Addiction 2016; DOI: 10.1111/add.13545.
6. Mitchell A. D., Casben J. For DebateTrade law and alcohol
regulation: what role for a global alcohol marketing code?
Addiction 2016; DOI:10.1111/add.13606.
THE BENEFITS OF USING THE UPPS MODEL OF
IMPULSIVITY RATHER THAN THE BIG FIVE
WHEN ASSESSING THE RELATIONSHIP
BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND PROBLEM
GAMBLING
Brunborg et al.srecentpaper[1]focusesontherelevance
of personality factors associated with problem gambling
(PG) in a study in which personality traits were examined
along with subclinical gambling problems in a large-scale
epidemiological sample. The study offers new insights into
specic personality traits associated with PG, namely high
neuroticism and low conscientiousness [1]. If these traits
are indeed the most relevant personalitydimensions linked
to PG, then in our communication we would like to suggest
using the UPPS model of impulsivity [2] as a more suitable
framework to help us understand the relationship between
personality and PG, rather than the Big Five model [3].
Based on the UPPS model [2,4], impulsivity is an
umbrella construct reected by four distinct dimensions:
(i) negative urgency, dened as the tendency to act rashly
while faced with intense negative emotional contexts
(emotion-laden impulsivity); (ii) premeditation, dened as
the tendency to take into account the consequences of an
act before engaging in that act; (iii) perseverance, dened
as the ability to remain focused on a task that may be
boring and/or difcult; and (iv) sensation-seeking,
considered as a tendency to enjoy and pursue activities
that are exciting and openness to trying new experiences.
In relation to the arguments developed in the current
letter, it is important to emphasize that each factor has
an analogue among the facets of the Big Five personality
traits.
Negative urgency is related strongly to neuroticism
(and not to conscientiousness, as stated erroneously by
Brunborg et al. [1]). Furthermore, according to Settles
et al. [5], negative urgency explains variance in exter-
nalizing behaviours beyond that accounted for by other
personality factors that correlate strongly with
neuroticism. Moreover, it has been shown that urgency is
the impulsivity facet that distinguishes more strongly
between treatment-seeking pathological gamblers and
matched control participants [6]. However, in comparison
to neuroticism, negative urgency is a narrower construct
that has been linked to specic executive and affective
mechanisms (e.g. poor pre-potent response inhibition,
heightened emotion reactivity) [7,8], allowing the
development of more focused prevention and treatment
strategies [9].
Premeditation and perseverance relate, respectively, to
the deliberationand self-disciplinefacets of conscien-
tiousness. These two UPPS components rely upon distinct
underlying mechanisms [10], which are not linked identi-
cally to PG [11]. In fact, the lack of premeditation has been
associated with poor decision-making abilities, which is an
established hallmark of PG [12,13], whereas the lack of
perseverance has been linked rather to attentional
processes that are not necessary altered in PG. Accordingly,
it is not surprising that low premeditation (but not low
perseverance) is elevated in PG. It is thus likely that
screening tools able to distinguish between premeditation
and perseverance are more relevant in relation to the
prevention and treatment of PG than measures capturing
only a broad and multi-determined construct of conscien-
tiousness (such as the one used in Brunborg et al.[1]).
Finally, the construct of sensation-seeking (as measured
by the UPPS) is probably more relevant in relation to PG
than the broader construct of extraversion (measured by
the Big Five and unrelated to PG in Brunborg et al.[1]).
Indeed, although linked inconsistently to PG per se,a
heightened level of sensation-seeking has been associated
consistently with certain gambling preferences (e.g.
gambling frequency, favoured types of games) [14,15],
and thus constitutes relevant information when tailoring
prevention efforts in gamblers (for example, in relation to
specicgametypes).
Declaration of interests
None.
Keywords Gambling, impulsivity, personality,
treatment, UPPS, urgency.
NATALE CANALE
1
,ALESSIOVIENO
1
,
HENRIETTA BOWDEN-JONES
2
& JOËL BILLIEUX
3
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of
Padova, Padova, Italy
1
National Problem Gambling Clinic, London,
UK
2
and Laboratory for Experimental Psychopathology, Psychological
Science Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain,
Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
3
E-mail: natale.canale@unipd.it
References
1. Brunborg G., Hanss D., Mentzoni R., Molde H., Palleson S.
Problem gambling and the Five Factor model of personality:
a large population-based study. Addiction 2016; 111:
142835.
© 2016 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction,112,370373
372 Letters to the Editor
2. WhitesideS.P.,LynamD.R.Theve factor model and impul-
sivity: using a structural model of personality to understand
impulsivity. Pers Indiv Differ 2001; 30:66989.
3. Costa P. T., McCrae R. R. Revised NEO Personality Inventory
(NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)
Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources; 1992.
4. Cyders M. A., Smith G. T. Emotion-based dispositions to rash
action: positive and negative urgency. Psychol Bull 2008;
134:80728.
5. Settles R. E., Fischer S., Cyders M. A., Combs J. L., Gunn R. L.,
Smith G. T. Negative urgency: a personality predictor of
externalizing behavior characterized by neuroticism, low
conscientiousness, and disagreeableness. J Abnorm Psychol
2012; 121:16072.
6. Michalczuk R., Bowden-Jones H., Verdejo-Garcia A., Clark L.
Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in pathological gamblers
attending the UK National Problem Gambling Clinic: a
preliminary report. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 262535.
7. Wilbertz T., Deserno L., Horstmann A., Neumann J.,
Villringer A., Heinze H. J. et al. Response inhibition and its re-
lation to multidimensional impulsivity. Neuroimage 2014;
103:2418.
8. Lannoy S., Heeren A., Rochat L., Rossignol M., Van der Linden
M., Billieux J. Is there an all-embracing construct of emotion
reactivity? Adaptation and validation of the emotion
reactivity scale amonga French-speaking community sample.
Compr Psychiatry 2014; 55: 19607.
9. Canale N., Vieno A., Grifths M. D., Rubaltelli E., Santinello M.
How do impulsivity traits inuence problem gambling
through gambling motives? The roles of perceived gambling
risk/benets. Psychol Addict Behav 2015; 29:81323.
10. Cyders M. A., Smith G. T. Clarifying the role of personality dis-
positions in risk for increased gambling behavior. Pe rs In div
Differ 2008; 45:5038.
11. Canale N., Scacchi L., Grifths M. D. Adolescent gamblingand
impulsivity: does employment during high school moderate
the association? Addict Behav 2016; 60:3741.
12. Brevers D., Bechara A., Cleeremans A., Noël X. Iowa
Gambling Task (IGT): twenty years aftergambling disorder
and IGT. Front Ps ych ol 2013; 4:14.
13. Torres A., Catena A., Megías A., Maldonado A., Cándido A.,
Verd e j o-Garc í a A . et al. Emotional and non-emotional
pathways to impulsive behavior and addiction. Front Psychol
2013; 7:11.
14. Smith G. T., Fischer S., Cyders M. A., Annus A. M., Spillane N.
S., McCarthy D. M. On the validity and utilityof discriminating
among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment 2007; 14:15570.
15. Bonnaire C., Bungener C., Varescon I. Pathological gambling
and sensation seekinghow do gamblers playing games of
chance in cafés differ from those who bet on horses at the
racetrack? Addict Res Theory 2006; 14:61929.
© 2016 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction,112,370373
Letters to the Editor 373
... Impulsivity can be defined as the tendency to engage rapidly in a behavior without appropriately considering the potential negatives consequences of the behavior 10 . Among the different models used to assess impulsivityrelated constructs, we chose to focus here on the UPPS-P model developed by Whiteside and Lynam 11 and revised by Cyders and Smith 12 because it considers impulsivity as a multidimensional construct, because it has demonstrated its clinical relevance in persons with addictive disorders 13,14 and because it assesses specifically emotion-related impulsivity, a core risk factors for addictive disorders and externalizing behaviors 15,16 . According to these authors Whiteside and Lynam 11 and Cyders and Smith 12 , impulsivity can be described as a multidimensional personality trait that comprises five different interrelated traits: positive urgency (i.e., tendency to act rashly in response to intensely positive emotional states), negative urgency (i.e., tendency to act rashly when distressed), sensation seeking (i.e., tendency to seek sensory pleasure, excitement and novel experiences), lack of premeditation (i.e., tendency to act without forethought) and lack of perseverance (i.e., inability to remain focused on a task and to quit when a task becomes difficult or boring). ...
... According to these authors Whiteside and Lynam 11 and Cyders and Smith 12 , impulsivity can be described as a multidimensional personality trait that comprises five different interrelated traits: positive urgency (i.e., tendency to act rashly in response to intensely positive emotional states), negative urgency (i.e., tendency to act rashly when distressed), sensation seeking (i.e., tendency to seek sensory pleasure, excitement and novel experiences), lack of premeditation (i.e., tendency to act without forethought) and lack of perseverance (i.e., inability to remain focused on a task and to quit when a task becomes difficult or boring). These five impulsivity facets can be assessed using the validated UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale that is extensively used in patients with addictive disorders, including tobacco 9,13,16 . According to some authors, the UPPS-P has the potential to characterize the influence of these distinct aspects of impulsivity on addictive behaviors 17 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Impulsivity dimensions have been shown to be associated with smoking status and tobacco use disorder severity. However, it is important to determine the specific impulsivity traits associated with smoking relapse. This study aimed at investigating the associations between impulsivity traits and smoking cessation success among adult smokers at 12 months after a quit attempt. Participants were 68 adult smokers enrolled in a 3-month course of simvastatine or placebo associated with behavioral cessation support, with a 9-month follow-up (ADDICSTATINE study). They were classified in 3 groups according to smoking status: abstinent, reduction ≥ 50%baseline or reduction < 50%baseline at 3 and 12 months. Impulsivity traits were assessed using the UPPS-P-scale. At 12 months, abstainers and participants who reduced smoking by 50% or more had significantly lower scores in negative and positive urgency compared to participants who reduced smoking by less than 50% (p = 0.011 and 0.0059). These urgency traits scores at 12 months were significantly and negatively correlated with smoking reduction at 12 months (p = 0.017 and 0.0012). These impulsivity traits were also associated with the smoking cessation success at 3 months. Patients who were abstinent at 3 months had also lower negative and positive urgency (p = 0.017 and 0.0039). Smoking cessation success at 3 and 12 months were not associated with the other impulsivity traits, sensation seeking, lack of premeditation or perseverance. Our findings suggest that positive and negative urgency are associated with smoking cessation success. Proposing better tailored-based-treatment targeting these impulsivity traits in combination with conventional treatment may help improving smoking treatment success.
... The personality traits reflect individuals' consistent, inherent psychological characteristics that influence their mental processes, emotions, and timeinvariant behaviors (Luo et al. 2018). Numerous techniques describe personality traits, including the Sixteen personality factor questionnaire (Bahner and Clark 2020) and the five-factor model (Canale et al. 2017;Nieben et al. 2020). The development of the big five models is an essential milestone in studying personality traits (Shi et al. 2018 This study aims to contribute in multiple ways to the existing body of knowledge. ...
Article
Full-text available
The organization must rely on new approaches and ways to succeed in current environments. One of these methods is to improve ethical leadership behavior to create a more expressive and productive work environment. Employee innovative behavior is a key factor contributing to businesses' long-term survival and growth. This study focuses on the impact of ethical leadership on the link between employee innovative behavior and considering personality as a mediator. Data were collected from 220 employees in the construction industry. The data were analyzed using partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) to assess the measurement and structural models. The study findings show that ethical leadership has a significant positive influence on employee innovative behavior. Personality mediates the relationship between ethical leadership and employee innovative behavior. This study has implications for officials who desire to identify and promote leadership techniques that encourage employee innovative behavior. By understanding the significance of ethical leadership, management can improve from this study, which can assist in strengthening and developing essential employee innovative behavior, which can eventually have a beneficial effect on organizational performance. This research provides a significant practical impact on construction industry management executives. Additionally, this research contributed to the literature for scholars concentrating on the outcomes of ethical leadership. JEL Classification: C99; L29; M10.
... Thus, this model of impulsivity may identify the specific dimensions of impulsivity involved in the co-occurrence of ADHD and behavioral addictions. Previous studies showed that dimensions of the UPPS-P model can explain variance in externalizing behaviors such as pathological gambling and predict both ADHD and behavioral addiction status (Canale et al., 2017;Lopez et al., 2015). The UPPS-P dimensions would be differentially involved in addictive behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study aimed to investigate ADHD in adult outpatients seeking treatment for a behavioral addiction and to identify the specificity of psychopathological features if the behavioral addiction cooccurs with adult ADHD. Method Sixty-five outpatients consulting for a behavioral addiction were assessed for ADHD (DIVA-5), addictive disorder (alcohol, tobacco, cannabis, gambling, gaming, food, and sex), impulsivity (UPPS-P), and emotion dysregulation (DERS-36). Results In our sample of outpatients seeking treatment for a behavioral addiction, adult ADHD was independently associated with higher compulsive sexual behavior disorder severity, “sensation seeking,” “positive urgency,” difficulties in “goal-directed behavior,” “impulse control,” and use of “emotion regulation strategies” in the context of intense emotions. A 29% of the sample was diagnosed for adult ADHD. Conclusion The association of adult ADHD with specific dimensions of impulsivity and emotion dysregulation, pave the way for future clinical and research perspectives.
... In addition, PG has been associated with impulsivity, particularly its emotional components (21). According to the evidence, emotion-driven impulsivity and ED are intimately linked (30). Compulsive gambling might be motivated by an impulsive need to avoid undesirable mood states (31). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: There is a high prevalence of suicidal behavior among Nigerian youths. However, the connections between suicidal behavior and precipitating factors, such as gambling disorder and emotional dysregulation (ED) in this population, are not well researched. Objectives: The present study examined the associations between gambling disorder, ED, and suicidal behavior in Nigerian university undergraduate students. Methods: This study was carried out during October 2020 - April 2021 on 1338 undergraduates with a mean ± SD age of 19.84 ± 3.22 years selected using random and purposive sampling for the universities and participants respectively. Participants responded to the Attitudes Towards Gambling Scale, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale, and Redeemer's University Suicidality Scale. Descriptive statistics (simple percentages) and inferential statistics (Pearson’s correlation and regression analysis) were used for data analysis. Results: The Pearson's correlation revealed that suicidal behaviour had significant positive correlations with the nonacceptance of emotional responses or distress (r = 0.22, P = 0), difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviours (r = 0.21, P = 0), difficulties in impulse control (r = 0.27, P = 0), the lack of emotional awareness (r = 0.14, P = 0), limited access to emotional regulation strategies (r = 0.33, P = 0), the lack of emotional clarity (r = 0.32, P = 0), and difficulties in emotional regulation (r = 0.4, P = 0). Problematic gambling (PG) independently predicted suicidal behavior, while PG and ED dimensions jointly predicted suicidal behavior. The ED dimensions contributed 15% incrementally to the prediction of suicidal behavior and beyond the 2% variance accounted for PG in undergraduates. Conclusions: Our results showed positive links between PG, ED, and suicidal behavior among Nigerian undergraduates. It suggests that the combination of PG and ED increases the severity of suicidal behavior among students.
... Similar to the BIS-11, there are several aspects of the UPPS-P that have been identified as particularly important in disordered gambling. Specifically, previous studies have reported a robust relationship between the affective components of the UPPS-P (positive and negative urgency) and gambling (Canale, Vieno, Bowden-Jones, & Billieux, 2017;Kim, Poole, Hodgins, McGrath, & Dobson, 2019;Navas et al., 2017;Rogier, Colombi, & Velotti, 2020). Impulsivity has long been proposed to influence the strength of AB in substance use (Field & Cox, 2008), and this relation was supported in a meta-analytic review (Coskunpinar & Cyders, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims: Attentional bias to gambling-related stimuli is associated with increased severity of gambling disorder. However, the addiction-related moderators of attentional bias among those who gamble are largely unknown. Impulsivity is associated with attentional bias among those who abuse substances, and we hypothesized that impulsivity would moderate the relationship between disordered electronic gaming machine (EGM) gambling and attentional bias. Methods: We tested whether facets of impulsivity, as measured by the UPPS-P (positive urgency, negative urgency, sensation seeking, lack of perseverance, lack of premeditation) and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (cognitive, motor, non-planning) moderated the relationship between increased severity of gambling disorder, as measured by the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), and attentional bias. Seventy-five EGM players participated in a free-viewing eye-tracking paradigm to measure attentional bias to EGM images. Results: Attentional bias was significantly correlated with Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) motor, positive urgency, and negative urgency. Only positive and negative urgency moderated the relationship between PGSI scores and attentional bias. For participants with high PGSI scores, higher positive and negative urgency were associated with larger attentional biases to EGM stimuli. Discussion: The results indicate that affective impulsivity is an important contributor to the association between gambling disorder and attentional bias.
... In GD, high levels of both impulsive action (27) and impulsive choice (26,28) have been observed, using self-report or behavioral measures, although there is a lack of consistency in explaining their association with GD severity (27,(29)(30)(31). GD has also been linked to impulsive tendencies (32,33). Specifically, positive and negative urgency levels and lack of perseverance are dimensions that may best distinguish individuals with and without GD (34)(35)(36). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Objective: Complex associations between gambling disorder (GD) and impulsivity have been identified. However, little is known regarding how compulsivity associates with different impulsivity domains in GD. In this study, we examined associations between self-reported and behavioral measures of impulsivity–assessed through the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) and the Experiential Discounting Task (EDT), respectively- and compulsivity-measured using the Padua Inventory and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), respectively-, in an adult sample with GD (N = 132, 94 men and 38 women, ages ranging from 18 to 69 years). GD severity was assessed using the South Oaks Gambling Screen. Methods: Structural Equation Modeling was used to examine relationships between impulsivity and compulsivity measures, age, and GD severity. Results: BIS-11 non-planning and BIS-11 total scores positively correlated with GD severity. The standardized coefficients for the SEM showed direct positive contributions of BIS-11 non-planning, Padua and EDT scores to GD severity. Only participants' ages directly contributed to WCST perseverative errors, and no direct or indirect effects were found with respect to GD severity. Conclusion: The findings suggest that specific aspects of impulsivity and compulsivity contribute to GD severity. Interventions specifically targeting domains that are most relevant to GD severity may improve treatment outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research indicates that the invigorating effect of stress sensitivity on gambling behavior might be moderated by individual differences. The current preregistered study tested whether gender and negative urgency (i.e. an emotion-related impulsivity trait) moderate the relationship between perceived stress and laboratory gambling following experimentally induced sadness. One hundred twenty college students were randomly assigned to a sadness versus a control condition before completing a laboratory gambling task. Although the distribution of the main study variables forced us to slightly deviate from the preregistered data analysis plan, we were able to show that heightened stress sensitivity affects gambling behavior and that this effect differs by gender (but not in terms of negative urgency) under conditions of sadness versus neutral mood. Men with high stress sensitivity gambled more money and more frequently selected the riskier betting option in the sadness condition, whereas women with heightened stress sensitivity display the same pattern in the neutral condition. Our study is relevant from a methodological standpoint and answers recent calls for endorsing open-science practices in gambling research. Findings also suggest that more research into female gambling is warranted and that emotion-regulation skills should be a central component of problem gambling prevention.
Article
Background and aims Emotion regulation (ER) and impulsivity impairments have been reported in patients with gambling disorder (GD). However, both constructs have not been studied in depth jointly in clinical samples. Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyze ER and impulsive tendencies/traits in a sample of n=321 treatment-seeking individuals with GD by differentiating them according to their gambling preference (n=100 strategic; n=221 non-strategic). Methods Our sample was assessed through the DERS (ER), the UPPS-P (impulsivity), and the DSM-5 (GD severity). Results The non-strategic group included a higher proportion of women and reported greater ER impairments, and more impulsive traits/tendencies compared to strategic gamblers. GD severity was associated with all DERS subscale (except for awareness) and with urgency dimensions of the UPPS-P. Discussion and Conclusions Our findings confirm that strategic and non-strategic gamblers differ in their ER processes and impulsive tendencies, showing the first clinical group a more adaptive profile. These results suggest the relevance of assessing these ER and impulsivity in order to tailor better treatment approaches.
Article
Background Students often simultaneously deal with shifting support networks, stressful life changes and psychological distress which may affect academic achievement. Methods: 285 students completed the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) to assess depression and the Computerized Life Events Assessment Record (CLEAR), to establish life events and supportive relationships. Module grades were used to measure academic achievement. A general linear model was used with student grade as the dependent variable and life events, depression and supportive relationships as independent variables. Confounding variables included age and sex. Results: A three-way interaction between life events, depression and lack of supportive relationships was found. It indicated the performance of depressed students depended on whether they had supportive relationships and that this interaction also depended on whether they had experienced a life event in the past year. Conclusions: Universities need to provide more support to students with life stress as they transition into university life.
Article
Objective Substance use is a public health concern and cross-sectional studies have found that impulsivity and drinking motives influence substance use in emerging adults. Despite these findings, longitudinal studies with nuanced measures of impulsivity and drinking motives are needed. Participants: The current study investigated the three-year relationship between impulsivity-related traits, drinking motives, sex, and drinking outcomes in a sample of 509 college students (47.47% male; 81% White). Methods: The effects of impulsivity traits and drinking motives on problematic drinking outcomes were evaluated using linear mixed effects models. Results: The results confirmed the hypothesized relationship between traits of impulsivity, drinking motives, and alcohol outcomes over time. Further, sex significantly interacted with drinking motives longitudinally in its relationship with alcohol use outcomes. Conclusions: These results indicate that intervention efforts may need to be tailored to specific individual attributes to target direct correlates of alcohol use behavior to increase effectiveness.
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims: Knowledge of the personality characteristics of individuals who develop gambling problems is important for designing targeted prevention efforts. Previous studies of the relationship between the five-factor model of personality and gambling problems were based on small samples not representative of the general population. We estimated differences in Neuroticism, Extroversion, Intellect, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness between non-problem gamblers and individuals with low, moderate and severe gambling problems. Design: Cross-sectional survey Setting: Norway Participants: 10 081 (51.5% female) individuals aged 16 to 74 years (mean age 46.5 years). Measures: The Problem Gambling Severity Index, The Mini-International Personality Item Pool, and demographic variables. Differences between groups of gamblers were analyzed by ordinary least squares regression models separately for each personality trait adjusting for gender, age, cohabitation, level of education and work status. Findings: Gamblers with low level, moderate level and severe level of gambling problems differed significantly from non-problem gamblers in Neuroticism (b = 0.16, 0.34 and 0.66 respectively, all p < .001) and Conscientiousness (b = -0.13, -0.27, and -0.44 respectively, all p < .001). Moderate and severe problem gamblers differed from non-problem gamblers in Agreeableness (b = -0.21, p < .001 and b = -0.20, p = .028 respectively). In addition, gambling problems were much more prevalent among men than women, and more prevalent among those who live alone, individuals without tertiary education, and among those who are unemployed or on disability pension. Conclusions: A higher level of problem gambling severity appears to be associated with higher scores on Neuroticism, and with lower scores on Conscientiousness and Agreeableness in the Mini-International Personality Item Pool.
Article
Full-text available
Although substantial research suggests that motivations have been found to mediate the relationships between impulsivity traits and various forms of substance use, no studies have examined how gambling motives may mediate the relationships between impulsivity traits and problem gambling. The primary purpose of this study was to test an integrative model linking impulsivity traits and gambling problems, evaluating the mediating effects of gambling motives. Participants were 594 students (73% male; age, M = 19.92 years; SD = 2.91) enrolled in public high schools or universities. Young people who tend to act rashly in response to extremely positive moods showed higher enhancement and coping motives, which in turn were positively related to gambling problems. Individuals with higher levels of sensation seeking were more likely to have higher levels of enhancement motives, which in turn were also positively related to gambling problems. The model was examined in several groups, separately for the level of perceived gambling risk/benefits (lower perceived gambling risk, higher perceived gambling risk, lower perceived gambling benefits, and higher perceived gambling benefits). There were significant differences between these groups for this division. These findings suggest that prevention and/or treatment strategies might need to consider the model's variables, including impulsivity traits and gambling motives, in accordance with individual levels of perceived gambling risk/benefits. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) involves probabilistic learning via monetary rewards and punishments, where advantageous task performance requires subjects to forego potential large immediate rewards for small longer-term rewards to avoid larger losses. Pathological gamblers (PG) perform worse on the IGT compared to controls, relating to their persistent preference toward high, immediate, and uncertain rewards despite experiencing larger losses. In this contribution, we review studies that investigated processes associated with poor IGT performance in PG. Findings from these studies seem to fit with recent neurocognitive models of addiction, which argue that the diminished ability of addicted individuals to ponder short-term against long-term consequences of a choice may be the product of an hyperactive automatic attentional and memory system for signaling the presence of addiction-related cues (e.g., high uncertain rewards associated with disadvantageous decks selection during the IGT) and for attributing to such cues pleasure and excitement. This incentive-salience associated with gambling-related choice in PG may be so high that it could literally "hijack" resources ["hot" executive functions (EFs)] involved in emotional self-regulation and necessary to allow the enactment of further elaborate decontextualized problem-solving abilities ("cool" EFs). A framework for future research is also proposed, which highlights the need for studies examining how these processes contribute specifically to the aberrant choice profile displayed by PG on the IGT.
Article
Full-text available
Impulsivity is tightly linked to addiction. However, there are several pathways by means of which impulsive individuals are more prone to become addicts, or to suffer an addiction more intensely and for a longer period. One of those pathways involves an inadequate appraisal or regulation of positive and negative emotions, leading to lack of control over hazardous behaviors, and inappropriate decisions. In the present work, we assessed cocaine-dependent individuals (CDI; n = 20), pathological gamblers (PG; n = 21), and healthy controls (HC; n = 23) in trait impulsivity measures (UPPS-P model's dimensions), and decision-making tasks (Go/No-go; delay-discounting task). During the Go/No-go task, electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was recorded, and Go/No-go stimuli-evoked potentials (ERP) were extracted. Theory-driven ERP analyses focused on the No-go > Go difference in the N2 ERP. Our results show that negative urgency is one of the several psychological features that distinguish addicts from HC. Nevertheless, among the dimensions of trait impulsivity, negative urgency is unique at independently covarying with gambling over-pathologization in the PG sample. Cocaine-dependent individuals performed more poorly than gamblers in the Go/No-go task, and showed abnormal Go/No-go stimuli-evoked potentials. The difference between the No-go stimulus-evoked N2, and the Go one was attenuated by severity and intensity of chronic cocaine use. Emotional dimensions of impulsivity, however, did not influence Go/No-go performance.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to study the sensation seeking personality trait in two groups of French pathological gamblers: one group who play games available in cafés (n = 57) and one group who bet on horses at the racetrack (n = 42). Sensation seeking was measured with the Zuckerman's Sensation Seeking Scale; intensity of gambling behavior with the South Oaks Gambling Screen and the DSM-IV criteria of pathological gambling. Pathological gamblers who bet at the racetracks had significantly higher scores on Sensation Seeking than those who play games available in cafés. These findings are consistent with the idea of identifying clinically distinct subgroups of gamblers essential in the treatment pathological gambling. One subtype is made of those who play “active” games who gamble for the arousal produced by the game. The second subtype would be one made up of those who play “passive” games who gamble to avoid unpleasant emotional states.