ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Video-Based Interaction on the Development of Second Language Listening Comprehension Ability: A Longitudinal Study

Authors:

Abstract

In our precursor research (Saito & Akiyama, 2017 in Language Learning), we reported that one academic semester of video-based L2 interaction activity was facilitative of various dimensions of the Japanese learners’ spontaneous production ability development (e.g., comprehensibility, fluency and vocabulary). In this paper, we aimed to revisit the dataset to examine the effects of long-term interaction on the development of L2 comprehension ability. Consistent with the interactionist account of L2 comprehension ability development, the results showed that longitudinal interaction enhanced Japanese learners’ comprehension (measured via a general listening proficiency test), as it provided opportunities for comprehensible input and output (measured via video-coding analyses) during meaning-oriented discourse.
Accepted for publication in TESOL Quarterly (Wiley Blackwell)
Effects of Video-Based Interaction on the Development of Second Language
Listening Comprehension Ability: A Longitudinal Study
Kazuya Saito & Yuka Akiyama
Abstract
In our precursor research (Saito & Akiyama, 2017 in Language Learning), we reported that one
academic semester of video-based L2 interaction activity was facilitative of various dimensions of
the Japanese learners’ spontaneous production ability development (e.g., comprehensibility,
fluency and vocabulary). In this paper, we aimed to revisit the dataset to examine the effects of
long-term interaction on the development of L2 comprehension ability. Consistent with the
interactionist account of L2 comprehension ability development, the results showed that
longitudinal interaction enhanced Japanese learners’ comprehension (measured via a general
listening proficiency test), as it provided opportunities for comprehensible input and output
(measured via video-coding analyses) during meaning-oriented discourse.
2
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Effects of Video-Based Interaction on the Development of Second Language Listening
Comprehension Ability: A Longitudinal Study
One of the most extensively researched topics in the field of second language acquisition
(SLA) concerns the relationship between interaction, negotiation for meaning and second
language (L2) development. According to the interaction hypothesis (e.g., Long, 1996), much
learning happens precisely when non-native speakers (NNSs) engage in meaningful
conversations with native speakers (NSs), and encounter communication breakdowns for
language-related reasons. To retrieve impaired meaning, NSs must make an intuitive and/or a
conscious effort to facilitate NNSs’ comprehension by way of negotiation strategies—
clarification requests, confirmation checks and repetition—in addition to recasting NNSs’
erroneous production (i.e., comprehensible input). As a result of negotiation for meaning, NNSs
are induced to notice and understand the gap between their own interlanguage systems and the
incoming input, and then produce more targetlike forms (i.e., comprehensible output).
To date, many researchers have claimed that opportunities to negotiate meaning through
interaction facilitate comprehension (e.g., Ellis, Tanaka, & Yamazaki, 1994). In light of an L2
listening research perspective, negotiation for meaning can provide acquisitionally-rich contexts
for the development of bottom-up processing (drawing on phonological, temporal, lexical and
grammatical information) as well as top-down processing (connecting the linguistic knowledge
with world knowledge) (Rost, 2011). In the face of communication breakdowns, NNSs can
receive a great deal of comprehensible input thanks to NSs’ use of negotiation strategies.
According to observational studies (e.g., Long, 1983), such interactionally modified speech
typically contains linguistic characteristics beneficial for L2 comprehension, including the
repetition, paraphrasing and simplification of original utterances (Jensen &Vinther, 2003) with
3
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
slower speech rate (Zhao 1997). To avoid and/or repair communication breakdowns during L2
interaction, NNSs obtain successful comprehension by searching for the most logical
possibilities via the effective use of context and prior knowledge (e.g., topic familiarity, cultural
background) (Goh, 2002). Through this, these learners are assumed to become more aware of the
importance of using various comprehension strategies, such as selective attention, problem
solving, planning, evaluation and monitoring, all of which are essential for the development of
listening ability (Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010).
Over the past 50 years, a number of empirical studies have extensively expounded the
effect of interaction on L2 comprehension development with pre- and post-test designs. There is
ample evidence showing that L2 learners who negotiate for meaning through actual interaction
with NSs tend to show relatively immediate and large gains in language ability, especially
compared to learners who are merely exposed to pre-modified and simplified input (for a
comprehensive review, Mackey, 2012). Though revealing, these previous studies have brought to
light several methodological problems which make further investigation worthwhile. According
to Mackey and Goo’s (2007) research synthesis, for example, most of studies have involved a
very brief amount of interactional treatment (< one hour), corresponding to a general lack of
longitudinal work in the field of SLA (Ortega & Byrnes, 2008). Furthermore, these studies have
exclusively focused on the acquisition of specific L2 vocabulary and grammar features, without
giving much attention to the development of pronunciation, fluency and listening comprehension
skills. Given that L2 speech research has examined intentional (rather than incidental) focus on
form via explicit instruction, form-focused tasks and interactional feedback (Saito, 2012;
Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010), it remains unclear how L2 learners can improve their global
4
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
oral and listening proficiency through negotiation for meaning during natural conversations with
NSs.
To advance the existing literature on this topic, we conducted an experimental research
with a pre/post-test design focusing on inexperienced Japanese college students’ L2 English
speech learning (comprehension, production) in a foreign language context, where L2 use is
extremely limited outside of classrooms. To create communicatively authentic conversational
opportunities under such restricted L2 learning conditions, the participants engaged in weekly,
dyadic conversation exchanges with NSs in the US by way of a video-conferencing tool (i.e.,
Google Hangouts) beyond the regular curriculum over one academic semester (12 weeks).
Unlike naturalistic environments, where L2 learners have access to a great deal of input and
interaction on a daily basis (e.g., study-abroad), this specific research setting—video-based
interaction in foreign language classrooms—could be considered as an interesting testing ground,
particularly for longitudinal analysis of L2 interaction, as it allowed us to monitor the quality and
quantity of their conversational experience throughout the experiments (see below).
In our precursor research (Saito & Akiyama, 2016), we reported that one academic
semester of video-based L2 interaction activity was facilitative of various dimensions of the
Japanese learners’ spontaneous production ability development (e.g., comprehensibility, fluency
and vocabulary). In this paper, we aimed to revisit the dataset to examine the effects of long-term
interaction on the development of L2 comprehension ability. Following the interactionist account
of L2 comprehension ability development (e.g., Ellis et al., 1994), we predicted that longitudinal
interaction would enhance Japanese learners’ comprehension (measured via a general listening
proficiency test), as it could provide opportunities for comprehensible input and output during
meaning-oriented discourse.
5
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Method
Participants
A total of 30 freshman and sophomore Japanese students at a university in Japan (NNS
learners), and 15 native speakers of English (NS interlocutors) participated in the current study.
NNS learners. For the purpose of recruitment, we created two kinds of flyers: (a) one on
conversational activities through conversational exchanges with college students in the US; and
(b) the other on vocabulary/grammar activities with the goal of attaining higher scores in Test of
English for International Communication (TOEIC)—the same test format as the pre-/post-test
measures. Among the interested participants, a total of 30 students were selected based on their
relatively homogeneous L2 English backgrounds and status as conversationally inexperienced L2
learners.
First, they had studied English for six years only through foreign language education
(typically with grammar-translation methods) since Grade 7 before entering the university
without any extensive experience overseas (< 1 month). Second, the learners’ exposure to L2
English was highly limited. All of them belonged to the same program (business and marketing)
and were required to take three hours of language-focused lessons per week. As specified in the
department syllabus, our casual classroom observations confirmed that the content of these
lessons mainly consisted of reading and listening activities. Finally, all participants reported
lacking any experience at private, conversational English language schools during the project,
indicating that their L2 use with NSs was highly limited on a daily basis.
NS interlocutors. The NS interlocutors were L2 Japanese learners at several universities
in the US who volunteered for conversational exchanges. Their L2 Japanese proficiency widely
varied (beginner to advanced).
6
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Research Design
First, the NNS learners individually took the pre-test in the researchers’ office (Week 1),
and were assigned to either the experimental group (n = 15) or the comparison group (n = 15)
based on their preference (videoconferencing for intercultural interaction with NSs vs.
vocabulary/grammar exercise as prep for TOEIC). Next, they proceeded to an orientation session
(Week 2) during which they received explanation on the procedure for the video-based
conversation activities (for the experimental group) and the vocabulary/grammar exercise
activities (for the comparison group) (Weeks 3-11). After finishing all sessions, they revisited the
researchers’ office to take the post-test (Week 12).
Experimental Group
The experimental treatment was organized as a language-exchange program. Each
session lasted for 60 minutes with the first part in English (30 min) and the second part in
Japanese (30 min). For both parts, a two-way information exchange task was used: NNSs
prepared two visuals corresponding to a different theme each week (e.g., education, food) which
they thought represented Japan and the US, and prepared two discussion questions for each
visual. This task was chosen following Lee’s (2002) suggestion that two-way exchanges of
information on real-life topics that are thematic and minimally structured allow L2 learners to
recycle ideas and reinforce language skills. Respecting the principle of learner autonomy in
language exchange, we did not provide pre-determined visuals. Instead, NNSs were responsible
for exploring cultural differences/similarities via the autonomous selection of the visuals.
Due to the time difference between Japan and the US, the participants were allowed to
arrange the sessions according to their individual schedules, and engaged in the activities using
their own computers. To keep track of their attendance and participation, the participants were
7
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
required to record and submit their own sessions to the researchers (by using a function of
Google Hangouts) upon the completion of every session.
As operationalized in previous L2 interaction research (e.g., Mackey, 2012), NS
interlocutors were trained to provide interactional feedback in the form of recasts, but only when
they perceived the NNSs’ errors to hinder the comprehensibility of their L2 speech. To maintain
the communicative nature of the interaction, the interlocutors were told to pay primary attention
to completing the tasks successfully, providing interactional feedback where natural and
appropriate.
The quality of the L2 interaction treatment during the project was analyzed at the onset
(T1: the second session/Week 4) and endpoint (T2: the eighth session/Week 8) by tallying three
key elements of interaction: (a) the number of linguistic errors made by the NNSs; (b) the
amount of feedback provided by the NS interlocutors in the form of negotiation strategies (after
communication breakdowns) and recasts (following communicatively harmful errors); and (c)
the number of attempts made by NNSs to correct their own errors (i.e., self-modified output)
(e.g., Mackey, 2012).
Comparison Group
The 15 NNS learners in the comparison group were asked to complete weekly take-home
assignments which consisted of a variety of vocabulary/grammar exercise activities, such as
vocabulary recall tests (i.e., comprehension practice) and fill-in-the-blank grammar questions
(i.e., production practice). The materials were piloted prior to the project, and each assignment
took approximately 30 minutes. The learners submitted the assignment to the researchers for
grading every week.
8
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
The purpose of including the comparison group in the current study was two-fold. First,
given that similar tests were used during the pre/post-test sessions (see below), examining the
comparison group’s performance was expected to reveal any test-retest effects. Second, since all
of the NNS learners were enrolled in three-hour English lessons during the project, the
comparison group served as a baseline to reveal the gains which Japanese learners typically
exhibit after one semester of foreign language learning without any opportunities for L2
interaction.
Outcome Measures
Materials. In line with the L2 listening literature (e.g., Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari,
2010), the participants’ comprehension was measured via a composite proficiency test (TOEIC).
This type of assessment is assumed to tap into L2 learners’ ability to process various kinds of
realistic spoken language in order to “understand linguistic information unequivocally included
in the text and to make inferences implicated by the content of the text” (Vandergrift &
Tafaghodtari, 2010, p. 477), and is thus are as a good fit for the current project, whose main
objective was to examine the impact of interaction as a whole (comprehensible input and output)
on L2 learners’ overall listening proficiency.
Two versions (A, B) of the TOEIC test were chosen from the New Official Workbook
(Educational Testing Service, Vol.4), with Version A used for the pre-tests and Version B for the
post-tests.
1
The participants marked their answers on a score sheet. Each test lasted for
approximately 50 minutes. Each version consisted of three components—Part 2 Question-
Response, Part 3 Conversations, and Part 4 Talks.
1
The materials were reprinted by permission of Educational Testing Service. No endorsement of any kind
by the copyright owner should be inferred.
9
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
1. Question-Response (30 questions): The participants selected the best response (out of
three options) for a single-sentence question (5-10 words). Their performance on this
section was assumed to reflect basic-level L2 comprehension proficiency (understanding
linguistically and semantically simple input).
2. Conversations (30 questions): The participants listened to a dialogue between a male
and a female speaker (80-100 words), and selected the best response (out of four options)
to three comprehension questions, respectively. This section was assumed to measure L2
learners’ comprehension ability of interactional speech with frequent turn taking (20-25
words per turn).
3. Talks (30 questions): The participants listened to a business announcement spoken by a
single person (80-100 words), and selected the best response (out of four options) to three
comprehension questions, respectively. This section was assumed to tap into the NNS
learners’ advanced comprehension proficiency (understanding of linguistically and
semantically complex input).
10
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Table 1. Lexical, Grammatical and Discoursal Characteristics of Aural Texts in the
Comprehension Test
Part 2: Question-
Response Part 3: Conversations Part 4: Talks
Task type Comprehension of a short
question (5-10 words)
Comprehension of a
dialogue (80-100 words)
Comprehension of a
talk (80-100 words)
Test version A (pre) B (post) A (pre) B (post) A (pre) B (post)
A. Vocabulary
Diversity (Measure of
Textual Lexical Diversity) 112.1 113.2 94.2 94.2 124.5 113.3
Concreteness 372.1 364.3 373.1 363.0 379.5 385.8
Familiarity 579.6 584.5 581.6 584.5 570.2 575.7
Sophistication
3000 word-families +
proper nouns (%)
a
97.8 98.0 98.8 98.5 98.1 97.4
6000 word-families +
proper nouns (%)
a
99.7 100 99.9 99.7 99.7 99.1
Frequency (CELEX Log) 3.03 3.05 3.09 3.10 2.97 2.99
B. Grammar
No. of words per sentence 5.79 5.15 12.19 11.36 14.87 15.89
Left embeddedness (the
number of words before main
verbs)
0.95 0.88 1.24 1.86 3.01 2.81
No. of modifiers per noun
phrase 0.65 0.59 0.68 0.57 0.83 0.90
C. Discourse
Connectives (incidence) N.A. N.A. 73.8 72.4 81.5 74.5
Cohesion
Stem overlap in adjacent
sentences N.A. N.A. 0.11 0.15 0.31 0.31
Semantic overlap in
adjacent sentences N.A. N.A. 0.06 0.10 0.14 0.14
Note.
a
Both 3000- and 6000-word families were measured based on Vocab Profiler (Cobb,
2012); the other vocabulary, grammar and discourse factors were analyzed via Coh-Metrix
(McNamara et al., 2014).
11
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Following the research framework of Révész and Brunfaut (2013), the processing
difficulty of the aural text (versions A, B) was analyzed via Coh-Metrix (McNamara, Graesser,
McCarthy, & Cai, 2014) and Vocab Profiler (Cobb, 2015). As summarized in Table 1, the three
tasks—Question-Response, Conversations, Talks—noted somewhat similar lexical profiles of
spontaneous spoken in English especially in terms of lexical frequency: 97-98% lexical coverage
by 3000 word-families and proper nouns and 98-99% lexical coverage by 6000-word families
and proper nouns (Nation, 2006).
At the same time, the three tasks differed in terms of the complexity of grammar and
discourse. Question-Response featured less complex grammar structures than Conversations and
Talks according to the number of words per sentence (5-6 vs. 12-16) and the number of words
before main verbs (0.9-1 vs. 1-3). Conversations and Talks were also different in several
respects. Not only did Conversations contain more frequent words than Talks, but the aural text
of the former was less complex than that of the latter according to all grammatical complexity
and discourse connective/cohesion factors.
In terms of the different levels of difficulty between the two test versions, Version A used
slightly less familiar words than Version B did in Parts 1, 2 and 3 (familiar ratings: M = 570.2-
581.6 vs. M = 575.7-584.5). However, no other consistent patterns were observed in terms of the
other domains of the aural texts (lexical, grammatical features and discourse complexity).
Taken together, the three components (Question-Response, Conversations, Talks) had a
lexical frequency range similar to that typically found in English conversational interactions
(3000-6000 word families) (Nation, 2006), but at the same time were ranked by cognitive
demand as follows: Talks > Conversations > Question-Response. The results of the text analyses
indicated that Version A may have been slightly more difficult than Version B. To follow up on
12
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
the tentative pattern of test difficulty (Version A > Version B), we used the comparison group’s
performance as baseline data (see below).
Results
Based on the video-coded data at the outset and end of the project, we first explored the
nature of the interaction treatment that the experimental group received. According to the
descriptive statistics (summarized in Table 2), negotiation for meaning episodes (as a result of
communication breakdowns) occurred only a few times per session (M = 2.3 times [5.3%] for
T1, 2.2 times [4.1%] for T2), and the NS interlocutors selectively recasted only salient linguistic
errors (M = 7.8 errors [17.6%], 4.9 errors [9.3%] per session). In response to such feedback
moves, the NNSs attempted to modify their own errors with relatively high uptake ratio (47.9-
71.4% for negotiation; 36.4-65.4 for recasts). The results here indicated that the NNS learners
processed a certain amount of comprehensible input and output while maintaining their primary
focus on meaning throughout the sessions.
13
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Table 2
Overall Interaction Patterns of Total Errors, Negotiation Strategies and Recasts, and Attempts to
Self-Repair
Errors
Feedback
Uptake
T1 T2
T1 T2
T1 T2
All
linguistic
domains
n =
664
n =
795
No
feedback
n = 34.1
(77.1%)
n =45.8
(86.5%)
Recasts n = 7.8
(17.6%)
n = 4.9
(9.3%)
Uptake n = 2.8
(36.4%)
n = 3.2
(65.4%)
No
uptake
n = 4.9
(63.6%)
n = 1.7
(34.6%)
Negotiation
n = 2.3
(5.3%)
n = 2.2
(4.1%)
Uptake n = 1.1
(47.9%)
n = 1.5
(71.4%)
No
uptake
n = 1.2
(52.1%)
n = 0.6
(28.5%)
Note. T1 = 2
nd
session, T2 = 8
th
session out of 9 sessions over one academic semester
Next, we investigated the longitudinal development of comprehension skills by the
participating students in the experimental group, and compared it with those in the comparison
group. Due to the relatively small size of the dataset (n = 15 per groups), a series of
nonparametric tests were conducted. The alpha level was set at p < .05 and adjusted to p < .0.25
via Bonferroni correction. With respect to pre-existing differences between the two groups, the
results of Mann-Whitney tests showed that they were found comparable for Question-Response
(z = -1.33, p = .187), Conversations (z = -1.91, p = .056), and Talks (z = -1.97, p = .202) at the
beginning of the project.
14
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Table 3
Descriptive Results of the Comprehension Test Scores over Time
Pre-test
(30 points)
Post-test
(30 points)
Improvement
(pre →post)
M SD M SD z p d
Question-Response
Experimental Group
19.6
3.45
23.1
2.79
-2.85
.004*
1.15
Comparison Group 18.0
3.76
19.6
3.77
-1.96
.049 0.42
Conversations Experimental Group
18.6
4.80
23.5
3.44
-3.30
.001*
1.17
Comparison Group 15.4
4.23
19.9
5.00
-3.01
.003*
0.97
Talks Experimental Group
14.8
5.09
21.6
4.86
-3.18
.001*
1.36
Comparison Group 13.0
3.96
16.8
6.63
-2.51
.011*
0.63
Note. * stands for a statistically significant improvement at a p < .025 level.
To examine the presence/absence of any significant improvement over time, a set of
nonparametric Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests were then performed for the experimental and
comparison groups, respectively. The magnitude of their improvement over time (pre → post)
was measured by Cohen’s d analysis, as suggested by Plonsky and Oswald (2014). As
summarized in Table 3, the comparison group noted somewhat limited improvement in their
comprehension scores from pre- to post-tests (a significant gain was found in Conversations and
Talks but not in Question-Response). In contrast, the experimental group significantly increased
their scores in all of three sections (p < .025), and, more importantly, the amount of their
improvement could be interpreted as relatively large (d > 1.00) in keeping with the research
standards in instructed SLA research (Plonsky & Oswald, 2014).
Discussion and Conclusion
The current study took a first step towards examining the effectiveness of video-based
interaction on the longitudinal development (one academic semester) of Japanese college
students’ L2 English comprehension skills in a foreign language setting. Building on recent L2
listening research (e.g., Révész & Brunfaut, 2013; Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010),
participants’ performance was analyzed via a composite test tapping into various dimensions of
15
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
L2 comprehension proficiency, such as the processing of short and simple input (Question-
Response), interactional input with frequent turn taking (Conversations), and long and complex
input (Talks). According to the results of the pre-/post-test data, both the experimental and
comparison groups equally developed their listening skills over time. Yet, analysis of the effect
sizes revealed that the amount of the experimental group’s improvement was equally large under
all task conditions (d > 1), although the comparison group’s improvement was limited in two out
of three tasks (Conversations, Talks) with small-to-medium effects (d < 1) (Plonsky & Oswald,
2014).
The results presented here allow us to assume that the extracurricular drill activities and
classroom listening activities received by the comparison group may be effective for L2
lexicogrammar learning, which could in turn help develop L2 listening skills to some degree,
even without any opportunities to interact with NSs. Other interpretations could be
methodological: (a) the gains resulting from the comparison group may simply indicate test-
retest effects (taking the similar tests twice can result in improved comprehension scores), as
frequently reported in many English proficiency test settings (Liao & Qu, 2010)
2
; and/or (b) the
pre-test materials (Version A) could have been more difficult than the post-test materials
(Version B), as suggested by our text analysis presented earlier.
Compared to the comparison group’s performance as a baseline, it is crucial to emphasize
that the participants in the experimental group attained significant and robust comprehension
skill acquisition with large effects regardless of task condition (i.e., different levels of processing
2
It is important to remember here that the comparison group did not improve on the simplest part of the
listening comprehension test (Question-Response), but improved on the two more difficult sections
(Conversations, Talks). This in turn corresponds to previous research evidence that the magnitude of test-
retest effects is relatively large especially when L2 learners participate in repeated, cognitively demanding
tasks (e.g., Kim & Tracy-Ventura, 2013).
16
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
difficulty: Question-Response < Conversations < Talks). As shown in the video-coded data of
the experimental group, the participants received feedback (recasts, negotiation) approximately
10 times per session (30 min). The nature of interaction could be comparable to other
observation studies which descriptively looked at the feedback frequency during meaning-
oriented interaction (e.g., Mackey, 2012). This in turn suggests that the NNS learners could work
on the development of their L2 listening skills with constant and immediate assistance from their
NS partners for prolonged periods of time. That is, the NS interlocutors occasionally led the
NNS learners to attain better comprehension via negotiation strategies in the case of
communication breakdowns (i.e., comprehensible input). Different from existing L2 listening
studies (e.g., Vandergrift & Tafaghodtari, 2010), where L2 learners focus on incoming input in a
receptive mode, the NS interlocutors also encouraged the NNS learners to repair certain
communicatively-harmful errors in production via recasts (i.e., comprehensible output). Self-
modified output is believed to push NNSs to align their linguistic representations more closely
with native speakers’ models (Swain, 2005), which will, in theory, stimulate the development of
more advanced and robust comprehension ability in a complementary fashion (Field, 2008).
Whereas our study shed some light on the acquisitional value of video-based interaction
as a whole from a longitudinal perspective, the findings should be considered as tentative in
nature, and thus need to be replicated with a larger sample size in different L1/L2 contexts. In
particular, future studies need to scrutinize precisely which aspects of L2 interaction treatment
are relatively important for L2 speech learning in the long run by controlling a range of affecting
factors, such as different types of interactional feedback (recasts, negotiation), opportunities for
repair, quality and quantity of turn taking, NSs’ specific linguistic use, and NNS learners’
proficiency levels (see Mackey, 2012). Additionally, the experimental group’s gain could be
17
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
related to L2 learners’ individual differences in motivation, willingness to communicate,
aptitude, cognition (working and phonological memory), and personality traits (extroversion vs.
introversion). Finally, more qualitative analyses may be needed to examine whether certain
students particularly benefit from L2 interaction of this kind, because they have practiced, or
prepared earnestly for each conversation session with their NS partners.
18
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
References
Cobb, T. (2015). The compleat lexical tutor. Retrieved from http://www.lextutor.ca/vp/
Ellis, R., Tanaka, Y., & Yamazaki, A. (1994). Classroom interaction, comprehension and the
acquisition of word meanings. Language Learning, 44, 449–491.
Field, J. (2008). Emergent and divergent: A view of second language listening research. System,
36, 2–9.
Goh, C. (2002). Exploring listening comprehension tactics and their interaction patterns. System
30, 185–206.
Jensen, E., & Vinther, T. (2003). Exact repetition as input enhancement in second language
acquisition. Language Learning, 53, 373–428.
Kim, Y., & Tracy-Ventura, N. (2013). The role of task repetition in L2 performance
development: What needs to be repeated during task-based interaction? System, 41, 829-
840.
Lee, L. (2002). Enhancing learners’ communication skills through synchronous electronic
interaction and task-based instruction. Foreign Language Annals, 35, 16-24.
Liao, C. W., & Qu, Y. (2010). Alternate forms test-retest reliability and test score changes for the
TOEIC speaking and writing tests. TOEIC Compendium.
Long, M. H. (1983). Native speaker/non-native speaker conversation and the negotiation of
comprehensible input. Applied Linguistics, 4, 126-141.
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W.
C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of language acquisition. Vol. 2: Second
language acquisition (pp. 413-468). New York: Academic Press.
19
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Mackey, A. (2012). Input, interaction, and corrective feedback in L2 learning. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research
synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language
acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 407–452). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
McNamara, D. S., Graesser, A. C., McCarthy, P. M., & Cai, Z. (2014). Automated evaluation of
text and discourse with Coh-Metrix. Cambridge University Press.
Nation, I. S. P. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed for reading and listening? Canadian
Modern Language Review, 63, 59–82.
Ortega, L., & Byrnes, H. (Eds.). (2008). The longitudinal study of advanced L2 capacities. New
York: Routledge.
Plonsky, L., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is ‘big’? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research.
Language Learning, 64, 878-912.
Révész, A., & Brunfaut, T. (2013). Text characteristics of task input and difficulty in second
language listening comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 31-65.
Rost, M. (2011). Teaching and researching listening. London: Longman.
Saito, K. (2012). Effects of instruction on L2 pronunciation development: A synthesis of 15
quasi-experimental intervention studies. TESOL Quarterly, 842-854.
Saito, K., & Akiyama, Y. (2017). Video-based interaction, negotiation for comprehensibility,
and second language speech learning: A longitudinal study. Language Learning, 67(1).
Vandergrift, L., & Tafaghodtari, M. (2010). Teaching L2 learners how to listen does make a
difference: An empirical study. Language Learning, 60, 470-497.
20
ROLE OF INTERACTION IN L2 LISTENING COMPREHENSION
Zhao, Y. (1997). The effects of listeners’ control of speech rate on second language
comprehension. Applied Linguistics, 18, 49–68.
... SCMC can be undertaken in different modes, such as text-based, audio-based, and video-based modes, or some combination of them. Its effect on L2 learning has been widely researched [5,6]. During SCMC activities, L2 learners are able to receive more authentic linguistic input and prompt feedback, and they have more language-use opportunities to interact with native speakers of their target language or more competent L2 users, such as peers or teachers [4,7,8]. ...
... In addition to improved speaking abilities, a study showed that Korean learners at a university in the United States who worked with their assigned videoconferencing interlocutors in Korea reported, via surveys, journals, and interviews, an improvement in their listening abilities [27]. The positive effect of videoconferencing on listening-comprehension ability was also confirmed in Levak and Son's study [2], as well as that of Saito and Akiyama [6,26], both of which measured participant listening abilities in listening tests and found improvements in the test outcome. Aside from the positive gains in speaking and listening skills, L2 learners could improve their vocabulary knowledge in videoconferencing tasks. ...
... One study stated having participants at the beginning-proficiency level [25], one had intermediate-level participants [39], and one had participants whose proficiency ranged from pre-intermediate to intermediate levels [23]. The studies conducted by Saito and Akiyama [6,26] merely stated that their participants had learned the target language for six years. Two main language features were investigated in these studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
To mitigate the unexpected closure of educational institutions during the COVID-19 pandemic, e-learning has become a practical alternative to conventional face-to-face instruction. Videoconferencing, a synchronous computer-mediated communication (SCMC) approach, has been adopted as a venue to continue student learning activities. However, in the field of second-language (L2) education, videoconferencing had already been integrated into learning tasks, enabling L2 learners to have more opportunities to access authentic linguistic input and participate in interactions with more proficient users or native speakers of the target languages. Research has reported the pedagogical benefits of learners’ L2 achievement that are provided by videoconferencing, whereas some studies have reached a different conclusion. To further ascertain the effectiveness of videoconferencing in L2 learning, meta-analysis can be used to provide statistical evidence of the significance of study results, which serves as a useful reference for the application of videoconferencing to current e-learning practices. Thus, systematic meta-analysis was used in this study to synthesize the findings from experimental and quasi-experimental research into the effectiveness of videoconferencing for L2 learning. Videoconferencing approaches led to positive, medium overall effects in control/experimental group comparisons (g = 0.35, p < 0.5) on the L2 language development of listening and speaking abilities. However, this conclusion is based on five studies and, thus, needs to be treated cautiously. The implications of the findings and suggestions for future studies are discussed.
... For the purposes of the current study, FPs were defined and coded as both lexical and non-lexical. , well, in that case, so, like, entonces, bueno, pero Non-lexical uh, u:h, um, u:m, eh, em, er, erm, mm Hesitation Cluster uh...u:h, um...u:m, uh...er, mm...um This study implemented constructs of oral fluency based on previous literature on measures of HP (e.g., LENNON, 1990;MÖHLE, 1984;OLYNYK;D'ANGLEJAN;SANKOFF, 1990;RAUPACH, 1984RAUPACH, , 1987RIGGENBACH, 1991;SAJAVAARA, 1987;SEGALOWITZ;DEWEY, 2004;O'BRIEN, 2014). The analysis consisted primarily of an investigation of FP rather than a holistic view of fluency (e.g., speech rate, duration, etc.); therefore, only the number of FP were included in the data analysis. ...
... FPs were coded for each participant in 2 ways: 1) as the total sum, 2) as a ratio of the number of FPs over the number of total words (including the FPs in the total word count). While previous research on FPs did not include whether FPs were included in the analysis as words (e.g., LENNON, 1990;MÖHLE, 1984;OLYNYK;D'ANGLEJAN;SANKOFF, 1990;RAUPACH, 1984RAUPACH, , 1987RIGGENBACH, 1991;SAJAVAARA, 1987;SEGALOWITZ;DEWEY, 2004;O'BRIEN, 2014), the current study opted to include them. Below is an example of the coding procedure. ...
Article
The frequency of hesitational phenomena, such as Filled Pauses (FP) are inversely correlated to the perceived fluency of L2 learners (e.g., DERWING; ROSSITER; MUNRO; THOMSON, 2004; LENNON, 1990; ULLAKONOJA, 2008). The current study investigated the production of these FPs by 66 naive Spanish L2 learners with an L1 of English in two types of oral tasks and compared their frequency in regard to task type and whether they participated in Face-to-Face or Telecollaboration (Teletandem) communicative dyads. Results indicate that while the medium of the tasks did not impact fluency, the task type did.
... Alghasab et al., 2019;Ioannou et al., 2015;Labadat, 2004) while others have looked into the effect of videoconferencing on the improvement of language learners' speaking proficiency (e.g. Alshahrani, 2016;Rassaei, 2017;Saito & Akiyama, 2017;Tecedor & Campos-Dintrans, 2019); nonetheless, ones that focus on spontaneous oral interaction in synchronous online audio-visual environments remain scarce, especially in the field of English Language Teaching (ELT). Given the central importance attributed to interaction in language learning, this paper seeks to answer the following question: ...
Article
Full-text available
The importance of oral interaction in language learning is well recognised, and this paper examines the factors that hinder English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners’ active participation in online discussions during Synchronous Online Communication. Using focus groups with 51 Cypriot Greek-speaking students from three tertiary institutions in Cyprus, thematic analysis revealed that the perceived prominent factor inhibiting oral interaction in the online environment was the absence of eye gaze and its associated social functions. Specifically, eye gaze was identified as a signal for turn-taking, turn-yielding, and turn-denying, whose absence hindered learners’ ability to interpret their peers’ and teachers’ intentions and expectations. The paper concludes with pedagogical implications for technologists, software developers, students, language teachers, and language teacher educators.
... This approach to language learning is supported by existing research, which underscores the importance of interactive practice in developing communicative abilities. Studies by Saito and Akiyama (2017) have shown that engaging in real-time conversations significantly improves both speaking and listening skills, providing learners with the opportunity to apply their language knowledge in practical, everyday contexts. Similarly, Lee (2008) found that interactive communication helps learners develop better fluency and comprehension, as it closely mimics the natural flow of conversation. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This study investigates the case of an Indonesian English learner who increased her linguistic proficiency through the self-taught use of mobile applications and identifies insights into the potential of mobile technology for autonomous language learning. Employing an instrumental case study approach, this qualitative research explores the strategies and features within mobile applications that were most effective in fostering self-taught language learning and developing communicative skills. The subject of the case study was an undergraduate teacher-trainee majoring in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) who demonstrated an exceptionally high increase in proficiency in English acquired independently using mobile applications over a three-year period. Data was collected through in-depth interviews to provide details of the subject's language learning journey. Thematic analysis revealed that a combination of interactive communication practices, immersive learning environments, social learning, motivation, and consistent practice were pivotal in enhancing the increase in the subject's language proficiency. Language input from Fondi's virtual spaces (Plaza, Lounge, and Bar) and a variety of English-language media significantly improved comprehension and fluency in speaking and listening. The study highlights the importance of incorporating interactive and gamified elements, real-time feedback, and personalized learning experiences via mobile language learning apps. Future research should focus on longitudinal studies and include diverse learner populations to expand the applicability of these findings. The insights from this study lay a foundation for further innovation and research, aiming to enhance the effectiveness and accessibility of autonomous language learning technologies.
... It is influenced by both personal qualities and acquired motivational directions, which enhance the overall personal attributes of the future specialist. The deepening and strengthening of these qualities should be aimed at the activation of independent work of students and other persons [8] [9]. ...
... Language learners can reach native speakers or more competent target-language users through videoconferencing and construct knowledge through dialogues (Vygotsky, 1978). Consequently, communicative competence can be developed more effectively (Saito and Akiyama, 2018;Tecedor and Campos-Dintrans, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to examine EFL university students’ perceptions about cross-cultural videoconferencing presentations for professors in the United States. Nineteen Taiwanese English-as-foreign-language university students who studied in a night program for a bachelor’s degree attended the study. Each of them was free to choose a topic related to Taiwanese cultures and individually made a 25-min presentation plus a five-minute question-and-answer session. After the presentation, they watched recordings of their presentations, completed a five-point Likert-scale attitude survey, and wrote down their reflections. The results showed that the participants were positive about the activity. Based on the analysis of their reflections, the participants acknowledged various aspects of the cross-cultural presentations, such as the opportunities to use English meaningfully, motivation to prepare for the project, and the authentic nature of the interaction. However, the participants had concerns about their language abilities, anxiety, and limited preparation time. Finally, they mentioned the impact the presentation had on them. Pedagogical implications and suggestions for future research are provided.
... An example of this can be seen in Saito and Akiyama's (2018) investigation of the English spoken language development by Japanese learners of English following a semester-long VE with native ...
Article
Full-text available
This goal of this paper is to enhance VE practitioners’ language assessment literacy. To do so, it begins with an overview of assessment practices commonly used in VE for evaluating the complex and multifaceted nature of language competence. These include the following: (1) approaches that evaluate change in learners’ language use over time, (2) approaches that employ pre- and posttest to evaluate learning outcomes, and (3) approaches that rely on student self-report or self-documentation of learning. Based on this overview, we then look to the field of language assessment for guidelines on the selection and use of classroom-based and standardized assessment tools and practices. Using an existing VE practice scenario, the authors provide an example of language assessment selection and development.
... As a mode of communication, video can play a role in how "identities" are performed (Dooly & Davitova, 2018), learning is acquired (Saito & Akiyama, 2018), ICC is developed (Ware, 2013), face is negotiated, interactional troubles are addressed, and social distance is managed (Ko, 2012;Yamada, 2009). Following these findings, it is possible that the video context mediated the use of RBBs by encouraging students to be more attentive to their conversational topics, as students must appear engaged when visible and cannot multitask in the same way as text-based platforms. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates how topics are managed in virtual exchanges between tertiary-level English language learners from Turkey and Kazakhstan. The study reports on a previously unexplored interactional device referred to as “rolling the ball back” (RBB) that invites a co-participant to maintain a current topic of discussion through reciprocation of a question asked previously. Using multimodal conversation analysis, the findings show that RBBs accomplish a range of discursive actions through this reciprocation including requesting for information, asking for opinions, changing speakership, and creating a space for topic extension. RBBs are pedagogically significant and interactionally salient in that “rolling it back” allows students to maintain topic progressivity by asking reciprocating questions in online interaction. These findings contribute to the literature on topic maintenance, as well as existing work concerned with how the discursive organization in virtual exchange can be used to enhance teaching and learning. RBBs can be used by language researchers and practitioners to design new learning practices and materials that facilitate robust learner talk in and out of language classrooms.
Article
The relevance of the study in this paper is due to the problems of ecology in the modern world and the formation of ecological culture among the younger generation within the framework of the educational process to introduce them the ecological values in their future professional activities to preserve and restore the ecosystem of the planet. The purpose of the study is to form a model for the didactic foundations of ecologically-oriented teaching of English to students of pedagogical specialities. The leading method for the study of this issue is the a survey using the M. Rokich test for diagnosing personal value orientations, which makes it possible to determine social values, psychological aspirations by identifying the existing self-tasks of the person under survey, features of his or her self-determination and self-regulation of behaviour within the framework of ecological culture. The study presents didactic foundations of ecologically-oriented teaching of English to students of pedagogical specialities with formation of value orientations including value-motivational, cognitive-informational and modeling of creative ecological-cognitive professional activity on the basis of which the level of ecological culture of students can be created. The formed didactic foundations model of ecologically-oriented teaching of English to students of pedagogical specialties enables to create ecological culture with value standards of careful attitude to the environment and all levels of nature, which will allow them to introduce it in the educational process in their future professional activity to educate students about high ecological culture and creative behavior concerning natural objects and the whole ecosystem of life in general, which has practical significance for the education system and well-being of society.
Article
Full-text available
Today's society thinks more realistically and critically in choosing educational institutions for their families. Therefore, educational institutions, through the principal as a manager, must carry out their duties with a transformative leadership style so that the positive image of the school increases so that it becomes the community's first choice to send their families to the school. This study presents a transformative leadership strategy for improving the school's image so that it exists amid competition between educational institutions. This research uses a qualitative case study approach. Methods of data collection using observation, interviews, and documentation. The object of this research is Islamic Senior High School of Nurul Jadid. The results showed that: Transformative leadership can create and enhance a positive image of the school where the positive image of Islamic Senior High School of Nurul Jadid can be seen from the achievements in academic and non-academic fields; The strategy used by the principal in improving the positive image of Islamic Senior High School of Nurul Jadid is by empowering and increasing the discipline of all school members which in its implementation is influenced by supporting factors, namely academic achievement, pleasing personality, and high socialization while the inhibiting factor is the lack of awareness of new teachers in educating students.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the impact of video‐based conversational interaction on the longitudinal development (one academic semester) of second language production by college‐level Japanese English‐as‐a‐foreign‐language learners. Students in the experimental group engaged in weekly dyadic conversation exchanges with native speakers in the United States via telecommunication tools. The native speaker interlocutors were trained to provide interactional feedback (recasts) when the nonnative speakers’ utterances hindered successful understanding (i.e., negotiation for comprehensibility). The students in the comparison group received regular foreign language instruction without any interaction with native speakers. The coded video data showed that the experimental students worked on improving all linguistic domains of language, likely in response to their native speaker interlocutors’ interactional feedback (recasts, negotiation) during the treatment. The pretest–posttest data of the students’ spontaneous production showed that they made significant gains in the dimensions of comprehensibility, fluency, and lexicogrammar but not in those of accentedness and pronunciation. Open Practices This article has been awarded an Open Materials badge. All materials are publicly accessible in the IRIS digital repository at http://www.iris‐database.org . Learn more about the Open Practices badges from the Center for Open Science: https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki .
Article
Full-text available
The calculation and use of effect sizes—such as d for mean differences and r for correlations—has increased dramatically in second language (L2) research in the last decade. Interpretations of these effects, however, have been rare and, when present, have largely defaulted to Cohen's levels of small (d = .2, r = .1), medium (.5, .3), and large (.8, .5), which were never intended as prescriptions but rather as a general guide. As Cohen himself and many others have argued, effect sizes are best understood when interpreted within a particular discipline or domain. This article seeks to promote more informed and field-specific interpretations of d and r by presenting a description of L2 effects from 346 primary studies and 91 meta-analyses (N > 604,000). Results reveal that Cohen's benchmarks generally underestimate the effects obtained in L2 research. Based on our analysis, we propose a field-specific scale for interpreting effect sizes, and we outline eight key considerations for gauging relative magnitude and practical significance in primary and secondary studies, such as theoretical maturity in the domain, the degree of experimental manipulation, and the presence of publication bias.
Article
Full-text available
Over the past 25 years second language (L2) acquisition researchhas paid considerable attention to the effectiveness of instruction onL2 morphosyntax development, and the findings of relevant empiricalstudies have been extensively summarized using narrative review meth-ods (e.g., Ellis, 2002) as well as meta-analytic review methods (e.g.,Spada & Tomita, 2010). These researchers have reached a consensusthat (a) integrating language focus into meaning-oriented classroomsis more effective than a purely naturalistic approach, and (b) contextu-alized grammar teaching methods (e.g., focus-on-form instruction,form-focused instruction) is more effective than decontexualized gram-mar teaching methods (e.g., focus-on-formS instruction, grammar-translation method). What is surprising in this vein of L2 acquisitionstudies, however, is the lack of research in the area of L2 pronuncia-tion development. Pronunciation teaching has been notorious for itsoverdependence on decontextualized practice such as mechanicaldrills and repetition, reminiscent of the audiolingual teaching meth-ods of several decades ago (for discussion, see Celce-Murcia, Brinton, 1Goodwin, & Griner, 2010). Furthermore, very few language teachersactually receive adequate training in the specific area of pronunciationteaching (Foote, Holtby, & Derwing, 2011).1234567891011121314151617181920212223242526272829303132333435363738394041424344
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the effects of a group of task factors on advanced English as a second language learners’ actual and perceived listening performance. We examined whether the speed, linguistic complexity, and explicitness of the listening text along with characteristics of the text necessary for task completion influenced comprehension. We also explored learners’ perceptions of what textual factors cause difficulty. The 68 participants performed 18 versions of a listening task, and each task was followed by a perception questionnaire. Nine additional students engaged in stimulated recall. The listening texts were analyzed in terms of a variety of measures, utilizing automatized analytical tools. We used Rasch and regression analyses to estimate task difficulty and its relationship to the text characteristics. Six measures emerged as significant predictors of task difficulty, including indicators of (a) lexical range, density, and diversity and (b) causal content. The stimulated recall comments were more reflective of these findings than the questionnaire responses.
Book
Now in its third edition, Teaching and Researching Listening renews its commitment to provide language educators, practitioners, and researchers in the fields of ESL, TESOL, and Applied Linguistics with a state-of-the-art treatment of the linguistic, psycholinguistic, and pragmatic processes underpinning oral language use. This revised edition incorporates signifi cantly updated sections on neurological processing, pragmatic processing, automated processing, and pragmatic assessment, as well as coverage of emerging areas of interest in L1 and L2 instruction and research. Boxes throughout such as “Concepts” and “Ideas From Practitioners” help to both reinforce readers understanding of the topics covered and ground them in a practical context. In addition, the updated section “Exploring listening” provides access to a range of tools and technologies to explore new perspectives on listening. Combining detailed overviews of theunderlying processes of listening with an exhaustive set of practical resources, this third edition of Teaching and Researching Listening serves as an authoritative and c omprehensive survey of issues related to teaching and researching oral communication for language teachers, practitioners, and researchers.
Book
The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition presents an integrated discussion of key, and sometimes controversial, issues in second language acquisition research. Discusses the biological and cognitive underpinnings of SLA, mechanisms, processes, and constraints on SLA, the level of ultimate attainment, research methods, and the status of SLA as a cognitive science. Includes contributions from twenty-seven of the world's leading scholars. Provides an invaluable resource for all students and scholars of human cognition, including those in linguistics, psychology, applied linguistics, ESL, foreign languages, and cognitive science.
Article
Coh-Metrix is among the broadest and most sophisticated automated textual assessment tools available today. Automated Evaluation of Text and Discourse with Coh-Metrix describes this computational tool, as well as the wide range of language and discourse measures it provides. Section I of the book focuses on the theoretical perspectives that led to the development of Coh-Metrix, its measures, and empirical work that has been conducted using this approach. Section II shifts to the practical arena, describing how to use Coh-Metrix and how to analyze, interpret, and describe results. Coh-Metrix opens the door to a new paradigm of research that coordinates studies of language, corpus analysis, computational linguistics, education, and cognitive science. This tool empowers anyone with an interest in text to pursue a wide array of previously unanswerable research questions..
Article
Researchers and educators routinely call for longitudinal research on language learning and teaching. The present volume explores the connection between longitudinal study and advanced language capacities, two under-researched areas, and proposes an agenda for future research. Five chapters probe theoretical and methodological reflections about the longitudinal study of advanced L2 capacities, followed by eight chapters that report on empirical longitudinal investigations spanning descriptive, quasi-experimental, qualitative, and quantitative longitudinal methodologies. In addition, the co-editors offer a detailed introduction to the volume and a coda chapter in which they explore what it would take to design systematic research programs for the longitudinal investigation of advanced L2 capacities. The scholars in this volume collectively make the argument that second language acquisition research will be the richer, theoretically and empirically, if a trajectory toward advancedness is part of its conceptualization right from the beginning and, in reverse, that advancedness is a particularly interesting acquisitional level at which to probe contemporary theories associated with the longitudinal study of language development. Acknowledging that advancedness is increasingly important in our multicultural societies and globalized world, the central question explored in the present collection is: How does learning over time evolve toward advanced capacities in a second language?