ChapterPDF Available

Universal Human Fears

Authors:
U
Universal Human Fears
Pavol Prokop
University of Trnava, Trnava, Slovakia
Synonyms
Aversion;Phobia
Definition
A distressing emotion caused by anticipation or
awareness of danger.
Introduction
Darwin states, in his book The Expression of the
Emotions in Man and Animals which supported
the theory of natural selection, that fear is the
most depressing of all the emotions(Darwin
1872, p. 36). A century later, basic emotion theo-
rists have proposed that certain emotions are
innate, in part because they are expressed the
same in people around the world (e.g., Ekman
1977). There is growing agreement among
researchers that fear is an adaptive emotion that
motivates ght or ybehavior, ultimately pro-
moting self-protection. This entry is focused on
(1) domains of human fear and (2) preparedness
learning, (3) perceptual biases, and (4) gender
differences to support its evolutionary origin.
Fear is associated with ancient parts of the
brain; the amygdala areas, which are located
deep and medially within the temporal lobes of
the brain, have been involved in fear conditioning
in a variety of mammals, including rats, mice,
rabbits, and monkeys (for a review, see LeDoux
2012). Almost universal physiological and behav-
ioral responses to fear-related stimuli are escape,
avoidance behavior, a stronger heart rate, blood
pressure, respiration, and adrenaline and gluco-
corticoid release. It is suggested that fear-related
responses are adaptive, because they prepare our
bodies for a ght or escape from a potential dan-
ger. Both types of responses require accelerated
metabolism via glucose release by adrenaline and
oxygen supply to the muscles by rapid breathing
in order to make energy available to the muscles
for ght or ight (Marks and Nesse 1994).
Evolutionary psychologists have suggested
that fear has developed for mostly ancient threats
(e.g., certain animals such as snakes, spiders and
rats, heights, storms, darkness, blood, strangers,
and separation or leaving the safety of home), but
not to current threats (e.g., cigarettes, cars, elec-
trical equipment, or handguns). Three lines of
evidence support this idea. First, fear stimuli are
nonrandomly distributed and are related to real
(albeit ancient) threats (Marks and Nesse 1994).
People rarely display, for example, a fear of trees,
stones, or the sky which were not harmful to our
ancestors, but fears of harmful objects or events
#Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016
T.K. Shackelford, V.A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2996-1
are relatively common. Second, Martin Seligman
(1971) has proposed that fears reect evolution-
arily prepared learning to fear events and situa-
tions that have provided survival threats in our
evolutionary past. This can be documented with
classic experiments carried out by Cook and
Mineka (1990,1991). Laboratory-reared, but not
wild, monkeys do not manifest strong fearful
responses to snakes suggesting that their reactions
are learned, rather than innate. Monkeys are, how-
ever, able to quickly acquire a fear of a predator by
observing other monkeys expressing fear in inter-
action with the predator. When laboratory-reared
monkeys observed a wild-reared monkey
displaying fear of a live and toy snake, they were
rapidly conditioned to fear snakes (Cook and
Mineka 1990). In consequent experiments, the
laboratory-reared monkeys observed displays of
fear in response to toy snakes (predators) and
owers (harmless objects) or to toy crocodiles
(predators) and rabbits (harmless objects). Fear
responses were conditioned only for predators,
but not for owers and toy rabbits (Cook and
Mineka 1991). Since the laboratory-reared mon-
keys had no previous experiences with the stimuli
used in the experiment, these results provide sup-
port for an evolutionary basis to the prepared
learning (Öhman and Mineka 2001).
Third, common human fears seem to develop
at a time when the danger might be encountered.
Fears of heights and strangers, for example,
emerge in infants at about 6 months of age,
when infants begin to crawl away from their
mothers thereby increasing the risk of a fall
and/or kidnapping. The higher risk of an infant
being killed by strangers has been documented
both in nonhuman primates as well as in modern
humans. At the age of 2 years, when infant explo-
ration begins to be more intense, animal fears and
fear of darkness emerge (for a review, see Gullone
2000). Humans, unlike nocturnal animals, have
limited vision making them more vulnerable to
predators when their visual capacity is limited.
Fear of the dark restricts infant mobility thereby
decreasing vulnerability to predators. Later, when
the young child leaves the home base, agorapho-
bia, the fear of being in crowds or public places,
which makes escape difcult, may develop
(Marks and Nesse 1994). The intensity of fears
in children tend to decrease with age (Gullone
2000) which can be explained by their higher
physical and emotional independence from
parents.
Evolutionary Evidence for Fear
of Dangerous Animals
Snakes are viewed as prototypical stimuli of com-
mon fears of animals (Öhman and Mineka 2001).
Snakes are present on all continents except Ant-
arctica, and their putative fossils date back as far
as 150 million years. At this time, constrictor
snakes were critical predators of the small ances-
tors of modern placental mammals (Isbell 2009).
Note that carnivore predators occurred much later,
only about 4060 million years ago. Transitional
primate-like creatures were evolving ca. 65.5 mil-
lion years ago, and recent primates are still
attacked by snakes (Cook and Mineka 1991).
This evidence suggests that the evolution of pri-
mate defense against predators was inuenced by
snakes.
The second line of evidence regarding danger
from snakes comes from introducing an effective
venom a new powerful weapon in the
predatorprey arms race with primates. Fossils
of snakes with venomous fangs from at least the
Lower Miocene have been discovered, i.e., far
before the rst ancestors of Homo appeared.
This evidence strongly suggests that interactions
between snakes and early humans could be
expected. Snake bite caused substantial human
mortality and disability, although these issues
have been largely neglected by health organiza-
tions. It is estimated that snake bites cause roughly
about 20,000125,000 deaths worldwide,
although these estimates are based on incomplete
data (Warrell 2010), partly because reporting is
not mandatory in many regions of the world. It can
be suggested that snake mortality was much
higher in our evolutionary past, when human
interaction with nature occurred daily and medical
help was highly limited (if any).
The early coexistence of dangerous snakes
with their mammalian prey may be the reason
2 Universal Human Fears
why snakes are frequently sources of phobias.
Robins and Regier (1991), for example, deter-
mined that bugs, mice, snakes or batswere the
most frequently cited category of phobic fears.
Indeed, snakes are traditionally labeled as the
least liked animals in a range of surveys on animal
fears, while certain other terrestrial predators such
as wolves or bears received more positive rank-
ings by people (Schlegel and Rupf 2010; Almeida
et al. 2014).
Threatening Objects Promote Visual
Attention
Coevolution with snakes as signicant primate
predators predicts that primates, including
humans, should have developed a specic visual
sensitivity for snake (i.e., danger) cues that pro-
motes vigilance for an early detection of stimuli to
escape and/or avoid potentially a deadly encoun-
ter with the predator and hence survive and repro-
duce (Öhman et al. 2001a). Neurobiological
research supports this idea, because amygdala
tunes visual brain areas for rapid perception of
fear-related stimuli. Furthermore, it seems that
specic neurons in the primate brain respond
especially quickly to images of snakes (Van Le
et al. 2013). Humans, similarly to nonhuman pri-
mates, seem to have no innate fear of snakes
(DeLoache and LoBue 2009), but show similar
abilities to condition snakes with something fear-
ful (LoBue and DeLoache 2010). Rakison and
Derringer (2008) found, for example, that
5-month-old infants look longer at a schematic
image of a spider or a snake relative to scrambled
versions of the same schematic image and look
equally long at nonthreatening images such as
owers.
To support the rapid detection of snakes rela-
tive to other non-treating stimuli, Öhman
et al. (2001a) compared the detection of a fear-
relevant target (a snake or a spider) among eight
fear-irrelevant distractors (owers or mushrooms)
with that of a fear-irrelevant target (a ower or a
mushroom) among fear-relevant distractors
(snakes or spiders). As expected, snakes and spi-
ders were detected more quickly than owers and
mushrooms. These ndings were repeatedly
reported in a number of studies, involving both
children and adults (for a review, see LoBue and
Rakison 2013). It can be argued that humans
should also be sensitive to a visual detection of
the emotional cues in the faces of other members
in the social group because signicant numbers of
violent deaths can be attributed to conicts
between conspecics similarly as with chimpan-
zees. The same paradigm suggesting that fear-
relevant stimuli receive rapid detection in order
to avoid potential threat, Öhman et al. (2001b)
found that threatening faces are detected by
humans among neutral faces more quickly than
friendly faces, a nding which was successfully
replicated by other researchers (for a review,
Öhman et al. 2001b; LoBue and Rakison 2013).
These ndings suggest that there is a clear bias for
threat in visual detection tasks.
Gender Differences in Common Fears
Girls exhibit more fears than boys (Arrindell
et al. 2003), and at least some of them can emerge
very early (Rakison 2009). In particular, girls
reported being more fearful of darkness, strange
sights and sounds, loneliness, personal relation-
ships, animals, injury, and being kidnapped,
robbed, or killed (Gullone 2000; Arrindell
et al. 2003). Boys reported being more fearful of
not being good and getting into trouble, night-
mares, and imaginary creatures including mon-
sters, gorillas, and dinosaurs (for a review, see
Gullone 2000). Gender socialization experiences
may contribute to the reporting of fear difference
by males and females. Girls may learn that it is
permissible to express fears, whereas boys are
expected to inhibit fears. Rakison (2009) found,
however, that 11-month-old female but not male
infants learn rapidly to associate negative facial
emotions with fear-relevant stimuli (snakes and
spiders) suggesting that socialization need not to
be the key factor responsible for gender differ-
ences in fearfulness.
Although gender differences in fears were
reported in a number of studies, their evolutionary
origin is much less clear. Certain researchers have
Universal Human Fears 3
suggested that a higher fear of large carnivore
predators in females can be explained by females
lower physical condition compared with the phys-
ical condition of males (Røskaft et al. 2003;
Prokop and Fančovičová 2013). Indeed, females
and children who are physically weaker than
males are less likely to survive predatory attacks
of large carnivores (Treves and Naughton-Treves
1999). The disease avoidance hypothesis suggests
that vulnerability to infectious diseases makes
people more sensitive to a potential danger.
Indeed, females are more disease vulnerable than
males, and more disease-vulnerable people per-
ceive animals as more dangerous than less
disease-vulnerable people (Prokop and
Fančovičová 2013). These two hypotheses can
be viewed as compatible, because disease vulner-
ability is associated with a poorer perceived phys-
ical condition (Prokop and Fančovičová 2013).
The parental investment hypothesis proposes
that females take care of children and have to be
more sensitive to danger in order to protect their
offspring (Røskaft et al. 2003). This hypothesis
has not received empirical support as yet.
Why Humans Display a Fear of Harmless
Stimuli
About 100 years ago, John Watson demonstrated
that an infant (9-month-old Little Albert) can be
conditioned to fear a white rat by pairing the
presentation of the rat with a loud aversive noise.
Upon seeing the rat, Little Albert became
extremely distressed, crying and crawling away.
Interestingly, Little Alberts fear was later gener-
alized to the sight of several other furry objects,
such as a rabbit, a furry dog, and a sealskin coat
and even a Santa Claus mask with white cotton
balls in the beard. It was later proposed of course
that classic conditioning is not the only way one
acquires fear. Observations, verbally transmitted
information, and some other mechanisms (LoBue
2013) can also develop fear.
This simple example of Little Alberts gener-
alization of fear may explain why humans mani-
fest a fear of certain fear-irrelevant stimuli:
harmless snakes (Arrindell et al. 2003)or
wormlike invertebrates (Schlegel et al. 2015) are
considered dangerous or unlikeable, although
they do not pose an actual danger. All of these
stimuli are associated with potentially dangerous
cues, because they supercially look like danger-
ous animals. The supercial perception of poten-
tially harmful stimuli is favored by natural
selection in order to minimize the likelihood of
failing to register the presence of genuine danger
(the smoke detector principle, Nesse 2005).
Although the avoidance of harmless snake could
be perceived as erroneous(false-positive error),
it is still less risky than erroneous detection of a
truly harmful snake (false-negative error). That is,
predictions guided by the evolutionary theory do
not propose that all fears should have reasonable
explanations in our ancestral past, but that certain
fears can also stem from similarities with fear-
relevant stimuli. Finally, some fears are condi-
tioned in everyday life. Children can, for example,
easily acquire fear of a medical doctor simply
because pain from vaccination is associated with
the doctor. The same mechanism can be applied to
dogs showing a fear of veterinarians.
Conclusion
The protective emotion of fear was highly bene-
cial in the ancestral environment where it evolved.
Modern society, where the risk of being attacked
by a predator is low, may perceive certain fears as
irrational, but a better understanding of our evo-
lutionary past would seem to be helpful in
reaching an understanding of our emotions. Com-
parative research on nonhuman primates and
human infants where socialization is minimal
seems to be promising in uncovering the origins
and adaptive functions of common fears.
Acknowledgments I thank David Livingstone for
improving the English. This study was partly funded by
grant VEGA no. 1/0104/16.
4 Universal Human Fears
Cross-References
Evolution
Fear
Predators
References
Almeida, A., Vasconcelos, C., & Strecht-Ribeiro,
O. (2014). Attitudes toward animals: A study of Portu-
guese children. Anthrozoös, 27, 173190.
Arrindell, W. A., Eisemann, M., Richter, J., Oei, T. P.,
Caballo, V. E., van der Ende, J., ... Sica, C. (2003).
Phobic anxiety in 11 nations: Part I: Dimensional con-
stancy of the ve-factor model. Behaviour Research
and Therapy,41, 461479.
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in
the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus mon-
keys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 16, 372389.
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1991). Selective associations in
the origins of phobic fears and their implications for
behavior therapy. In P. Martin (Ed.), Handbook of
behavior therapy and psychological science: An inte-
grative approach (pp. 413434). Oxford: Pergamon
Press.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man
and animals. London: Fontana Press.
DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in
the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear.
Developmental Science, 12, 201207.
Ekman, P. (1977). Facial expression. In A. Siegman &
S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal communication and
behavior (pp. 97126). New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Gullone, E. (2000). The development of normal fear:
A century of research. Clinical Psychology Review,
20, 429451.
Isbell, L. A. (2009). The fruit, the tree, and the serpent.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: A view
through fear. Progress in Brain Research, 195,
431442.
LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early
attention shapes our most common fears. Child Devel-
opment Perspectives, 7,3842.
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of
threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Sci-
ence, 13, 221228.
LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most:
A developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli.
Developmental Review, 33, 285303.
Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and tness: An
evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethology
and Sociobiology, 15, 247261.
Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the regulation
of defenses: A signal detection analysis of the smoke
detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26,
88105.
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fear, phobias and pre-
paredness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear
learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483522.
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001a). Emotion
drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130,
466478.
Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001b). The face
in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with sche-
matic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 80, 381396.
Prokop, P., & Fančovičová, J. (2013). Self-protection ver-
sus disease avoidance: The perceived physical condi-
tion is associated with fear of predators in humans.
Journal of Individual Differences, 34,1523.
Rakison, D. H. (2009). Does womens greater fear of
snakes and spiders originate in infancy? Evolution
and Human Behavior, 30, 438444.
Rakison, D. H., & Derringer, J. L. (2008). Do infants
possess an evolved spider-detection mechanism? Cog-
nition, 107, 381393.
Robins, L. N., & Regier, D. A. (1991). Psychiatric disor-
ders in America: The epidemiologic catchment area
study. New York, NY: Free Press.
Røskaft, E., Bjerke, T., Kaltenborn, B. P., Linnell, J. D. C.,
& Andersen, R. (2003). Patterns of self-reported fear
towards large carnivores among the Norwegian
public. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 184198.
Schlegel, J., & Rupf, R. (2010). Attitudes towards potential
animal agship species in nature conservation:
A survey among students of different educational insti-
tutions. Journal for Nature Conservation, 18, 278290.
Schlegel, J., Breuer, G., & Rupf, R. (2015). Local insects as
agship species to promote nature conservation?
A survey among primary school children on their atti-
tudes toward invertebrates. Anthrozoös, 28, 229245.
Seligman, M. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behav-
iour Therapy, 2, 307320.
Treves, A., & Naughton-Treves, L. (1999). Risk and
opportunity for humans coexisting with large carni-
vores. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 275282.
Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Nguyen, M., Hori,
E., Maior, R. S., Tomaz, C., Tran, A. H., Ono, T., &
Nishijo, H. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobio-
logical evidence of past selection for rapid detection of
snakes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
ences, 110, 1900019005.
Warrell, D. A. (2010). Snake bite. The Lancet, 375,7788.
Universal Human Fears 5
... Por el contrario, en otras generan repudio, rechazo y, sobre todo, temor, llegando hasta ser la representación en la Tierra del mismísimo Ángel Negro en el Libro del Génesis, tan influyente para la humanidad. Luego de millones de años de coevolución, las serpientes parecen representar un arquetipo de fobia humana que automáticamente produce miedo (Gomes et al. 2017;Öhman and Mineka 2003;Öhman et al. 2012;Prokop 2016). Así, en muchos sectores del planeta, las serpientes están entre los animales que más repulsión causan a los seres humanos, y por ello se las persigue y aniquila de forma indiscriminada (da Costa Cardoso et al. 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Los accidentes por ofidios son un problema para los habitantes de las pequeñas poblaciones rurales del noroeste de Córdoba, quienes viven y trabajan en contacto con los ambientes donde habitan las serpientes. Por consiguiente, resulta relevante estudiar y conocer la percepción y los saberes ancestrales de los pobladores de esta zona en relación con la fauna de ofidios, con el objetivo de diagnosticar de manera adecuada esta problemática en un marco regional, y realizar actividades de prevención de accidentes ofídicos. Se realizaron entrevistas confeccionadas con preguntas relativas al conocimiento de las serpientes de la zona, a la nomenclatura popular, al uso, a las creencias o mitos sobre estos animales y a las acciones que se realizan ante sus mordeduras y para prevenirlas. Se emplearon imágenes de las especies más comunes de la zona a fin de verificar los nombres vernáculos mencionados por los entrevistados. Los resultados muestran que los habitantes de la región conocen 15 de las 20 especies que —se sabe— habitan el lugar. Se registraron conocimientos ecológicos, nombres vernáculos y mitos asociados a las especies de serpientes conocidas. Se destaca la claridad en la identificación de las 4 consideradas peligrosas. El 62% de los encuestados matan o matarían al animal ante un encuentro. Con respecto a las acciones ante mordeduras, se registró el uso de prácticas domésticas basadas en plantas o en acciones mecánicas sobre la mordedura (en general, poco recomendadas). Los resultados de este estudio aportan elementos a la generación de herramientas útiles para desarrollar planes de manejo comunitario tendientes al uso y la conservación de especies de serpientes, y a la mejora de la convivencia entre la gente y estos animales.
... Although mortality due to European Vipera spp. bites is generally low, the fear and disgust regarding snakes is widespread and deeply rooted [7][8][9][10][11][12]25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction/Objective A countrywide survey of venomous snakebites was never conducted in Serbia: the making of a central register was initiated only in 2018. We collected and analyzed the available data regarding venomous snakebites in the Užice region (Western Serbia). The previous analysis from this hospital was published in 1968. Methods We retrospectively analyzed the data regarding the venomous snakebite patients treated at the Užice General Hospital between 2006 and 2018 and compared these with the data from the 1960s, from one more hospital in Serbia, and from two ex-Yugoslav countries. Results In 13 years, 249 persons were treated. Of all cases, 10.4% were with inconspicuous symptoms (mild pain at the place of bite), 68.7% were with mild to moderate symptoms, and 20.9% were more or less severe. No fatalities were recorded. Conclusion Although usually not a life-threatening issue, venomous snakebites are quite common and can cause serious complications. With proper education, many can be avoided. Also, bearing in mind not only the biodiversity per se but also the importance of snakes' venoms for the making of various medically important products, we emphasize the need for proper protection of all three venomous snake species in Serbia, namely Vipera ammodytes (nose-horned viper), V. berus (European adder), and V. ursinii (meadow viper).
... Such responses are known to be especially triggered by wild animals such as snakes, tigers and wolves, such as the one used in our design [38]. Moreover, such fears are considered 'universal', due to their evolutionary nature, meaning that they are experienced by all humans [61]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışmada, siyasal bağnazlığın demografik değişkenler ve korku biçimleri ile ilişkisi analiz edilmiştir. Bağnazlık, fikirde, dinde ve ideolojide sıklıkla karşılaşılan bir davranış biçimi olup bir inanışa aşırı ölçüde bağlanmak, başka bir inanışı kabul etmemektir. Bağnazlık köklerini önyargıdan alır ve önyargının katılaşmış hali olarak da görülebilir. Bu davranışın sosyal yaşam içinde bir çok faktörün etkisiyle ortaya çıktığı söylenebilir. Bu faktörler, güvensizlik ve korku, kişilik yapısı, bilgisizlik, çıkar, toplumdaki hızlı yapılaşma, felsefi düşünme eksikliği, kendi eksikliğini örtme ve kendine güvensizliktir. Bu araştırmanın örneklem grubu seçkisiz olmayan amaçsal örneklemeye göre ve Elazığ ili evreninde kartopu örnekleme yöntemide dahil edilerek çevresi ve kendisi tarafından bağnaz kabul edilen kişilerden oluşturulmuştur. Araştırma, bazı parti merkezlerine ulaşarak ve toplum içerisinde kartopu örneklem yöntemi kullanılarak 423 kişiden elde edilen verilerle yapılmıştır. Araştırmada sonuçlar SPSS 22 programıyla değerlendirilmeye tabi tutulmuştur. İstatistiksel olarak değerlendirmeler, ortalama, T testi, Anova analizi olarak yapılmıştır. Ortalamalar standart sapma ile birlikte verilmiştir. Araştırma sonucunda siyasal bağnazlık ile eleştirilme, alay edilme, yalnızlık, cezalandırılma, iş bulma korku biçimleri ve bazı demografik değişkenler ile anlamlı ilişki bulunmuştur.
Article
Full-text available
Many of the body’s adaptive responses, such as pain, fever, and fear, are defenses that remain latent until they are aroused by cues that indicate the presence of a threat. Natural selection should shape regulation mechanisms that express defenses only in situations where their benefits exceed their costs, but defenses are often expressed in situations where they seem unnecessary, with much resulting useless suffering. An explanation emerges from a signal detection analysis of the costs and benefits that shaped defense regulation mechanisms. Quantitative modeling of optimal regulation for all-ornone defenses and for continuously variable defenses leads to several conclusions. First, an optimal system for regulating inexpensive all-or-none defenses against the uncertain presence of large threats will express many false alarms. Second, the optimum level of expression for graded defenses is not at the point where the costs of the defense and the danger are equal, but is instead where the marginal cost of additional defense exceeds the marginal benefit. Third, in the face of uncertainty and skewed payoff functions, the optimal response threshold may not be the point with the lowest cost. Finally, repeated exposures to certain kinds of danger may adaptively lower response thresholds, making systems vulnerable to runaway positive feedback. While we await quantitative data that can refine such models, a general theoretical perspective on the evolution of defense regulation can help to guide research and assist clinical decision making.
Article
Full-text available
Fear is one of our most salient emotions, and one that is shared among humans and nonhumans alike. Traditional and modern views of how we acquire fear suggest that it is learned through conditioning or observation. However, an interesting aspect of human fears is that they are not all created equal—some fears are more likely to be experienced than others. In this article, I discuss some recent developmental research that sheds new light on why we are more likely to experience certain fears over others, and how attention and learning might work together to produce some of our most common fears and anxieties.
Article
Full-text available
In this study we analyzed the attitudes toward different animals in 210 Portuguese children: 107 boys and 103 girls, aged between 8 and 10 years, attending the 3rd; and 4th; years of primary school. We used a questionnaire with two distinct parts. In the first part, the children were asked about the degree to which they liked 25 different animals, using a scale ranging from –7 (strongly dislike) to 7 (like very much), and to give their reasons for the value attributed. In the second part, they were asked whether in the event of the animals being threatened with extinction, it would be important to save any of them. We also asked for the reasons for their opinion. The most popular animals were big mammals and also birds. Certain gender differences were present, with boys preferring predators and other animals with a traditionally bad image, like bats and sharks. The most disliked animals were insects, but also those that were thought of as a danger to humans. However, we found a moderate positive correlation between liking and saving an animal, although this was lower in the case of the girls toward several animals. This shows that a negative perception of an animal does not always mean a negative attitude toward it. In part, the reasons for liking an animal were different from the reasons for saving it. All the results are important for the design of primary school teaching activities involving animals, including the fact that some reasons that the children gave revealed a lack of knowledge about the meaning of certain behaviors of the animals and of their ecological role.
Article
Full-text available
Fear is one of our most basic emotions. It is an important social signal and alerts us to when a situation is safe or risky. Interestingly, not all fears are created equal: Several researchers have proposed that humans develop specific fears, such as fear of threatening stimuli, more readily than others. Here we discuss three major theories of fear acquisition, and consider the possibility that some fears are privileged in learning. Second, we review a growing literature that suggests that humans have perceptual biases that quickly draw attention to threatening stimuli in the environment. In particular, we highlight recent developmental work that shows that even infants and young children respond rapidly to the presence of threat well before they acquire any threat-relevant fears. Finally, we argue that such biases may play a causal role in privileging fear learning for certain threats, and we suggest directions for future work that can clarify whether early biases in perception indeed facilitate the development of our most common fears.
Article
Participants searched for discrepant fear-relevant pictures (snakes or spiders) in grid-pattern arrays of fear-irrelevant pictures belonging to the same category (flowers or mushrooms) and vice versa. Fear-relevant pictures were found more quickly than fear-irrelevant ones. Fear-relevant, but not fear-irrelevant, search was unaffected by the location of the target in the display and by the number of distractors, which suggests parallel search for fear-relevant targets and serial search for fear-irrelevant targets. Participants specifically fearful of snakes but not spiders (or vice versa) showed facilitated search for the feared objects but did not differ from controls in search for nonfeared fear-relevant or fear-irrelevant, targets. Thus, evolutionary relevant threatening stimuli were effective in capturing attention, and this effect was further facilitated if the stimulus was emotionally provocative.
Article
An evolved module for fear elicitation and fear learning with 4 characteristics is proposed. (a) The fear module is preferentially activated in aversive contexts by stimuli that are fear relevant in an evolutionary perspective. (b) Its activation to such stimuli is automatic. (c) It is relatively impenetrable to cognitive control. (d) It originates in a dedicated neural circuitry, centered on the amygdala. Evidence supporting these propositions is reviewed from conditioning studies, both in humans and in monkeys; illusory correlation studies; studies using unreportable stimuli; and studies from animal neuroscience. The fear module is assumed to mediate an emotional level of fear learning that is relatively independent and dissociable from cognitive learning of stimulus relationships.
Article
Flagship species play an important role in promoting nature conservation. However, although the significance of invertebrates in biodiversity and ecosystem services is undisputed, they are rarely used as flagship species. A focused approach to better understand the drivers of differences in attitudes toward invertebrates, and insects in particular, would be helpful for selecting and establishing insects as flagship species, especially in a local context for local conservation purposes. Using a standard questionnaire, a total of 363 children, predominantly aged 10 to 12 years, were asked about their attitudes toward 18 invertebrate species indigenous to Switzerland. The species, 14 insect species and four other invertebrates, were individually presented in a color photograph without any background information. Based on ordinal regression models, the survey revealed substantial affinity rating differences across the invertebrates selected. Gender, species knowledge, preferred leisure activities, and family membership of a nature protection organization proved to be significant predictors for children’s attitudes in general, and for some specific species in particular. Additionally, existing species knowledge was analyzed and was found to neither depend on school location (urban/rural) nor on gender. The authors propose the inclusion of local invertebrates and species knowledge in the curriculum in early years at primary school while applying teaching methods that allow for real-life experience.