ArticlePDF Available

The Sooner, the Better: Early Reading to Children

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

As reading to children plays an important role in language development, primary caregivers are often encouraged to read to their children from a very young age. However, little is known about the age at which such reading should start. The linguistic skills of 104 children were assessed shortly before school entry. Their parents were asked how old their children were when they first read to them and how often they had read to their children. Almost half of the study children were read to before they were 6 months old. The age at which children were first read to was closely associated with family characteristics such as socioeconomic status, the frequency with which children were read to as preschoolers, and with children’s linguistic and cognitive competencies. The findings imply that reading books to very young children indeed contributes meaningfully to a favorable home literacy environment and supports children’s language development
Content may be subject to copyright.
SAGE Open
October-December 2016: 1 –11
© The Author(s) 2016
DOI: 10.1177/2158244016672715
sgo.sagepub.com
Creative Commons CC-BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License
(http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of
the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages
(https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Article
Introduction
Children’s language and literacy competence does not begin
when children enter school—Children’s literacy learning
starts well before formal schooling, and studies have shown
that children are sensitive to speech even prenatally (e.g.,
Moon, Lagercrantz, & Kuhl, 2013; Partanen et al., 2013).
Parents and primary caregivers (subsequently referred to as
parents) are highly influential in a child’s early learning as
parent–child interactions are frequent and ongoing. Indeed,
research shows that the home literacy environment (HLE) is
the context in which children first acquire the language and
literacy skills that equip them to make sense of, describe, and
participate in the world (e.g., Liebeskind, Piotrowski,
Lapierre, & Linebarger, 2014; Niklas & Schneider, 2013;
Raikes et al., 2006).
Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological theory offers a theo-
retical framework for the influence of the HLE on child
development. According to Bronfenbrenner, children belong
to a complex and evolving social and cultural ecology. His
theory posits that distal elements of a child’s ecological sys-
tem such as extended family, community, and society have
some impact on children, but proximal factors such as a
child’s immediate family have the most influence on a child’s
development. Similarly, distal family characteristics such as
family socioeconomic status (SES) based on income, occu-
pation, or education and family migration background are far
less influential than proximal family characteristics such as
parent–child interactions (Farrant & Zubrick, 2012; Niklas,
2015; Niklas, Möllers, & Schneider, 2013).
Other theoretical frameworks relevant to a discussion of
the influence of the HLE include Vygotsky’s (1978) social
constructivist theory and Bourdieu’s (1986) social theory.
According to Vygotsky, children learn through observation
and interaction with knowledgeable others in social contexts.
In the context of language and literacy learning, the impor-
tance of parents modeling literacy activities and supporting
their children’s emerging literacy skills is clear. Bourdieu
theorized that the cultural capital to which an individual or a
family has access is a key component of the HLE. This cul-
tural capital includes cultural objects and resources such as
books and works of art, as well as the ability to utilize and
learn from such objects and resources. Consequently, a fam-
ily’s cultural practices such as reading and writing are closely
associated with the family’s cultural capital.
The multifaceted HLE incorporates various literacy-
related activities such as parental reading behavior, library
visits, teaching of letters and sounds, and owning books at
home (Niklas, 2015). However, reading to children is a core
672715SGOXXX10.1177/2158244016672715SAGE OpenNiklas et al.
research-article2016
1University of Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Corresponding Author:
Frank Niklas, Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of
Melbourne, 4/100 Leicester Street, Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia.
Email: frank.niklas@unimelb.edu.au
The Sooner, the Better: Early
Reading to Children
Frank Niklas1, Caroline Cohrssen1, and Collette Tayler1
Abstract
As reading to children plays an important role in language development, primary caregivers are often encouraged to read to
their children from a very young age. However, little is known about the age at which such reading should start. The linguistic
skills of 104 children were assessed shortly before school entry. Their parents were asked how old their children were when
they first read to them and how often they had read to their children. Almost half of the study children were read to before
they were 6 months old. The age at which children were first read to was closely associated with family characteristics such
as socioeconomic status, the frequency with which children were read to as preschoolers, and with children’s linguistic and
cognitive competencies. The findings imply that reading books to very young children indeed contributes meaningfully to a
favorable home literacy environment and supports children’s language development.
Keywords
storybook reading, onset of reading to a child, home literacy environment (HLE), parent–child interactions, linguistic
competencies
2 SAGE Open
element of the HLE (Bus, van IJzendoorn, & Pellegrini,
1995; Niklas, Nguyen, Cloney, Tayler, & Adams, 2016).
Many studies have focused on the impact that reading to
children has on children’s linguistic competencies (e.g.,
Edwards, 2014; Raikes et al., 2006), but little is known about
the age at which parents should start reading to their chil-
dren, whether the age of first reading predicts the frequency
with which a child is read to later on, or whether the age of
first reading predicts children’s competencies as preschool-
ers (e.g., DeBaryshe, 1993).
In this study, parents were asked how old their child was
when first read to. This first-reading age was then tested to
explore its relationship with various child outcomes shortly
prior to school entry.
Early Linguistic Development
The ability to read and write letters, with comprehension, is
the basis for success in formal education. Adults who show
greater literacy competencies often earn higher incomes,
have better job prospects, lower risk of mental illness, and
enjoy better health (Fawcett, 2003; Lyon, 2002). However,
no clear-cut borders exist between the precursors of reading
and spelling, such as children’s vocabulary and phonological
awareness, and children’s later reading and spelling skills
(Bowman, Donovan, & Burns, 2003; Schatschneider,
Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004; Torgesen,
2002; Torppa et al., 2007). Oral language skills such as
vocabulary and code-related skills such as phonological
awareness are interrelated domains of emergent literacy.
Consequently, the early ability to understand language and to
use or manipulate language expressively is very important
(Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Understanding the initial
stages of linguistic development and identifying aspects of
early literacy experiences that support children’s later read-
ing proficiency present opportunities to have a positive
impact on early linguistic skills of very young children.
As language and literacy experiences in the first three
years of life set the scene for children’s later literacy compe-
tencies, these experiences are critical. Passive listening and
nonverbal communication are a child’s first step toward
becoming an active talker, and differences in children’s abil-
ity to segment conversational speech signals are predictors
of later language development (Newman, Ratner, Jusczyk,
Jusczyk, & Dow, 2006). As children develop, language com-
prehension becomes increasingly important. For instance, in
a study conducted by Flax, Realpe-Bonilla, Roesler,
Choudhury, and Benasich (2009), children’s language com-
prehension at three years of age predicted various language
and reading measures at seven years of age. Furthermore,
children with stronger early linguistic skills tend to outper-
form children with weak early linguistic skills on assess-
ments of literacy abilities in school, and this gap appears to
widen over time (Bast & Reitsma, 1998; Torppa, Poikkeus,
Laakso, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2006). Parents who support
their children’s language and later emergent literacy learning
intensively by providing a high-quality HLE facilitate chil-
dren’s vocabulary acquisition (Mol, Bus, de Jong, & Smeets,
2008; Niklas & Schneider, 2015). Consequently, the HLE
appears to play an important role in children’s early linguis-
tic development.
HLE and Reading to Children
Research demonstrates that home learning environments are
associated with children’s numeracy abilities (e.g., Kleemans,
Peeters, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2012; Niklas & Schneider,
2014) and behavioral outcomes (e.g., Schmiedeler, Niklas, &
Schneider, 2014), and predict children’s literacy competen-
cies (e.g., Niklas & Schneider, 2013; Sénéchal & LeFevre,
2002). For instance, Niklas and Schneider (2013) demon-
strated that aspects of the HLE such as parental reading,
reading to the child, and the number of books in a household
predicted preschoolers’ vocabulary and phonological aware-
ness as well as the further development of literacy competen-
cies even when controlling for a range of child and family
background variables.
Sénéchal and LeFevre (2002, 2014) showed specific asso-
ciations of different aspects of the HLE with children’s lin-
guistic and literacy competencies. The informal literacy
environment consisted of variables measuring the shared
reading behavior in the family, whereas the formal literacy
environment took parents’ literacy teaching into account.
Parents’ literacy teaching predicted letter knowledge and
phonological awareness, whereas the informal literacy envi-
ronment was associated with vocabulary growth.
Despite the fact that the HLE is a multifaceted construct,
reading to children is still a fundamental element of this con-
struct. Exposure to books supports vocabulary acquisition
(Bus et al., 1995; Farrant & Zubrick, 2012; Prevoo et al.,
2014) as books typically contain more complex vocabulary
than common usage vocabulary (Sénéchal, LeFevre, Hudson,
& Lawseon, 1996). Early meta-analyses conducted by
Scarborough and Dobrich (1994) and Bus and colleagues
(1995) revealed that reading to children explained about 8%
of the variance in children’s linguistic competencies. This
finding is important as reading to children can be more easily
manipulated than, for instance, family SES. In addition, one
can assume that early effects of a positive learning environ-
ment are cumulative over time (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2001;
cf. Stanovich, 1986).
In addition to the frequency of reading and quality of read-
ing (Lever & Sénéchal, 2011; L. M. Phillips, Norris, &
Anderson, 2008; Sim & Berthelsen, 2014), the onset of read-
ing seems to play an important role (DeBaryshe, 1993; Niklas,
Cohrssen, Tayler, & Schneider, 2016; see also B. M. Phillips
& Lonigan, 2009). In their study, B. M. Phillips and Lonigan
(2009) analyzed the relationship of different measures of the
HLE—among these the onset of reading to children—with
various background characteristics of the family. Here, higher
Niklas et al. 3
quality HLE was associated with greater family income, better
caregiver education, and lower educator stress level. However,
no child outcome measures were included in the analyses, and
the focus was not on the onset of reading to children.
In her study, DeBaryshe (1993) found that the age at
which 41 two-year-old children were first read to was a bet-
ter predictor of oral language skills than the frequency with
which children were read to or visited the library. It should
be noted that correlational data only were used and no con-
trol variables for family or child characteristics were included
in this analysis. Given the small sample size, the young sam-
ple, and the fact that important family characteristics such as
family language or migration background were not taken
into account, the findings of DeBaryshe need to be regarded
as preliminary—important as they are.
In a German study (Niklas, Cohrssen, Tayler, & Schneider,
2016), the age children were first read to was a significant
predictor of reading frequency in kindergarten, children’s
rhyming abilities, vocabulary, and, to a lesser extent, other
cognitive abilities such as numeracy skills and intelligence
as well. However, the onset of shared reading was assessed
according to categories only and about 75% of the sample
fell into the earliest category (shared reading started before
the child age of two years). In addition, although research
indicates that the association of the HLE with children’s lin-
guistic and cognitive outcomes seems to be comparable
across countries, there is also evidence that the HLE might
be more closely associated with children’s vocabulary in
German-speaking contexts than in English-speaking con-
texts (Niklas, Tayler, & Schneider, 2015).
Although these studies indicate that starting shared read-
ing early seems to be beneficial for children’s linguistic
development, little is known about whether the age at which
parents first read to children is a specific predictor of English-
speaking preschoolers’ linguistic competencies, even when
controlling for child and family characteristics. In addition,
we do not know whether children start to benefit from par-
ents reading to them from a particular age.
The Current Study
Research has shown that literacy-based parent–child interac-
tions in general, and reading to children in particular, support
the development of children’s linguistic competencies (Bus
et al., 1995; Liebeskind et al., 2014; Sénéchal, Pagan, Lever,
& Ouellette, 2008). However, despite a few studies analyz-
ing the onset of reading (e.g., DeBaryshe, 1993; Niklas,
Cohrssen, Tayler, & Schneider, 2016), there are still ques-
tions to answer. This study uses data from an Australian
study to analyze the association of the onset of parent–child
reading with (a) the frequency with which children were read
to in the year prior to commencing formal school education,
and (b) various child outcomes.
We expected the age at which children were first read to
would be closely related to the frequency with which children
were read to in the year before school as well as to children’s
linguistic abilities, and would predict these even when con-
trolling for child and family characteristics. As shared reading
seems to be a fairly specific predictor for children’s linguistic
competencies, we expected weaker associations with other
cognitive child abilities such as concentration, numeracy
competencies, and intelligence. We also anticipated that the
onset of shared reading would not predict these abilities sig-
nificantly when controlling for child and family characteris-
tics. In an additional exploratory approach, we tested whether
and how the associations between the onset of shared reading
and the outcome variables changed for different subgroups of
children by incrementally excluding children whose parents
started reading to their children comparatively late.
Method
Participants
All 104 preschoolers attended one of four child care centers
located across one local government area in Melbourne.
After obtaining approval from local government, formal con-
sent to conduct the study was obtained from the center coor-
dinators, directors, and teachers. Each family registered as
using the various early childhood settings was invited to par-
ticipate in the study. Members of the research team were on
hand to obtain consent from parents and caregivers and to
answer questions at morning drop-off and afternoon pickup
times.
Children were assessed toward the end of their final term
in kindergarten. In the sample, boys (55.8%) outnumber girls
(44.2%), with a mean age of approximately 5 years 2 months
(SD = 4.0 months) at the time of assessment. About 6% of the
sample spoke a language other than English as the main lan-
guage at home. In 37% of the sample, at least one of the
parents, or the child, was born outside Australia. However,
when participants born in a country in which English is an
official language were excluded from the migrant group, the
overall percentage of children with a migration background
decreased to 20.6%.
The assessment of children took place in the kindergarten
rooms during November and December 2014, and thus about
two months before the children entered school as the school
year in Australia starts at the end of January. Multiple assess-
ment tasks were used and those relevant to the analyses are
briefly described below. In addition, parents were asked to
complete surveys on family background characteristics and
their reading behavior at three points during 2014.
Measures—Assessment of Children’s Linguistic
and Other Cognitive Competencies
Several subtests were drawn from the Woodcock–Johnson
III Tests of Cognition and Achievement (WJ III; Mather &
Woodcock 2001a, 2001b; McGrew, Woodcock, & Mather,
4 SAGE Open
2001). The WJ III is a standardized, normed measure fre-
quently used to assess children’s achievement (e.g., Chien
et al., 2010; Duncan et al., 2007).
Verbal Comprehension. This test has four subtests: Picture
Vocabulary, Synonyms, Antonyms, and Verbal Analogies.
Each subtest starts with a sample item to demonstrate how to
approach the question and for the researcher to provide feed-
back to the child. Thereafter, no feedback is provided.
Together, these subtests measure different aspects of children’s
acquired vocabulary skills. For four- to six-year-old children,
estimated reliabilities on Verbal Comprehension range from
.89 to .90.
Sound Awareness, subtest: Rhyming. Here, children had to pro-
vide a word that rhymed with a given word or sentence. This
subtest becomes increasingly complex the further one
advances through it. The median reliability for WJ III Sound
Awareness is .81.
Concept Formation. This task requires rule application and
frequent switching from one rule to another. It is a broad
measure of the ability to reason, form concepts, and solve
problems, using unfamiliar information or novel procedures.
For each presented item, the child tries to figure out the rule
that divides a set of symbols into two groups. Other than in
similar assessment tools, during the assessment of concept
formation, the child receives feedback on the performance,
and in case of wrong answers the correct one is pointed out
and explained (median test reliability = .94).
In addition to the WJ III subtest, two other assessments
were used to measure children’s short-term concentration
and their numeracy abilities.
Concentration. The Frankfurt Tests for Five-Year-Olds–Con-
centration (FTF-K; cf. Esser, 2002) measures children’s abil-
ity to work quickly, to remain goal-oriented, and to
concentrate. In this timed, paper-and-pencil worksheet test,
children are required to draw a line through as many pears as
possible in 90 seconds while disregarding the apples. Pears
and apples are black line drawings presented in rows and fill-
ing one side of an A4 page (retest reliability for up to 3
weeks: rtt = .79-.85).
Numeracy abilities. Four subtests were used to assess children’s
numeracy abilities: Counting, Identifying Number Symbols,
Stating Number Values, and Estimating Amounts. In the
Counting subtest, children were asked to count forward and
backward and to name numbers that immediately precede or
follow a spoken number word. They were also shown number
symbols from 1 to 21 in a nonnumerical sequence and asked to
name the number. The number values task required children to
match cards depicting different numbers of stick-figure draw-
ings of children with a spoken quantity or with an indicated
numeral on a number line from 1 to 10. Finally, children were
presented with a subitizing task. Dotted cards were revealed
for 3 s and the children were first required to state how many
dots had been seen and then to compare the number of dots
presented. The sum scores of each of the four subtests were
combined in a total sum score that measured children’s early
numeracy ability (Cronbach’s α = .70).
Measures—Parent Survey
Parents were requested to complete surveys about child and
family characteristics and the HLE, and 98 surveys were
returned. In addition to providing information on migration
background and main language spoken at home (see sample
description), respondents were asked about their occupation
and their partner’s occupation to assign prestige values to
these occupations (cf. Wegener, 1988). Here, values from 20
(an unskilled laborer) to 186.8 (a physician) are assigned.
The household occupation awarded the highest prestige was
used as an indicator of family SES (M = 99.7, SD = 33.5).
Parents were asked whether they read to their child, inde-
pendent of the language used, and how old the child was when
first read to (parents were required to enter years and months
in the space provided). Only one family reported not having
read to their child. More than one third of the sample (37.5%)
indicated that they started reading to their child within the first
three months after the child’s birth and four families reported
that they read to their child from birth. This early start was
sometimes attributed to the participating child having older
siblings whom they joined for reading sessions. Another third
of the families started reading to their child when they were
four to six months old. Six months was the mode (24%) and
6.34 months the mean (SD = 5.90) for this sample. These find-
ings align very well with the reported average onset of reading
in a study by B. M. Phillips and Lonigan (2009). Almost all
families had started reading to their children by the time the
children were 15 months old (93.8%). Two parents reported
starting to read to their children when they were 18 months
old, three parents first read to their children when they were
two years old, and one family started reading to their child
when he was three years old (see Figure 1).
Finally, on three occasions during 2014, parents were
asked to report the number of days during the preceding
week on which the study child had been read to. The average
of these three values (Cronbach’s α = .89; rtt = .65-.80) was
used to indicate the average number of days a child was read
to in a typical week during the year prior to commencing
school. Almost half of the families reported having read to
their child every day (49%). Another 25% of the respondents
read to their children on five or six days per week, and 11.2%
read to their child on three days or fewer.
Analytic Approach
First, correlations and descriptive statistics for all study vari-
ables are reported. Pearson’s r was used as correlation
Niklas et al. 5
coefficient for all correlations except for the correlations of
onset age of shared reading with the other variables as this
variable was slightly positively skewed (skewness = 2.3).
Here, Spearman’s rho was used instead. Thereafter, correla-
tions (Spearman’s ρ) of first reading to a child with the other
variables are reported for reduced samples, excluding fami-
lies that report late onset of first parent–child reading. Here,
we started with the full sample of children with available
data (child age when the child was first read to 36 months)
and reduced the sample to children aged 12, 9, 6, or 3 months
and less when read to for the first time. This exploratory
approach tested whether the associations of the study vari-
ables change for these subsamples. Finally, regression analy-
ses were conducted to predict both current reading frequency
and children’s linguistic, and other cognitive competencies
by the age the child was first read to, while controlling for
child age, sex, and main language as well as family SES and
migration background. Here, in a first model, the control
variables are used as predictors, and in a second model, first
parent–child reading is introduced into the model as addi-
tional predictor.
Results
Table 1 presents the correlations and descriptive statistics for
all study variables. The later that parents started reading to
their child, the less frequently they read to their child in the
year prior to school. No significant associations of first read-
ing with child sex, age, and main language and with concen-
tration and numeracy abilities were found. However, children
who were read to earlier showed greater rhyming ability, ver-
bal comprehension, and concept formation. In addition, first
reading was significantly associated with family background
variables. Here, families with a migration background or
with a lower SES began reading to their children later.
Children in these families also showed significantly lower
verbal comprehension. As expected, all linguistic and
cognitive abilities were highly correlated. Current shared
reading frequency showed similar associations with the
study variables as onset of shared reading with the exception
that the frequency children were read to was significantly
associated with children’s numeracy abilities.
In a next step, correlations were again calculated, this
time for subsamples excluding families who started reading
to their children late. As can be seen in Table 2, excluding
families from the analysis who started reading to their chil-
dren after their child’s first birthday, or after the child was 9
months old, did not change the picture much although first
reading was no longer significantly associated with family
background variables. However, including only those fami-
lies who started reading to their children when the child was
6 months old or younger led to small, nonsignificant correla-
tions of first reading with the frequency with which the child
was read to in the final year of preschool and with children’s
verbal comprehension. Rhyming and concept formation
were still significantly associated with the onset of reading in
that children who were read to earlier within this subsample
still showed greater performance on these assessment tasks.
Finally, when we included only those families who started
reading to their child when the child was three months old or
younger, no significant correlations were found at all. In
addition, most correlations were close to 0.
In a last step, regression analyses were used to predict
reading frequency in the final preschool year, and children’s
linguistic and cognitive abilities by first reading, when con-
trolling for child and family characteristics (see Table 3).
Even after controlling for various child and family character-
istics, the age at which a child was first read to was a very
strong predictor of parent–child reading frequency in the
year prior to school (large effect size) and a significant pre-
dictor of children’s rhyming abilities and verbal comprehen-
sion (small to medium effect size). In comparison, first
reading was not a significant predictor of children’s concen-
tration and numerical abilities (p > .60) and only a margin-
ally significant predictor of children’s concept formation (p
< .10; 3% of additional explained variance by age the child
was first read to; small effect).
In addition, older children showed greater competencies
but were not read to more frequently, and there was a ten-
dency that children who spoke English as their main lan-
guage and came from higher SES families performed better
in the linguistic tasks. Finally, boys were read to more fre-
quently in our sample, and girls performed better on the
rhyming task and slightly better on concept formation and
concentration.
Discussion
Early support of children’s linguistic competencies is essen-
tial as linguistic and literacy skills play a major role in every-
day life and are important for later academic achievement
and life success in general (Fawcett, 2003; Lyon, 2002).
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
012345678912 14 15 18 24 36
Child age in months when the child was first read to
%
Figure 1. Distribution of child age in months when the child was
first read to.
6 SAGE Open
Table 2. Correlational Analyses (Spearman’s ρ) for Age, the Child Was First Read to in Months, With the Study Variables for Different
Subgroups of Children.
First reading
(child age in
months) n
Reading
frequency Sexa
Age in
months
Migration
backgroundb
Main
languagecSES Rhyming
Verbal
comprehension
Concept
formation Concentration
Numeracy
abilities
Age
36 96 −.41** .10 .06 .25* −.16 −.29** −.36** −.33** −.38** −.15 −.13
12 88 −.30** −.03 .01 .25* −.04 −.14 −.29** −.25* −.35** −.07 −.08
9 81 −.22* .00 −.09 .17 −.07 −.03 −.31** −.25* −.39** −.11 −.18
6 68 −.04 .06 −.02 .12 −.04 −.04 −.32** −.17 −.31* −.13 −.14
3 36 −.06 .14 .19 .07 −.02 .28 −.07 .05 −.24 .08 .24
Note. SES = socioeconomic status.
a0 = female; 1 = male.
b0 = no migration background; 1 = one or both parents and/or child born abroad.
c0 = another language than English is child’s main language; 1 = English is main language.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
Results in this study demonstrate that early onset of parent–
child reading is favorable for children’s linguistic competen-
cies (cf. DeBaryshe, 1993; Niklas, Cohrssen, Tayler, &
Schneider, 2016). Whereas onset of shared reading explained
a similar amount of variance in both, children’s rhyming
abilities and verbal comprehension, research indicates that
the onset of shared reading should be a better predictor of
children’s linguistic abilities such as vocabulary, as opposed
to literacy competencies such as sound awareness (Niklas,
Cohrssen, Tayler, & Schneider, 2016; Sénéchal & LeFevre,
2002, 2014).
Furthermore, the age at which children were first read to
was highly correlated with the frequency with which they
were read to in the year before school. This finding indicates
that the early parent–child reading onset may be regarded as
a marker for general literacy habits in a family. Clearly, the
age at which children were first read to represents an impor-
tant aspect of the HLE (cf. Niklas, Cohrssen, Tayler, &
Schneider, 2016; B. M. Phillips & Lonigan, 2009).
In a set of additional regression analyses, we included the
current frequency with which the child was read to as an addi-
tional predictor of child outcomes. This led to the onset of
shared reading becoming a nonsignificant predictor of chil-
dren’s linguistic competencies. Whereas children’s vocabu-
lary was predicted by current frequency of shared reading, no
significant amount of variance of rhyming was explained any
longer by these two variables. These findings may be partly
due to multicollinearity for which we found evidence.
However, they also indicate that current reading behavior
seems to be a better predictor of child outcomes than the onset
of shared reading, while the onset of shared reading may be
more of an indicator of the later HLE. Small sample size may
partly explain the reduced and nonsignificant amount of
explained variance in child linguistic competencies by onset
of shared reading, given that in a larger German study, onset
of shared reading remained a significant predictor of child
vocabulary and rhyming abilities when current frequency of
shared reading was controlled (cf. Niklas, Cohrssen, Tayler,
Table 1. Correlational Analyses and Descriptive Statistics (Means and Standard Deviations) for all Study Variables.
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 M SD
First read to (child age in months) −.41** .10 .06 .25* −.16 −.29** −.36** −.33** −.38** −.15 −.13 6.34 5.90
Reading frequency (2) .12 .00 −.05 .21* .28** .26* .35** .21* .12 .23* 5.74 1.59
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) (3) .26** .14 −.14 −.12 −.23* −.13 −.20* −.16 −.07 0.56 0.50
Age in months (4) −.01 .08 −.22* .12 .16 .14 .18 .28** 52.74 3.96
Migration backgrounda (5) −.25* −.06 −.15 −.24* −.23* −.13 −.09 0.37 0.49
Main languageb (6) .06 .18 .33** .12 .11 .11 0.94 0.24
SES (7) .29** .26* .19 .03 .11 99.71 33.53
Rhyming (8) .63** .34** .36** .39** 6.55 4.17
Verbal comprehension (9) .51** .40** .49** 22.09 5.64
Concept formation (10) .31** .43** 9.93 6.36
Concentration (11) .43** 28.41 5.87
Numeracy abilities (12) 21.45 5.97
Note. SES = socioeconomic status.
a0 = no migration background; 1 = one or both parents and/or child born abroad.
b0 = another language than English is child’s main language; 1 = English is main language.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
Niklas et al. 7
Table 3. Regression Analyses to Predict Frequency, Children Were Read to in the Last Year Before Formal Schooling, Children’s
Rhyming Abilities, Verbal Comprehension, Concept Formation, Concentration, and Numeracy Abilities.
Variable
Unstandardized
regression
coefficient B SE t value
Explained
variance R2
Gain ΔR2 by
first readinga
Reading frequency
Model 2 .32** .17**
Age −0.00 0.04 −0.03
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) 0.72 0.30 2.39*
Migration backgroundb0.18 0.30 0.59
Main languagec0.66 0.63 1.05
SES 0.01 0.01 1.21
First readinga−0.13 0.03 −4.62**
Rhyming
Model 2 .24** .04*
Age 0.24 0.11 2.31*
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) −1.71 0.83 −2.05*
Migration backgroundb−0.42 0.84 −0.50
Main languagec0.71 1.76 0.40
SES 0.03 0.01 2.10*
First readinga−0.17 0.08 −2.15*
Verbal comprehension
Model 2 .28** .04*
Age 0.32 0.14 2.33*
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) −0.93 1.10 −0.85
Migration backgroundb−1.47 1.11 −0.13
Main languagec4.57 2.31 1.97
SES 0.03 0.02 1.92
First readinga−0.21 0.10 −2.04*
Concept formation
Model 2 .18* .03
Age 0.37 0.17 2.21*
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) −2.36 1.32 −1.79
Migration backgroundb−2.12 1.33 −1.59
Main languagec−0.75 2.78 −0.27
SES 0.03 0.02 1.23
First readinga−0.21 0.12 −1.70
Concentration
Model 2 .09 .00
Age 0.35 0.16 2.15*
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) −2.25 1.28 −1.76
Migration backgroundb−1.00 1.29 −0.77
Main languagec0.71 2.69 0.26
SES 0.00 0.02 0.21
First readinga−0.06 0.12 −0.49
Numeracy abilities
Model 2 .13* .00
Age 0.51 0.16 3.20*
Sex (0 = female; 1 = male) −1.46 1.27 −1.14
Migration backgroundb−0.49 1.29 −0.38
Main languagec0.85 2.68 0.32
SES 0.03 0.02 1.29
First readinga−0.05 0.12 −0.40
Note. SES = socioeconomic status.
aFirst reading to child (child age in months).
b0 = no migration background; 1 = one or both parents and/or child born abroad.
c0 = another language than English is child’s main language; 1 = English is main language.
p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.
8 SAGE Open
& Schneider, 2016). Finally, current reading frequency also
significantly predicted children’s numeracy abilities. This
finding may be due to shared reading being associated with
numeracy-related activities in the home which were not con-
trolled (Niklas, 2015) or to the numeracy abilities assessed
depending to some extent on children’s verbal skills.
In exploratory analyses, the onset of shared reading
proved to be closely associated with later reading frequency
and with children’s linguistic competencies, even when
excluding those families from the sample who started read-
ing to their children comparatively late. Consequently, the
significant associations were not due to child and family
characteristics in the few cases in which the child was first
read to very late. The associations changed meaningfully
only when all children were removed from the sample who
were first read to at 6 months of age or later. This may be due
in part to the somewhat diminished variance in onset of read-
ing in the remaining subsamples. However, the variance in
all other study variables remained stable for all analyzed sub-
samples, suggesting that the findings are not the result of
lack of variance only. Consequently, parents should be
encouraged to start reading to their young children early.
Although it is unlikely to make much difference whether
parents start reading to children at the age of two or four
months, our findings indicate that reading aloud to a child
before the child reaches the age of six months appears to be
advantageous to the child’s linguistic development. Research
shows that children are sensitive to language even prenatally
(Moon et al., 2013; Partanen et al., 2013), yet in the first few
months after a child is born, general language experiences
are more likely to make a difference than reading aloud.
However, as no negative effects of a very early start of read-
ing to a child are to be expected, the sooner the better appears
to be best.
Whereas some studies found comprehensive measures of
the HLE to be a predictor of children’s numeracy abilities as
well (Anders et al., 2012), the onset of reading to a child
proved to be a specific predictor of children’s linguistic com-
petencies, but not of numeracy competencies or of concen-
tration. Obviously, shared book reading events present rich
opportunities for children to acquire new vocabulary, to
rehearse new words in extended conversations, to play with
language, and to experience the purpose of print media. All
of these experiences support children’s linguistic develop-
ment (Bus et al., 1995; Mol et al., 2008; Schatschneider
et al., 2004; Torgesen, 2002; Torppa et al., 2007).
The age at which children were first read to was also
related with children’s concept formation in correlational
analysis. Furthermore, first reading was a marginally signifi-
cant predictor of concept formation, after controlling for
child and family characteristics (p < .10, small effect size).
This aligns with recent studies that reported small to medium
effect size associations of the home learning environment
with intelligence (Frumkin, 2013; Kleemans et al., 2012;
Niklas, Cohrssen, & Tayler, 2016).
Also aligning with results from earlier research, both the
onset of parent–child reading and reading frequency were
closely associated with background variables such as family
SES and migration background (Aikens & Barbarin, 2008;
Edwards, 2014; Niklas et al., 2013; Prevoo et al., 2014).
However, the association of the age at which children were
first read to with children’s linguistic outcomes remained
significant when “late onset reading” children were excluded
from the sample, whereas the association with family back-
ground variables diminished. In addition, first reading
remained a significant predictor of children’s linguistic out-
comes when controlling for SES, migration background, and
child’s main language. The onset of shared reading is thus an
important predictor of both later shared reading frequency
and child linguistic competencies, independent of the family
background.
Some further interesting results were observed. As
expected in samples of children as young as these, child age
was a significant predictor of all child outcomes. In addition,
boys were read to more frequently, whereas girls showed
somewhat greater cognitive abilities. We can only speculate
on these findings. Given the small sample size, these results
may be random. Alternatively, the parents of boys in our
sample may have thought that their children would need
greater support and thus read to them more frequently.
Limitations
The onset and frequency of parent–child reading were self-
reported in parent surveys and such survey responses are sus-
ceptible to perceived social desirability. Prospective
longitudinal studies would be needed for more accuracy as
parents might also not remember correctly the age of their
child when they first started reading to him or her. However,
previous home learning environment studies have shown that
surveys provide reliable data and lead to findings that are
closely related to those obtained with other measures (cf.
Burgess, 2002). Consequently, although our approach cer-
tainly has to be regarded as exploratory and preliminary, and
the findings interpreted with caution, our correlational analy-
ses with subsamples may help to better understand the associa-
tion of onset of shared reading and various outcome variables,
and may provide a first hint at the child age at which shared
reading supports children’s linguistic development.
Beyond the limitation of self-reported, retrospective esti-
mations of the onset of shared reading by parents, other limi-
tations mark this study. The sample size was small and
characterized by, on average, middle to high SES families.
One may expect to find later onsets of reading and less fre-
quent reading to children in samples with a low to middle
family SES (Niklas, Cohrssen, Tayler, & Schneider, 2016).
Further research needs to test whether the associations found
in this study would change with a wider range of family SES.
Nonetheless, the onset of reading and the frequency with
which children were read to align very well with figures
Niklas et al. 9
reported in other international studies (Duursma, 2014; B.
M. Phillips & Lonigan, 2009).
In addition, it is not possible to interpret our results caus-
ally due to the high likelihood of gene–environment interac-
tions, that is, inherent parent and child characteristics
influencing both, the HLE and child outcomes. Future
research should test whether onset age of shared reading var-
ies between families with or without a genetic risk for dys-
lexia and analyze possible interactions.
We also did not assess whether children in families with a
background of migration were read to in English or in an
language other than English. As the child outcomes were all
assessed in English, this might have played a role. However,
in families with a migration background, children not only
had more limited vocabularies, but onset of parent–child
reading also occurred later, independent of the language
used.
In our study, we focused only on the age at which children
were first read to and the frequency with which they were
read to in the final preschool year. Consequently, we do not
consider other aspects of shared book reading that play a
major role in children’s linguistic development such as the
quality of the reading (e.g., Mol et al., 2008; L. M. Phillips
et al., 2008) or the appropriateness of the book (Kucirkova,
Messer, & Whitelock, 2012). Future studies need to explore
the relationship between onset of shared book reading and
the quality of the book reading.
Unfortunately, we were not able to include children’s
actual reading or spelling abilities in school or literacy com-
petencies in kindergarten as outcome measures to test
whether the age children were first read to also predicts these
outcomes. However, children’s rhyming abilities and vocab-
ulary are important predictors of later reading and spelling
abilities (e.g., Bowman et al., 2003; Schatschneider et al.,
2004). Consequently, the onset of shared reading should also
be associated with later academic achievement in school.
Conclusion
Despite these limitations, this is one of the first studies to
focus first on the influence of the age at which children were
first read to on the development of children’s linguistic
skills, and second on how this first reading is associated
with later literacy experiences in the home. The findings
indicate that starting early can support the development of
children’s language abilities and that the onset of shared
reading seems to be a good and specific indicator of the
overall HLE. Consequently, parents should be encouraged
to start reading to their children when they are very young—
the sooner, the better.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research and/or authorship of this article: This work was
supported by a fellowship within the Postdoctoral Programme of
the German Academic Exchange Services (DAAD).
References
Aikens, N. L., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Socioeconomic differences in
reading trajectories: The contribution of family, neighborhood,
and school contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
235-251.
Anders, Y., Rossbach, H. G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S.,
Lehrl, S., & von Maurice, J. (2012). Home and preschool
learning environments and their relations to the development
of early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
27, 231-244.
Bast, J., & Reitsma, P. (1998). Analyzing the development of
individual differences in terms of Matthew effects in read-
ing: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study. Developmental
Psychology, 34, 1373-1399.
Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. Richardson (Ed.),
Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of educa-
tion (pp. 241-258). New York, NY: Greenwood Press.
Bowman, B. T., Donovan, M. S., & Burns, M. S. (Eds.). (2003).
Eager to learn: Educating our preschoolers. Washington, DC:
National Academies Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Burgess, S. R. (2002). The influence of speech perception, oral
language ability, the home literacy environment, and pre-read-
ing knowledge on the growth of phonological sensitivity: A
one-year longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 709-737.
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint
book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-
analysis on inter-generational transmission of literacy. Review
of Educational Research, 65, 1-21.
Chien, N. C., Howes, C., Pianta, R., Burchinal, M., Ritchie, S.,
Bryant, D., . . . Barbarin, O. (2010). Children’s classroom
engagement and school readiness gains in prekindergarten.
Child Development, 81, 1534-1549.
DeBaryshe, B. D. (1993). Joint picture-book reading correlates
of early oral language skill. Journal of Child Language, 20,
455-461.
Duncan, G. J., Claessens, A., Huston, A., Pagani, L., Engel,
M., Sexton, H., . . . Duckworth, K. (2007). School readi-
ness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43,
1428-1446.
Duursma, E. (2014). Parental bookreading practices among fami-
lies in the Netherlands. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy,
14, 435-458.
Edwards, C. M. (2014). Maternal literacy practices and toddler’s
emergent literacy skills. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy,
14, 53-79.
Esser, G. (2002). BUEVA – Basisdiagnostik für umschriebene
Entwicklungsstörungen im Vorschulalter [BUEVA—Basic
diagnostics for developmental disorders at preschool age].
Göttingen, Germany: Beltz.
10 SAGE Open
Farrant, B. M., & Zubrick, S. R. (2012). Early vocabulary develop-
ment: The importance of joint attention and parent-child book
reading. First Language, 32, 343-364.
Fawcett, A. J. (2003). The international adult literacy survey in
Britain: Impact on policy and practice. Dyslexia, 9, 99-121.
Flax, J. F., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Roesler, C., Choudhury, N., &
Benasich, A. (2009). Using early standardized language mea-
sures to predict later language and early reading outcomes
in children at high risk for language-learning impairments.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 61-75.
Frumkin, L. A. (2013). Young children’s cognitive achievement:
Home learning environment, language and ethnic background.
Journal of Early Childhood Research, 11, 222-235.
Kleemans, T., Peeters, M., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012).
Child and home predictors of early numeracy skills in kinder-
garten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 471-477.
Kucirkova, N., Messer, D., & Whitelock, D. (2012). Parents reading
with their toddlers: The role of personalization in book engage-
ment. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 13, 445-470.
Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2011). Discussing stories: On how a dia-
logic reading intervention improves kindergartners’ oral narra-
tive construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
108, 1-24.
Liebeskind, K. G., Piotrowski, J. T., Lapierre, M. A., & Linebarger,
D. L. (2014). The home literacy environment: Exploring how
media and parent-child interactions are associated with chil-
dren’s language production. Journal of Early Childhood
Literacy, 14, 482-509.
Lyon, G. R. (2002). Reading development, reading difficulties,
and reading instruction educational and public health issues.
Journal of School Psychology, 40, 3-6.
Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. (2001a). Woodcock–Johnson III tests
of achievement: Examiner’s manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Publishing.
Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. (2001b). Woodcock–Johnson III tests
of cognitive abilities: Examiner’s manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Publishing.
McGrew, K., Woodcock, R., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock–
Johnson III technical manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008).
Added value of dialogic parent–child book readings: A meta-
analysis. Early Education and Development, 19, 7-26.
Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H., & Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Language experi-
enced in utero affect vowel perception after birth: A two-coun-
try study. Acta Paediatrica, 120, 156-160.
Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Dow,
K. A. (2006). Infants’ early ability to segment the conversa-
tional speech signal predicts later language development: A
retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology, 42, 643-
655.
Niklas, F. (2015). Die familiäre Lernumwelt und ihre Bedeutung
für die kindliche Kompetenzentwicklung [The learning envi-
ronment provided by the family and its impact on the devel-
opment of child competencies]. Psychologie in Erziehung und
Unterricht, 62, 106-120.
Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., & Tayler, C. (2016). Home learning
environment and concept formation: A family intervention
study with kindergarten children. Early Childhood Education
Journal, 44, 419-427.
Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., Tayler, C., & Schneider, W. (2016). Erstes
Vorlesen: Der frühe Vogel fängt den Wurm [Early reading
to children: The early bird catches the worm]. Zeitschrift für
Pädagogische Psychologie, 30(1), 35-44.
Niklas, F., Möllers, K., & Schneider, W. (2013). Die frühe
familiäre Lernumwelt als Mediator zwischen strukturellen
Herkunftsmerkmalen und der basalen Lesefähigkeit am Ende
der ersten Klasse [The early learning environment provided by
the family as a mediator between structural family background
and basic reading abilities at the end of Grade 1]. Psychologie
in Erziehung und Unterricht, 60, 94-111.
Niklas, F., Nguyen, C., Cloney, D., Tayler, C., & Adams, R.
(2016). Self-report measures of the home learning environ-
ment in large scale research: Measurement properties and
associations with key developmental outcomes. Learning
Environments Research, 19, 181-202. doi:10.1007/s10984-
016-9206-9
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2013). Home literacy environment
and the beginning of reading and writing. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 38, 40-50.
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2014). Casting the die before the die
is cast: The importance of the home numeracy environment
for preschool children. European Journal of Psychology of
Education, 29, 327-345.
Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2015). With a little help: Improving
kindergarten children’s vocabulary by enhancing the home lit-
eracy environment. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary
Journal, 28, 491-508.
Niklas, F., Tayler, C., & Schneider, W. (2015). Home-based lit-
eracy activities and children’s cognitive outcomes: A compari-
son between Australia and Germany. International Journal of
Educational Research, 71, 75-85.
Partanen, E., Kujala, T., Näätänen, R., Liitola, A., Sambeth, A., &
Huotilainen, M. (2013). Learning induced neural plasticity of
speech processing before birth. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 110, 15145-15150.
Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the home
literacy environment of preschool children: A cluster analytic
approach. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 146-174.
Phillips, L. M., Norris, S. P., & Anderson, J. (2008). Unlocking the
door: Is parents’ reading to children the key to early literacy
development? Canadian Psychology, 49, 82-88.
Prevoo, M. J. L., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Emmen, R. A. G., Yeniad,
N., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Linting, M. (2014). Predicting
ethnic minority children’s vocabulary from socioeconomic sta-
tus, maternal language and home reading input: Different path-
ways for host and ethnic language. Journal of Child Language,
41, 963-984.
Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Gayle, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-
Gunn, J., Constantine, J., . . . Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Mother–
child reading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes
during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77,
924-953.
Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of read-
ing to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245-302.
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D.,
& Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of read-
ing skills: A longitudinal comparative analyses. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 96, 265-282.
Niklas et al. 11
Schmiedeler, S., Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2014). Symptoms
of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and home
learning environment (HLE): Findings from a longitudinal
study. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29(3),
467-482.
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2001). Storybook reading and par-
ent teaching: Links to language and literacy development. In P.
R. Britto & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), The role of family literacy
environments in promoting young children’s emerging literacy
skills (pp. 39-52). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2002). Parental involvement in the
development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudi-
nal study. Child Development, 73, 445-460.
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014). Continuity and change
in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in
vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85, 1552-1568.
Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J.-A., Hudson, E., & Lawseon, P. E. (1996).
Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s
vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 520-536.
Sénéchal, M., Pagan, S., Lever, R., & Ouellette, G. P. (2008).
Relations among the frequency of shared reading and 4-year-old
children’s vocabulary, morphological and syntax comprehen-
sion, and narrative skills. Early Education and Development,
19, 27-44.
Sim, S., & Berthelsen, D. (2014). Shared book reading by parents
with young children: Evidence-based practice. Australasian
Journal of Early Childhood, 39(1), 50-55.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some conse-
quences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.
Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
Torgesen, J. K. (2002). The prevention of reading difficulties.
Journal of School Psychology, 40, 7-26.
Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A. M., Laakso, M. L., Eklund, K., &
Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge
development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement
among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia.
Developmental Psychology, 42, 1128-1142.
Torppa, M., Tolvanen, A., Poikkeus, A. M., Eklund, K., Lerkkanen,
M. K., Leskinen, E., & Lyytinen, H. (2007). Reading devel-
opment subtypes and their early characteristics. Annals of
Dyslexia, 57, 3-32.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher
psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Wegener, B. (1988). Kritik des Prestiges [Criticism of the prestige].
Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and
emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848-872.
Author Biographies
Frank Niklas is a developmental and educational psychologist at
the University of Würzburg, Germany. He also has an honorary
appointment at the University of Melbourne. He conducts research
on the home learning environment, the development of children’s
mathematical and literacy competencies, and children’s school
readiness.
Caroline Cohrssen is a senior lecturer on the Master of Teaching
(Early Childhood) at the University of Melbourne. Her research
interests include the influence of the home learning environment on
children’s literacy and numeracy skills, children’s demonstrations
of mathematical thinking and their implications for teaching and
learning in early childhood education, and teacher perceptions of
school readiness.
Collette Tayler is professor at the University of Melbourne and
holds the Chair in Early Childhood Education and Care in the
Melbourne Graduate School of Education. Collette’s work
addresses program access and engagement; public and private
investments; program standards and quality; the curriculum and
pedagogy applied in different services; leadership and staff devel-
opment; child and family involvement and program outcomes. Her
research seeks to explain both universal principles and contextual
variations needed to provide exemplary care and education for
young children.
... Prior research has established the HLE as a predictor of later theory of mind (Ebert et al., 2020) and language and literacy skills (Burgess, Hecht, & Lonigan, 2002;Griffin & Morrison, 1997;Weigel, Martin, & Bennett, 2006). The HLE is also thought to be a stable characteristic and present as early as infancy (Niklas, Cohrssen, & Tayler, 2016). Thus, our analyses assume that both parent education and the HLE are stable characteristics that precede the theory of mind developments that are occurring in the preschool years. ...
... Numerous studies have established the validity of parent-reported HLE such as the Stony Brook Family Reading Survey (Whitehurst, 2001). How often children are read to, the number of books in the home, trips to the library, children's reading interest, parents' reading interest, and the age at which children are first read to are positively related to objective measures of children's concurrent and later language and literacy skills (e.g., Bracken & Fischel, 2008;Burgess et al., 2002;Carroll et al., 2019;de Jong & Lesemen, 2001;Dunst, Simkus, & Hamby, 2012;Myrtil et al., 2019;Niklas et al., 2016;Tichnor-Wagner et al., 2015;Wirth, Ehmig, Drescher, Guffler, & Niklas, 2020). ...
... We found that parents with higher levels of education tended to provide greater book exposure, which in turn was related to children's greater theory of mind. This finding is consistent with prior work showing an association between parental education and the variables included in the book exposure component-number of books, age of onset of reading to children, and trips to the library (e.g., Bracken & Fischel, 2008;Niklas et al., 2016;Sénéchal & Lefevre, 2002). ...
Article
Personal reading history, in particular reading fiction, is related to theory of mind in adulthood; yet little work has examined reading experiences and theory of mind in early childhood. This study examined the home literacy environment (HLE) and theory of mind in 516 3- to 6-year-olds (Mage = 4;10 years; 53% female; 80% White). Eleven aspects of the HLE were examined and four HLE components were extracted. Most aspects of the HLE (e.g., reading frequency, number of books) were correlated with children’s theory of mind; one component, book exposure, was moderately related (r = 0.48) to theory of mind and remained a significant predictor controlling for several covariates (e.g., child age, parent education, family size). This component included the onset of reading to children, the number of children’s books in the home, and how frequently children visited the library. There was a significant interaction between child age and book exposure; the strength of the association between book exposure and theory of mind was relatively stronger for younger children than for older children. Book exposure also partially mediated the relation between parent education and theory of mind. These results contribute to our understanding of family predictors of children’s theory of mind.
... [10] The number of books at home, the age at which parent-child book reading was initiated, the frequency of trips to the library, and the frequency and enjoyment of reading by the primary caregiver are the aspects of the HLE [11] which are important predictors of children's language and literacy development. [12,13] According to Petrill et al., the number of literacy materials in the home may also be influenced by the parents' educational level and intelligence. [14] Maternal reading beliefs also influence children's emergent literacy outcomes. ...
... Total score ranges from 3 to 29. Families are classified into five groups: upper class (26)(27)(28), upper-middle class (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25), lower-middle class (11)(12)(13)(14)(15), upper-lower class (5-10), and lower class (<5). The Questionnaire on "Parental Perspectives on Storybook Reading in Indian Home Contexts" [23] was used. ...
... [10] The number of books at home, the age at which parent-child book reading was initiated, the frequency of trips to the library, and the frequency and enjoyment of reading by the primary caregiver are the aspects of the HLE [11] which are important predictors of children's language and literacy development. [12,13] According to Petrill et al., the number of literacy materials in the home may also be influenced by the parents' educational level and intelligence. [14] Maternal reading beliefs also influence children's emergent literacy outcomes. ...
... Total score ranges from 3 to 29. Families are classified into five groups: upper class (26)(27)(28), upper-middle class (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25), lower-middle class (11)(12)(13)(14)(15), upper-lower class (5-10), and lower class (<5). The Questionnaire on "Parental Perspectives on Storybook Reading in Indian Home Contexts" [23] was used. ...
... This study focuses on the long-term effects of families' participation in Bookstart. Bookstart aims to promote language and literacy skillsthe best predictors of school achievement (e.g., Barnes & Puccioni, 2017;Niklas, Cohrssen, & Tayler, 2016;Pace, Burchinal, Alper, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2019). The program's primary mechanism is an early start with book reading, preferably in the first year of children's life. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study tested whether Bookstart – a program promoting book reading in infancy – continues to have an impact well into Kindergarten. We distinguished between children who were more or less challenging to read to in infancy (more or less temperamentally reactive). Eighty percent (n = 471) of a sample participating in a study when the children were one year old – about half involved in Bookstart – agreed to complete a home literacy survey when the children were, on average, 72.1 months. A smaller group (n = 318) also consented to collect tests concerning language and math at children's Kindergarten. The findings show that language development when they are about to start learning to read still profits from Bookstart. Especially the temperamentally most reactive 50 % shows benefits (d = 0.21). Bookstart also improved children's home literacy environment (longer book reading sessions), but this effect did not explain Bookstart's impact in Kindergarten.
Article
Many children are at risk for reading difficulties because of inadequate emergent literacy skills. It is widely accepted that development of emergent literacy skills is strictly related to children’s early literacy experiences at home and school. Dialogic reading is an evidence-based intervention to promote the language skills of preschool children. This study examined the feasibility and efficacy of a parent-focused dialogic book reading intervention that aimed to foster the early language and literacy skills of pre-school children. A sample of 40 Italian preschoolers (Mage = 62.9 months, SD = 6.3) and their parents were divided into three groups: treatment group (n = 12); information group (n = 12) and control group (n = 16). The efficacy of the intervention for oral language skills was examined by analyzing the improvements from pre- to post-intervention in children’s oral language outcomes, through ad hoc and standardized tasks; specifically, by measuring proximal and distal abilities. Additionally, we analyzed the intervention effects on parent–child interaction and dialogic behaviors during shared book reading. Results suggest that a relatively brief intervention (6 weeks) using dialogic book reading strategies can lead to sustained improvements in early language and literacy skills in preschoolers (both proximal and distal) and in parent dialogic behaviors during shared book reading.
Article
Background: This study is informed by research connecting parent-child book reading and attachment and by the concept of maternal-foetal attachment. Aim: To explore experiences of pregnant mothers who read picture books marketed to be read while their babies are in utero. Study design: This study utilised a qualitative, multiple case study design involving 10 pregnant women from the Midwestern region of the United States. Participants read picture books marketed to be read by expectant families and participated in an interview focused on their experiences and reflections, and completed a follow-up survey 24-27 months later. Results: Themes identified included (1) Learning and Love; (2) Seeing the Baby as a Person; (3) Natural or Weird?; (4) Fathers' Involvement; (5) Slowing Down: Focusing on Now and Envisioning the Future; and (6) Who is this for? Discussion: Findings suggest providing books to expectant families may be a promising practice and should be further researched.
Article
Children in America today struggle with finding themselves in the books they read due to societal expectations. From an early age, children are dictated on the correct way to speak and write in “American,” which can leave children and their home languages feeling unseen and dismissed. To help further the conversation and promotion of linguistic diversity in American society, this capstone analyzes dialectal representation in children’s books, with a heavy focus on attitudinal linguistic principles rather than prescriptive mechanics. The secondary research explores current literature and resources that discuss literacy acquisition in adolescents, trends in dialects in America, and childhood sense of self at an early age. Additionally, this research discusses current trends in the publishing industry regarding voice and African American, Latinx/Spanish-speaking, and Appalachian/Southern linguistic representation in fictional children’s and young adult literature; this conversation regarding trends in the industry will lead to an analysis of the relationship between authenticity of voice and the #OwnVoices movement. Finally, the primary research dissects and critically assesses first-hand insights from children’s book publishing professionals as to how the industry can best include and promote the importance of seeing and hearing children’s voices in the books they read. The results of this study indicate that secretly sinister gatekeeping practices threaten true growth and inclusion regarding diverse voices in children's literature, and the capstone concludes with potential calls to action for linguists, educators, and children's literature publishers alike.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to examine the role of different types of home learning activities, such as reading, singing, painting, playing games, and letters and numbers (ABCs and 123 s), in the development of nonverbal reasoning skills in young children. Although much previous research has focused on the role of the home learning environment in the development of language and numeracy skills, few studies have explored other aspects of cognitive development such as nonverbal reasoning. The data were drawn from the Growing Up in Ireland study, a nationally representative longitudinal birth cohort study. We examined whether learning activities were associated with scores on standardized nonverbal reasoning and vocabulary tests of the British Ability Scales in a sample of 9793 3-year-old children. The regression models also controlled for other factors that potentially influence cognitive development such as the parent–child relationship and maternal education. The findings indicate that activities such as reading, games, and painting/drawing have a small but statistically significant association with nonverbal reasoning scores, as well as with vocabulary scores, even after controlling for other factors in the model. Teaching the alphabet or numbers did not make significant contributions to the model. The findings of the study highlight the importance of considering the role of different types of home learning activities, as well as other environmental factors, in different aspects of cognitive development. We consider the implications of the findings for theories of cognitive development and for supporting cognitive development in young children.
Article
Full-text available
English language development is a critical component for young children’s school readiness. In this study, we examined the effect of Read it again-Pre-K! (Justice and McGinty in Read it again!-Prek: a preschool curriculum supplement to promote language and literacy foundations, Crane Center for Early Childhood Research and Policy, Columbus, 2013), a literacy curriculum designed to prepare young children’s school readiness on the English literacy skills of Korean dual language learners in general education. Using a multiple probe design, eight 4- to 5-year-old Korean dual language learners (1 female, 7 males) received 1:1 online synchronous daily instruction over 2 months during the summer before entering their kindergarten programs. Through the intervention, all eight children demonstrated increases in the use of English vocabulary, story comprehension, and oral fluency. Post-intervention data on vocabulary and reading fluency through three standardized tests, Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, and Expressive Vocabulary Test, showed improvement over baseline for most children. Discussion and implications for future research were provided.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The spread of the coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) has escalated the need for studying the home learning environment (HLE). With learners spending more time at home, understanding about learning at home, especially in disadvantaged communities, is paramount. Aim: The aim of this research study was to explore the HLE for foundation phase learners by analysing data from an intervention that aspired to support organised learning at home during the COVID-19 pandemic. Setting: This research study was conducted telephonically with families from 11 schools (10 in the Waterberg district, Limpopo and one in Soweto, Gauteng). Methods: Qualitative data (recorded phone interviews) were collected from caregivers and learners from 17 households. The interview recordings were transcribed and translated into English. The constant comparative method of data analysis was used to analyse interview transcripts. Results: Three themes emerged from the analysis: (1) challenges of learning at home, (2) perceived benefits of learning at home and (3) the desire to continue with the programme even after schools re-opened. The data showed that having access to learning material facilitated learning at home. Additionally, caregivers noted benefits to learners from engaging in learning activities. However, there were also challenges in facilitating learning at home. These include caregivers not following the learning guidelines provided. Conclusion: Caregivers appreciated having access to learning material and were willing to facilitate learning at home. However, the challenges they faced resulted in them not executing the programme effectively. Future studies and interventions should explore how to provide adequate and accessible support to caregivers.
Article
Full-text available
Favourable home learning environments (HLEs) support children’s literacy, numeracy and social development. In large-scale research, HLE is typically measured by self-report survey, but there is little consistency between studies and many different items and latent constructs are observed. Little is known about the stability of these items and constructs over time when used in either longitudinal research or studies with children with a wide range of ages. A review of the literature shows commonalities and differences between approaches in research on HLE. When we tested the psychometric properties of a short-form measure of HLE with a Rasch item-response-model using longitudinal data from over 1600 Australian families, there was support for two dimensions of HLE—formal and informal learning frequency. We found that this measure was stable over the transitional period from pre-K to school as well as between gender and family language background. There were small but significant associations between the HLE measures and cognitive and behavioural developmental outcomes. We recommend that other measures of HLE could be similarly validated to assess their suitability for use in longitudinal research on learning environments. Recommendations are made for the future development of measures of broader constructs of the HLE.
Book
1 Die Vorbereitungen fUr diese Arbeit reichen bis in das Jahr 1979 zuriick Ihre Niederschrift ist aber eine Sache der letzten zwolf Monate. Fur die Dauer dieser Zeit war ich an zwei amerikanischen Universitaten zu Gast: zuerst in Wisconsin 2 Madison, dann in Harvard Mehr als ich es zunachst wahrgenommen habe, haben beide Forschungsaufenthalte durch die Anregungen, die sie mir vermitteln konnten, das Gesicht meiner Untersuchung verandert. Der Grund war ihre Gegensatzlichkeit: Das Soziologie-Department der Universitat von Wisconsin ist als Heimstatt der sogenannten Wisconsin-Schule der empirischen Mobilitats forschung bekannt. In Cambridge dagegen ist auch heute noch der Geist Talcott Parsons' lebendig. Beide Ausrichtungen sind keine naturlichen Partner. Ich glaube aber, daB sie sich auf wichtige Weise erganzen. Die Absicht, das aufzuzei gen, wurde zum Leitmotiv meiner Arbeit. Die Frage nach Sozialprestige eignet sich fUr ein solches synthetisches Unter nehmen in besonderer Weise. Ich will versuchen, den diesbezuglich wichtigsten Gedanken im Vorgriff anzudeuten. Die Eigentumlichkeit der Vorstellung von Prestige ist, daB sie sich auf eine subjektive Wahrnehmung bezieht, zugleich aber auch auf eine objektive, die Gesellschaft in bestimmter Hinsicht strukturierende Gegebenheit. Prestige ist also eine Ordnungsinstanz fUr unser Handeln, die in bezug auf Individuen ihrer Herkunft nach sowohl intern als auch extern ist. Diese Doppelnatur hat die Konturen des Begriffs stets verschwommen erscheinen lassen beziehungsweise sie hat dort, wo wissenschaftliche Eindeutigkeit als Standard galt, zur Ausklam merungjeweils des einen Teils - zu Reduktionismus - gefUhrt.
Book
Ziel der Vorschultestbatterie ist es, Teilleistungsstörungen bereits vor Schulbeginn zu erkennen und damit einer frühzeitigen Förderung zuzuführen. Das diagnostische Vorgehen zeichnet sich durch eine hohe Durchführungsökonomie und einen breiten Anwendungs-bereich aus. Insgesamt werden bei Vierjährigen die allgemeine Intelligenz, die Artikulation, die expressive und die rezeptive Sprache sowie die Visuomotorik in 20 bis 25 Minuten erfasst, bei den Fünfjährigen kommen zusätzlich noch Prüfungen der Aufmerksamkeit hinzu, was die Durchführung um fünf bis zehn Minuten gegenüber den Vierjährigen verlängert. Die vorliegende Testbatterie kann auch im Rahmen von Schuleingangsuntersuchungen verlässlich eingesetzt werden.
Article
Primary caregivers are often encouraged to read to their children from a very young age as we know that reading to children plays a very important role in their literacy development. However, little is known about whether the onset of reading to a child is a specific predictor of language abilities when controlling for child and family characteristics. Literacy skills and other cognitive abilities of 746 German children were assessed shortly before school entry and results were compared to those of a recent Australian study. Parents were asked how old their children were when they were first read to and how often they currently read to their children. In both countries, the age at which children were first read to was closely associated with the frequency with which children were read to as pre-schoolers and with their literacy abilities. It was less closely associated with other cognitive competencies. The findings imply that reading books to infants early may contribute meaningfully to a favourable home literacy environment and support children's literacy development.
Article
The current review is a quantitative meta-analysis of the available empirical evidence related to parent-preschooler reading and several outcome measures. In selecting the studies to be included in this meta-analysis, we focused on studies examining the frequency of book reading to preschoolers. The results support the hypothesis that parent-preschooler reading is related to outcome measures such as language growth, emergent literacy, and reading achievement. The overall effect size of d = .59 indicates that book reading explains about 8% of the variance in the outcome measures. The results support the hypothesis that book reading, in particular, affects acquisition of the written language register. The effect of parent-preschooler reading is not dependent on the socioeconomic status of the families or on several methodological differences between the studies. However, the effect seems to become smaller as soon as children become conventional readers and are able to read on their own.
Article
Children’s cognitive development has a neural basis, yet children’s learning is facilitated by interactions with more knowledgeable others. Young children experience such interactions in the context of the home learning environment (HLE), when parents support children’s thinking and learning during everyday activities. Consequently, one way to improve children’s cognitive abilities may be to enhance the quality of the HLE. In this study a non-intensive intervention was developed to improve both, HLE and children’s cognitive abilities. The sample consisted of 113 Australian 4-year-old children and their parents. All parents were invited to participate in a two-part intervention that included firstly attending a group meeting at which information regarding the HLE was provided, and secondly participating in an additional individual session that introduced the principles of counting and dialogic reading. HLE and children’s concept formation, as an indicator of fluid reasoning, were assessed before and after the intervention. Families and children in the intervention group showed significantly greater gains than the control group, both in the quality of HLD and the children’s concept formation than members of the control group. Results indicate that non-intensive family interventions may positively impact on HLE and children’s fluid reasoning.
Article
Home-based literacy activities (HBLA) such as reading with a child and their impact on cognitive competencies have been widely investigated. However, few comparison studies between different countries have been conducted. This study compares HBLA in Germany and Australia, and their associations with different control and outcome variables using longitudinal data of some 900/2500 children. More than two-thirds of the parents in both samples engaged in literacy interactions with their children more or less daily. Parents in both countries who reported more HBLA had higher levels of education, were less likely to speak another language than German or English (respectively) at home, and their children performed better in cognitive outcomes. These differences proved to be stable for the whole preschool period.
Article
Maternal emergent literacy practices during shared-reading interactions with 18-36-month-old toddlers were examined. The investigation of emergent literacy behaviours of both mothers and toddlers investigated included the examination of phonological awareness and written language awareness. Participants included 15 mother-toddler dyads from middle to upper socio-economic status (SES) backgrounds. The main data collection measures included: (1) home literacy environment observation; (2) shared reading observations; and (3) parent questionnaire. Data were coded and organized into emergent literacy domains, phonological awareness and written language awareness. The results indicate that these mothers engaged in several emergent literacy behaviours with their toddlers during shared-reading interactions. Some of these behaviours were also exhibited by their children. The behaviours observed in mothers and toddlers were related to the written language awareness domain. These and other findings are discussed in relation to their theoretical foundations (e.g. Purcell-Gates, 2001) and implications for early intervention practices and programme development. © The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav.