ArticlePDF Available

Metacognition and Professional Judgment and Decision Making in Coaching: Importance, Application and Evaluation

Authors:

Abstract

Previous research has emphasised the dynamic nature of coaching practice and the need to consider both individual performer needs and necessary contextual trade-offs in providing optimum solutions. In this regard, a Professional Judgment and Decision Making framework has been suggested to facilitate an optimum blend of actions against these complex and dynamic demands. Accordingly, we extend this work and address recent calls for greater focus on expertise-oriented assessments, by postulating on the aspirant/developing coach’s capacity for and development of metacognition (i.e., active control over cognitive processes) as a ‘tool’ within the reflective process. Specifically, we propose that metacognition enables essential active cognitive processing for deep learning and impactful application, together with construction and refinement of useable knowledge to inform coaching decisions. Metacognition, therefore, helps to contextualise knowledge provided in training, further optimising the experience, particularly before certification. Finally, we exemplify how metacognition can be developed in coaches through the use of cognitive apprenticeships and decision training tools; and evaluated via a series of observed coaching episodes, with reasoning articulated through pre and postsession interview. Despite challenging traditional competency-based approaches to coach education, we believe that a considered mixed approach represents a vital next step in further professionalising sports coaching.
1
This is a pre-proof corrected manuscript, as accepted for publication, of an article published
1
by Human Kinetics in International Sport Coaching Journal on 22nd October 2016, available
2
online at: http://journals.humankinetics.com/doi/pdf/10.1123/iscj.2016-0037
3
4
5
6
Metacognition and Professional Judgment and Decision Making in Coaching: Importance,
7
Application and Evaluation
8
9
Loel Collins*, Howie J. Carson and Dave Collins
10
Institute for Coaching and Performance, University of Central Lancashire, Preston, UK
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Loel Collins, Institute for
27
Coaching and Performance, University of Central Lancashire, Preston, UK, PR1 2HE. Email:
28
lcollins2@uclan.ac.uk.
29
2
Abstract
30
Previous research has emphasised the dynamic nature of coaching practice and the need to
31
consider both individual performer needs and necessary contextual trade-offs in providing
32
optimum solutions. In this regard, a Professional Judgment and Decision Making framework
33
has been suggested to facilitate an optimum blend of actions against these complex and
34
dynamic demands. Accordingly, we extend this work and address recent calls for greater
35
focus on expertise-oriented assessments, by postulating on the aspirant/developing coach’s
36
capacity for and development of metacognition (i.e., active control over cognitive processes)
37
as a ‘tool’ within the reflective process. Specifically, we propose that metacognition enables
38
essential active cognitive processing for deep learning and impactful application, together
39
with construction and refinement of useable knowledge to inform coaching decisions.
40
Metacognition, therefore, helps to contextualise knowledge provided in training, further
41
optimising the experience, particularly before certification. Finally, we exemplify how
42
metacognition can be developed in coaches through the use of cognitive apprenticeships and
43
decision training tools; and evaluated via a series of observed coaching episodes, with
44
reasoning articulated through pre and postsession interview. Despite challenging traditional
45
competency-based approaches to coach education, we believe that a considered mixed
46
approach represents a vital next step in further professionalising sports coaching.
47
Key words: Assessment; Coach education; Development; Expertise; Training
48
49
3
Metacognition and Professional Judgment and Decision Making in Coaching: Importance,
50
Application and Evaluation
51
Coaching practice is recognised and demonstrated as a dynamic process (e.g.,
52
Abraham & Collins, 2011b; L. Collins & Collins, 2012, 2015; Martindale & Collins, 2012).
53
Such work highlights the need to consider both individual performer needs and contextual
54
trade-offs in providing optimum solutions. For example, despite a coach predominantly
55
working to develop long-term performance, they might deviate from this approach to give a
56
short-term boost to confidence at the expense of skill retention (i.e., a trade-off).
57
Consequently, the ability to respond quickly and efficiently to selected, or preselected,
58
subsets of factors is a crucial skill for any coach.
59
Influenced by the practices of other professions, a process of Professional Judgment
60
and Decision Making (PJDM) has been suggested within the sport psychology and coaching
61
literature, to facilitate an optimum blend of actions against such demands. This process,
62
involving reflection during coaching (in action; Schön, 1983), post coaching activity (on-
63
action; Schön, 1983) and by creating time within the coaching session/process for reflection
64
(on-action/in-context; L. Collins & Collins, 2015; Schön, 1987) has, to date, been implicit
65
within these suggestions. As such, this Insights paper extends these ideas by postulating on
66
the requisite cognitive skills for a coach to employ a PJDM approach and, consequently, the
67
implications for training and evaluation.
68
Successful operationalisation of the PJDM process relies on a coach’s declarative
69
understanding of ‘what needs to be done’ (e.g., blocked practice to generate a rapid
70
performance gain or random practice to promote better long-term retention and transferable
71
skills) which, in turn, cyclically links back to their intentions (Abraham, Collins &
72
Martindale, 2006); in short, knowing why particular action(s) should be taken in response to
73
the multifactorial demands of a situation (cf. Winter & Collins, 2015). Of course, knowing
74
4
how to enact those decisions is also important. We suggest that integrated application of the
75
what, why (declarative knowledge) and how (procedural knowledge) of a PJDM approach are
76
facilitated by metacognitive skills. Specifically, metacognition underpins the ability for
77
reflection in-action, on-action and on-action/in-context, enabling the essential consideration
78
and weighing up of alternative coaching options within the PJDM process (Cruickshank,
79
2013). Crucially, such reflection supports coaches to recognise and address novel or complex
80
problems while coaching. By addressing the coach’s capacity for and development of
81
metacognition, we aim to stimulate thought and debate within this developing avenue of
82
research.
83
Such concepts will apply across most, if not all, sports; since the PJDM process is
84
apparent between different contexts (e.g., open vs. closed skill sports), levels of challenge
85
(e.g., practice vs. competition) and within different environments (e.g., indoor vs. outdoor).
86
However, our interests lead to a particular focus on Adventure Sports Coaching (ASC); a
87
hyper-dynamic environment that is especially demanding on coaches’ ability to make
88
effective decisions (see L. Collins & Collins, 2012, 2015; L. Collins, Collins & Grecic,
89
2015). Accordingly, the paper is presented in two stages: (1) we introduce and explore
90
metacognition as a ‘tool’ within the reflective process and (2) we propose how metacognition
91
can be trained and evaluated in developing/aspirant coaches.
92
Metacognition and Reflective Thinking within the PJDM Process
93
In part, the practical success of a PJDM framework relies on a coachs understanding
94
of the situational demands (Abraham & Collins, 2011a). However, less attention has been
95
directed towards coaches knowing how to apply aspects of their knowledge, that is, the
96
process of translating theory into practice. In offering a potential solution, Abraham and
97
Collins (2011b) proposed that PJDM requires a process of nested decisions that are
98
developed via nuanced in-action, on-action and on-action/in-context reflective processes.
99
5
Inevitably, therefore, alternative actions are always generated, contextualised and critically
100
considered against intended outcomes when using this approach. Working without reflection
101
could explain why coaches sometimes make suboptimal decisions based on heuristic
102
constructs from personal experience (Collins & Collins, 2016b). In other words, Naturalistic
103
Decision Making processes are potentially weakened by the coach’s lack of breadth and
104
depth in experience (Klien, 2008; Lyle, 2003). Accordingly, it would appear essential that
105
coaches develop metacognitive skills as a necessary adjunct to increasing declarative
106
knowledge (Abraham & Collins, 2011a), if they are to safeguard themselves against such
107
potential pitfalls associated with narrowly formed heuristics or ‘recipe coaching’.
108
When considering the scope of metacognition, Kruger and Dunning (1999) argue that
109
“the skills that engender competence in a particular domain are often the very same skills
110
necessary to evaluate competence in that domain—one’s own and anyone else’s” (p. 1121).
111
Indeed, Kruger and Dunning’s findings imply that those metaskills, including metacognition,
112
are an important aspect of a coach’s performance evaluation. Crucially within ASC,
113
understanding one’s own coaching and personal ability has safety implications and
114
developmental impact (Collins & Collins, 2012). The highly-dynamic coaching environment
115
in adventure sports, coupled with the inherent risk and requirement for the coach to engage in
116
the adventure activity, means that the coach must comprehend the interaction between the
117
task, environment and participant (L. Collins & Collins, 2016a). In summary, Kruger and
118
Dunning suggest that knowledge used to produce coherent judgments about a situation is the
119
same as that which underlies the ability to recognise good judgment.
120
Action, reason and deliberation are central to the Aristotelian notion of phronesis
121
(practical wisdom). The judgements that are required to exercise practical wisdom, link the
122
capacity to deliberate, evaluate and take action in a practical way. The constant audit of the
123
coaching process (D. Collins, Collins & Carson, 2016; L. Collins & Collins, 2016b) includes
124
6
an evaluation of the decision making process, itself a metacognitive process. Indeed, these
125
skills are well suited to the complex coaching environment and presumably, if they can be
126
articulated can also be taught. Fenichel and Eggbeer (1990) described this process of
127
enacting phronesis as “the ability to do the right thing, at the right time, for the right reason”
128
(p. 21); notably, this quote has become increasingly synonymous with wisdom and is
129
similarly utilised in the educational domain. In this regard, we can describe phronesis as
130
good judgment (the how), which differs from the knowledge of coaching (the what) and
131
could be considered a metaskill. Crucially, however, Claxton and Lucas (2007) proposed
132
that merely being taught to think is insufficient, being taught to think well is most
133
appropriate. With these distinctions in place, it is worth exploring the mechanisms which
134
underpin thinking well as opposed to thinking per se (cf. cognition and metacognition), if we
135
are to encourage an adaptive, flexible and creative coaching workforce.
136
In applying effective decision making within a PJDM framework, we suggest that
137
metacognition is used to operationalise the knowledge generated by coaches’ reflective
138
process. Consequently, this enables the modification of existing schema and generation of
139
new versions through a multilooped comparative audit in which current experience and
140
potential coaching solutions are contrasted and considered (Collins & Collins, 2013). This
141
adaptation and generation of new, accessible and internalised schemata allows the coach to
142
be adaptive, flexible and creative in response to situational demands as they unfold. In short,
143
coaches become capable of accurately selecting and activating an optimum behaviour from a
144
broader repertoire under naturalistic conditions; that is, a heuristic for adaptive expertise (cf.
145
de Oliveira, Lobinger & Raab, 2014).
146
More specifically, metacognition utilises both analogous and metaphoric dimensions
147
to problem solving. Using analogies, the coach is able to create understanding through a
148
contextual relationship between the known and the newly experienced coaching scenario (cf.
149
7
Carbonell, 1985) and, from this, to select a best fit rather than optimum solution which, in
150
turn, may be adapted in situ (adaptability and flexibility): for example, linking a carved turn
151
on skis with a carved turn in a kayak, when a kayaker is on skis for the first time. When
152
encountering novel and/or poorly defined challenges, the coach reconceptualises the
153
challenge in a metaphoric way by aligning the experience more broadly with a range of
154
known strategies and approaches, considering the challenges in a more thematic, or
155
principled, manner; as shown when asking a skier to “crush a grape under your big toe” to
156
encourage use of an edging with a ski. Font, Bolite and Acevedo (2010) proposed that such
157
metaphoric thinking would enable coaches to anticipate, solve and address the novel
158
problems that are encountered in dynamic environments. In both analogous and metaphoric
159
thinking, however, there is a requirement for a higher level of contextual thinking skill that is
160
fundamental to the PJDM process, namely metacognition. The coach processes the flow of
161
information in each coaching situation (micro level), at an intervention level (meso) and
162
programme (macro) level. Metacognitive capacity allows the coach to better organise,
163
prioritise and make accessible (e.g., the metaphoric or analogous strategies) newly
164
constructed or adapted information across long-term timescales, in this capacity
165
metacognition improves the flow of information.
166
Despite this seeming advantage towards designing high-level practice, Collins,
167
Collins and Carson (2016) identified that metacognition cannot always be articulated by the
168
coach. Such inability raises concern over how the coach could communicate such nuances
169
while training or mentoring others. In order to act as a coach educator therefore, an ability to
170
consider and apply necessary decisions from reflections on-action/in-context (e.g., when
171
facing new situations or the need to implement trade-off decisions) becomes a critical skill; in
172
simple terms, an ability to provide a commentary of one’s own metacognition in practice.
173
8
The need for metacognitive skills in coach educators is, therefore, an important aspect of
174
coach education (cf. Kruger & Dunning, 1999).
175
Metacognition is also important because it enables the active cognitive processing that
176
is essential for deep learning (Claxton & Lucas, 2007; Schön, 1987) and application,
177
construction and refinement of useable knowledge. Metacognition helps the coach to
178
contextualise the knowledge acquired in training, further optimising the experience between
179
training and certification by providing the tools for reflection and supporting the
180
developmental aspect of professional practice. As such, we now address how metacognition
181
might be developed and assessed by training organisations (e.g., national governing bodies)
182
when implementing a PJDM framework within coach education.
183
Developing and Evaluating Metacognition within the PJDM Process
184
A PJDM focus in coach education would need to be in concert with the developments
185
of an expertise focus for evaluation (EFE) of coaching practice. Furthermore, education and
186
evaluation would need to reflect the appropriate synergy of skills required in the coaches’
187
role. Realistically, and despite recent criticisms of competency-based approaches (see
188
Collins, Bruke, Martindale & Cruickshank, 2015), some aspects of the coach’s performance
189
will be suitable for competency focused assessment methods. These are essentially the
190
components of the coaching process (e.g., equipment setup, maintenance, aspects of safety),
191
the essential content which often has a right or wrong catagorisation, while an expertise-
192
oriented assessment would measure the interactional and decision making aspects of
193
coaching in practice; a situation where shades of grey solutions (or ‘it depends’) are more
194
appropriate. In simple terms, our proposal here is not for an either/or approach, but that
195
current competency-based approaches, best utilised for specific and stereotypic skills, ought
196
to also emphasise an expertise-based approach for the complex situations such as coaching.
197
A mixed assessment strategy in which competency and expertise foci coexist clearly offers a
198
9
more valid and reliable assessment of requisite skills. Accordingly, the PJDM tools (e.g.,
199
metacognition, reflectivity, adaptability and flexibility) will need to be understood by
200
educators and coaches; they will need to know how knowledge interacts between these
201
various factors and demonstrate an ability to articulate and utilise them. Therefore, coach
202
educators should be skilful coaches and educators who can articulate the dynamics of the
203
coaching process.
204
Reflecting the teaching of PJDM, this would need to identify flexible, as opposed to
205
repeated, mental processes (cf. our earlier conceptions of metacognition). In turn, these
206
require developing coaches to plan, explain and evaluate their own thinking and learning in
207
addition to their coaching. Both Bolton (2010) and Moon (1999) identify that nonroutine,
208
open-ended learning tasks involving a degree of uncertainty serve to encourage higher quality
209
thinking and metacognition. This approach may be challenging for coaches or training
210
programmes that encourage a routine or proceduralised process. Indeed, recent study
211
suggests that firmly fixed beliefs in one solution can counter the acceptance and
212
implementation of others, even when the alternative is proven to be more efficacious
213
(Yarritu, Matute & Luque, 2015). Accordingly, the shift towards PJDM enables learners to
214
construct meaning, make judgments and produce multiple solutions to new or unique
215
problems and to challenge doctrine and dogmatism; all promoted perhaps by a greater
216
tolerance, acceptance or even pursuit of productive ambiguity. As such, upfront selling and
217
gaining long-term commitment to this approach will be essential as a fundamental
218
requirement for intentional, goal-directed change of well-established behaviours (cf. Carson
219
& Collins, 2011; Prochaska, DiClemente & Norcross, 1992).
220
Crucially, explicit pedagogies associated with the teaching of metacognition and
221
PJDM must ensure that the learning transfers beyond the context in which it is taught. In
222
turn, this must be supported by suitable theoretical underpinning, metacognitive ability,
223
10
curriculum design, delivery materials, an explicit epistemology, pedagogy and infrastructure.
224
In particular, an educational environment in which these skills are valued and demonstrated
225
as elements of expert practice, a shift towards an adaptive notion of expertise. Notably, this
226
may necessitate some focused work on broader coach and coach educational cultures before
227
it can be achieved (cf. Cruickshank & Collins, 2012; Stoszkowski & Collins, 2012).
228
Metacognitive Approaches in Coach Education
229
Addressing the combined tuition of practical and cognitive performance elements, the
230
constructivist approach of a cognitive apprenticeship (CA; Collins, Brown & Newman, 1987)
231
offers one pedagogic mechanism to this learning. In practice, using approaches such as CA
232
exposes the implicit processes associated with performing complex skills. In doing so, the
233
CA approach focuses on articulating and identifying the tacit processes within the
234
complexity, encouraging students to observe, identify and practice them with help from the
235
tutor coach. For example, the decisions associated with selecting and placing an anchor
236
while rock climbing provide opportunity for such an approach. CA requires the learner to
237
consciously engage in the cognitive aspect of the process, be motivated to learn and to
238
accurately reproduce the cognitive and motoric aspects of the skill. Adding ecological
239
strength to such practice, the activity being taught is modelled in a real-world context
240
utilising explicit coachtrainee interactions. Following this, situated cognition (A. Collins et
241
al., 1987; Godden & Baddeley, 1975) then aids the development of metacognitive processes
242
by assisting at the skill level just beyond what the learner could accomplish themselves; that
243
is, the zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978).
244
To exemplify how a CA may be achieved in the sporting context, consider Vickers’
245
(2007) decision training model. Indeed, this model reflects a sophisticated epistemological
246
position (Schommer, 1994) that accepts the integrated nature of practical and cognitive
247
performance. It may also align with concepts such as Christensen, Sutton and McIlwain’s
248
11
(2016) mesh theory that advocates a motoric and cognitive aspect to performance and
249
learning. Both Vickers’ decision training model and Christensen, Sutton and McIlwain’s
250
mesh theory provide a pragmatic integration of cognitive and motoric aspects of performance
251
and offer an alternative to purely technically-focused syllabi. Such approaches may allow the
252
integration of PJDM into both the education and practice of the coach.
253
Staying with the constructivist paradigm, problem-based learning strategies focus on
254
engaging learners in a process of collaborative and self-directed inquiry (Jones & Turner,
255
2006). Here, the role of the teacher is to guide, facilitate and challenge the learning process
256
rather than strictly provide knowledge. Accordingly, learners are presented with an authentic
257
problem and, through discussion within their learning group, prior knowledge is used to
258
address the problem; thus formulating a shared mental model to explain the problem (Ojala &
259
Thorpe, 2015). This framework, on which students can construct knowledge relating to the
260
problem, is managed by the coach educator. Following the generation of a shared mental
261
model, students work independently in self-directed study to research the specific aspects of
262
the problem. Finally, the students re-group to discuss and refine their initial explanations
263
based on what they learnt. As such, students are agents in this socioconstructivist process in
264
which meaning and interpretations of the world are based on experiences and interactions;
265
learning becomes a continuous and lifelong process. Identifying a suitable line through a
266
white water rapid prior to allowing a group to paddle it provides an opportunity with a group
267
of trainee coaches. In this case, the problem is to descend the rapid in a safe and controlled
268
manner with a group. Students are allowed to inspect the rapid, individually, prior to
269
developing a strategy for descent that draws on their previous experiences. Then, the trainee
270
coaches share each possible approach and construct a shared mental model to descend the
271
rapid. After paddling the rapid the strategy is reviewed by the team.
272
As another possible method, transformative teaching strategies address psychological
273
12
and behavioural characteristics in an attempt to alter a learner’s perspective relating to an
274
experience of activity from fundamentally rational and analytical positions (Taylor & Collins,
275
2016). The approach focuses on altering the learner’s philosophy by challenging the
276
underlying premises of their perspective. Facilitating such understanding is the goal of a
277
transformative approach and, in that respect, develops autonomous thinking. Mezirow
278
(1997) describes the construction of dilemma by providing options and forcing a choice by
279
the learners. In this way the teacher can facilitate transformation. Transformative
280
approaches have value in the coach education process: For example, Taylor and Collins
281
(2016) highlight a transformational approach in addressing a novice coachs epistemology,
282
transforming a naive epistemological position towards a sophisticated position (Schommer,
283
1994).
284
Clearly, the development of metacognition plays a pivotal role in these approaches.
285
However, an important aspect must also be considered, that of the right approach in the right
286
place at the right time alluded to earlier. We have advocated that a single approach to
287
assessment is flawed and we must, de facto, extend such observation to teaching approaches
288
(Collins, Collins & Willmott, 2016); this seems to simply strengthen the need for
289
metacognition in both coaching and coach education practice.
290
An EFE process (and the professional development which accompanies it) could
291
potentially be the nature of the decisions that accompany and drive the adaptability,
292
flexibility and creativity within the coaching process, not just the coaching tools. Aligning
293
the philosophy of coaching, education and assessment within the scheme becomes
294
imperative; in this context, a coaching philosophy that values and reflects adaptive expertise.
295
This philosophical position would be aligned with a core of declarative knowledge and
296
declarative skill. This differs from presenting basic techniques for instruction; the emphasis
297
becomes to construct the fundamental techniques from these declarative elements.
298
13
Throughout the educative process, the explicit interaction between declarative elements is
299
illustrated and articulated (i.e., the PJDM process). This would be achieved via a reduction in
300
the instruction of basic content in favour of declarative content, metacognitive skills and
301
PJDM to utilise and operationalise that knowledge. Thus, the focus of assessment becomes
302
how and why we teach, rather than solely the what; the situation which exists at present in
303
competency-based assessments.
304
What could an Evaluation of Adaptive Coaching Expertise look like?
305
A variety of different approaches exist, although all (we suggest) would incorporate
306
some form of questioning on the whys of decisions taken. For example, the evaluation of
307
adaptable coaching skills could be assessed via a series of observed coaching episodes, with
308
reasoning articulated through pre and postsession interview. In simple terms, the coach is
309
asked to overtly discuss the reasoning through which decisions were reached, what
310
alternatives were considered and under what circumstances such alternatives would have
311
been used (cf. the big five approach; Collins et al., 2015). To enhance validity, both coaching
312
session and interview could be recorded, the footage being used to assist in stimulating the
313
coaches’ recall of the session and the audio to form part of a professional development log.
314
Encapsulated within this concept would be the need to generate a constantly learning coach,
315
with an improvement in thinking skill, sophistication and practice being expected at each
316
assessed session. Evaluation would extend over a series of nonlinked sessions in which
317
preplanning, adaptation of that plan and its underpinning rationalisation can be articulated.
318
Indeed, distributing sessions has been shown to facilitate more accurate judgments of
319
learning; that is, metacognition (cf. Dail & Christina, 2004). To avoid the potential for post
320
hoc rationalisation of actions, consideration could be given to developing the reflective
321
process as an articulation of the coach’s internal dialogue (not unlike the commentary
322
provided in advanced driver training, blue light response training or those training in
323
14
emergency care). Noninterventionist approaches to assessment may be challenged by such a
324
notion and some would argue that this influences the coaches’ performance and that the
325
assessment is compromised. However, the focus of evaluation is not to measure performance
326
in that instance but rather, to evaluate the rate and nature of development, the individual’s
327
trajectory of development. Consequently, evaluation and feedback would initially be largely
328
formative, a mentoring process or the CA approach highlighted earlier, then developed to a
329
point at which the trainee is operating with full autonomy. Alongside development in the
330
metacognitive aspects of performance, developments in practice should be observed and
331
greater autonomy demonstrated by the coach.
332
Alignment between the desired learning outcomes (adaptive expertise) and delivery
333
(declarative knowledge and skills, PJDM (reflection and metacognition)) would need to be
334
matched with a suitably skilled workforce of trainers, examiners and quality assurance.
335
Indeed, the nuances of coaching and educative practices may differ such that an expert coach
336
may not philosophically be an effective or skilled coach educator.
337
The use of case study approaches and constructing case formulations (Martindale &
338
Collins, 2012) is another way in which the nested nature of planning may be evaluated. This
339
would be particularly relevant from Level 3 upwards (based on the current UK Coaching
340
Certification formulation of levels) as coaches’ decision making becomes increasingly
341
layered; as per the first example presented at the start of this paper. The point here is that, as
342
the timespan of the coaching relationship extends, there is an inevitable need for long-
343
(macro) and short- (micro) term decisions to increase in coherence. As above, metacognition
344
on these levels is essential if such longer-term relationships (which characterise higher
345
performance contexts) are to be optimised. These considerations notwithstanding however,
346
we would suggest that there is strong merit in introducing elements of EFE at the earliest
347
stages of a coach’s education journey. The sense that ‘it depends’ is the correct answer to
348
15
many elements of the coaching process is an important consideration; not one that should
349
suddenly appear at a specific level.
350
Conclusion
351
In this paper we have explained how coaches could develop the metacognitive skills
352
required in adaptive and flexible coaching situations. We proposed that a mixed assessment
353
could be employed to evaluate coaching. Developing metacognition alongside declarative
354
knowledge and skill presents a contrast to more proceduralised notions of coach education
355
and coaching. In this context, universal employment of competency-based approaches does
356
not cater for the often complex reality of coaching and, we suggest, is leading to suboptimal
357
professional standards. As such, we anticipate that adopting a mixed approach will foster and
358
encourage adaptive expertise alongside competency, but with challenge, since the perception
359
of performance is, in itself, influenced by a lack of metacognition. However, through our
360
ongoing systematic, considered and applied-focussed research, we believe that this is a
361
necessary next step in the development and further professionalisation of sports coaching.
362
16
References
Abraham, A., & Collins, D. (2011a). Effective skill development: How should athletes’ skills
be developed? In D. Collins, A. Button, & H. Richards (Eds.), Performance
psychology: A practitioner’s guide (pp. 207229). Oxford: Elsevier.
Abraham, A., & Collins, D. (2011b). Taking the next step: Ways forward for coaching
science. Quest, 63, 366384. doi:10.1080/00336297.2011.10483687
Abraham, A., Collins, D., & Martindale, R. (2006). The coaching schematic: Validation
through expert coach consensus. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 549564.
doi:10.1080/02640410500189173
Bolton, G. (2010). Reflective practice, writing and professional development. London: Sage.
Carbonell, J. G. (1985). Derivational analogy: A theory of reconstructive problem solving
and expertise acquisition (1534). Retrieved from Carnegie Mellon University
Computer Science Department:
http://repository.cmu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2533&context=compsci
Carson, H. J., & Collins, D. (2011). Refining and regaining skills in fixation/diversification
stage performers: The Five-A Model. International Review of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 4, 146167. doi:10.1080/1750984x.2011.613682
Christensen, W., Sutton, J., & McIlwain, D. (2016). Cognition in skilled action: Meshed
control and the varieties of skill experience. Mind and Language, 31, 3766.
doi:10.1111/mila.12094
Claxton, G., & Lucas, B. (2007). The creative thinking plan. London: BBC Books.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1987). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the
craft of reading, writing and mathematics (Technical Report No. 403). BBN
Laboratories, Cambridge, MA: Centre for the Study of Reading, University of Illinois.
17
Collins, D., Burke, V., Martindale, A., & Cruickshank, A. (2015). The illusion of competency
versus the desirability of expertise: Seeking a common standard for support
professions in sport. Sports Medicine, 45, 17. doi:10.1007/s40279-014-0251-1
Collins, D., Collins, L., & Carson, H. J. (2016). “If it feels right, do it”: Intuitive decision
making in a sample of high-level sport coaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 504.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00504
Collins, D., Collins, L., & Willmott, T. (2016). Over egging the pudding? Comments on
Ojala and Thorpe. International Sport Coaching Journal, 3, 9093.
doi:10.1123/iscj.2015-0068
Collins, L., & Collins, D. (2012). Conceptualizing the adventure-sports coach. Journal of
Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning, 12, 8193.
doi:10.1080/14729679.2011.611283
Collins, L., & Collins, D. (2013). Decision making and risk management in adventure sports
coaching. Quest, 65, 7282. doi:10.1080/00336297.2012.727373
Collins, L., & Collins, D. (2015). Integration of professional judgement and decision-making
in high-level adventure sports coaching practice. Journal of Sports Sciences, 33, 622
633. doi:10.1080/02640414.2014.953980
Collins, L., & Collins, D. (2016a). Professional judgement and decision-making in adventure
sports coaching: The role of interaction. Journal of Sports Sciences, 34, 12311239.
doi:10.1080/02640414.2015.1105379
Collins, L., & Collins, D. (2016b). Professional judgement and decision making in the
planning process of high level adventure sports coaching practice. Journal of
Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning, Advane online publication.
doi:10.1080/14729679.2016.1162182
18
Collins, L., Collins, D., & Grecic, D. (2015). The epistemological chain in high-level
adventure sports coaches. Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning, 15,
224238. doi:10.1080/14729679.2014.950592
Cruickshank, A. (2013). Case study 1: A Professional Judgement and Decision Making
(PJDM) approach to consultancy with an elite junior judo player. Sport and Exercise
Psychology Review, 9, 1523.
Cruickshank, A., & Collins, D. (2012). Culture change in elite sport performance teams:
Examining and advancing effectiveness in the new era. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 24, 338355. doi:10.1080/10413200.2011.650819
Dail, T. K., & Christina, R. W. (2004). Distribution of practice and metacognition in learning
and long-term retention of a discrete motor task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 75, 148155. doi:10.1080/02701367.2004.10609146
de Oliveira, R. F., Lobinger, B. H., & Raab, M. (2014). An adaptive toolbox approach to the
route to expertise in sport. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 709.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00709
Fenichel, E., & Eggbeer, L. (1990). Preparing practitioners to work with infants, toddlers
and their families: Issues and recommendations for educators and trainers.
Arlington, VA: National Center for Clinical Infant Programs.
Font, V., Bolite, J., & Acevedo, J. (2010). Metaphors in mathematics classrooms: Analyzing
the dynamic process of teaching and learning of graph functions. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 75, 131152. doi:10.1007/s10649-010-9247-4
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural
environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325331.
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
19
Jones, R. L., & Turner, P. (2006). Teaching coaches to coach holistically: Can Problem-
Based Learning (PBL) help? Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy, 11, 181202.
doi:10.1080/17408980600708429
Klien, G. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Human Factors, 50, 456460.
doi:10.1518/001872008X288385
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties recognising
one's own incompetence lead to inflated self assessments. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 77, 11211134.
Lyle, J. (2003). Stimulated recall: A report on its use in naturalistic research. British
Educational Research Journal, 29, 861878. doi:10.1080/0141192032000137349
Martindale, A., & Collins, D. (2012). A professional judgment and decision making case
study: Reflection-in-action research [Special issue]. The Sport Psychologist, 26, 500
518.
Mezirow, J. (1997). Transformative learning: Theory to practice. New Directions for Adult
and Continuing Education, 1997, 512. doi:10.1002/ace.7401
Moon, J. (1999). Reflection in learning and professional development: Theory and practice.
London: Kogan Page.
Ojala, A.-L., & Thorpe, H. (2015). The role of the coach in action sports: Using a problem-
based learning approach. International Sport Coaching Journal, 2, 6471.
doi:10.1123/iscj.2014-0096
Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C. (1992). In search of how people
change: Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 47, 11021114.
doi:10.1037/0003-066x.47.9.1102
20
Schommer, M. A. (1994). Synthesising epistemological belief of research: Tentative
understandings and provocative confusions. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 293
319. doi:10.1007/BF02213418
Schön, D. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. Aldershot,
UK: Ashgate.
Schön, D. (1987). Educating the reflective practitioner. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Stoszkowski, J., & Collins, D. (2012). Communities of practice, social learning and
networks: Exploiting the social side of coach development. Sport, Education and
Society, 19, 773788. doi:10.1080/13573322.2012.692671
Taylor, W. G., & Collins, L. (2016). Jack Mezirow: Transformative learning in coaching. In
L. Nelson, R. Groom , & P. Potrac (Eds.), Learning in sports coaching: Theory and
application (pp. 150160). Abingdon: Routledge.
Vickers, J. (2007). Perception, cognition, and decision training: The quiet eye in action.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Winter, S., & Collins, D. (2015). Why do we do, what we do? Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 27, 3551. doi:10.1080/10413200.2014.941511
Yarritu, I., Matute, H., & Luque, D. (2015). The dark side of cognitive illusions: When an
illusory belief interferes with the acquisition of evidence-based knowledge. British
Journal of Psychology, 106, 597608. doi:10.1111/bjop.12119
... Similarly, as in sports officials, coaches' own motor experiences might also play a role in their decisions. For instance, the selection and placement of anchors in rock climbing requires coaches to simulate the cognitive and motor aspects of the practiced skill (Collins et al., 2016). Generally, motor components however seem to be much less integral to the decision processes of coaches compared to the other two population, reflecting low cognition-action interaction in majority of their decisions. ...
... Despite including coaches as a comparison group in the sample, the aforementioned studies only focused on tasks that reflected decisions of athletes and sports officials. The potential influence of sensorimotor experiences on coach's decision process therefore still remains to be considered and might be particularly relevant in cases such as previously mentioned selection and placement of anchors in rock climbing (Collins et al., 2016). Interactors (and reactors) indicate their decisions using hand signals and whistle (Helsen et al., 2023) and face high physical demands (Bloß et al., 2020), which have been acknowledged in few studies to date. ...
Article
Full-text available
Athletes, sports officials, and coaches make decisions while facing varying levels of cognitive and motor demands. While classic judgment and decision-making (JDM) theories have focused on the cognitive demands, emerging approaches emphasize the interaction of cognitive and motor processes. As the theoretical understanding of JDM is not independent of the experimental tasks used to assess them, the present scoping review aimed to determine to what extent existing decision-making tasks in sports account for dynamic cognition–action interaction. Three databases were systematically searched. Studies were included if they comprised healthy participants, who completed a decision-making task with sport-specific stimuli, and decision-making parameters were assessed as main dependent variables. Upon screening, 430 studies were included, comprising 371 distinct decision-making tasks. The majority of the included studies examined decision-making processes in athletes (70.3%), followed by officials (26.7%) and coaches (3.0%). Despite the frequent use of video stimuli, the presentation of options was deemed rather static in the majority of the laboratory tasks (83.0%). The requisite responses in studies on sports officials and coaches were rather discrete (88.7%), largely constituting micromovements, while 66.4% of athletes’ responses were categorized as continuous, predominantly featuring whole-body movements. Understanding of the cognition–action interaction in JDM is currently undermined by relying on static rather than dynamic options, a focus on simulated rather than executed movements, and dependent variables that are not sufficiently sensitive to capture the cognition–action interaction. We encourage future research to tackle the identified limitations by making use of emerging theoretical approaches and technological developments.
... They are characteristic of expertise (Larrick & Feiler, 2015) that sits within the Type 1 dual processing theory of fast thinking. There is no doubt that practitioners rely on fast heuristic style decision-making within their practice due to the complex dynamic environments in which they work, and consequently, it would be reasonable to assume that it is a characteristic of their expertise (Collins et al., 2016;Lyle, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Practitioners operating in performance sports are required to problem solve, enabling them to offer tailored performance solutions while making expert decisions with high precision. Problem-solving and decision-making are intertwined and tangled in practice. However, the reality is that practitioners require two independent skill sets. This study aims to investigate performance practitioner’s approaches to problem-solving and decision-making, analysing the meta-cognitive skills required by multidisciplinary team (MDT) practitioners to be effective in their daily practice. Using a 71-statement Likert scale survey, 115 performance- and medical-related MDT practitioners were surveyed to gain insight into their strengths and perceptions of how they think about problem-solving and decision-making in their work. We tabulated descriptive data and created heat maps to visualise correlations between responses. Findings suggest that practitioners rely on a mixed bag of approaches, cognitively toggling between problem types, approaches, and decision styles. In this study, practitioners preferred skilled procedural doing and intuitive expertise to overcome simple problems over rationalistic, logical innovation to address complex problems. Findings suggest the need for MDT practitioners to differentiate between problem types, problem-solving approaches, and decision-making styles while deepening our comprehension of practitioners’ expertise. It offers insight into the cognition that forms the foundation of their approaches, providing a valuable perspective.
... For instance, within the context of outdoor education, instructors with expertise have demonstrated heightened situational awareness and the capacity to recognise the demands of their environment through cues (30). The capacity to notice key information results in loops of perception, comprehension, and projection, which supports experts to filter salient information to make optimal decisions which diverge away from original plans of action (31,32). Thus, following the guidance of others, adaptability is the essential ingredient for expertise to be realised (33,34). ...
Article
Full-text available
Within the domain of coach education researchers have long called for a paradigm shift, whereby the quality of coaching practice is no longer measured against a checklist of prescribed competencies. This desire to evolve coach education and development, has been aligned to the need to better identify, understand and utilise what adaptive skill and expertise looks, sounds and feels like across specific sport coaching contexts. This paper outlines a broader research plan for the Premier League to drive the progress of research informed practice, in turn shaping a coach development agenda focused on developing adaptive and skilful coaches within Academies. In turn, this is a core feature of the Premier Leagues institutional aim of developing the most skilful coaches in the world. However, in order to begin the process of initiating such a shift in the way things work, there is a need to seek first to understand, before being understood. Therefore, to demonstrate an evidence informed basis to this shift within coach education and development, we ask three questions; (1) Do we understand what the coaches with the highest level of expertise can do? (2) How should we identify coaches with expertise across different contexts? (3) What does coaching expertise research need to do? In answer to these questions, we present the lack of empirical investigation previously conducted in the sports coaching discipline to explore coaching expertise and draw on wider domains to offer possible capacities of skilful coaches who possess expertise. To identify coaches with expertise, coherent with the broader expertise literature, we suggest that this is best conducted via means of social validation. Finally, we offer a road map of investigation designed to explore expertise, formed of a mix of evidence informed methodologies which have not yet been utilised in sport coaching research.
... Periodic evaluations help identify successes and constraints in coaching and measure the effectiveness of resource use. Through structured and systematic evaluations, sports clubs or organizations can identify the achievement of coaching goals, recognize obstacles that may arise, and measure the effectiveness of the use of resources that have been allocated (Collins et al., 2016;Griffo et al., 2019); (3) Alignment with achievement goals. These findings encourage the authors to focus more resources on achievement coaching. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Taekwondo sports coaching system necessitates effective organisational management. However, managing a sports organisation unquestionably demands a suitable and accurate concept, one that will ultimately allow the sports organisation to accomplish its objectives. The purpose of this study was to examine the basic interpretation of historical aspects, development systems, management resources, and achievement development. The research method used in this study is qualitative descriptive research, employing a phenomenological research design. The research sample consists of 5 informants, including 1 club founder, 2 club executives, and 2 athlete coaches, selected for direct interviews. Data analysis involves (i) introduction and organization; (ii) coding and reduction; (iii) interpretation and representation. The findings revealed that the coaching system, management resources and Taekwondo achievement development at the Mahameru Club in Sukoharjo Regency were functioning effectively. The limitations of this study are the sample used and the scope of the study which is only one area. As a result, future research should compare and broaden the scope of the study by investigating the factors that influence the implementation of the coaching system, management resources, and achievement development, such as psychological, sociological, and environmental aspects for athletes. The conclusion of this research indicates that this research makes a notable contribution by delving into the uncharted territory of Taekwondo sports coaching at Mahameru Club. It not only fills a knowledge gap but also furnishes practical guidance for the improvement of coaching strategies.
... Specific examples include being trapped against sea cliffs while tidal flows created massive waves an hour earlier than forecast, attending to climbers who had fallen from ice falls and sustained critical injuries, or improvising a pully system to rescue a kayaker trapped in a waterfall. These are all real examples of chaotic learning activities which were not planned, but were expected within the context of regular exposure to open-ended adventurous environments (Boyes & O'Hare, 2003;Collins et al., 2016). In the chaotic domain the learning is most likely focussed on developing metacognition. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Learning Design continues to emerge as a distinct area of professional practice in both corporate and education contexts. This practice must now adapt to meet the challenges of increased expectation of learner experience, an exponential rate of global change, and the need for learning experiences to be future-fit. However, there is a lack of a coherent framework to guide Learning Design practice in addressing these challenges. There is a lack of a framework that supports the learning designer as a leader of change for a thriving future. This thesis proposes an emergent framework: Leadership by Learning Design. The researcher adopts a pragmatic approach, which draws upon both existing research and professional experience across a range of design related themes including anthro-complexity, future studies, decolonisation, transformational learning, and systems transformation. Key literature in each of these areas is explored, contextualised to Learning Design, then applied in a manner that pushed beyond the boundaries of prevalent practice. Together these themes form an emergent, yet coherent, framework which allows Learning Designers to explore their role as leaders for a thriving future - Leadership by Learning Design.
Article
Purpose This paper aims to address the United Nations’ Sustainable Development Goal 4 “quality education” through the adoption of heutagogic ideas in improving work-based learning for individuals. Design/methodology/approach This paper locates a discussion of heutagogy within the wider context of vocational and higher education, as well as through reference to the theoretical discourse on learning theory. Findings Heutagogy is concerned with the development of individual capability through the creation of a learner-determined curriculum that is attuned to professional goals. Organizations that adopt heutagogic approaches enhance corporate adaptiveness through personalized learning. Originality/value This briefing provides the reader with an accessible insight into both the individual and organizational benefits of adopting heutagogic ideas in professional development. As organizations confront an increasingly uncertain external environment, this paper offers an insight into how to develop individual capability.
Article
Full-text available
Coach education and continuing career development have become a significant focus of global discussion within the sport domain. Current mainstream strategies for developing and assessing coaches in most countries, including China, are based on competency-based systems. However, there are many shortcomings of this system, especially when considering the varied practical challenges and needs of coaches and athletes; in short, such an approach does not facilitate enough adaptability. The purpose of this article is to critically review the literature, exploring both competence- and expertise-based coach development systems and their implications for coaching practice in China. Firstly, we introduce and discuss the competency-based approach, including its strengths and weaknesses and how this applies within the Chinese development system. Next, we introduce and evaluate an alternative, expertise-based development system characterised by adaptability and greater inclusiveness within the coaching domain, which is underpinned by a distinct set of cognitive decision-making skills from the coach’s perspective. In addition, we expand this discussion by explaining the implications of this approach for coach assessment and offer some future suggestions for research in this area.
Article
Full-text available
The ability of coaches to make effective decisions that can impact positively on a team’s performance during competition is a fundamental skill in coaching, especially in fast, dynamic team sports such as soccer. Yet, there has been little research attention given to exploring the thought processes underpinning coaches’ decision-making during soccer match-play. We used a think aloud protocol analysis to explore the cognitions of skilled and less-skilled soccer coaches who were required to watch and coach a team during representative video clips of a soccer match first half. At the end of the first half of the match, coaches were also asked to verbalise their thoughts of what they would do or say to the team at half-time. We further assessed the quality of decisions made at half-time. During first-half match-play, skilled coaches verbalised more thoughts related to performance and tactical evaluations, and the planning of actions than less-skilled coaches, who mostly monitored the ongoing game actions or events. Moreover, during half-time skilled coaches made more appropriate decisions which were underpinned by more relevant planning strategies aimed at improving team performance for the second half than less-skilled participants. Findings enhance our understanding of cognitive expertise in coaches’ decision-making performance during competition.
Article
Full-text available
Comprehensive understanding and application of decision making is important for the professional practice and status of sports coaches. Accordingly, building on a strong work base exploring the use of professional judgment and decision making (PJDM) in sport, we report a preliminary investigation into uses of intuition by high-level coaches. Two contrasting groups of high-level coaches from adventure sports (n = 10) and rugby union (n = 8), were interviewed on their experiences of using intuitive and deliberative decision making styles, the source of these skills, and the interaction between the two. Participants reported similarly high levels of usage to other professions. Interaction between the two styles was apparent to varying degrees, while the role of experience was seen as an important precursor to greater intuitive practice and employment. Initially intuitive then deliberate decision making was a particular feature, offering participants an immediate check on the accuracy and validity of the decision. Integration of these data with the extant literature and implications for practice are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
We present a synthetic theory of skilled action which proposes that cognitive processes make an important contribution to almost all skilled action, contrary to influential views that many skills are performed largely automatically. Cognitive control is focused on strategic aspects of performance, and plays a greater role as difficulty increases. We offer an analysis of various forms of skill experience and show that the theory provides a better explanation for the full set of these experiences than automatic theories. We further show that the theory can explain experimental evidence for skill automaticity, including evidence that secondary tasks do not interfere with expert performance, and evidence that experts have reduced memory for performance of sensorimotor skills.
Article
Full-text available
In a recent paper in ISCJ, Ojala and Thorpe offered a culturally based observation that questions the role and application of coaching in action sports. Their critique is focused on the action sport of snowboarding which, despite its' comparatively recent inclusion in the Olympics, retains a different, almost collaborative rather than competitive culture more akin to other action sports such as skateboarding and surfing. Ojala and Thorpe then present Problem Based Learning (PBL) as the solution to many of these perceived ills, describing the positive characteristics of the approach and promoting its cultural fit with action sport environments and performers. In this paper we offer a different perspective, which questions the veracity of the data presented and the unquestioningly positive view of PBL as the answer. Our alternative, data-driven perspective suggests that action sport athletes are increasingly positive, or even desirous of good coaching, of which PBL is a possible approach; suitable for some athletes some of the time.
Book
Athletes must be able to make split-second decisions under the pressures of competition, but often this vital learning is left to chance. With Perception, Cognition, and Decision Training: The Quiet Eye in Action, readers gain access to the research foundations behind an innovative decision-training system that has been used successfully for years in training athletes. Certain to become the definitive guide to decision making in sport, this text presents three innovations solidly based in research. The first is the vision- in-action method of recording what athletes actually see when they perform. The second is the quiet eye phenomenon that has attracted considerable media attention. The third innovation is decision training to identify not only how athletes make performance decisions but also how to facilitate visual perception and action to enhance performance. Author Joan Vickers—who discovered the quiet eye and developed the vision-in-action method—takes the next step by integrating all three innovations into a system for helping athletes improve. Together, these advances provide scientific evidence of the effectiveness of perception– action coupling in athletes’ training.
Article
This investigation examined the planning and decision-making processes in adventure sports coaching. We utilised a thematic analysis approach to investigate the planning decision-making practices of a sample of high-level adventure sports coaches over a series of sessions. The investigation discovered that, in planning coaching activity, high-level adventure sports coaches draw on their epistemological values and domain-specific expertise, employ a synergy of classic and naturalistic decision-making processes, and continually audit the evolving coaching process. Based on these findings, implications for professional training, accreditation and development of adventure sports coaches are presented.
Article
Reflective practice is key to the meaningfulness, effectiveness and personal and professional satisfaction of the therapeutic endeavour. Expressive and explorative writing offers a swift, dynamic, and challenging route to reflective practice. This paper draws upon my experience running groups for therapists, counsellors, clinical psychologists and other clinicians. The processes of writing, and making reflective use of that writing, are examined.
Article
This case study of an elite judo player recovering from injury provides an exemplification of a practitioner's Professional Judgment and Decision Making (PJDM) using a 'reflection-in-action research' methodology. The process of "reflectionin-action" Schön (1991) and in particular the concept of 'framing' offer insight into how professionals think in action. These concepts assisted the practitioner in organizing, clarifying and conceptualizing the client's issues and forming intentions for impact. This case study exemplifies the influence of practitioner PJDM on implementation at multiple levels of practice including planning the overall program of support, designing specific interventions to aid client recovery and moment-to-moment in-situ decision making session-by session. It is suggested that consideration of practitioner PJDM should be a strong feature of case study reporting and that this approach carries the potential to extend our use of case studies within applied sport psychology practice.
Article
This qualitative study presents the view that coaching practice places demands on the coach's adaptability and flexibility. These requirements for being adaptive and flexible are met through a careful process of professional judgement and decision-making based on context-appropriate bodies of knowledge. Adventure sports coaches were selected for study on the basis that adventure sports create a hyper-dynamic environment in which these features can be examined. Thematic analysis revealed that coaches were generally well informed and practised with respect to the technical aspects of their sporting disciplines. Less positively, however, they often relied on ad hoc contextualisation of generalised theories of coaching practice to respond to the hyper-dynamic environments encountered in adventure sports. We propose that coaching practice reflects the demands of the environment, individual learning needs of the students and the task at hand. Together, these factors outwardly resemble a constraints-led approach but, we suggest, actually reflect manipulation of these parameters from a cognitive rather than an ecological perspective. This process is facilitated by a refined judgement and decision-making process, sophisticated epistemology and an explicit interaction of coaching components.