Content uploaded by Sten Helmfrid
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Sten Helmfrid on Oct 21, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
Studies on Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Graded
Exercise Therapy for ME/CFS are misleading
Sten Helmfrid
Assoc. Prof. of Physics, Member of the Swedish ME Association
This is a translation of an article published in Socialmedicinsk tidskrift, Stockholm,
Sweden, on September 28
th
, 2016. http://socialmedicinsktidskrift.se
Link to the original article in Swedish (vol. 93, issue 4, pp. 433–44):
http://socialmedicinsktidskrift.se/index.php/smt/article/view/1450/1255
Citation: Helmfrid S. Studier av kognitiv beteendeterapi och gradvis ökad träning vid
ME/CFS är missvisande. Soc Med Tidskr. 2016;93(4):433–44.
Abstract
There have been a number of studies on Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and
Graded Exercise Therapy (GET) for ME/CFS based on a treatment model where the
disease is perpetuated by cognitive processes. Although the studies are flawed and
the model lacks scientific support, the treatments are described as evidence based.
The studies are non-blinded and rely on subjective outcomes. There are no objective
measures of adherence. The diagnostic criteria vary, and the participating patients
often have one or several psychiatric diagnoses apart from suffering from chronic
fatigue. The underlying model has no theoretical foundation and is at odds with
physiological findings. Surveys suggest that the efficacy of CBT is no better than
placebo and that GET is harmful. Therefore, cognitive behavioral therapy and graded
exercise therapy for ME/CFS are not evidence based.
Keywords: myalgic encephalomyelitis, chronic fatigue syndrome, treatment,
biopsychosocial, PACE
— 1 —
Introduction
ME/CFS—sometimes also referred to as chronic fatigue syndrome—is a severely de-
bilitating disease [1]. It is classified since 1969 as a neurological disease by the World
Health Organization (WHO) [2]. Although ME/CFS has unknown etiology and patho-
genesis, a number of abnormalities have been observed in patients, including immu-
ne and autonomic dysfunction, neuroendocrine abnormalities, brain and mitochond-
rial dysfunction, and neurocognitive defects [3].
In the late 1980s, a group of British psychiatrists—the so-called Oxford school—
introduced a biopsychosocial treatment model for ME/CFS. They claimed that the
disease is perpetuated by avoidance behavior and deconditioning and that this
pattern can be reversed by cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) [4]. The cognitive
therapy is often combined with graded exercise therapy (GET), which means that the
patients gradually increase their activity level to agreed targets in order to reverse
the deconditioning. The psychiatrists created their own criteria for chronic fatigue
syndrome, which only requires prolonged fatigue and therefore selects a larger and
more heterogeneous group of patients than what is normally understood by ME/CFS
[5].
There have been numerous studies published on cognitive behavioral therapy and
graded exercise therapy for ME/CFS, for example the British PACE study [6]. It was
published in the Lancet in 2011, and attracted a lot of media attention. The studies
have serious methodological flaws, and evidence that contradicts the model has been
neglected. Negative results have been played down. Despite criticism from research-
ers, doctors, journalists, and patients’ organizations, which often has been harsh [7–
15], the proponents continue to claim positive outcomes for the treatments. This
article provides an overview of the methodological flaws of the studies and of the
evidence that contradicts the underlying model. Cognitive behavioral therapy and
graded exercise therapy according to the Oxford model has no positive effect in
patients with ME/CFS, but on the contrary often leads to deterioration.
There is no improvement in objective outcomes
In treatment studies, the group or groups receiving the experimental treatment are
always compared with a control group. To avoid systematic errors in the comparison
due to the placebo effect or reporting bias, the study must either be blinded or use
objective outcomes [16]. Several factors contribute to the systematic error, for exam-
ple the attitude of the therapists. It is therefore expected that the errors are large in
studies of cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exercise therapy, as it is part of
the treatment model to convince patients that the method works.
The studies of CBT and GET according to the Oxford model have almost exclusively
relied on subjective self-reports [6], which makes it impossible to determine the real
efficacy of the treatment. In the few cases where objective outcomes have been re-
ported, the results have been disappointing. One way of obtaining an objective mea-
sure of fatigue is to register the activity level of the patient by a so-called accelero-
meter, a device roughly the size of a watch that is attached to the wrist or the ankle.
A Dutch group reviewed three earlier studies in which activity data were recorded by
accelerometers, but no analysis was carried out. Their post-analysis showed that
— 2 —
there was no increase in the objective activity levels of the patients, in spite of re-
ported improvement in the subjective fatigue [17]. In two other uncontrolled studies
of CBT with accelerometers, subjective improvement did not result in any increase in
the objective activity level [18,19]. In a study that compared neuropsychological test
results before and after intervention with cognitive behavioral therapy, the self-
reported cognitive impairment improved, but the objective test scores remained un-
changed [20].
The PACE study included a test of how far the patients were able to walk in six
minutes. All treatment groups improved slightly during the study, but the group that
followed the graded exercise program improved more than the others [6]. This was
portrayed as objective support for GET by the authors. However, the walking test is a
rough measure of improvement, as it is not possible to control what effort the parti-
cipants are making. In the complementary fitness test that was carried out during
the study, but was published only four years later, there was no improvement for any
of the groups—including the one that followed the graded exercise program [21]. This
suggests that the attitude of the patients changed, but that there was no improve-
ment in physical functioning [22]. It should be pointed out that the result of the GET
group in the walking test, 379 m, was very modest. A result of 400 m corresponds to
severe functional impairment and has been suggested as a marker when lung pati-
ents should be listed for transplantation [23]. The normal result for healthy adults is
about 640 m [24].
Other objective measures of improvement are return to productive work or studies.
A follow-up of PACE showed that the treatment did not result in fewer sick days, nor
in a reduction in levels of benefits or payments from insurance companies. The same
picture emerged in an audit of four Belgian rehabilitation centers. There was no im-
provement in the physical fitness of the patients after 6–12 months of treatment, and
the number of hours in employment decreased [26].
When activity is used as an objective measure in studies of ME/CFS, it is important
to register the overall activity for a period of time. Patients are often compensating
for increased activity by cutting back on other commitments. Children with ME/CFS,
for example, usually abandon leisure activities in order to keep up at school [27]. In a
Dutch study of internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy for adolescents (FIT-
NET), school attendance increased alongside with improvements in subjective out-
comes, which was interpreted as a big success by the authors [28]. The fact that nei-
ther activities out of school nor performance in school were monitored makes it im-
possible to determine if there was any improvement in the physical or cognitive capa-
city of the patients. The results of FITNET are at odds with previous experiences of
objective outcomes and must therefore be interpreted with caution.
The risks are underestimated because there are no objective measures of
adherence
Patients’ organizations have often pointed out that graded exercise therapy may lead
to deterioration in ME/CFS. Reports of adverse events in published studies have not
always been easy to interpret, but treatment risks are usually dismissed by propo-
nents of the Oxford model. In the PACE study, more cases of severe deterioration
— 3 —
were reported in the GET group than in other groups. However, the authors thought
that the difference was negligible and concluded that graded exercise therapy is a
safe intervention [6].
When the risks of a treatment are assessed, it is important to know how the patients
adhere to the protocol. The lack of objective measures of the activity level in most
studies of graded exercise therapy means that there are also no objective measures of
adherence. Studies in which accelerometers were used show that self-reported acti-
vity levels are unreliable [19]. In a case study of graded exercise therapy, the patient
was unable to follow the training program and reduced the activity level [29]. Similar
results were obtained in another study where patients should walk a certain distance
every day [30]. The authors of the PACE study claim that the adherence to the exer-
cise protocol was good, but the poor results in the fitness test suggest otherwise. The
relatively low number of adverse events that has been reported in published studies
therefore does not mean that graded exercise therapy according to the protocol is
safe.
The inclusion criteria are not consistent
Another problem is that the inclusion criteria in the studies have been inconsistent.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) published the first criteria for the
disease in 1988 after an epidemic in Lake Tahoe, and introduced the term Chronic
Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) [31]. CDC singled out persistent fatigue as the cardinal
symptom, although experienced physicians that were involved in several earlier
epidemics had suggested that muscle fatigability is the most characteristic symptom
[32,33]. The criteria were revised in 1994, the so-called Fukuda criteria [34]. The
patients must experience severe fatigue over a period of six months and have addi-
tionally four of eight specified neurological and immunological symptoms. Criteria of
this kind—where some of the symptoms from a larger pool of symptoms should be
present—are called polythetic and have the disadvantage that the selected group of
patients becomes more heterogeneous.
The Oxford school emphasized the symptom fatigue and suggested that the other
symptoms and the etiology were of secondary importance. They published criteria of
their own in 1991, which only required persistent, severe fatigue [5]. They used the
name CFS, although it was already used in the CDC construct with a slightly diffe-
rent meaning, and despite the word syndrome usually referring to a combination of
symptoms rather than a single symptom. This has caused a lot of confusion in the
scientific literature. CFS according to the Oxford criteria selects a larger and more
heterogeneous group than the Fukuda criteria and includes many patients with
psychiatric diagnoses. Studies of exercise programs are difficult to interpret, as pati-
ents with ME/CFS often feel worse after exercise, whereas fatigued patients with
major depressive disorder (MDD) often feel better [35].
In 2003, an expert panel selected by Health Canada published a consensus document
on chronic fatigue syndrome. They revived the earlier term ME for the disease and
introduced the compromise term ME/CFS. They also proposed a new and more
restrictive set of criteria for the disease, the Canadian Consensus Criteria (CCC)
[36]. The objective was to select a more homogeneous group of patients. Post exer-
— 4 —
tional malaise became mandatory for diagnosis. In addition, the patients should have
a number of neurological, immunological and endocrine symptoms. These criteria are
used by the International Association for CFS/ME (IACFS/ME) [3] and are recom-
mended by most biomedical researchers in the field.
The evaluation and the comparison of treatment studies on ME/CFS have been comp-
licated not only by different inclusion criteria, but also because the criteria have been
incorrectly operationalized by some investigators. To operationalize usually means
that abstract formulations are interpreted, so that they can be applied in a practical
situation. In many treatment studies of CBT and GET, the investigators have pushed
the boundaries and changed or removed some of the conditions that should be met. In
some cases, this has caused uncertainty whether the patients in the study really re-
flect the group selected by the inclusion criteria. For example, a Dutch group claimed
that the Fukuda criteria were used in a study, but they "operationalized" the criteria
by removing the four additional symptoms [37].
Most of the early studies of cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exercise therapy
used the Oxford criteria [38], but in more recent studies the Fukuda criteria have
also been used [39]. It is unclear if the results from a heterogeneous group of patients
may be particularized to a more strictly defined group, for example patients who
meet the Canadian consensus criteria. A study of patients with chronic fatigue in the
UK primary care suggested that a strong predictor for poor outcome of cognitive
behavioral therapy and graded exercise therapy is that the patients meet the Fukuda
criteria for ME/CFS [40].
The Oxford criteria were used in the PACE study, but the study included a sub-group
analysis that compared the results for the main group with the results for patients
who met the so-called London criteria [41] and CDC's revised criteria from 2003 (in
practice the same as the Fukuda criteria) [42]. The conclusion was that there were no
significant differences between the compared diagnostic criteria. Unfortunately, the
sub-groups in the comparison were biased. Patients who were recruited to PACE met
the Oxford criteria, which require fatigue to be the predominant symptom [5]. From
this group, patients who fulfilled the other diagnostic criteria were then selected. The
process excludes patients who fulfill the London criteria or CDC criteria, but who
have predominantly neurological or immunological symptoms. The correct procedure
would be to recruit the patients with different diagnostic criteria separately, not from
the group of Oxford patients. On top of this, a follow-up publication two years after
the original study revealed that the Fukuda criteria were operationalized in PACE.
The four additional symptoms needed to be present only for a week, rather than for
six months [43]. The six-month limit is important to rule out other conditions [44],
and it is therefore unclear which type of patients were actually included in the com-
parative analysis of PACE.
There is no scientific support for the underlying “theory”
There is no scientific support for the treatment model used by the Oxford school, and
its proponents have caused confusion by using inaccurate terms in the scientific lite-
rature and by presenting exaggerated claims. In science, a theory or a model is a de-
scription of the mechanisms behind a phenomenon [45]. (A model should not be con-
— 5 —
fused with a treatment model, which is merely a flow chart for the therapeutic
efforts.) If a theory, model or other scientific proposition does not yet have empirical
support, it is called a hypothesis [46]. The interventions with cognitive behavioral
therapy and graded exercise therapy are based on a hypothesis that the disease is
perpetuated by avoidance behavior and that symptoms are caused by a lack of fit-
ness. Although the Oxford school have not described any underlying mechanisms, nor
presented any evidence for the presumed causation, they refer to their hypotheses
either as theories [6] or models [47]. This gives the impression of scientific support,
which in fact does not exist.
When the hypotheses of the Oxford school first were introduced, the authors descri-
bed their ideas with an analogy. They accepted that infections often are triggers for
ME/CFS, but they pointed out that for example in a hit-and-run accident, pursuing
the guilty party is not necessary in order to get started with the rehabilitation of the
victim [4]. Similarly, it is not necessary to know what triggered ME/CFS in the first
place. The analogy misses an important point. When victims of a traffic accident are
rehabilitated, doctors must know what injuries they have sustained—fractures requi-
re different therapies than burns—and when ME/CFS is treated, the rehabilitation
strategy must resonate with the true perpetuating factors. The hit-and-run traffic
analogy gives no hint as to whether the proposed strategy really works.
The hypotheses of the Oxford school are implausible given what we know about ME/
CFS. The activity level of patients often follows a push–crash pattern [48]. Patients
push themselves to their limit, which leads to a deterioration (“crash”), and eventual-
ly to a recovery and to a new push period. This is inconsistent with the assumption of
avoidance behavior. Another problem is that post-infective fatigue only has been rep-
orted after a limited number of infections [49,50]. The hypotheses do not explain why
some pathogens do not trigger ME/CFS. The same perpetuating cognitive factors
should be present after any infection. It is also unlikely that lack of fitness could
cause ME/CFS symptoms. In this case similar symptoms should be observed in
people who are inactive for other reasons, such as people in plaster for a long time or
prisoners in isolation cells.
There have been several attempts to provide a theoretical basis for a biopsychosocial
understanding of ME/CFS. Vercoulen et al. published a structural equation model for
ME/CFS and concluded that behavioral and cognitive factors are involved in the per-
petuation of the symptoms [51]. However, this is a misinterpretation of the results.
Structural equation models can be used to test assumptions on how different factors
are related to each other, but the causal relations are always ambiguous [52]. It is
impossible to determine which of the biological, behavioral and cognitive factors are
antecedents and which are consequents without first having an understanding of the
underlying mechanisms, and this is not provided by the article. Moreover, Vercoulen
used a heterogeneous group of patients. When other researchers tried to replicate the
results, they obtained poor agreement for patients with ME/CFS, but good agreement
for patients with psychiatric conditions [53].
— 6 —
Physiological abnormalities verify that patients deteriorate after exercise
In graded exercise therapy, patients should gradually increase their level of activity
according to a predetermined plan. This treatment strategy has often been criticized,
as exertion may trigger a relapse in patients. Today there is strong evidence that
post-exertional malaise—which is regarded as a cardinal symptom of ME/CFS—has a
biomedical basis. Several independent studies show that patients cannot reproduce
their test parameters on day two in a repeated cardiopulmonary exercise test (CPET)
[54–56]. ME/CFS patients have reduced capacity to recover from acidosis as com-
pared to healthy controls [57]. Immunological changes [58] and cognitive impairment
[59] after exertion have also been demonstrated.
The principal investigator from the PACE trial, Peter White, has dismissed the criti-
cism of graded exercise therapy and claims that the activity level is below the level
that triggers physiological changes [60]. However, he admits that the graded-exercise
programs have been adapted so that patients may abstain from an increase in the
activity level or if necessary return to a previous lower level of activity. His defense is
thus an indirect confirmation that post-exertional deterioration does occur and that
the Oxford model does not describe the disease correctly.
Surveys suggest that CBT and GET are of no benefit
Patients’ organizations have for a long time used surveys to evaluate the efficacy of
different therapies. Data is available from eleven independent surveys conducted in
four different countries with replies from more than 15,000 patients [61–64]. The
results give a completely different picture than the published studies of the Oxford
model. The surveys suggest that graded exercise therapy carries a high risk of dete-
rioration. More than 4,700 patients have tried this treatment, and 53% reported that
they felt worse.
One of the largest surveys was conducted by the ME Association in the UK. In a
comparison between different therapies, graded exercise therapy had the second low-
est proportion of patients who reported an improvement, and the highest proportion
who reported deterioration [63]. More than 56% of the patients felt worse, and 33%
said they became much worse. The proportion of patients who improved was smaller
both for graded exercise therapy and cognitive behavior therapy than for homeopa-
thy—that is, in practice placebo. The same pattern appeared in a Norwegian patient
survey [62].
Conclusions
In the late 1980s, British psychiatrists introduced a treatment model for ME/CFS,
which has been tested in a number of non-blinded studies. A moderate improvement
in subjective parameters has been demonstrated in some cases, but the results have
been negative when objective parameters have been assessed. Patients have not been
able to return to productive work after treatment. Even though there are no objective
measures of adherence, the treatments are described as safe. Most studies have been
carried out on patients with chronic fatigue, but it has been assumed that the results
apply to more strictly defined ME/CFS. There is no scientific support for the hypo-
— 7 —
thesis that cognitive processes perpetuate the disease. A treatment where psycho-
therapy is used to convince the patients about the validity of an unproven hypothesis
can hardly be regarded as evidence based.
Both the treatment model and the results of the studies are contradicted by other
evidence. There are many reports that suggest a biomedical basis for post-exertional
malaise. This is inconsistent with the assumption that there is pathological avoidan-
ce of exertion in ME/CFS, and makes it difficult to believe that exercise leads to im-
provement. Patient surveys suggest that the efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy
according to the Oxford model is no better than placebo, and that more than half of
the patients are made worse by graded exercise therapy.
PACE is so far the largest study on cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exercise
therapy. Even though it does not nearly meet the requirements for pharmacological
studies, it is described as a randomized controlled trial [47]. It is astonishing that so
many voices in the debate have been completely uncritical. The problems became
known to a wider audience in 2015, when an American health reporter demolished
the PACE study on the popular website Virology blog [15]. The initial article was
followed by several other critical blog posts [65]. Eventually, six prominent scientists
wrote an open letter to the editor-in-chief of the Lancet and demanded an indepen-
dent review of PACE [66]. The letter was ignored, but was followed by another letter
with forty two signatories [67]. More criticism has come from the 2013 Bill Silverman
prize winner, Professor James Coyne [68], and from the Director of Stats.org, Profes-
sor Rebecca Goldin [69].
It is ultimately up to healthcare providers to critically assess the quality of treatment
studies. This article shows that there is no evidence for benefits of CBT according to
the Oxford model in ME/CFS and that graded exercise therapy often leads to deterio-
ration.
References
1. Beyond Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Redefining an Illness.
Committee on the Diagnostic Criteria for Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue
Syndrome; Board on the Health of Select Populations; Institute of Medicine. Washington,
D.C.: National Academies Press, 2015.
2. International Classification of Diseases – Manual of the International Statistical
Classification of Diseases, Injuries, and Causes of Death. Volume 1. Based on the
Recommendation of the Eighth Revision Conference, 1965, and Adopted by the
Nineteenth World Health Assembly. Geneva: World Health Organisation, 1967, p. 158.
3. Chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis. Primer for clinical practitioners.
2014 Edition. Chicago: International Association for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome/Myalgic
Encephalomyelitis, 2014.
4. Wessely S, David A, Butler S, Chalder T. Management of chronic (post-viral) fatigue
syndrome. J R Coll Gen Pract. 1989;39(318):26–9.
5. Sharpe MC, Archard LC, Banatvala JE, et al. A report – chronic fatigue syndrome:
guidelines for research. J Royal Soc Med. 1991;84(2):118–21.
6. White PD, Goldsmith KA, Johnson AL, et al. Comparison of adaptive pacing therapy,
cognitive behavior therapy, graded exercise therapy, and specialist medical care for
chronic fatigue syndrome (PACE): a randomized trial. Lancet. 2011;377(9768):823–36.
7. Williams M. Statement of Concern about CBT/GET provided for the High Court Judicial
— 8 —
Review of February 2009. 2009. http://www.investinme.org/Article-
361%20Statements%20of%20Concern%20-%20CBT-GET%20JR%20Feb09.htm.
8. Twisk FNM, Maes M. A review on cognitive behavorial therapy (CBT) and graded exercise
therapy (GET) in myalgic encephalomyelitis (ME) / chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS):
CBT/GET is not only ineffective and not evidence-based, but also potentially harmful for
many patients with ME/CFS. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2009;30(3):284–99.
9. ME Association. ME Association press statement about the results of the PACE study.
2011. http://www.meassociation.org.uk/2011/02/me-association-press-statement-on-the-
pace-trial-results/.
10. Mitchell JT Jr. The PACE trial in chronic fatigue syndrome. Lancet. 2011;377(9780):1831.
11. Stouten B, Goudsmit EM, Riley N. The PACE trial in chronic fatigue syndrome. Lancet.
2011;377(9780):1832–3.
12. Kindlon T. The PACE trial in chronic fatigue syndrome. Lancet. 2011;377(9780):1833.
13. Chaudhuri A. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome. Lancet.
2001;358(9277):238.
14. Vermeulen RCW, Scholte HR, Bezemer PD. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic fatigue
syndrome. Lancet. 2001;358(9277):238.
15. Tuller D. TRIAL BY ERROR: The Troubling Case of the PACE Chronic Fatigue Syndrome
Study. 2015. http://www.virology.ws/2015/10/21/trial-by-error-i/,
http://www.virology.ws/2015/10/22/trial-by-error-ii/,
http://www.virology.ws/2015/10/23/trial-by-error-iii/.
16. Wood L, Egger M, Gluud LL, et al. Empirical evidence of bias in treatment effect
estimates in controlled trials with different interventions and outcomes: meta-
epidemiological study. Br Med J. 2008;336(7644):601–5.
17. Wiborg JF, Knoop H, Stulemeijer M, et al. How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce
fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychol
Med. 2010;40(8):1281–7.
18. Heins MJ, Knoop H, Burk WJ, et al. The process of cognitive behaviour therapy for
chronic fatigue syndrome: Which changes in perpetuating cognitions and behaviour are
related to a reduction in fatigue? J Psychosom Res. 2013;75(3):235–41.
19. Friedberg F, Sohl S. Cognitive-Behavior Therapy in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Is
Improvement Related to Increased Physical Activity? J Clin Psychol. 2009;65(4):423–42.
20. Knoop H, Prins JB, Stulemeijer M, et al. The effect of cognitive behaviour therapy for
chronic fatigue syndrome on self-reported cognitive impairments and neuropsychological
test performance. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(4):434–6.
21. Chalder T, Goldsmith KA, White PD, et al. Rehabilitative therapies for chronic fatigue
syndrome: a secondary mediation analysis of the PACE trial. Lancet Psychiatry.
2015;2(2):141–52.
22. Knoop H, Wiborg JF. What makes a difference in chronic fatigue syndrome? Lancet
Psychiatry. 2015;2(2):113–4.
23. Kadikar A, Maurer J, Kesten S. The Six-Minute Walk Test: a guide to assessment for lung
transplantation. J Heart Lung Transplant. 1997;16(3):313–9.
24. Hill K, Wickerson L, Woon LJ. The 6-min walk test: responses in healthy Canadians aged
45 to 85 years. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2011;36(5):643–9.
25. McCrone P, Sharpe M, Chalder T, et al. Adaptive Pacing, Cognitive Behaviour Therapy,
Graded Exercise, and Specialist Medical Care for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: A Cost-
Effectiveness Analysis. PLos One. 2012;7(8):e40808.
26. Stordeur S, Thiry N, Eyssen M. Chronisch Vermoeidheidssyndroom: diagnose,
behandeling en zorgorganisatie. KCE reports 88A. Brussels: Federaal Kenninscentrum
voor de Gezondheidszorg, 2008.
27. Van Hoof ELS, De Becker PJ, Lapp C, et al. How do adolescents with chronic fatigue
syndrome perceive their social environment? A quantitative study. Bull IACFS ME.
2009;17(1):16–31.
— 9 —
28. Nijhof SL, Bleijenberg G, Uiterwaal CS, et al. Effectiveness of internet-based cognitive
behavioural treatment for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome (FITNET): a
randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2012;379(9824):1412–8.
29. Friedberg F. Does graded activity increase activity? A case study of chronic fatigue
syndrome. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiat. 2002;33(3–4):203–15.
30. Black CD, McCully KK. Time course of exercise induced alterations in daily activity in
chronic fatigue syndrome. Dyn Med. 2005;4:10.
31. Holmes GP, Kaplan JE, Gantz NM, et al. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: A Working Case
Definition. Ann Intern Med. 1988;108(3):387–9.
32. Ramsay AM. Myalgic Encephalomyelitis and Postviral Fatigue States. The saga of Royal
Free disease. Second edition. London: Gower Medical Publishing, 1988.
33. Dowsett EG. Conversation piece. Interview by P. D. Welsby. Postgrad Med J.
1992;68(795):63–5.
34. Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, et al. The Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: A Comprehensive
Approach to Its Definition and Study. Ann Intern Med. 1994;121(12):953–9.
35. Van Ness M. An Exercise in Objectivity: A Realistic Approach Exercise in ME/CFS.
Muntlig presentation vid Exercise and ME/CFS, Bristol Watershed, 5 februari 2014.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=q_cnva7zyKM.
36. Carruthers BM, Jain AK, DeMeirleir KM, et al. Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic
Fatigue Syndrome: Clinical Working Case Definition, Diagnostic and Treatment
Protocols. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. 2003;11(1):7–115.
37. Prins JB, Bleijenberg G, Bazelmans E, et al. Cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fati-
gue syndrome: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Lancet. 2001;357(9259):841–7.
38. Sharpe M, Hawton K, Simkin S, et al. Cognitive behavior therapy for chronic fatigue
syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. Br Med J. 1996;312(7022):22–6.
39. Núñez M, Fernández-Solà J, Núñez E, et al. Health-related quality of life in patients with
chronic fatigue syndrome: group cognitive behavioural therapy and graded exercise versus
usual treatment. A randomised controlled trial with 1 year of follow-up. Clin Rheumatol.
2011;30(3):381–9.
40. Darbishire L, Seed P, Ridsdale L. Predictors of outcome following treatment for chronic
fatigue. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:350–1.
41. Tyrrell DAJ. Report from the National Task Force on Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS),
Post Viral Fatigue Syndrome (PVFS) and Myalgic Encephalomyelitis (ME). Bristol:
Westcare, 1994.
42. Reeves WC, Lloyd A, Vernon SD, et al. Identification of ambiguities in the 1994 chronic
fatigue research case definition and recommendations for resolution. BMC Health Serv
Res. 2003;3(1):25.
43. White PD, Goldsmith K, Johnson AL, et al. Recovery from chronic fatigue syndrome after
treatments given in the PACE trial. Psychol Med. 2013;43(10):2227–35.
44. Nisenbaum R, Reyes M, Mawle AC, et al. Factor analysis of unexplained severe fatigue
and interrelated symptoms: overlap with criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome. Am J
Epidemiol. 1998;148(1):72–7.
45. Thompson RP. Theories and Models in Medicine. Philosophy of Medicine, Gifford F, Ed.,
North Holland, 2011, pp. 115–136.
46. Niiniluoto I. Evaluation of Theories. General Philosophy of Science: Focal Issues, Kuipers
TA, Ed., North Holland, 2007, pp. 175–218.
47. White PD, Sharpe MC, Chalder T, et al. Protocol for the PACE trial: A randomised
controlled trial of adaptive pacing, cognitive behaviour therapy, and graded exercise as
supplements to standardised specialist medical care versus standardised specialist
medical care alone for patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic
encephalomyelitis or encephalopathy. BMC Neurol. 2007;7:6.
48. Pemberton S, Cox DL. Experiences of daily activity in chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic
encephalomyelitis (CFS/ME) and their implications for rehabilitation programmes.
— 10 —
Disabil Rehabil. 2014;36(21):1790–7.
49. Kondo K. Post-Infectious Fatigue. Japan Med Assoc J. 2006;49(1):27–33.
50. Hickie I, Davenport T, Wakefield D, et al. Post-infective and chronic fatigue syndromes
precipitated by viral and non-viral pathogens: prospective cohort study. Br Med J.
2006;333(7568):575.
51. Vercoulen JHMM, Swanink CMA, Galama JMD, et al. The persistence of fatigue in
chronic fatigue syndrome and multiple sclerosis: Development of a model. J Psychosom
Res. 1998;45(6):507–17.
52. Bollen KA. Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York: Wiley, 1989, p. 70.
53. Song S, Jason LA. A population-based study of chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) experi-
enced in differing patient groups: An effort to replicate Vercoulen et al.’s model of CFS. J
Ment Health. 2005;14(3):277–89.
54. Snell CR, Stevens SR, Davenport TE, et al. Discriminative Validity of Metabolic and
Workload Measurements for Identifying People With Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Phys
Ther. 2013;93(11):1484–92.
55. Keller BA, Pryor JL, Giloteaux L. Inability of myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue
syndrome patients to reproduce VO2peak indicates functional impairment. J Transl Med.
2014;12:104.
56. Vermeulen RCW, Kurk RM, Visser FC, et al. Patients with chronic fatigue syndrome
performed worse than controls in a controlled repeated exercise study despite a normal
oxidative phosphorylation capacity. J Transl Med. 2014;8:93.
57. Jones DE, Hollingsworth KG, Jakovljevic DG, et al. Loss of capacity to recover from
acidosis on repeat exercise in chronic fatigue syndrome: a case–control study. Eur J Clin
Invest. 2012;42(2):186–94.
58. Nijs J, Nees A, Paul L, et al. Altered immune response to exercise in patients with chronic
fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis: A systematic literature review. Exerc
Immunol Rev. 2014;20:94–116.
59. Meyer JD, Light AR, Shukla SK, et al. Post-exertion malaise in chronic fatigue syndrome:
symptoms and gene expression. Fatigue. 2013;1:190–209.
60. Clark LV, White PD. Chronic fatigue syndrome. J Rehabil Med. 2008;40(10):882–3.
61. Kindlon T. Reporting of Harms Associated with Graded Exercise Therapy and Cognitive
Behavioural Therapy in Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Bull
IACFS ME 2011;19:59–111.
62. Bringsli GJ, Gilje A, Getz Wold BK. ME-foreningens brukerundersøkelse. ME-syke i
Norge – fortsatt bortgjemt? Oslo: Norges myalgisk encefalopati-forening, 2013.
63. ME Association. Managing my M.E. What people with ME/CFS and their carers want
from the UK’s health and social services. 2010. http://www.meassociation.org.uk/wp-
content/uploads/2010/09/2010-survey-report-lo-res10.pdf.
64. ME Association. ME/CFS Illness Management Survey Results. “No decisions about me
without me”. Part 1. 2015. http://www.meassociation.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2015-ME-
Association-Illness-Management-Report-No-decisions-about-me-without-me-30.05.15.pdf.
65. Virology blog. ME/CFS. http://www.virology.ws/mecfs/.
66. Davis RW, Edwards JCW, Jason LA, et al. An open letter to Dr. Richard Horton and The
Lancet. 2015. http://www.virology.ws/2015/11/13/an-open-letter-to-dr-richard-horton-and-
the-lancet/.
67. Davis RW, Edwards JCW, Jason LA, et al. An open letter to The Lancet, again. 2016.
http://www.virology.ws/2016/02/10/open-letter-lancet-again/.
68. Coyne J. Why the scientific community needs the PACE trial data to be released. 2015.
http://blogs.plos.org/mindthebrain/2015/11/11/why-the-scientific-community-needs-the-
pace-trial-data-to-be-released/.
69. Goldin R. PACE: The research that sparked a patient rebellion and challenged medicine.
2016. http://www.stats.org/pace-research-sparked-patient-rebellion-challenged-medicine/.