ArticlePDF Available

Different Strokes for Different Folks: Gender and Emotions in an Environmental Game

Canadian Center of Science and Education
Sustainable Agriculture Research
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Females are often expected to behave more environmentally-friendly than males, to be more sensitive to nuances in wording/framing, to be more emotionally expressive, and to be more likely to act on these emotions. Do they actually behave according to these expectations? Previous research found mixed evidence on gender effects. The purpose of our study is to examine whether there are gender differences in reaction to framing, expressing of emotions and acting in response to emotions in the environmental context and to determine whether these differences (if present) follow the “stereotypical” expectations. To investigate this, we conducted a framed laboratory experiment in the water quality context. Our findings show that there is a gender effect and it is highly context-dependent with respect to environmental decisions and with respect to the likelihood of expression of positive and negative emotions. Furthermore, we find that females sharing behavior is not sensitive to empathy vs. self-interest framing, while males sharing behavior is. Our results indicate that the payoff-relevant factors are, generally, more important than gender. We conclude that males and females are responding to different stimuli (“different strokes for different folks”), thus empirically testing behavior in a specific context is paramount when trying to predict responses by gender and designing environmental policies.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Sustainable Agriculture Research; Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
ISSN 1927-050X E-ISSN 1927-0518
Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education
81
Different Strokes for Different Folks: Gender and Emotions in an
Environmental Game
Marianna Khachaturyan1 & Natalia V. Czap2
1Independent researcher, Brazil, BEEP Lab Research Fellow, University of Michigan-Dearborn, MI, USA
2Associate Professor of Economics, Department of Social Sciences, Director of the Behavioral and Experimental
Economics and Policy Laboratory, University of Michigan-Dearborn, Dearborn, MI; CAFIO-PRG Research
Fellow, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
Correspondence: Natalia V. Czap, Department of Social Sciences (Economics), University of
Michigan-Dearborn, 4901 Evergreen Rd., Dearborn, MI 48103, USA. E-mail: nczap@umich.edu
Received: August 19, 2016 Accepted: September 12, 2016 Online Published: October 13, 2016
doi:10.5539/sar.v5n4p81 URL: http://dx.doi.org/sar.v5n4p81
Abstract
Females are often expected to behave more environmentally-friendly than males, to be more sensitive to nuances
in wording/framing, to be more emotionally expressive, and to be more likely to act on these emotions. Do they
actually behave according to these expectations? Previous research found mixed evidence on gender effects. The
purpose of our study is to examine whether there are gender differences in reaction to framing, expressing of
emotions and acting in response to emotions in the environmental context and to determine whether these
differences (if present) follow the “stereotypical” expectations. To investigate this, we conducted a framed
laboratory experiment in the water quality context. Our findings show that there is a gender effect and it is highly
context-dependent with respect to environmental decisions and with respect to the likelihood of expression of
positive and negative emotions. Furthermore, we find that females sharing behavior is not sensitive to empathy
vs. self-interest framing, while males sharing behavior is. Our results indicate that the payoff-relevant factors are,
generally, more important than gender. We conclude that males and females are responding to different stimuli
(“different strokes for different folks”), thus empirically testing behavior in a specific context is paramount when
trying to predict responses by gender and designing environmental policies.
Keywords: gender differences, environmental behavior, economic experiment, water quality, emotions, framing
1. Introduction
This study attempts to investigate whether there are gender differences in the reaction to framing, expressing of
emotions, and acting in response to emotions in the environmental context and to determine whether these
differences (if present) follow the “stereotypical” expectations. Understanding the differences, or lack thereof,
will allow decision makers to formulate more effective environmental policies.
Increased attention to gender differences in economic behavior has led to a number of studies investigating
gender effects in economic experiments involving other-regarding preferences. Evidence from these studies is,
however, mixed. For instance, in dictator game experiments, Eckel and Grossman (1998) and Kamas, Preston,
and Baum (2008) find that women donate more than men, while Bolton and Katon (1995) report no gender
differences. In a modified dictator game, Andreoni and Vesterlund (2001) find that men behave more
altruistically when it is cheap to do so. In their public good experiments, Brown-Kruse and Hummels (1993) and
Anderson, DiTraglia, and Gerlach (2011) find gender differences with men contributing more than women;
Seguino, Stevens, and Lutz (1996), on the other hand, report that women contributed more than men, while Frey
and Meier (2004) find no significant differences. In a solidarity game, Selten and Ockenfels (1998) report that
giving nothing is much more common for men. Brown and Taylor (2000) find gender differences in a
hypothetical valuation treatment but not in a real valuation treatment in their public good experiment. In
prisoner’s dilemma games, Ortmann and Tichy (1999) report that women cooperate more in the first round. In
subsequent rounds, however, the difference becomes smaller and non-significant. In their experiment of
individual contributions to group projects, H. Czap N. Czap, and Bonakdarian (2010) find that women contribute
significantly more than men. In terms of sharing behavior, females are often more empathetic than males if they
are in a position of a victim. However, in a position of a polluter both genders are equally empathetic (N. Czap,
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
82
H. Czap, Burbach, & Lynne, 2014).
Evidence from the environmental economics literature on gender differences in pro-environmental attitudes and
behavior is also mixed. Surveys on gender differences show that women have higher levels of environmental
concern (Mohai, 1992; Mohai, 1997; Xiao & Dunlap, 2007), they are more concerned about species preservation
(Czech, Devers, & Krausman, 2001) and have stronger ecocentrism (concern for nature and living organisms;
Zelezny, Chua, & Aldrich, 2000). As a result, they feel more responsible for improving the environment, and thus
are more likely to contribute to the environment (Israel, 2007). Based on interviews of people regarding their
attitudes and behavior towards wildlife, Kellert and Berry (1987) established that females value wild animals as
objects of affection and are concerned with the exploitation of wildlife, while males value animals for practical
and recreational reasons. Davidson and Freudenburg (1996) report that, although gender differences in general
environmental concern are modest, women exhibit much higher levels of concern in their attitudes towards
specific forms of environmental risk. Caiazza and Barrett (2003) claim that women are concerned more than men
about environmental risks at the local level. However, in a survey of rural residents, Luzar and Cosse (1998) find
no significant difference between men and women in their willingness to pay for rural water quality. N. Czap and
H. Czap (2010) report that women not only exhibit higher stated environmental concern than men, but also
higher levels of revealed environmental concern (i.e., demonstrate more environmentally-friendly behavior).
Furthermore, the differences in conservation behavior depend on the type of (dis)incentives provided for its
violation. Specifically, females are more sensitive to soft nudging via empathy than males are. However, neither
gender responds well to monetary fines (N. Czap et al., 2014).
The papers discussed above are studying gender differences in economic and environmental decisions under the
assumption that there are either psychological, or neurological, or socialization differences between females and
males. Other researchers point out that a lack of gender differences or inconsistencies in the results might be
explained by the differences in the type of experiments, the experimental design, the context, the framing, or
motivational factors (Eckel & Grossmann, 2008; Croson & Gneezy, 2009; Fujimoto & Park, 2010). These
inconsistencies suggest that further research is needed in identifying gender differences and their impact on
economic and environmental decisions.
The purpose of this paper is to determine whether there are gender differences and whether they are following
the expected pattern in: (1) costly pro-environmental choices, (2) sensitivity to framing, (3) willingness to
provide a costly emotional feedback (both positive and negative), (Note 1) and (4) changes in behavior in
response to such emotional feedback. The rest of the paper is structured in the following way. In the next section
we discuss the theoretical background and develop testable hypotheses. In the third section we explain the
experimental design. In the fourth section we present and discuss the results. In the last section we conclude our
analysis and offer the implications for environmental policy.
2. Theoretical Background and Study Hypotheses
As noted above, there are numerous studies examining gender differences in environmental behavior. The
majority of these studies claim that there is a significant difference between choices made by females and males.
Caiazza and Barrett (2003) report that women are more likely than men to give money to (or volunteer for)
environmental causes. According to Tindall, Davies, and Mauboulès (2003), women display more
environmentally-friendly behavior on a daily basis. Menges and Traub (2009) and N. Czap and H. Czap (2010)
find that females are contributing more to the environment than males. This leads to the first hypothesis of this
study:
Hypothesis 1: Females choose more environmentally-friendly actions than males.
According to psychological research, women are more sensitive to social cues in determining their behavior
compared to men (e.g., Gilligan, 1982). Women are also found to be more sensitive than men to the design and
context of the experiment (Leon-Mejia & Miller, 2007; Perugini, Tan, & Zizzo, 2010; Miller & Ubeda, 2012;
Rodriguez-Lara, 2014), to the decision-making context (Eckel & Grossman, 1996; Cox & Deck, 2006), and to
framing effects (Druckman & McDermott, 2008; Fujimoto & Park, 2010). However, according to Batson et al.
(1997), empathy manipulation was effective in both men and women. Croson and Gneezy (2009), in their review
of economic experiments on gender differences in preferences, argue that the small differences in experimental
design and implementation can have an influence on social cues, resulting in women being more other-regarding in
some experiments and less other-regarding in others. Based on these studies, the next hypothesis is:
Hypothesis 2: Females are more prone to framing effects in environmental context. Specifically, females are
more sensitive to empathy/self-interest framing than males.
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
83
The general public believes that women are more emotional (Graham & Ickes, 1997) and women confirm that
they experience negative and positive emotions to a higher degree than men do (Alexander & Wood, 2000).
Although this is disputed by Kring and Gordon (1998), who find that women do not report experiencing more
emotions than men. The difference in perception of the general public and what the women actually feel could be
explained by the difference in experiencing emotions and expressing them. Specifically, several researchers
(Kring & Gordon, 1998; Gross & John, 1995; Brody & Hall, 2010) found that females are more emotionally
expressive. Furthermore, women tend to rate themselves as more emotionally expressive (Simon & Nath, 2004)
in terms of both positive and negative expressivity (Gross and John, 1998). This leads to the next hypothesis of
this study:
Hypothesis 3: Females are more likely to send costly emotional feedback (positive or negative) than males.
If sending a negative emotional feedback can be considered as expressing sadness, then, according to some
literature on self-reported measures of emotions (e.g., Fischer, 1993), females express sadness more.
Alternatively, if negative emotion can be considered as expressing anger (or desire to punish) then males express
it more (e.g., Fischer, 1993). Additionally, rewards and punishment can be thought of as the behavioral means to
express positive and negative emotions (N. Czap, H. Czap, Khachaturyan, Burbach, & Lynne, 2013).
Furthermore, as has been mentioned above, there has been reported sensitivity to social cues in the behavior of
women (Croson & Gneezy, 2009; Miller & Ubeda, 2012). Gross and John (1998) find that “women show greater
reactivity to emotional stimuli” (p. 177). Yuan et al. (2009) argue that females are more susceptible to negative
emotions. Furthermore, N. Czap et al. (2014) find females to be more affected by potential social disapproval.
This leads to the last hypothesis of this study:
Hypothesis 4: Females react more to emotional feedback than males. Specifically, emotional feedback
encourages females to undertake environmentally-friendly actions more than it encourages males.
3. Experimental Design
3.1 Context of the Game and the Players
We tested the above hypotheses using data from a framed laboratory experiment in the context of downstream
water pollution. Downstream water pollution is a negative externality. It occurs when the upstream water users
are letting pollutants enter rivers, which subsequently negatively impacts the water quality of the downstream
rivers and lakes. As a result, the downstream water users must undertake costly water clean-up operations. The
cases of shared water resources are numerous in the world. Both men and women can be polluters (upstream
farmers) and victims (downstream water users). However, there may be a difference in how they behave in each
role. Our goal is to investigate these behavioral gender differences.
The downstream water pollution problem was modeled in the form of a three-player game, including Upstream
Farmer (UF), Downstream Water User (DWU), and Upstream Farmer/Downstream Water User (UF/DWU,
dual-role player). UF operates upstream and decides what tillage technology to implement on his/her land. UF
has two tillage options: conservation and intensive. Conservation tillage (CT) is not as invasive on the
environment as intensive tillage (IT) because CT results in less soil erosion and less chemical runoff into the
rivers. However, CT is more costly for the farmers to implement than IT. As a consequence of using CT, the
water downstream is less polluted and needs less expensive cleaning before it can be used. As such, CT is more
beneficial for DWU who gets their drinking water from downstream rivers and lakes. Finally, UF/DWU is a
player performing activities of both UF and DWU (playing a dual role): they farm upstream and decide on the
tillage technology, while, at the same time, draw drinking water from a downstream river or lake.
The experiment participants were presented with a simplified description of agricultural technologies. CT was
presented as a relatively lower profit practice, but with a relatively mild environmental impact. IT was presented
as a relatively higher profit practice, but with a relatively stronger negative environmental impact.
3.2 Treatments
The experiment consisted of three treatments: Empathy framing, Self-interest framing, and Neutral framing (or
No framing). In Empathy treatment, the players were referred to as UF, DWU, and UF/DWU. All participants
were presented with the description of the upstream-downstream situation. In addition, the farmers were told that
“The choice of tillage by farmers will greatly affect the water quality of the lake and the payoff for the
Downstream Water User. A cleaner lake and higher payoff for DWU will be assured if the farmers choose to
place more land under Conservation Tillage.” Then UFs and UF/DWUs were asked to make decisions regarding
how much of their land (500 acres) to place under CT. The CT chosen by UF and UF/DWU was used to
calculate the level of the lake cleanliness.
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
84
In the Self-interest treatment, the players were referred to as UF, DWU, and UF/DWU as well. Similarly to the
previous treatment, all participants were presented with the description of the upstream-downstream situation.
However, in this treatment, the farmers were told that “The choice of tillage by farmers will greatly affect their
own profit. The farmers get higher profit if they choose to place more land under Intensive Tillage.” Then UFs
and UF/DWUs were asked to make decisions regarding how much of their land (500 acres) to place under IT.
The IT chosen by UF and UF/DWU was used to calculate the level of the lake pollution.
In the Neutral treatment, the players were referred to as Player 1, Player 2, and Player 3. They were presented
with a situation written in a neutral context-free language (without mentioning the upstream-downstream
situation, CT, IT, cleanliness, or pollution of the lake). Players 1 and Player 2 had to allocate chips between
Options A and Option B (with associated returns in line with the respective types of tillage).
3.3 Payoffs of the Players
3.3.1 Empathy Treatment
Farmers decide how much of 500 acres to place under CT. UF’s payoff () in tokens is defined as:
  󰇛 󰇜(1)
DWU’s payoff () in tokens is defined as:
   󰇛󰇜 (2)
UF/DWU’s payoff ( ) in tokens is defined as:
    󰇛󰇜 (3)
Note that each acre not placed under CT gives UF/DWU a payoff of two tokens, while each acre placed under
CT gives UF/DWU a payoff of one token. Hence, similarly to UF, UF/DWU benefits more from not placing the
land under CT. However, their opportunity costs of doing conservation are lower. The cleanliness of the lake
depends on the proportion of land placed under CT by the two farmers (UF and UF/DWU). The cleanliness,
therefore, is defined as:
 
  (4)
Therefore, if, for instance, both farmers decide to place all of their land (500 acres) under CT, then the lake
cleanliness will be 100 percent.
The game represents a zero-sum game with three players sharing 3000 tokens, i.e., total social welfare is fixed.
The structure of the payoffs is such that if the farmers choose    acres, which leads to
 , each player gets an equal payoff of 1000 tokens.
The Nash equilibrium in Empathy framing for both decision making players (UF and UF/DWU) is to choose a
zero level of conservation tillage technology, since it gives them the highest payoff of 1500 and 1000,
respectively.
3.3.2 Self-interest Treatment
Farmers decide how much of 500 acres to place under IT. The payoffs in this treatment are mirroring the payoffs
in Empathy framing. UF’s payoff () in tokens is defined as:
  󰇛󰇜 (5)
UF’s payoff () in tokens is defined as:
   󰇛 󰇜 (6)
UF/DWU’s payoff ( ) in tokens is defined as:
   󰇛 󰇜 (7)
Note, that each acre placed under IT gives UF/DWU a payoff of two tokens, while each acre not placed under IT
gives UF/DWU a payoff of one token. The pollution of the lake depends on the proportion of land placed under
IT by the two farmers (UF and UF/DWU). The lake pollution, therefore, is defined as:
 
  (8)
If the farmers choose    acres leading to  , then each
player gets an equal payoff of 1000 tokens.
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
85
To be consistent and allow for comparison of decisions of farmers across both treatments, the tillage decisions
were converted into conservation tillage via    and lake pollution was converted into lake
cleanliness via:
   (9)
Since the payoff structure in Empathy and Self-interest framing treatments are the same, any differences that are
observed in the experiment are due to the treatment effect.
The Nash equilibrium in the Self-interest framing for both decision-making players (UF and UF/DWU) is to
place everything under intensive tillage technology, since it gives them the highest payoff of 1500 and 1000,
respectively. Choosing any other amounts of CT (or IT) corresponds to altruistic giving in a dictator game. The
economic theory predicts that a selfish dictator will keep the whole amount to themselves, without sharing with
another player. Experimental evidence, however, shows that dictators do share and give, on average, a positive
amount even to the anonymous respondents (e.g., Bolton & Katok, 1995; Eckel & Grossmann, 1998).
3.3.3 Neutral Treatment
In this scenario, the subjects were referred to as Player 1, Player 2, and Player 3. Player 1 and Player 2 have to
choose how many chips to allocated to Option A and Option B. The profits were calculated the same way as in
the framing treatments. The language used in this treatment was context-free.
3.4 Sending and Receiving Emotional Feedback
Participants were randomly assigned their roles in the game. Round 1 started with both UF and UF/DWU
choosing how much of their land acreage to place under CT (in Empathy treatment) or IT (in Self-interest
treatment) or Players 1 and Player 2 choosing how much of their chips to allocate to Option A (in Neutral
treatment). Next, the cleanliness or pollution level of the lake was shown to UF (Player 3). This was done to
reflect the real life situation, in which the downstream water users do not observe the choices made by upstream
farmers (CT or IT), but only the outcomes of those choices (cleanliness or pollution of water). DWU/Player 3
had an option to provide a costly (costs =50 tokens) emotional feedback to the farmers/Player 1 and Player 2.
Specifically, they could express their positive or negative emotions by sending a smiley/happy face or a
frowney/unhappy face to both farmers (Figure 1). Prior to this step none one of the three players were
informed that DWU/Player 3 will have an option to send an emotional feedback. This was done to prevent
strategic behavior by UF and UF/DWU in eliciting or evading such feedback.
Figure 1. Empathy framing an example of a decision screen seen by Downstream Water User
Note. This is Empathy framing. Thus, cleanliness of the lake is displayed. In Self-interest framing, the “%
pollution of the lake” is displayed, while in Neutral framing, “% of chips in Option B” is displayed.
If an emoticon was sent by DWU it appeared on the screens of both farmers accompanied by a message. In the
case when a smiley emoticon was sent, the message was After seeing the % pollution of the lake the
Downstream Water User sent to you and the other farmer:
”. In the case of a frowney emoticon, the message
was After seeing the % cleanliness of the lake the Downstream Water User sent to you and the other farmer:
”.
If DWU decided not to send anything, nothing appeared on the screen of farmers.
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
86
In Round 2, UF and UF/DWU chose how much of their land acreage to place under CT or IT (after receiving
emoticons, in cases when they were sent). Cleanliness or pollution level of the lake was again presented to DWU;
however, they were not able to provide any additional feedback. The payoffs of all players were shown to all of
them after the game was over.
3.5 Procedures
The experiment was conducted in the Experimental and Behavioral Economics Laboratory at the University of
Nebraska-Lincoln. The subjects were recruited on campus via flyers (posted and distributed across campus) and
emails, inviting them to participate in the decision-making experiment in the context of water usage. There was
no mentioning of gender either during the recruitment process or while running the experiment. Moreover, the
subjects did not know the gender of the other participants in the group. In total 216 students and members of the
public participated in the experiment: 84 subjects in Empathy, 84 in Self-interest, and 48 in Neutral framing.
Overall, 45.4% of the participants were female (see Table 1 for details). All sessions were computerized and
administered using the economics experimental software z-Tree (Fischbacher, 2007). Each session lasted 70-100
minutes.
Table 1. Percent of females by treatment and role
UF
UF/DWU
DWU
Average
Empathy
64.3%
42.9%
35.7%
47.6%
Self-Interest
32.1%
53.6%
32.1%
39.3%
Neutral
43.8%
43.8%
68.8%
52.1%
Average
47.2%
47.2%
41.7%
45.4%
Upon arrival to the laboratory, participants were greeted and seated in the waiting area. Before starting the
experiment, they read and signed the informed consent form. When the appropriate number of the participants
for the session arrived, they were ushered to the experimental computer laboratory and randomly seated at the
computers separated by privacy screens. To ensure anonymity and confidentiality, subjects were assigned a
5-digit random number, which they used to sign in on their individual computers.
Each subject received two handouts: (1) a payoffs table with some of the possible combinations of strategies and
(2) formulas to calculate the payoffs. (Note 2) To ensure that the instructions are public knowledge, they were
read to the subjects aloud and presented on the computer screens. After the instructions were read, the
experimenter answered questions and the participants were given a quiz. (Note 3) The quiz checked their
understanding of the instructions and ability to calculate the payoffs (or to use the payoff table). The game did
not start until all the participants successfully completed the quiz. The tokens that the participants earned during
the experiment were converted into dollars (at the rate of US $1=70 tokens). The participants were paid privately
in cash. The average earnings were US $28.9. (Note 4)
4. Results and Discussion
4.1 Gender and Pro-environmental Behavior
In Hypothesis 1 we conjectured that females behave in a more environmentally-friendly manner. We found
partial support for this hypothesis. In all but one case we observed economically significant, positive differences
between the average conservation level chosen by female and male farmers (Figure 2). However, the difference
was statistically significant at 1% in only one case: UF in Self-interest treatment (according to Wilcoxon rank
sum test, p-value=0.0052).
Furthermore, we explored the heterogeneity of the behavior of both genders when it comes to zero conservation,
equal sharing, and high levels of conservation (Figures 3a, 3b). Males were leading in the proportion of zero
conservation: 45% of male UFs and 29% of male UF/DWUs chose not to conserve versus 21% of female UFs
and 12% of female UF/DWUs. We classified CT of 250 acres as “equal sharing” because if both farmers choose
it then all three players (UF, UF/DWU, and DWU) get an equal payoff. In the case of UF, twice as many females
chose CT=250 than males, while in the case of UF/DWU the situation was reversed: males chose it more often.
Finally, “high levels of conservation” refers to CT>250. Choosing this level of conservation suggests that the
participants have either a very strong preference for pro-environmental and/or sharing behavior or a desire to
compensate for the potentially lower conservation level by another player. Females led in high conservation: 26%
of female UFs and 38% of female UF/DWUs implemented CT on more than 250 acres, while the numbers for
males were 13% and 21%, respectively.
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
87
Figure 2. Conservation tillage chosen by farmers in Round 1 by gender
These results offer an intriguing observation about the heterogeneity of female and male behavior in the
dual-role situation. By playing dual-role, UF/DWU can truly empathize and “walk-in-the-shoes-of-others” (N.
Czap, H. Czap, Khachaturyan, Lynne, & Burbach, 2012; Lynne, N. Czap, H. Czap, & Burbach, 2016), in this
case in the shoes of DWUs. However, males and females responded differently to this situation. One can
speculate that male UF/DWUs chose conservation levels of 250 acres as if expecting that their group partner will
do the same leading to a close-to-equal payoff for everyone. Such behavior can be summarized as “I do my part
and I expect you [UF] will too”. Female UF/DWUs, on the other hand, chose higher levels of conservation
(CT>>250) as if not expecting that their group partner will conserve a lot and willing to compensate for it. Such
behavior can be summarized as “I will pick up some of your [UF’s] slack”.
Figure 3a. Percentage of Upstream Farmers choosing a specific level of conservation tillage by gender
Figure 3b. Percentage of Upstream Farmers/Downstream Water Users choosing a specific level of conservation
tillage by gender
4.2 Gender and Framing Effect
In Hypothesis 2 we conjectured that females are more susceptible to framing effects, specifically, to
empathy/self-interest manipulation than males. We found no support for this hypothesis. Levels of CT chosen by
female UFs in Empathy and Self-interest treatments (250 and 200 acres, respectively) are not statistically
significantly different. The difference between female UFs in Empathy and Neutral treatment is statistically
significant at 10% (according to the Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon test). As it is evident from Figure 2, there is even
less difference in the female UF/DWUs behavior (these differences are also not statistically significant).
Male UFs, however, were more sensitive to framing effects: the difference between the behaviors of male UFs in
the Empathy and Self-interest treatment (215 vs. 50 acres, respectively) is statistically significant at 1%, and
statistically significant at 10% (according to the Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon test) between the Self-interest and
Neutral treatment. The behavior of male UF/DWUs, on the other hand, does not statistically differ across the
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
88
treatments.
To summarize, neither female upstream farmers nor females playing the dual role are very sensitive to the
difference in framing. Instead, they are choosing the conservation levels close to the one that Empathy framing
tries to achieve in almost all treatments. In that sense sharing and empathetic behavior seems to be a norm for them.
It is intriguing that some deviation from that norm happens only in the Neutral treatment (but not in the
Self-interest treatment). The results are different for males. Male UFs, but not male UF/DWUs, are quite
susceptible to the Self-interest, but not to the Empathy framing. Overall, the conservation levels that seem to be a
norm for males are much higher than the Nash prediction, but not as high as the norm for females. At the same time,
when they are nudged for self-interest, male UFs respond and demonstrate lower conservation efforts.
4.3 Gender and Provision of Emotional Feedback
In Hypothesis 3 we conjectured that females are more likely to send (costly) emotional feedback. We found that
the likelihood to send emotional feedback depends on the context. At the end of Round 1, after UFs and
UF/DWUs made a decision about conservation, lake cleanliness level was presented to DWUs and they were
invited to provide emotional feedback to the farmers: either a smiley emoticon or a frowney emoticon .
Choosing to send such feedback was costly for DWU (their payoff was reduced by 50 tokens). Overall, DWUs
expressed high willingness to provide costly feedback/express their emotions (see Figure 4 and the note below it
for the graph explanation). As it is evident from Figure 4, males were more likely to send emotional feedback in
both the Empathy and Self-interest treatments, while in the Neutral treatment females were more likely to do so.
It is intriguing that females shied away from offering emotional feedback in the framed treatments. There are two
possible explanations of such behavior: (1) the disbelief in the effectiveness of the feedback in the farmer-water
user situation and (2) the acceptance of the right of the farmers to behave the way they did. By extension, in the
Neutral treatment (free of the pre-conceived notions of how one should behave in such context) females felt the
need to intervene and correct the behavior, while males were more accepting of the right of Player 1 to behave
the way they pleased. Both genders were more eager to show their disapproval/negative emotions by sending a
frowney than to show their positive emotions with a smiley.
Figure 4. Proportion of Downstream Water Users sending emotional feedback by gender
Note. The height of each column represents the proportion of DWU players, sending emotional feedback, by
gender. For example, the first column reveals that 10% of females in Empathy treatment sent a smiley and 40%
of females sent a frowney, which implies that 50% of females (not shown) did not sent any feedback.
To further evaluate whether there are systematic gender differences in the proportions of emotional feedback
across all treatments, several multinomial logit regressions were estimated (Table 2).
Based on the regression analysis, the emotion expression does not depend on gender. The gender dummy (in
Models 1-3) and the interaction terms (in Models 4-5) are not statistically significant. Lake cleanliness is the
only variable that is highly significant across all models. Moreover, including cleanliness in Model 2 as
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
89
compared to Model 1 increases the index for goodness of fit, Nakelkerke R-sq., from 0.014 to 0.46. These results
indicate that expression of emotions is mainly triggered by the cleanliness of the lake and, as such, it depends on
the payoff-relevant aspects of actions of others, rather than on other factors including gender. This, in turn,
means that Hypothesis 3 is not supported by data.
Table 2. Gender in the context of sending emotional feedback (multinomial logit)
Variable
Model 1
Model 2
Model 4
Model 5
Intercept
-0.44
-2.90**
-3.19***
-3.33***
0.31
2.21***
2.33***
2.63***
Female
-0.66
-0.81
-0.15
0.36
Cleanliness
0.05**
0.05**
0.08**
-0.07***
-0.07***
-0.11***
Empathy frame
Self-interest frame
Cleanliness x Female
-0.01
-0.02
0.01
0.02
Cleanliness x Empathy frame
-0.02
0.04
Cleanliness x Self-interest frame
-0.03
0.04
Log-Likelihood
-74.0
-55.8
-56
53.1
Nagelkerke R-squared
0.014
0.46
0.46
0.51
Note: Dependent variable is the type of emotional feedback (smiley, frowney, no feedback). Significance levels:
*** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.10.
4.4 Gender and Response to Emotional Feedback
In Hypothesis 4 we conjectured that females react more to emotional feedback than males. In the context of this
experiment, we tested whether females chose higher levels of conservation tillage than males after they received
emotional feedback. We estimated two models for the change of conservation levels between Round 1 and
Round 2, depending on the type of feedback received and gender (Table 3).
Table 3. Gender and response on emotional feedback (tobit)
Model 6
Model 7
Dependent Variable change in CT
by UF
by UF/DWU
by UF
by UF/DWU
Intercept
68.36
-31.8
29.38
-34.62
(1=smiley dummy)
-39.13
-25.65
-11.52
-55.07
(1=frowney dummy)
28.96
111.24**
73.33*
120.82**
Female (1=female dummy)
-67.5**
-5.64
* Female
-42.86
53.97
* Female
-81.16*
-21.56
Log-Likelihood
-434.3
-446.7
-435.2
-446.3
Nagelkerke Rsq.
0.14
0.17
0.12
0.17
Note: Dependent variable is the difference between conservation tillage acreage from Round 2 as compared to
Round 1. Significance levels: ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.10.
The results of the regressions demonstrate that positive emotional feedback is not effective in increasing the
levels of conservation regardless of gender. Negative emotional feedback, however, turned out to be more
effective: receiving a frowney encouraged the farmers, on average, to increase CT levels next round. Gender
effect depended on the role that the participants played. Notably, and, in contrast to our expectations, female UFs
were less affected by negative emotional feedback than male UFs. Specifically, female UFs increased CT by
lower levels than the male UFs did. However, female and male UF/DWUs increased CT by the same level, on
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
90
average.
5. Conclusion
The discussion of gender differences and similarities is always a fashionable topic, both for the scientific
community and for the public (Sanz de Acedo Lizarraga et al., 2007). Typically people expect females to behave
more environmentally-friendly and be more emotionally expressive. The previous literature on these topics is
inconclusive: with some researchers reporting the presence of gender effects and some finding the absence of
such. In the current paper, we joined this investigation and added an exploration on whether there are gender
differences in pro-environmental behavior and how individuals act upon emotions. We tested the gender effects
using a framed laboratory experiment on water quality.
The first part of our exploration dealt with pro-environmental behavior and framing. In our experiment, we find
limited evidence that females, on average, behave more environmentally-friendly than males. Instead, the gender
effect is highly context-dependent. Specifically, females are more likely to choose high levels of conservation
than males. Males are more likely than females to choose an outcome that will lead to close-to-equal
distributions when their payoff depends on both their own choice and the actions of others (the role of UF/DWU).
Females are more likely than males to choose an outcome that leads to close-to-equal distribution when they can
unilaterally determine own payoff (the role of UF). Males are more sensitive than females to framing when
playing one role. Both females and males are sensitive to the dual role manipulation. From our results we can
also conclude that the payoff-relevant factors are, overall, more important than gender. However, the genders can
respond differently to these factors.
The second part of our exploration dealt with the expression of emotions and reacting on emotions. Females are,
typically, considered to be a more emotional gender. However, research does not support this stereotype. After
reviewing empirical studies on gender differences in emotions, Fischer (1993) concludes that “the general idea
that women are more emotional than men tells us more about Western sex stereotypes than about women’s actual
emotions” (p. 303). Fischer (1993) points out the differences between self-reported measures and direct
measures of emotions. In this paper, we used an incentive-compatible experiment to directly measure emotional
expression and acting on the expressed emotions of others. We found that the likelihood of expression of
emotions (both positive and negative) using emoticons and does not depend on gender, but rather on the
payoff-relevant factors. Furthermore, both genders were more likely to show their negative emotions than their
positive emotions. In terms of acting in response to emotions, we found that the gender differences depend on
the players’ role. When the role allowed the participants to determine their own payoff unilaterally, in contrast to
the expectations, females acted less on the emotional feedback than males. When the role implied that the payoff
depended on both their own choice and the actions of others, the gender differences were not observed. These
findings further underlined the complexity of the gender differences.
In the last decades experimental methods have gained recognition as a valuable tool for understanding human
behavior. Understanding human behavior and its motivation helps the society to design more effective public
policies. Many environmental problems are rooted in human behavior and, thus, can be managed by changing
the relevant behavior in order to reduce its negative environmental impacts (Steg & Vlek, 2009). This paper
contributes to the literature by experimentally investigating whether there are gender differences and whether
these differences follow the expected patterns in environmental context. Our findings suggest that different
strokes for different folks”: (1) the gender difference or the absence thereof depend on specific stimuli and the
decisional context and (2) if the difference is present, it does not always correspond to typical
expectations/stereotypes. Thus, we caution the policy makers and general public against predicting behavioral
responses by gender without empirically testing the specific context.
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the funding from the USDA-CSREES National Integrated Water quality program. The
two authors are the part of the team that developed and ran the experiment. The rest of the team included Dr.
Gary D. Lynne, Dr. Hans J. Czap, and Dr. Mark E. Burbach. We are very grateful to the rest of our team for
generously allowing us to peruse the data to write this paper. We also thank Jonathan Bertin for helping to
administer the experiment and Darin Dolberg for the technical support.
References
Alexander, M. G., & Wood, W. (2000). Women, men, and positive emotions: A social role interpretation. In A. H.
Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 189-210). Cambridge and New
York: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511628191.010
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
91
Anderson, L. R., DiTraglia, F. J., & Gerlach, J. R. (2011). Measuring altruism in a public goods experiment: A
comparison of US and Czech subjects. Experimental Economics, 14(3), 426-437.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9274-8
Andreoni, J., & Vesterlund, L. (2001). Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. The Quarterly
Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293-312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/003355301556419
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizingsystemizing (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy
of Sciences, 1156(1), 68-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x
Barrett, L., Feldman, L., Robin, P., Pietromonaco, R., & Eyssell, K. M. (1998). Are women the more emotional
sex? Evidence from emotional experiences in social context. Cognition & Emotion, 12(4), 555-578.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026999398379565
Batson, C. D., Sager, K., Garst, E., Kang, M., Rubchinsky, K., & Dawson, K. (1997). Is Empathy-induced helping
due to self-other merging? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 495-509.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.495 Bolton, G. E., & Katok, E. (1995). An experimental test for
gender differences in beneficent behavior. Economics Letters, 48(3), 287-292.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.495
Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. (2010). Gender, emotion, and socialization. In J. C. Chrisler, & D. R. McCreary (Eds.),
Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 429-454). New York, NY: Springer.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1465-1_21
Brown, K. M., & Taylor, L. O. (2000). Do as you say, say as you do: Evidence on gender differences in actual and
stated contributions to public goods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 43(1), 127-139.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(00)00113-x
Brown-Kruse, J., & Hummels, D. (1993). Gender effects in laboratory public goods contribution: Do individuals
put their money where their mouth is? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 22(3), 255-267.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(93)90001-6
Caiazza, A., & Barrett, A. (2003). Engaging women in environmental activism: Recommendations for Rachel’s
network. Institute for Women’s Policy Research Publication #I913.
Cox, J. C., & Deck, C. A. (2006). When are women more generous than men? Economic Inquiry, 44(4), 587-598.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ei/cbj042
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 1-27.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448
Czap, H. J., Czap, N. V., & Bonakdarian, E. (2010). Walk the talk? The effect of voting and excludability in
public goods experiments. Economics Research International. 1-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2010/768546
Czap, N. V., & Czap, H. J. (2010). An experimental investigation of revealed environmental concern. Ecological
Economics, 69(10), 2033-2041. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2010.06.002
Czap, N. V., Czap, H. J., Burbach, M. E., & Lynne, G. D. (2014). Gender in environmental context: An effect of
property rights, fines, and empathy nudging. International Journal of Economics and Finance, 6(4), 11-23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijef.v6n7p11
Czap, N. V., Czap, H. J., Khachaturyan, M., Burbach, M. E., & Lynne, G. D. (2013). Smiley or frowney: The
effect of emotions and empathy framing in a downstream water pollution game. International Journal of
Economics and Finance, 5(3), 9-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijef.v5n3p9
Czap, N. V., Czap, H. J., Khachaturyan, M., Lynne, G. D., & Burbach, M. (2012). Walking in the shoes of others:
Experimental testing of dual-interest and empathy in environmental choice, Journal of Socio-Economics,
41(5), 642-653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2012.05.005
Czech, B., Devers, P. K., & Krausman, P. R. (2001). The relationship of gender to species conservation attitudes.
Wildlife Society Bulletin, 29(1), 187-194.
Davidson, D. J., & Freudenburg, W. R. (1996). Gender and environmental risk concerns a review and analysis of
available research. Environment and Behavior, 28(3), 302-339.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916596283003
Druckman, J. N., & McDermott, R. (2008). Emotion and the framing of risky choice. Political Behavior, 30(3),
297-321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9056-y
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
92
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1996). The relative price of fairness: Gender differences in a punishment game.
Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 30(2), 143-158.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(96)00854-2
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1998). Are women less selfish than men: Evidence from dictator experiments.
Economic Journal, 108(448), 726-735. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-0297.00311
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (2008). Differences in the economic decisions of men and women: Experimental
evidence. In C. R. Plott, & V. L. Smith (Eds.) Handbook of experimental economics results, Volume 1 (pp.
510-519). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1574-0722(07)00057-1
Fischbacher, U. (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics,
10(2), 171-178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4
Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism and Psychology, 3(3),
303-318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959353593033002
Frey, B. S., & Meier, S. (2004). Pro-social behavior in a natural setting. Journal of Economic Behavior &
Organization, 54(1), 65-88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2003.10.001
Fujimoto, H., & Park, E-S. (2010). Framing effects and gender differences in voluntary public goods provision
experiments. Journal of Socio-Economics, 39(4), 455-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2010.03.002
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge and London:
Harvard University Press.
Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women's intuitions is not greater than men's. In W. J. Ickes (Ed.)
Empathic accuracy (pp. 117-143). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1995). Facets of emotional expressivity: Three self-report factors and their correlates.
Personality and Individual Differences, 19(4), 555-568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00055-b
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1998). Mapping the domain of expressivity: Multimethod evidence for a hierarchical
model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 170.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.170
Israel, D. K. (2007). Charitable donations: Evidence of demand for environmental protection? International
Advances in Economic Research, 13(2), 171-182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11294-007-9080-4
Kamas, L., Preston, A., & Baum, S. (2008). Altruism in individual and joint-giving decisions: What’s gender got
to do with it? Feminist Economics, 14(3), 23-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13545700801986571
Kellert, S. R., & Berry, J. K. (1987). Attitudes, knowledge, and behaviors toward wildlife as affected by gender.
Wildlife Society Bulletin, 15(3), 363-371.
Kring, A. M., & Gordon, A. H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: expression, experience, and physiology.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 686-703.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.686
Leon-Mejia, A., & Miller, L. M. (2007). The devil is in the details. Sex differences in simple bargaining games.
Jena Economic Research Papers #2007-069.
Luzar, E. J., & Cosse, K. J. (1998). Willingness to pay or intention to pay: The attitude-behavior relationship in
contingent valuation. Journal of Socio-Economics, 27(3), 427-444.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1053-5357(99)80097-3
Lynne, G. D., Czap, N. V., Czap, H. J., & Burbach, M. E. (2016). A theoretical foundation for empathy
conservation: Toward avoiding the tragedy of commons. Review of Behavioral Economics, in press.
Menges, R., & Traub, S. (2009). An experimental study on the gap between willingness to pay and willingness to
donate for green electricity. FinanzArchiv: Public Finance Analysis, 65(3), 335-357.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1628/001522109x477804
Miller, L., & Ubeda, P. (2012). Are women more sensitive to the decision-making context? Journal of Economic
Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 98-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.06.014
Mohai, P. (1992). Men, women, and the environment: An examination of the gender gap in environmental
concern and activism. Society and Natural Resources, 5(1), 1-19.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08941929209380772
http://sar.ccsenet.org Sustainable Agriculture Research Vol. 5, No. 4; 2016
93
Mohai, P. (1997). Gender differences in the perception of most important environmental problems. Race, Gender
and Class, 5(1), 153-169.
Ortmann, A., & Tichy, L. K. (1999). Gender differences in the laboratory: Evidence from prisoner’s dilemma
games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 39(3), 327-339.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-2681(99)00038-4
Perugini, M., Tan, J. H. W., & Zizzo, D. J. (2010). Which is the more predictable gender? Public good
contribution and personality. Economic Issues, 15(1), 83-110. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.676806
Rodriguez-Lara, I. (2014). An experimental study of gender differences in distributive justice. Cuadernos de
Economía, 38(106), 27-36. DOI: 10.1016/j.cesjef.2014.01.001.
Sanz de Acedo Lizarraga, M.L., Sanz de Acedo Baquedano, M.T., & Cardelle-Elawar, M. (2007). Factors that
affect decision making: Gender and age differences. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological
Therapy, 7(3), 381-391.
Seguino, S., Stevens, T., & Lutz, M. (1996). Gender and cooperative behavior: economic man rides alone.
Feminist Economics, 2(1), 1-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/738552683
Selten, R., & Ockenfels, A. (1998). An experimental solidarity game. Journal of Economic Behavior &
Organization, 34(4), 517-539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2681(97)00107-8
Simon, R. W., & Nath, L. E. (2004). Gender and emotion in the United States: Do men and women differ in
self-reports of feelings and expressive behavior? American Journal of Sociology, 109(5), 1137-1176.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/382111
Steg, L., & Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research
agenda. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(3), 309-317.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004
Tindall, D. B., Davies, S., & Mauboulès, C. (2003). Activism and conservation behavior in an environmental
movement: the contradictory effects of gender. Social and Natural Resources, 16(10), 909-932.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/716100620
Xiao, C., & Dunlap, R. E. (2007). Validating a comprehensive model of environmental concern cross-nationally:
A U.S.-Canadian comparison. Social Science Quarterly, 88(2), 471-492.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2007.00467.x
Yuan, J., Luo, Y., Yan, J. H., Meng, X., Yu, F., & Li, H. (2009). Neural correlates of the females' susceptibility to
negative emotions: An insight into gender-related prevalence of affective disturbances. Human Brain
Mapping, 30(11), 3676-3686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20796
Zelezny, L. C., Chua, P-P., & Aldrich, C. (2000). Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. Journal
of Social Issues, 56(3), 443-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00177
Notes
Note 1. Alternatively, this point can be thought of as willingness to costly express their emotions. It is believed
that women experience and express emotions more than men; the emotion that men are believed to experience
and express more is anger (Barrett, Feldman, Robin, Pietromonaco, & Eyssell, 1998). In this experiment we
incorporate expression of emotions as punishment or reward, which are used to express social disapproval or
approval, respectively, and increase fairness and cooperation in social dilemmas (N. Czap et al., 2013).
Note 2. The handouts are available upon request.
Note 3. The quiz is available upon request.
Note 4. The subjects reported their average hourly wages (thus, opportunity costs were) to be US $15. This
average wage was quite high and after examining the data one outlier (or data entry error) of hourly wage was
found to be US $1,320. After removing this outlier, the opportunity costs are down to US $8.95.
Copyrights
Copyright for this article is retained by the author(s), with first publication rights granted to the journal.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
... ○ Females, on average, are more likely to choose higher levels of conservation than males, especially when they can unilaterally determine their own and their partner's payoff (Khachaturyan and Czap, 2016). ...
... ○ Payoff-relevant factors have a stronger influence on the decisions and expression of emotions (both positive and negative) than gender (Khachaturyan and Czap, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Environmental problems coupled with shrinking budgets for environmental agencies call for alternative strategies to improve the effectiveness of current and future environmental policies. Empathy conservation promises such an alternative approach. In this paper we summarize the findings from previous research testing various propositions of metaeconomics and dual-interest theory based on which we develop a conceptual framework for empathy conservation. Furthermore, we offer recommendations for using empathy conservation in environmental policy and programs.
... There are several economic studies that show that after nudging to induce situational empathy, women make more prosocial decisions than men (Van Rijn, Quiñones, & Barham 2018; Czap et al. 2014;Khachaturyan & Czap, 2016). The paper most closely related to this one, Willer, Wimer, & Owens (2015), posits that empathy may help explain gender differences in giving to charitable organizations in a dictator game. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This paper examines how empathy is associated with gender differences in prosocial behavior in some economic games. Examining dictator, charitable giving, public goods, and trust games, we find that accounting for empathy eliminates the significance of gender differences in prosocial behavior in all four games. When the data are pooled from all games, the reduction in the female coefficients are significant after including either total empathy or empathic concern. The strong effect of empathy on prosocial behavior in the four games persists with controls for college major and demographics. This pattern replicates itself only with empathic concern, not the other empathy subscales. We conclude that measured gender differences in prosocial behavior in the games are not due wholly to sex per se, but at least partially to personality traits such as empathy that both sexes share so that the most empathic men act as prosocially as do women.concern on prosocial behavior in the four games persists with controls for social preferences, college major and demographics. JEL Codes: C91, D03, D63, D64, J1
... However, such concern may not necessarily result in behavioral differences: Luzar and Cosse (1998), for instance, did not find a significant gender difference in the willingness to pay for rural water quality. Khachaturyan and Czap (2016) show that in laboratory experiments gender differences in pro-environmental behavior depend on framing and decision context. Following that we will test: Hypothesis 1. ...
Article
Full-text available
Hate crimes against Muslims in the United States have been on the rise since 2016 (FBI, 2022), discouraging this group’s participation in public life. Most Americans, therefore, encounter Muslims only via media representations. We investigated if two journalistic storytelling devices can kindle in white non-Muslim Americans empathy and supportive attitudes toward Muslim women who are victims of discrimination. Indeed, personalization and emotionalization of news stories increased empathy for Muslim victims among participants with high Muslim prejudice. Gender differences moderated the effect of emotionalization, with women participants reporting more empathy and willingness to help victims than men.
Article
This paper examines how empathy is associated with gender differences in prosocial behavior in some economic games. Examining dictator, charitable giving, public goods, and trust games, we find that accounting for empathy eliminates the significance of gender differences in prosocial behavior in all four games. When the data are pooled from all games, the reduction in the female coefficients are significant after including either total empathy or empathic concern. The strong effect of empathy on prosocial behavior in the four games persists with controls for college major and demographics. This pattern replicates itself only with empathic concern, not the other empathy subscales. We conclude that measured gender differences in prosocial behavior in the games are not due wholly to sex per se, but at least partially to personality traits such as empathy that both sexes share, and that the most empathic men act as pro-socially as do women.
Article
Full-text available
Increased interest in emotional expressivity has led to a proliferation of conceptions and measures. It is unclear, however, whether they all refer to the same construct and whether the domain of emotional expressivity is best conceptualized as unidimensional or multifaceted. Study 1 examined 6 common expressivity questionnaires, yielding 5 factors: Expressive Confidence, Positive Expressivity, Negative Expressivity, Impulse Intensity, and Masking. To develop a nomological network for these factors, the factors were related to broader personality taxonomies and their differential relations to sex and ethnicity were tested. Study 2 provided further evidence of discriminant validity in relation to (a) typical emotion expression in peer relationships, (b) ability to pose emotions in the laboratory, (c) likability, and (d) regulation of emotion and mood. These findings support a hierarchical model of individual differences in emotional expressivity.
Article
Full-text available
Experimental economics research shows that gender can often explain some of the variation in individual behavior in experiments. This is especially true for contextualized games (corruption, environmental protection) in which participants’ behavior is guided by homegrown values and predispositions. We examine the gender differences in environmental behavior and the sharing of payoffs between a farmer and a water user under two alternative property rights assignments (farmer/polluter vs. water user/victim) and three methods of feedback (inducing empathy vs. imposing fine vs. no feedback). We found mixed evidence of gender differences concerning the choice of levels of pollution. Overall, albeit not always statistically significant, it seems that females are sharing with their group members more than males. Specifically, the results suggest that females are often more empathetic than males when they are in a position of a victim (water user). In a position of a polluter (farmer), in contrast, females and males are almost equally empathetic. Overall imposing monetary fines is counterproductive and decreases environmentally friendly behavior (however it does not significantly affect sharing), while empathy nudging increases sharing behavior (however it does not significantly affect environmentally friendly behavior). Empathy nudging is more effective for females than for males. Imposing fines, however, has no significant gender effect for either conservation or sharing behavior. Our findings provide another argument for increased gender equality based on environmentally sustainable economic development and thus propose a push by national governments as well as international organizations to increase the economic role of women.
Article
Full-text available
Common-pool resources and other shared resources frequently suffer from overextraction/overuse and associated negative externalities. In this paper we design a framed laboratory experiment on downstream water pollution to investigate (a) the importance of empathy vs. self-interest framing in determining the behavior of upstreamers regarding the negative externalities, and (b) the potential of downstreamers to influence the choices of upstreamers using non-monetary sanctions and rewards, alleviating the need for intervention by the local governments and regulatory institutions. Our results show that empathy framing has a much more significant impact on individual behavior than self-interest framing. Overall subjects behaved more profit-oriented in the self-interest framing and more egalitarian in the empathy framing. Lastly, negative emotional feedback is a powerful tool for changing behavior of subjects towards more environmentally friendly actions. Interestingly, positive emotional feedback is counterproductive for that. In general our results indicate that explicit emotional feedback, even though not expressed by everyone, works to the same degree as the implicit appeal to emotions through framing.
Article
Full-text available
It has been over 10 years since Kellert and Berry's (1987) pioneering study on the influence of gender on attitudes toward wildlife. Since data were gathered for that study, several sociopolitical movements have entered the American ecopolitical scene, including the Sagebrush Rebellion and the Wise Use Movement, providing impetus for a current assessment of attitudes toward wildlife. Consequently, we conducted a nationwide mail survey to assess gender influences on attitudes toward wildlife conservation issues. In our study, women ascribed greater preservation value to nonhuman species than men, selected ecological importance as the most important factor in prioritizing species for conservation (as did men), exhibited a greater concern for species conservation relative to property rights than men, and expressed stronger support for the Endangered Species Act (ESA) than men. The importance men and women placed on the ecological value of species conservation suggested that the American public supports continuing efforts to protect and conserve endangered species.
Article
When do people call someone emotional? Why is it generally accepted that women are emotional and men are not? What are the actual differences between men and women with regard to specific emotions? Under what circumstances are these differences most pronounced? How can we explain these alleged differences? In this book a distinguished international group of scholars seek to address these and other questions in an attempt to disentangle the complex and fascinating relationship between gender and emotion. Presenting a systematic overview of the most recent social psychological research in this field, the contributors combine empirical evidence and theoretical explanations to examine a wide range of emotions and emotional expressions and how they vary according to gender and context.
Article
Females valued wild animals as objects of affection and expressed considerable concern regarding the consumptive exploitation of wildlife. Males were far more knowledgeable and less fearful of wildlife and more inclined to value animals for practical and recreational reasons. Efforts to broaden the scope and effectiveness of wildlife management should consider and understand the influence of gender. -Authors
Article
Este artículo muestra que las mujeres eligen con mayor frecuencia la asignación justa que les resulta más beneficiosa a su rentabilidad financiera. La evidencia experimental proviene de un juego del dictador con la producción, en el que los sujetos primero resuelven un cuestionario para acumular ganancias, y luego dividen estas ganancias eligiendo una de las 5 posibles asignaciones propuestas, algunas de las cuales se fundamentan en ideales de justicia. Los datos muestran también que las mujeres son más sensibles al contexto, dado que sus elecciones dependen de si han acumulado más o menos dinero que sus oponentes. Esto no ocurre en el caso de los hombres.