ArticlePublisher preview available

Visuo-oculomotor skills related to the visual demands of sporting environments

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

The aim of this study was to assess the visuo-oculomotor skills of gaze orientation in selected sport activities relative to visual demands of the sporting environment. Both temporal and spatial demands of the sporting environment were investigated: The latency and accuracy of horizontal saccades and the gain of the horizontal smooth pursuit of the sporting environment were investigated in 16 fencers, 19 tennis players, 12 gymnasts, 9 swimmers and 18 sedentary participants. For the saccade test, two sequences were tested: In the fixed sequence, participants knew in advance the time interval between each target, as well as the direction and the amplitude of its reappearance; in the Freyss sequence however, the spatial changes of the target (direction and amplitude) were known in advance by participants but the time interval between each target was unknown. For the smooth-pursuit test, participants were instructed to smoothly track a target moving in a predictable sinusoidal, horizontal way without corrective ocular saccades, nor via anticipation or head movements. The results showed no significant differences between specificities of selected sporting activities via the saccade latency (although shorter than in non-athletes), contrary to saccade accuracy and the gain of smooth pursuit. Higher saccade accuracy was observed overall in fencers compared to non-athletes and all other sportsmen with the exception of tennis players. In the smooth pursuit task, only tennis players presented a significantly higher gain compared to non-athletes and gymnasts. These sport-specific characteristics of the visuo-oculomotor skills are discussed with regard to the different cognitive skills such as attentional allocation and cue utilization ability as well as with regard to the difference in motor preparation. More information: http://rdcu.be/kJuq
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1 3
Exp Brain Res (2017) 235:269–277
DOI 10.1007/s00221-016-4793-3
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Visuo‑oculomotor skills related to the visual demands of sporting
environments
Hadrien Ceyte1,2 · Alexis Lion2,3 · Sébastien Caudron1,2 · Philippe Perrin1,2,4 ·
Gérome C. Gauchard1,2
Received: 29 December 2015 / Accepted: 29 September 2016 / Published online: 4 October 2016
© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016
accuracy and the gain of smooth pursuit. Higher saccade
accuracy was observed overall in fencers compared to non-
athletes and all other sportsmen with the exception of ten-
nis players. In the smooth-pursuit task, only tennis players
presented a significantly higher gain compared to non-ath-
letes and gymnasts. These sport-specific characteristics of
the visuo-oculomotor skills are discussed with regard to the
different cognitive skills such as attentional allocation and
cue utilization ability as well as with regard to the differ-
ence in motor preparation.
Keywords Visuo-oculomotor skills · Sporting activity ·
Saccade · Smooth pursuit
Introduction
Gaze control is characterized by two processes: gaze sta-
bilization through ocular reflexes from vestibular, visual
or cervical origin and gaze orientation through ocular sac-
cades and smooth-pursuit mechanisms. During gaze ori-
entation, the visuo-oculomotor system uses a combination
of saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements in order to
center the image of the object of interest on the retina.
Although many sporting activities require efficient gaze
orientation, the effect of practice on the visuo-oculomotor
skills remains unclear. Several seminal works found no dif-
ference between athletes and non-athletes on measures of
oculomotor efficiency (Falkowitz and Mendel 1977), sac-
cadic response time (Shank and Haywood 1987) and ocu-
lomotor balance (Blundell 1984). A possible explanation
of this absence of distinction between athletes and non-ath-
letes on gaze orientation could reside in the type of visuo-
oculomotor task chosen and the difficulty level of the vis-
ual task used. Conversely, more recent studies have shown
Abstract The aim of this study was to assess the visuo-
oculomotor skills of gaze orientation in selected sport activ-
ities relative to visual demands of the sporting environment.
Both temporal and spatial demands of the sporting envi-
ronment were investigated: The latency and accuracy of
horizontal saccades and the gain of the horizontal smooth
pursuit of the sporting environment were investigated in
16 fencers, 19 tennis players, 12 gymnasts, 9 swimmers
and 18 sedentary participants. For the saccade test, two
sequences were tested: In the fixed sequence, participants
knew in advance the time interval between each target, as
well as the direction and the amplitude of its reappearance;
in the Freyss sequence however, the spatial changes of the
target (direction and amplitude) were known in advance by
participants but the time interval between each target was
unknown. For the smooth-pursuit test, participants were
instructed to smoothly track a target moving in a predict-
able sinusoidal, horizontal way without corrective ocular
saccades, nor via anticipation or head movements. The
results showed no significant differences between specifici-
ties of selected sporting activities via the saccade latency
(although shorter than in non-athletes), contrary to saccade
* Alexis Lion
alexis.lion@lih.lu
1 UFR-STAPS, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Université de
Lorraine, 54603 Villers-lès-Nancy, France
2 EA 3450 DevAH « Development, Adaptation
and Disability», Université de Lorraine,
54500 Vandœuvre-lès-Nancy, France
3 Sports Medicine Research Laboratory, Luxembourg Institute
of Health, 76 Rue d’Eich, 1460 Luxembourg, Luxembourg
4 Service d’ORL Pédiatrique, Centre Hospitalier Régional et
Universitaire de Nancy, 54500 Vandœuvre-lès-Nancy, France
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... Responsible for planning, organization, and motor representation, it is essential in integrating sensory and mnemonic information. 26,27 The pair of electrodes (Fp1 and Fp2) presented similar results for the main group effect. A decrease in the absolute theta power in the left electrode (Fp1) was observed for the group of athletes. ...
... 32,33 Inferior prefrontal gyrus Traditionally, the inferior prefrontal gyrus is associated with language and semantic functions, as well as with working memory 34 and long-term memory processes. 27 The data of the present study suggest that prefrontal gyration is not limited to aspects of speech and understanding of speech. A decrease in the theta power in the athletes was verified for the electrode located in the left hemisphere, characterized by analytical processes (F7). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The saccadic eye movement is responsible for providing focus to a visual object of interest to the retina. In sports like volleyball, identifying relevant targets quickly is essential to a masterful performance. The training improves cortical regions underlying saccadic action, enabling more automated processing in athletes. Objective We investigated changes in the latency during the saccadic eye movement and the absolute theta power on the frontal and prefrontal cortices during the execution of the saccadic eye movement task in volleyball athletes and non-athletes. We hypothesized that the saccade latency and theta power would be lower due to training and perceptual-cognitive enhancement in volleyball players. Methods We recruited 30 healthy volunteers: 15 volleyball athletes (11 men and 4 women; mean age: 15.08 ± 1.06 years) and 15 non-athletes (5 men and 10 women; mean age: 18.00 ± 1.46 years). All tasks were performed simultaneously with electroencephalography signal recording. Results The latency of the saccadic eye movement presented a significant difference between the groups; a shorter time was observed among the athletes, associated with the players' superiority in terms of attention level. During the experiment, the athletes observed a decrease in absolute theta power compared to non-athletes on the electrodes of each frontal and prefrontal area. Conclusion In the present study, we observed the behavior of reaction time and absolute theta power in athletes and non-athletes during a saccadic movement task. Our findings corroborate the premise of cognitive improvement, mainly due to the reduction of saccadic latency and lower beta power, validating the neural efficiency hypothesis.
... Saccadic eye movements, which are rapid movements that position the object image on the central retina, are used to obtain clear visual information from the environment. The properties of saccade in athletes have been examined in numerous studies [1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11]. For instance, athletes such as clay shooters [1], volleyball players [9,11], and some open-skill sports athletes [2] have shorter saccade latencies compared to nonathletes. ...
Article
Full-text available
In ball game sports, binocular visual function is important for accurately perceiving the distance of various objects in visual space. However, the temporal coordination of binocular eye movements during saccades has not been investigated extensively in athletes. The purpose of the present study was to compare the characteristics found in the interocular timing differences in horizontal saccades between ball game players. The participants included 32 university baseball players and 54 university soccer players. They were asked to shift their gaze to the onset of the light-emitting diodes located at 10 deg of visual field eccentricity to the left and right and alternated every 2 s. Horizontal movements of the left and right eyes were recorded separately with the electro-oculogram. Temporal variables for each eye were calculated with digital differentiation, and timing differences between the left and right eyes were compared between participant groups. The overall results showed significant interocular differences between left and right eye movements for the temporal variables of binocular saccades. The comparison between the participant groups revealed that baseball players had smaller interocular timing differences between the left and right eyes than soccer players in the onset time, time to peak velocity, duration, and peak velocity. These results suggest that baseball players have a higher degree of temporal coordination in binocular eye movements, particularly during the initial phase of horizontal saccades, compared to soccer players. This enhanced coordination might be attributable to the sport-specific visual demands of baseball, where players require precise stereoscopic vision to track a small high-speed ball within their visual space.
... SVT could be useful to enhance fencers' attentional and visual skills. Considering that athletes heavily rely on their visual and proprioceptive systems for precise movements and actions, assessing the integration between the visual and motor systems is crucial, highlighting the need to train and enhance visuomotor skills [10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: This study aimed to evaluate the effects of a six-week visual training protocol, based on the Science Vision Training Academy (SVTA) method, on reaction times and executive functions in high-ranking fencers. Methods: Twenty-seven fencers, aged 17.34 ± 3.63 years, were randomly assigned to an experimental Visual Training Group (VTG = 16) and a Control Group (CG = 11). The VTG, in addition to regular fencing training, underwent SVTA training two times per week using six different visual modules, while the CG followed only their traditional fencing training. Simple and complex reaction times and movement times were assessed before and after the intervention using the Fit-Light System. Results: Both groups showed a significant improvement in all four reaction time tests: simple reaction time with and without a weapon and complex reaction time ability (motor inhibition ability) with and without a weapon (p < 0.001). No significant differences were observed between the groups. A significant Time* Group interaction was found in the short reaction time and movement time (p < 0.001). This trend suggests that, although genetically determined and difficult to significantly improve through training, short reaction time can be stimulated through SVTA protocols. Conclusions: Training in realistic conditions is always preferable to non-ecological protocols; however, the SVTA method may be beneficial to enhance simple reaction time in elite fencers.
... A previous study investigated the speed of information processing in athletes compared with a control group. 37 The results indicated the amount of practice allowed for the development of preparatory activities, which may suggest that the β dynamics indicate that the effects of physical exercise are correlated with cognitive improvement, meaning that tasks set by internal factors are associated with a cognitive effort dependent on executive functions, reflected by β activity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background High levels of physical conditioning are associated with improvements in cognitive performance. In this sense, electroencephalographic (ECG) correlates are used to investigate the enhancing role of physical exercise on executive functions. Oscillations in the β frequency range are proposed to be evident during sensorimotor activity. Objective To investigate the ECG changes influenced by aerobic and resistance exercises performed in an attention task by analyzing the differences in absolute β power in the prefrontal and frontal regions before, during, and after the oddball paradigm in practitioners and nonpractitioners of physical exercise. Methods There were 15 physical activity practitioners (aged 27 ± 4.71) and 15 nonpractitioners (age 28 ± 1.50) recruited. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was implemented to observe the main effect and the interaction between groups and moments (rest 1, pre-stimulus, and rest 2). Results An interaction between group and moment factors was observed for Fp1 ( p < 0.001); Fp2 ( p = 0.001); F7 ( p < 0.001); F8 ( p < 0.001); F3 ( p < 0.001); Fz ( p < 0.001); and F4 ( p < 0.001). Electrophysiological findings clarified exercisers' specificity and neural efficiency in each prefrontal and frontal subarea. Conclusion Our findings lend support to the current understanding of the cognitive processes underlying physical exercise and provide new evidence on the relationship between exercise and cortical activity.
... The visual resolution of the retina is highest at the central fovea and decreases exponentially with increasing distance from the fovea [3,4]. Saccadic eye movement (saccade) that quickly and precisely matches the fovea to the visual object accompanying an abrupt shift is important for perception of the object in various sports [5][6][7][8][9]. Saccades to the appearing visual object are called visually guided saccades or pro-saccades. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous studies indicated that substantial individual variation exists in the distribution of pro-saccade reaction times under gap condition. To investigate the influence of sports experience on the distribution, we examined distribution of the pro-saccade reaction time under overlap and gap conditions, for the basketball club, table tennis club, and non-sporting control groups. Methods Subjects performed pro-saccade tasks under the overlap and gap conditions, in which the intentional and reflexive disengagement of fixation are important, respectively. Under the overlap condition, the central fixation point was illuminated for a random duration of 1–3 s, then the fixation point was turned off. Just after the switch-off of the fixation point, one of the peripheral targets was illuminated for a duration of 1 s. The visual stimulus under the gap condition was almost the same as that under the overlap condition. However, only the temporal gap between the switch-off of the fixation point and the onset of the target differed between those conditions. The gap duration in the gap condition was set at 200 ms. The mean of median value of the bandwidth showing the earliest peak in the histogram was calculated for each group. Thereafter, for each subject, the bandwidth showing the earliest peak under the gap condition was defined as the criterion bandwidth (0 ms bandwidth). Based on this criterion bandwidth, the mean of the relative frequency was calculated for every 10 ms of bandwidth, for the overlap and gap conditions, in each group. Results Under the overlap condition, for all subjects, the pro-saccade reaction times showed unimodal distribution. The means of the median value of the bandwidth showing the earliest peak for the basketball and table tennis groups (approximate 170 ms) were significantly earlier than that for the control group (approximate 190 ms). Under the gap condition, the distribution was bimodal for 11 of 15 subjects in the basketball group and for 5 of 15 subjects in the control group. In the table tennis group, the distribution was not bimodal but unimodal for all 15 subjects. For the basketball group, mean of the relative frequency showed bimodal distribution with approximate 120 ms and 170 ms peaks. For the table tennis and control groups, the mean of the relative frequency showed unimodal distribution with approximate 130 ms and 140ms peak, respectively. Conclusions The present study indicated that under the gap condition, the sports experience influenced on the distribution of the pro-saccade reaction time. The pro-saccade reaction time under the condition would show a distinct bimodal distribution for the basketball group and show a distinct and early unimodal distribution for the table tennis group. It was suggested that the physiological factor leading the group difference in the distribution was the effect of sports experience on the disengagement function of fixation.
... 14 Dual-task assessments pair dynamic postural control with a cognitive task to further challenge the individual. 14 With the various clinician-based outcomes (e.g., balance, cognitive testing) currently in use, implementing validated patient reported outcome measures (PROM) like the Disablement in Physically Active Scale (DPAS) and Post-Concussion Symptom Scale (PCSS) could be beneficial in determining if the athlete themselves identifies any changes. Though specific questionnaires for concussion and subconcussion have not yet been developed, the DPAS and PCSS can identify functional limitations, impairments, quality of life changes in physically active individuals. ...
Article
Introduction: The understanding of subconcussive impacts is limited, with few assessments to determine effects in a recreationally active population due to existing tests being static and unidimensional. M ethods: This study investigated the effects of 10 soccer headers on pre and post-test measurements of patient reported outcome measures and single and dual-task tandem gait of 12 recreationally active college-aged participants. Results: No changes due to the heading session were observed; however, there was a detectable learning effect with participants walking faster and committing more gait errors but less cognitive errors. Conclusion: A bout of soccer heading may not pose an immediate risk to dynamic postural control and cognitive function
Article
Background Oculomotor and reaction time tests are frequently used assessments of vestibular symptoms, traumatic brain injury (TBI), or other neurological disorders in both clinical and research contexts. When interpreting these tests it is important to have a reference interval (RI) as a comparison for what constitutes a typical/expected response; however, the current body of research has only limited information regarding normative ranges calculated according to established standards or for a military-specific sample. Purpose The purpose of the present study was to describe RIs for oculomotor and reaction time tests in a cohort of service members and veterans (SMVs) for use as comparators by clinicians and scientists. Research Design Descriptive. Study Sample Participants were prospectively enrolled in the Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center-Traumatic Brain Injury Center of Excellence 15-year Longitudinal Traumatic Brain Injury Study. Only SMVs without a history of TBI or blast exposure were included in the RI calculations. Data Collection and Analysis The test paradigms included in this analysis were: smooth pursuit, prosaccades, antisaccades, saccades and reaction time, predictive saccades, optokinetic nystagmus, auditory reaction time, and visual reaction time. Nonparametric methods, based on the U.S. Food and Drug Administration's recognized consensus standards, were used to calculate 95% RIs. A comparison between the calculated RIs and those available from previously published research is provided. Results Summary statistics and RIs were calculated for 47 outcome parameters from 13 oculomotor and reaction time tests. Sample sizes and age ranges varied across outcome parameters depending on the availability of reference values for RI calculations. The sample sizes used to calculate RIs ranged from 51 to 69. The age of SMVs included in each RI ranged from 19 to 61 years with mean ages ranging from 37 to 39 years. Similarities/differences between the RIs in the present study and those in previously published research are highly dependent on the outcome parameter; however, in general, the RIs in the present study tended to be somewhat wider. Conclusions The RIs provided in this paper can serve as comparisons for clinicians and scientists who are utilizing these oculomotor and reaction time testing paradigms in similar cohorts of patients or research participants.
Article
In a variety of sports, visual functions in depth are important. Kinetic visual acuity and depth perception, which are existing methods for evaluating visual functions, have been used to reveal the property of visual functions in depth in athletes. However, since the existing methods do not directly reflect eye movements in depth, the property of vergence eye movements, which respond to visual stimuli in depth and are disconjunctive eye movements, in athletes is still uncertain. The existing methods and the ability of vergence eye movements could be independent of each other. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to reveal the differences between visual functions in baseball players and non-athletes using kinetic visual acuity, depth perception and vergence eye movements. The results showed that there was no difference in kinetic visual acuity and depth perception between baseball players and non-athletes. However, our results also showed that the latency and initial acceleration of vergence eye movements in baseball players were superior to those in non-athletes. Therefore, it is suggested that performance supported by the superior visual function in baseball players is associated with the properties of vergence eye movements.
Article
Significance Automated eye tracking could be used to evaluate saccade performance of patients with concussion history, providing quantitative insights about the degree of oculomotor impairment and potential vision rehabilitation strategies for this patient population. Purpose To evaluate the saccade performance of patients with concussion history based on automated eye-tracking test results. Methods We conducted a retrospective study of patients with concussion history, primarily from sports participation, who underwent oculomotor testing based on an eye-tracking technology at the Duke Eye Center vision rehabilitation clinic between June 30, 2017 and January 10, 2022. Patients’ saccade test results were reviewed, including saccade fixation and saccade speed/accuracy ratio. The outcomes were compared with age-matched normative population data derived from healthy individuals. Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to identify factors associated with saccade performance among patients with concussion history. Results: 115 patients with concussion history were included in the study. Patients with concussion on average had fewer fixations on self-paced horizontal and vertical saccade tests and lower horizontal and vertical saccade speed/accuracy ratios compared with normative ranges. Among patients with concussion history, multiple linear regression analyses showed that older age was associated with fewer fixations on horizontal and vertical saccade tests, while male sex was associated with more fixations on horizontal and vertical saccade tests (all P < .01). Additionally, older age was associated with lower horizontal saccade speed/accuracy ratio, after adjusting for sex, number of concussion(s), and time from most recent concussion to oculomotor testing (P < .001). Conclusions Patients with concussion history had lower saccade performance based on eye tracking compared with healthy individuals. We additionally identified risk factors for lower saccade performance among patients with concussion history. These findings support the use of saccade test results as biomarkers for concussion and have implications for post-concussion rehabilitation strategies.
Article
Full-text available
A major element in expert sports performance, particularly racket-and-ball games, is excellent anticipatory skill. A prestudy combined the temporal and spatial occlusion paradigms to ascertain which key stimuli badminton players use for anticipating the direction of overhead shots. The main study then evaluated a program for training anticipatory skills; 200 video clips were employed to orient attention toward these key stimuli. Participants were 63 badminton novices, 20 national league players, and 21 local league players. A transparent red patch (exogenous orienting) was used to orient attention toward the trunk up to 160 ms before racket-shuttle contact; the arm, from 160 ms to 80 ms before contact; and the racket, from 80 ms before to actual contact. Results showed that badminton novices who trained with this program significantly improved their anticipatory skill between post- and retention test compared with controls. Whereas local league players improved from pre- to posttest, training had no effect on expert national league players. It is concluded that using red transparent patches to highlight the most informative cues in perceptual training programs is a promising way to improve anticipatory skill.
Article
Full-text available
Hitters in fast ball-sports do not align their gaze with the ball throughout ball-flight; rather, they use predictive eye movement strategies that contribute towards their level of interceptive skill. Existing studies claim that (i) baseball and cricket batters cannot track the ball because it moves too quickly to be tracked by the eyes, and that consequently (ii) batters do not - and possibly cannot - watch the ball at the moment they hit it. However, to date no studies have examined the gaze of truly elite batters. We examined the eye and head movements of two of the world's best cricket batters and found both claims do not apply to these batters. Remarkably, the batters coupled the rotation of their head to the movement of the ball, ensuring the ball remained in a consistent direction relative to their head. To this end, the ball could be followed if the batters simply moved their head and kept their eyes still. Instead of doing so, we show the elite batters used distinctive eye movement strategies, usually relying on two predictive saccades to anticipate (i) the location of ball-bounce, and (ii) the location of bat-ball contact, ensuring they could direct their gaze towards the ball as they hit it. These specific head and eye movement strategies play important functional roles in contributing towards interceptive expertise.
Article
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
Information-processing theory predicts that sport performers gain information from their environment through use of a systematic pattern of eye movements/fixations. Of interest here was the visual-search pattern used in viewing an oncoming baseball pitch and whether this pattern differed for expert and novice batters. Ocular latencies were also examined during the task. Subjects were 9 varsity collegiate baseball players (expert) and 9 novice players. Each subject was shown 20 pitches on videotape, with the type of pitch (curve, fastball) and type of motion (wind-up, stretch) varied During the viewing eye movements were recorded by a photoelectric technique. Expert players fixated during wind-up on the anticipated release point and then, after a latency of about 150 msec. following release, moved their eyes to the oncoming ball. Novice batters tended to move their eyes before release or to fixate somewhere other than the release point, such as the pitcher's head. Performance differences in hitting may be partially attributable to these differences in visual search pattern. This latter finding supports the information-processing viewpoint, yet the existence of an eye-movement latency supports a direct perception perspective.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated long-term adaptations of smooth pursuit eye movement characteristics in high-level gymnasts and compared these responses to those of nonathletes. Gymnasts were selected because of their exceptional ability to spatially orient during fast, multiaxial whole body rotations. Participants were tested with standardized and supra-maximal sinusoidal smooth pursuit measurements. The results showed significantly higher gain values in top-level gymnasts, followed by young federal team gymnasts, followed by the nonathlete control group. By testing participants over the course of three years and also after periods of abstinence from training, changes to patterns of smooth pursuit over time are revealed. These results have interesting implications for understanding the characteristics of eye-movements in expert populations as well as understanding the general principles that underlie oculomotor adaptation.
Article
Analyzes the relationship between the sensorimotor and the semantic visual function in sport and considers how understanding and acting cooperate. Athletes' visual behavior (recorded by a video-oculographic technique) and motor behavior (recorded by video or rapid cinematography) in 2 characteristic open activities are studied. Table tennis is an activity in which the opponent both manipulates the uncertainty and imposes the time pressure externally. Rock climbing is an activity in which the uncertainty is conveyed by the physical environment and the time is self-paced. Different levels of processing are identified, then a general model describing the understanding–acting process in sport is presented. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Study design: Cross-sectional study. Objective: To determine whether adolescent idiopathic scoliosis (AIS) at onset is associated with oculomotor dysfunction and whether these oculomotor anomalies are correlated to the amplitude of the spine deformation. Summary of background data: AIS is related to abnormalities of postural control. To date, few studies have focused on visuo-oculomotor and vestibulo-ocular functions at early-stage AIS. Methods: Fifty-three adolescent girls were diagnosed with AIS (mean age: 11.6 ± 2.1 yr) on clinical and radiological criteria (mean Cobb angle: 14.8° ± 5.0°). Visuo-oculomotor and vestibulo-ocular functions were studied with video-oculography, including saccades, smooth pursuit, caloric test, and pendular rotation, with visual vestibular ocular reflex and vestibulo-ocular reflex sequences. Two patient groups were defined according to the mean Cobb angle: group 1 included 29 patients with a Cobb angle from 5° to 14° and group 2 included 24 patients with a Cobb angle from 15° to 25°. Results: The group 2 showed different saccade characteristics than group 1: higher latencies for saccade sequences characterized by temporal uncertainty and predictive direction; lower velocity regardless of the type of the saccades. No difference was observed for saccadic accuracy and smooth-pursuit gain. For the visual vestibular ocular reflex, group 2 showed lower total maximal slow-phase velocity than group 1, whereas the vestibulo-ocular reflex (tested in dark) did not differ between groups. No difference was observed concerning the caloric vestibular test. Conclusion: Patients with a Cobb angle of 15° or more presented normal vestibulo-ocular responses but altered visuo-oculomotor functions, especially for the saccadic latency and velocity. This could be the result of a dysfunction of oculomotor pathways at cerebellar and/or brainstem level. These central disorders may be incriminated in the development of AIS.
Article
The purpose of this cross-sectional study was to determine whether measures on a selected groups of visual and psychomotor variables distinguished between groups of elite, intermediate and novice table tennis players. The variables included commonly of Melbourne measured clinical functions such as static visual acuity, extent of visual field and oculomotor balance and more experimental visual tasks such as recognition of, peripheral targets, saccadic latency and dynamic visual acuity. Psychomotor performance was assessed by measures of simple reaction time, choice response time and hand movement time. Although elite level competitors had significantly better dynamic visual acuity, a wider visual field and superior recognition of peripheral targets compared to less skilled competitors, the magnitude, or practical significance of these differences was not great and individually accounted for less than 5 per cent of population variance. Elite competitors had significantly faster psychomotor responses than novice players with die skill variables individually accounting for between 21 per cent and 62 per cent of population variance in psychomotor performance. These results indicate that the psychomotor parameters could be a useful part of a test battery for talent identification amongst table tennis players.
Article
In this study, we examined differences in dynamic visual acuity between elite and subelite water polo players and sedentary students. To measure dynamic visual acuity binocularly, we asked participants to indicate the orientation of a broken ring, similar to the Landolt C, which increased in size as it moved across a computer screen. Two different speeds, three possible trajectories, and two different levels of contrast were evaluated. There were statistically significant differences between elite players and sedentary students for each combination of speed, contrast, and trajectory. Elite players achieved better dynamic visual acuity scores, and results also improved for some combinations of speed, contrast, and trajectory. Comparison between elite and subelite groups failed to reveal any