Gender bias in open source: Pull request acceptance of women versus men

Article (PDF Available) · January 2016with 766 Reads 
How we measure 'reads'
A 'read' is counted each time someone views a publication summary (such as the title, abstract, and list of authors), clicks on a figure, or views or downloads the full-text. Learn more
DOI: 10.7287/PEERJ.PREPRINTS.1733V1
Cite this publication
Abstract
Biases against women in the workplace have been documented in a variety of studies. This paper presents the largest study to date on gender bias, where we compare acceptance rates of contributions from men versus women in an open source software community. Surprisingly, our results show that women's contributions tend to be accepted more often than men's. However, when a woman's gender is identifiable, they are rejected more often. Our results suggest that although women on GitHub may be more competent overall, bias against them exists nonetheless.
Advertisement
Gender Bias in Open Source: Pull Request
Acceptance of Women Versus Men
Josh Terrell1, Andrew Kofink2, Justin Middleton2,
Clarissa Rainear2, Emerson Murphy-Hill2, Chris Parnin2
1Department of Computer Science, Cal Poly, San Luis Obispo, USA
2Department of Computer Science, North Carolina State University, USA
To whom correspondence should be addressed; E-mail: emerson@csc.ncsu.edu
Biases against women in the workplace have been documented in a variety of
studies. This paper presents the largest study to date on gender bias, where
we compare acceptance rates of contributions from men versus women in an
open source software community. Surprisingly, our results show that women’s
contributions tend to be accepted more often than men’s. However, when a
woman’s gender is identifiable, they are rejected more often. Our results sug-
gest that although women on GitHub may be more competent overall, bias
against them exists nonetheless.
Introduction
In 2012, a software developer named Rachel Nabors wrote about her experiences trying to fix
bugs in open source software.1Nabors was surprised that all of her contributions were rejected
by the project owners. A reader suggested that she was being discriminated against because of
1http://rachelnabors.com/2012/04/of-github-and-pull-requests-and-comics/
1
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
her gender.
Research suggests that, indeed, gender bias pervades open source. The most obvious illus-
tration is the underrepresentation of women in open source; in a 2013 survey of the more than
2000 open source developers who indicated a gender, only 11.2% were women (1). In Vasilescu
and colleagues’ study of Stack Overflow, a question and answer community for programmers,
they found “a relatively ’unhealthy’ community where women disengage sooner, although their
activity levels are comparable to men’s” (2). These studies are especially troubling in light of
recent research which suggests that diverse software development teams are more productive
than homogeneous teams (3).
This article presents an investigation of gender bias in open source by studying how software
developers respond to pull requests, proposed changes to a software project’s code, documen-
tation, or other resources. A successfully accepted, or ‘merged,’ example is shown in Figure 1.
We investigate whether pull requests are accepted at different rates for self-identified women
compared to self-identified men. For brevity, we will call these developers ‘women’ and ‘men,’
respectively. Our methodology is to analyze historical GitHub data to evaluate whether pull
requests from women are accepted less often. While other open source communities exist, we
chose to study GitHub because it is the largest (4), claiming to have over 12 million collabora-
tors across 31 million software repositories.2
The main contribution of this paper is an examination of gender bias in the open source
software community, enabled by a novel gender linking technique that associates more than
1.4 million community members to self-reported genders. To our knowledge, this is the largest
scale study of gender bias to date.
2https://github.com/about/press
2
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Figure 1: GitHub user ‘JustinAMiddleton’ makes a pull request; the repository owner ‘akofink’
accepts it by merging it. The changes proposed by JustinAMiddleton are now incorporated into
the project.
Related Work
A substantial part of activity on GitHub is done in a professional context, so studies of gender
bias in the workplace are relevant. Because we cannot summarize all such studies here, we
instead turn to Davison and Burke’s meta-analysis of 53 papers, each studying between 43 and
523 participants, finding that male and female job applicants generally received lower ratings
for opposite-sex-type jobs (e.g., nurse is a female sex-typed job, whereas carpenter is male
sex-typed) (5).
The research described in Davison and Burke’s meta-analysis can be divided into experi-
3
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
ments and field studies. Experiments attempt to isolate the effect of gender bias by controlling
for extrinsic factors, such as level of education. For example, Knobloch-Westerwick and col-
leagues asked 243 scholars to read and evaluate research paper abstracts, then systematically
varied the gender of each author; overall, scholars rated papers with male authors as having
higher scientific quality (6). In contrast to experiments, field studies examine existing data to
infer where gender bias may have occurred retrospectively. For example, Roth and colleagues’
meta-analysis of such studies, encompassing 45,733 participants, found that while women tend
to receive better job performance ratings than men, women also tend to be passed up for pro-
motion (7).
Experiments and retrospective field studies each have advantages. The advantage of ex-
periments is that they can more confidently infer cause and effect by isolating gender as the
predictor variable. The advantage of retrospective field studies is that they tend to have higher
ecological validity because they are conducted in real-world situations. In this paper, we use a
retrospective field study as a first step to quantify the effect of gender bias in open source.
Several other studies have investigated gender in the context of software development. Bur-
nett and colleagues analyzed gender differences in 5 studies that surveyed or interviewed a
total of 2991 programmers; they found substantial differences in software feature usage, tinker-
ing with and exploring features, and in self-efficacy (8). Arun and Arun surveyed 110 Indian
software developers about their attitudes to understand gender roles and relations but did not
investigate bias (9). Drawing on survey data, Graham and Smith demonstrated that women in
computer and math occupations generally earn only about 88% of what men earn (10). Lage-
sen contrasts the cases of Western versus Malaysian enrollment in computer science classes,
finding that differing rates of participation across genders results from opposing perspectives
of whether computing is a “masculine” profession (11). The present paper builds on this prior
work by looking at a larger population of developers in the context of open source communities.
4
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Some research has focused on differences in gender contribution in other kinds of virtual
collaborative environments, particularly Wikipedia. Antin and colleagues followed the activity
of 437 contributors with self-identified genders on Wikipedia and found that, of the most active
users, men made more frequent contributions while women made larger contributions (12).
There are two gender studies about open source software development specifically. The first
study is Nafus’ anthropoligical mixed-methods study of open source contributors, which found
that “men monopolize code authorship and simultaneously de-legitimize the kinds of social
ties necessary to build mechanisms for women’s inclusion” (13). The other is Vasilescu and
colleagues’ study of 4,500 GitHub contributors, where they inferred the contributors’ gender
based on their names and locations (and validated 816 of those genders through a survey); they
found that gender diversity is a significant and positive predictor of productivity (3). Our work
builds on this by investigating bias systematically and at a larger scale.
General Methodology
Our main research question was
To what extent does gender bias exist among people who judge GitHub pull re-
quests?
To answer this question, we approached the problem by examining whether men and women
are equally likely to have their pull requests accepted on GitHub, then investigated why differ-
ences might exist. While the data analysis techniques we used were specific to each approach,
there were several commonalities in the data sets that we used, as we briefly explain below and
in more detail in the Material and Methods appendix.
We started with the GHTorrent (14) dataset from April 1st, 2015, which contains public data
pulled from GitHub about users, pull requests, and projects. We then augmented this GHTorrent
5
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
data by mining GitHub’s webpages for information about each pull request status, description,
and comments.
GitHub does not request information about users’ genders. While previous approaches have
used gender inference (2,3), we took a different approach – linking GitHub accounts with social
media profiles where the user has self-reported gender. Specifically, we extract users’ email
addresses from GHTorrent, look up that email address on the Google+ social network, then, if
that user has a profile, extract gender information from these users’ profiles. Out of 4,037,953
GitHub user profiles with email addresses, we were able to identify 1,426,121 (35.3%) of them
as men or women through their public Google+ profiles. We are the first to use this technique,
to our knowledge.
As an aside, we believe that our gender linking approach raises privacy concerns, which
we have taken several steps to address. First, this research has undergone human subjects IRB
review,3research that is based entirely on publicly available data. Second, we have informed
Google about our approach to determine whether they believe that it’s a privacy violation of
their users to be able to link email address to gender; they responded that it’s consistent with
Google’s terms of service.4Third, to protect the identities of the people described in this study
to the extent possible, we do not plan to release our data that links GitHub users to genders.
Results
Are women’s pull requests less likely to be accepted?
We hypothesized that pull requests made by women are less likely to be accepted than those
made by men. Prior work on gender bias in hiring – that women tend to have resumes less
favorably evaluated than men (5) – suggests that this hypothesis may be true.
3NCSU IRB number 6708.
4https://sites.google.com/site/bughunteruniversity/nonvuln/
discover-your-name-based-on-e-mail-address
6
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
To evaluate this hypothesis, we looked at the pull status of every pull request submitted by
women compared to those submitted by men. We then calculate the merge rate and correspond-
ing confidence interval, using the Clopper-Pearson exact method (15), and find the following:
Open Closed Merged Merge Rate 95% Confidence Interval
Women 8,216 21,890 111,011 78.6% [78.45%, 78.87%]
Men 150,248 591,785 2,181,517 74.6% [74.56%, 74.67%]
The hypothesis is not only false, but it is in the opposite direction than expected; women
tend to have their pull requests accepted at a higher rate than men! This difference is statis-
tically significant (χ2(df = 1, n = 3,064,667) = 1170, p < .001). What could explain this
unexpected result?
Perhaps women’s high acceptance rate is because they are already well known in the projects
they make pull requests in. Pull requests can be made by anyone, including both insiders (ex-
plicitly authorized owners and collaborators) and outsiders (other GitHub users). If we exclude
insiders from our analysis, the women’s acceptance rate (64.4%) continues to be significantly
higher than men’s (62.7%) (χ2(df = 2, n = 2,473,190) = 492, p < .001).
Perhaps only a few highly successful and prolific women, responsible for a substantial part
of overall success, are skewing the results. To test this, we calculated the pull request acceptance
rate for each woman and man with 5 or more pull requests, then found the average acceptance
rate across those two groups. The results are displayed in Figure 2. We notice that women
tend to have a bimodal distribution, typically being either very successful (>90% acceptance
rate) or unsuccessful (<10%). But this data tells the same story as the overall acceptance rate;
women are more likely than men to have their pull requests accepted.
Why might women have a higher acceptance rate than men? In the remainder of this section,
we will explore this question by evaluating several hypotheses that might explain the result.
7
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
0% 25% 50% 75% 100%
% of Women
Women
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
0% 25% 50% 75% 100%
% of Men
Men
Acceptance Rate
Figure 2: Histogram of mean acceptance rate per developer for Women (Mean 69.3%, Median
78.6%) and Men (Mean 66.3%, Median 70.0%)
Do women’s pull request acceptance rates start low and increase over time?
One plausible explanation is that women’s first few pull requests get rejected at a disproportion-
ate rate compared to men’s, so they feel dejected and do not make future pull requests. After
all, Reagle’s account of women’s participation in virtual collaborative environments describes
an unreasonably aggressive argument style necessary to justify one’s own contributions, a style
that many women may find to be not worth the exhaustion of proving their competence over
and over (16). Thus, the overall higher acceptance rate for women would be due to survivorship
8
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
bias within GitHub; the women who remain and do the majority of pull requests would be better
equipped to contribute, and defend their contributions, than men. Thus, we might expect that
women have a lower acceptance rate than men for early pull requests but have an equivalent
acceptance rate later.
To evaluate this hypothesis, we examine pull request acceptance rate over time, that is, the
mean acceptance rate for developers on their first pull request, second pull request, and so on.
Figure 3 displays the results. Orange points represent the mean acceptance rate for women, and
purple points represent acceptance rates for men. Shaded regions indicate the 95% Clopper-
Pearson confidence interval.
The acceptance rate of women tends to fluctuate at the right of the graph, because the ac-
ceptance rate is affected by only a few individuals. For instance, at 128 pull requests, only
103 women are represented. Intuitively, where the shaded region for women includes the cor-
responding data point for men, the reader can consider the data too sparse to conclude that a
substantial difference exists between acceptance rates for women and men. Nonetheless, be-
tween 1 and 64 pull requests, women’s higher acceptance rate remains. Thus, the evidence
casts doubt on our hypothesis.
Are women making pull requests that are more needed?
Another explanation for women’s pull request acceptance rate is that, perhaps, women dispro-
portionately make contributions that projects need more urgently. What makes a contribution
“needed” is difficult to assess from a third-party perspective. One way is to look at which pull
requests link to issues in projects’ GitHub issue trackers. If a pull request references an issue,
we consider it to serve a more immediate, recognized need than an otherwise comparable one
that does not. To support this argument with data, we randomly selected 30 pull request de-
scriptions that referenced issues; in 28 cases, the reference was an attempt to fix all or part of
9
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
60%
70%
80%
1 2 4 8 16 32 64 128
Pull Requests
Acceptance Rate
Women
Men
Figure 3: Pull request acceptance rate over time
an issue. Based on this high probability, we can assume that when someone references an issue
in a pull request description, they usually intend to fix a real problem in the project. Thus, if
women more often submit pull requests that address an immediate need and this is enough to
improve acceptance rates, we would expect that these same requests are more often linked to
issues.
We evaluate this hypothesis by parsing pull request descriptions and calculating the per-
centage of pulls that reference an issue. To eliminate projects that do not use issues or do not
customarily link to them in pull requests, we analyze only pull requests in projects that have at
10
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
least one linked pull request. Here are the results:
without reference with reference %95% CI
Women 29,163 4748 14.0% [13.63%, 14.38%]
Men 1,071,085 182,040 14.5% [14.46%, 14.58%]
This data shows a statistically significant difference (χ2(df = 1, n = 1,287,036) = 7.3, p <
.007). Contrary to the hypothesis, women are slightly less likely to submit a pull request that
mentions an issue, suggesting that women’s pull requests are less likely to fulfill an immediate
need. Note that this doesn’t imply women’s pull requests are less valuable, but instead that the
need they fulfill appears less likely to be recognized and documented before the pull request was
created. Regardless, the result suggests that women’s increased success rate is not explained by
making more immediately needed pull requests.
Are women making smaller changes?
Maybe women are disproportionately making small changes that are accepted at a higher rate
because the changes are easier for project owners to evaluate. This is supported by prior work
on pull requests suggesting that smaller changes tend to be accepted more than larger ones (17).
We evaluated the size of the contributions by analyzing lines of code, modified files, and
number of commits included. The following table lists the median and mean lines of code
added, removed, files changed, and commits per pull request:
lines added lines removed files changed commits
Women median 29 5 1 2
mean 1591 596 5.2 29.3
Men median 21 4 1 2
mean 1002 431 4.9 26.8
For all four metrics of size, women’s pull requests are significantly larger than men’s (Wilcoxon
rank-sum test, p <=.001).
One threat to this analysis is that lines added or removed may exaggerate the size of a change
11
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
whenever a refactoring is performed. For instance, if a developer moves a 1000-line class from
one folder to another, even though the change may be relatively benign, the change will show
up as 1000 lines added and 1000 lines removed. Although this threat is difficult to mitigate
definitively, we can begin to address it by calculating the net change for each pull request as the
number of added lines minus the number of removed lines. Here is the result:
net lines changed
women median 11
mean 994
men median 7
mean 590
This difference is also statistically significant (Wilcoxon rank-sum test, p<.001). So even in
the face of refactoring, the conclusion holds: women make pull requests that add and remove
more lines of code, modify more files, and contain more commits.
Are women’s pull requests more successful when contributing code?
One potential explanation for why women get their pull requests accepted more often is that
the kinds of changes they make are different. For instance, changes to HTML could be more
likely to be accepted than changes to C code, and if women are more likely to change HTML,
this may explain our results. Thus, if we look only at acceptance rates of pull requests that
make changes to program code, women’s high acceptance rates might disappear. For this,
we define program code as files that have an extension that corresponds to a Turing-complete
programming language. We categorize pull requests as belonging to a single type of source code
change when the majority of lines modified were to a corresponding file type. For example, if
a pull request changes 10 lines in .js (javascript) files and 5 lines in .html files, we include
that pull request and classify it as a .js change.
Figure 4 shows the results for the 10 most common programming language files (top) and
the 10 most common non-programming language files (bottom). Each pair of bars represent
12
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
60%
70%
80%
.js
.rb
.py
.java
.php
.cpp
.cs
.c
.go
.m
Acceptance Rate
Women Men
60%
70%
80%
.md
.html
.xml
.json
.css
.txt
.yml
.rst
.markdown
.podspec
Acceptance Rate
Figure 4: Pull request acceptance rate by file type, for programming languages (top) and non-
programming languages (bottom)
a pull request classified as part of a programming language file extension, where the height
of each bar represents the acceptance rate and each bar contains a 95% binomial confidence
interval.
Overall, we observe that women’s acceptance rates dominate over men’s for every program-
ming language in the top ten, to various degrees.
13
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Is a woman’s pull request accepted more often because she appears to be a
woman?
Another explanation as to why women’s pull requests are accepted at a higher rate would be
what McLoughlin calls Type III bias: “the singling out of women by gender with the intention
to help” (18). In our context, project owners may be biased towards wanting to help women
who submit pull requests. Thus, we expect that women who can be perceived as women are
more likely to have their pull requests accepted than women whose gender cannot be easily
inferred.
We evaluate this hypothesis by comparing pull request acceptance rate of developers who
have gender-neutral GitHub profiles and those who have gendered GitHub profiles. We define
a gender-neutral profile as one where a gender cannot be readily inferred from their profile.
Figure 1 gives an example of a gender-neutral GitHub user, “akofink”, who uses an identicon,
an automatically generated graphic, and does not have a gendered name that is apparent from
the login name. Likewise, we define a gendered profile as one where the gender can be readily
inferred from the photo or the name. Figure 1 also gives an example of a gendered profile;
the profile of “JustinAMiddleton” is gendered because it uses a login name (Justin) commonly
associated with men, and because the picture depicts a person with masculine features (e.g., pro-
nounced brow ridge (19)). Clicking on a user’s name in pull requests reveals their profile, which
may contain more information such as a user-selected display name (like “Justin Middleton”).
To obtain a sample of gendered and gender-neutral profiles, we used a combination of au-
tomated and manual techniques. For gendered profiles, we included GitHub users who used a
profile image rather than an identicon and that Vasilescu and colleagues’ tool could confidently
infer a gender from the user’s name (2). For gender-neutral profiles, we included GitHub users
that used an identicon, that Michael’s tool could not infer a gender for, and that a mixed-culture
panel of judges could not guess the gender for.
14
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
60%
70%
80%
Gender−Neutral Gendered
Acceptance Rate
Women Men
60%
70%
80%
Gender−Neutral Gendered
Acceptance Rate
Women Men
Figure 5: Pull request acceptance rate by gender and perceived gender, with 95% Clopper-
Pearson confidence intervals, for insiders (left) and outsiders (right)
While acceptance rate results so far have been robust to differences between insiders (people
who are owners or collaborators of a project) versus outsiders (everyone else), for this analysis,
there is a substantial difference between the two, so we treat each separately. Figure 5 shows the
acceptance rates for men and women when their genders are identifiable versus when they are
not, with pull requests submitted by insiders on the left and pull requests submitted by outsiders
on the right.
For insiders, we observe little evidence of bias when we compare women with gender-
neutral profiles and women with gendered profiles, since both have about equivalent acceptance
rates. This can be explained by the fact that insiders likely know each other to some degree,
since they are all authorized to make changes to the project, and thus may be aware of each
others’ gender.
For outsiders, we see evidence for gender bias: women’s acceptance rates are 71.8% when
they use gender neutral profiles, but drop to 62.5% when their gender is identifiable. There is
15
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
a similar drop for men, but the effect is not as strong. Women have a higher acceptance rate
of pull requests overall (as we reported earlier), but when they’re outsiders and their gender is
identifiable, they have a lower acceptance rate than men.
Discussion
To summarize this paper’s observations:
1. Women are more likely to have pull requests accepted than men.
2. Women continue to have high acceptance rates as they gain experience.
3. Women’s pull requests are less likely to serve an immediate project need.
4. Women’s changes are larger.
5. Women’s acceptance rates are higher across programming languages.
6. Women have lower acceptance rates as outsiders when they are identifiable as women.
We next consider several alternative theories that may explain these observations as a whole.
Given observations 1–5, one theory is that a bias against men exists, that is, a form of
reverse discrimination. However, this theory runs counter to prior work (e.g., (13)), as well
as observation 6. With 6, we observed that when a contributor’s gender is identifiable, men’s
acceptance rates surpass women’s.
Another theory is that women are taking fewer risks than men. This theory is consistent with
Byrnes’ meta-analysis of risk-taking studies, which generally find women are more risk-averse
than men (20). However, this theory is not consistent with observation 4, because women tend
to change more lines of code, and changing more lines of code correlates with an increased risk
of introducing bugs (21).
16
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Another theory is that women in open source are, on average, more competent than men.
This theory is consistent with observations 1–5. To be consistent with observation 6, we need
to explain why women’s pull request acceptance rate drops when their gender is apparent. An
addition to this theory that explains observation 6, and the anecdote describe in the introduction,
is that discrimination against women does exist in open source.
Assuming this final theory is the best one, why might it be that women are more competent,
on average? One explanation is survivorship bias: as women continue their formal and informal
education in computer science, the less competent ones may change fields or otherwise drop
out. Then, only more competent women remain by the time they begin to contribute to open
source. In contrast, less competent men may continue. While women do switch away from
STEM majors at a higher rate than men, they also have a lower drop out rate then men (22),
so the difference between attrition rates of women and men in college appears small. Another
explanation is self-selection bias: the average woman in open source may be better prepared
than the average man, which is supported by the finding that women in open source are more
likely to hold Master’s and PhD degrees (1). Yet another explanation is that women are held
to higher performance standards than men, an explanation supported by Gorman and Kmec’s
analysis of the general workforce (23).
In closing, as anecdotes about gender bias persist, it’s imperative that we use big data to
better understand the interaction between genders. While our big data study does not definitely
prove that differences between gendered interactions are caused by bias among individuals, the
trends observed in this paper are troubling. The frequent refrain that open source is a pure
meritocracy must be reexamined.
References and Notes
1. L. Arjona-Reina, G. Robles, S. Dueas, The floss2013 free/libre/open source survey (2014).
17
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
2. B. Vasilescu, A. Capiluppi, A. Serebrenik, Interacting with Computers 26, 488 (2014).
3. B. Vasilescu, et al.,CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI (ACM,
2015), pp. 3789–3798.
4. G. Gousios, B. Vasilescu, A. Serebrenik, A. Zaidman, Proceedings of the 11th Working
Conference on Mining Software Repositories (ACM, 2014), pp. 384–387.
5. H. K. Davison, M. J. Burke, Journal of Vocational Behavior 56, 225 (2000).
6. S. Knobloch-Westerwick, C. J. Glynn, M. Huge, Science Communication 35, 603 (2013).
7. P. L. Roth, K. L. Purvis, P. Bobko, Journal of Management 38, 719 (2012).
8. M. Burnett, et al.,Proceedings of the 2010 ACM-IEEE International Symposium on Em-
pirical Software Engineering and Measurement (ACM, 2010), p. 28.
9. S. Arun, T. Arun, Journal of International Development 14, 39 (2002).
10. J. W. Graham, S. A. Smith, Economics of education review 24, 341 (2005).
11. V. A. Lagesen, Science, technology & human values 33, 5 (2008).
12. J. Antin, R. Yee, C. Cheshire, O. Nov, Proceedings of the 7th International Symposium on
Wikis and Open Collaboration (ACM, 2011), pp. 11–14.
13. D. Nafus, New Media & Society 14, 669 (2012).
14. G. Gousios, Proceedings of the 10th Working Conference on Mining Software Repositories,
MSR ’13 (IEEE Press, Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2013), pp. 233–236.
15. C. Clopper, E. S. Pearson, Biometrika pp. 404–413 (1934).
16. J. Reagle, First Monday 18 (2012).
18
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
17. G. Gousios, M. Pinzger, A. v. Deursen, Proceedings of the 36th International Conference
on Software Engineering, ICSE 2014 (ACM, New York, NY, USA, 2014), pp. 345–355.
18. L. A. McLoughlin, Journal of Engineering Education 94, 373 (2005).
19. E. BrownU, D. Perrett, Perception 22, 829 (1993).
20. J. P. Byrnes, D. C. Miller, W. D. Schafer, Psychological bulletin 125, 367 (1999).
21. A. Mockus, D. M. Weiss, Bell Labs Technical Journal 5, 169 (2000).
22. X. Chen, National Center for Education Statistics (2013).
23. E. H. Gorman, J. A. Kmec, Gender & Society 21, 828 (2007).
19
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Acknowledgments
Special thanks to Denae Ford for her help throughout this research project. Thanks to Jon
Stallings for his help with the statistics. Thanks to the Developer Liberation Front for their
reviews of this paper. For their helpful discussions, thanks to Tiffany Barnes, Margaret Burnett,
Tim Chevalier, Prem Devanbu, Ciera Jaspan, Saul Jaspan, Jeff Leiter, Ben Livshits, Peter Rigby,
and Bogdan Vasilescu. This material is based in part upon work supported by the National
Science Foundation under grant number 1252995.
Materials and Methods
GitHub Scraping
An initial analysis of GHTorrent pull requests showed that our pull request merge rate was
significantly lower than that presented in prior work on pull requests (17). We found a solution
to the problem that calculated pull request status using a different technique, which yielded a
pull request merge rate comparable to prior work. However, in a manual inspection of pull
requests, we noticed that several calculated pull request statuses were different than the statuses
indicated on the github.com website. As a consequence, we wrote a web scraping tool that
automatically downloaded the pull request HTML pages, parsed them, and extracted data on
status, pull request message, and comments on the pull request.
We determined whether a pull requester was an insider or an outsider during our scraping
process because the data was not available in the GHTorrent dataset. We classified a user as an
insider when the pull request listed the person as a member or owner, and classified them as an
outsider otherwise. This analysis has inaccuracies because GitHub users can change roles from
outsider to insider and vice-versa. As an example, about 6% of merged pull requests from both
outsider female and male users were merged by the outsider pull-requestor themselves, which
20
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
isn’t possible, since outsiders by definition don’t have the authority to self-merge. We emailed
such an outsider, who indicated that, indeed, she was an insider when she made that pull request.
This problem is presently unavoidable as GitHub does not keep data on role changes.
Gender Linking
To evaluate gender bias on GitHub, we first needed to determine the genders of GitHub users.
Our technique uses several steps to determine the genders of GitHub users. First, from
the GHTorrent data set, we extract the email addresses of GitHub users. Second, for each
email address, we use the search engine in the Google+ social network to search for users with
that email address. The search works for both Google users’ email addresses (@gmail.com),
as well as other email addresses (such as @ncsu.edu). Third, we parse the returned users’
About’ page to scrape their gender. Finally, we only include the genders ‘Male’ and ‘Female’
because there were relatively few other options chosen. We also automated and parallelized
this process. This technique capitalizes on several properties of the Google+ social network.
First, if a Google+ user signed up for the social network using an email address, the search
results for that email address will return just that user, regardless of whether that email address
is publicly listed or not. Second, signing up for a Google account currently requires you to
specify a gender (though ‘Other’ is an option)5, and, in our discussion, we interpret their use of
‘Male’ and ‘Female’ in gender identification (rather than sex) as corresponding to our use of the
terms ‘man’ and ‘woman’. Third, when Google+ was originally launched, gender was publicly
visible by default.6
5https://accounts.google.com/SignUp
6http://latimesblogs.latimes.com/technology/2011/07/google-plus-users-will-soon-be-able-to-opt-out-of-sharing-gender.
html
21
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Merged Pull Requests
Throughout this study, we measure pull requests that are accepted by calculating developers’
merge rates, that is, the number of pull requests merged divided by the sum of the number of pull
requests merged, closed, and still open. We include pull requests still open in the denominator
in this calculation because pull requests that are still open could be indicative of a pull requestor
being ignored, which has the same practical impact as rejection.
Determining Gender Neutral from Gendered Profiles
To determine gendered profiles, we first parsed GitHub profile pages to determine whether each
user was using a profile picture or an identicon. We then ran display names and login names
through a gender inference program, which maps a name to a gender.7. We classified a GitHub
profile as gendered if each of the following were true:
a profile image (rather than an identicon) was used, and
the gender inference tool output a gender at the highest level of confidence (that is, ‘male’
or ‘female,’ rather than ‘mostly male,’ ‘mostly female,’ or ‘unknown’).
To classify profiles as gender neutral, we added a manual step. Given a GitHub profile that
used an identicon (thus, a gender could not be inferred from a profile photo) and a name that
the gender inference tool classified as ‘unknown’, we manually verified that the profile picture
could not be easily identified as belonging to a specific gender. We did this in two phases. In
the first phase, we assembled a panel of 3 people to evaluate profiles for 10 seconds each. The
panelists were of American (man), Chinese (man), and Indian (woman) origin, representative
of the three most common nationalities on GitHub. We used different nationalities because
7This tool was builds on Vasilescu and colleagues’ tool (2), but we removed some of Vasilescu and col-
leagues’ heuristics to be more conservative. Our version of the tool can be found here: https://github.
com/DeveloperLiberationFront/genderComputer
22
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
we wanted the panel to be able to identify, if possible, the genders of GitHub usernames with
different cultural origins. In the second phase, we eliminated two inefficiencies from the first
phase: (a) because the first panel estimated that for 99% of profiles, they only looked at login
names and display names, we only showed this information to the second panel, and (b) because
the first panel found 10 seconds was usually more time than was necessary to assess gender, we
allowed panelists at the second phase to assess names at their own pace. Across both phases,
panelists were instructed to signal if they could identify the gender of the GitHub profile. To
estimate panelists’ confidence, we considered using a threshold like “90% confident of the
gender,” but found that this was too ambiguous in pilot panels. Instead, we instructed panelists
to signal if they would be comfortable addressing the GitHub user as ’Mister’ or ’Miss’ in an
email, given the only thing they knew about the user was their profile. We considered a GitHub
profile as gender neutral if all of the following conditions were met:
an identicon (rather than a profile image) was used,
the gender inference tool output a ’unknown’ for the user’s login name and display name,
and
none of the panelists indicated that they could identify the user’s gender.
Across both panels, panelists inspected 3000 profiles of roughly equal numbers of women and
men. We chose the number 3000 by doing a rough statistical power analysis using the results
of the first panel to determine how many profiles panelists should inspect during the second
panel to obtain statistically significant results. Of the 3000, panelists eliminated 409 profiles for
which at least one panelist could infer a gender.
23
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
Threats
One threat to this analysis is the existence of robots that interact with pull requests. For example,
“Snoopy Crime Cop”8appears to be a robot that has made thousands of pull requests. If such
robots used an email address that linked to a Google profile that listed a gender, our merge rate
calculations might be skewed unduly. To check for this possibility, we examined profiles of
GitHub users that we have genders for and who have made more than 1000 pull requests. The
result was tens of GitHub users, none of whom appeared to be a robot. So in terms of our merge
calculation, we are somewhat confident that robots are not substantially influencing the results.
Another threat is if men and women misrepresent their genders at different rates. In that
case, we may have inaccurately labeled some men on GitHub as women, and vice-versa.
Another threat is inaccuracies in our assessment of whether a GitHub member’s gender is
identifiable. For profiles we labeled as gender-neutral, our panel may not have picked out subtle
gender features in GitHub users’ profiles. Moreover, project owners may have used gender
signals that we did not; for example, if a pull requestor sends an email to a project owner, the
owner may be able to identify the requestor’s gender even though our technique could not.
Another threat is that of construct validity, whether we’re measuring what we aim to mea-
sure. One example is our inclusion of “open” pull requests as a sign of rejection, in addition to
the “closed” status. Rather than a sign of rejection, open pull requests may simply have not yet
been decided upon. Another example is whether pull requests that do not link to issues signals
that the pull request does not fulfill an immediate need.
Another threat is that of external validity; do the results generalize beyond the population
studied? While we chose GitHub because it is the largest open source community, other com-
munities such as SourceForge and BitBucket exist, along with other ways to make pull requests,
such at through the git version control system directly. Moreover, while we studied a large pop-
8https://github.com/snoopycrimecop
24
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
ulation of contributors, they represent only part of the total population of developers on GitHub,
because not every developer makes their email address public, because not every email address
corresponds to a Google+ profile, and because not every Google+ profile lists gender.
25
PeerJ PrePrints | https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj.preprints.1733v1 | CC-BY 4.0 Open Access | rec: 9 Feb 2016, publ: 9 Feb 2016
  • ... Only recently, a demographic attribute of contributors -gender, is found to influence evaluation of contributions in OSS projects. In a study of 1.4 million GitHub user profiles, Terrell et al. found that code contributions by female developers were less often accepted than their male counterparts when their gender was identifiable [25]. ...
    ... A recent study of 1.4 million GitHub user profiles reported bias in evaluation of code contributions when a demographic attribute of contributors -gender, was known to fellow contributors [25]. The study by Terrell et al. found that code contributions by female developers were less often accepted than their male counterparts when their gender was identifiable [25]. ...
    ... A recent study of 1.4 million GitHub user profiles reported bias in evaluation of code contributions when a demographic attribute of contributors -gender, was known to fellow contributors [25]. The study by Terrell et al. found that code contributions by female developers were less often accepted than their male counterparts when their gender was identifiable [25]. The results of this study suggests that other demographic attributes of contributors like geographical location, which are known to a significant percentage of fellow contributors [27], may influence evaluation of contributions. ...
    Conference Paper
    Background Open source software projects show gender bias suggesting that other demographic characteristics of developers, like geographical location, can negatively influence evaluation of contributions too. Aim This study contributes to this emerging body of knowledge in software development by presenting a quantitative analysis of the relationship between the geographical location of developers and evaluation of their contributions on GitHub. Method We present an analysis of 70,000+ pull requests selected from 17 most actively participating countries to model the relationship between the geographical location of developers and pull request acceptance decision. Results and Conclusion We observed structural differences in pull request acceptance rates across 17 countries. Countries with no apparent similarities such as Switzerland and Japan had one of the highest pull request acceptance rates while countries like China and Germany had one of the lowest pull request acceptance rates. Notably, higher acceptance rates were observed for all but one country when pull requests were evaluated by developers from the same country.
  • ... Researchers are beginning to investigate how gender biases play out in OSS communities. For example, one recent study reported that when the gender of women "outsiders" (newcomers/non-core contributors) was identifiable, their pull request acceptance rates were 12% lower than those of women whose gender was not identifiable from their profiles [55]. Several other investigations shed additional insights into gender bias in OSS [21,22,39,57]; we discuss these and others in the Related Work section (Section 4). ...
    ... A later investigation by Ford et al. showed that, because of the dearth of women in technical online communities, women disproportionately experience a lack of a notion they term "peer parity" (seeing other women contributing to their community) [21], but peer parity is important to women's continued contribution to the community. Terell et al. [55] mined GitHub projects to understand the extent to which gender bias affects pull request acceptance. They found when contributors are newcomers (non-core members/outsiders) men's and women's pull request acceptance rate is similar when their gender profiles are neutral. ...
    ... which we find manifest themselves in the tools and infrastructure. Beyond these, issues of culture, values, and community abound, as others have shown [21,22,39,53,55,57]. ...
    Conference Paper
    Research has revealed that significant barriers exist when entering Open-Source Software (OSS) communities and that women disproportionately experience such barriers. However, this research has focused mainly on social/cultural factors, ignoring the environment itself --- the tools and infrastructure. To shed some light onto how tools and infrastructure might somehow factor into OSS barriers to entry, we conducted a field study with five teams of software professionals, who worked through five use-cases to analyze the tools and infrastructure used in their OSS projects. These software professionals found tool/infrastructure barriers in 7% to 71% of the use-case steps that they analyzed, most of which are tied to newcomer barriers that have been established in the literature. Further, over 80% of the barrier types they found include attributes that are biased against women.
  • ... To better understand the reasons behind this disparity, and offer support for decision making, we and others have been engaged in empirical studies of activity in these online platforms, in which gender is one of the variables of interest [10,12,15,16,19]. However, since gender is not explicitly recorded, it is typically inferred by automatic "gender Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. ...
    ... Recently Terrell et al. [15] have also used Google+ to infer gender based on an email address. Since Stack Overflow profiles do not include email, we first searched for the Stack Overflow username on GitHub to obtain the email, and compare it with the MD5 hash of the email recorded in the September 2013 Stack Overflow data dump 7 . ...
    ... If the Stack Overflow username could not be found on GitHub or did not have an email address associated with it, we infer "unknown". Next, we use Google+ API to imitate the method of Terrell et al. [15]. Since we have discovered that Google+ API rarely allows one to associate the email address with a Google+ profile, we also manually consult Google+ to get the most precise answer one could hope for. ...
    Conference Paper
    Software development remains a predominantly male activity, despite coordinated efforts from research, industry, and policy makers. This gender imbalance is most visible in social programming, on platforms such as Stack Overflow. To better understand the reasons behind this disparity, and offer support for (corrective) decision making, we and others have been engaged in large-scale empirical studies of activity in these online platforms, in which gender is one of the variables of interest. However, since gender is not explicitly recorded, it is typically inferred by automatic "gender guessers", based on cues derived from an individual's online presence, such as their name and profile picture. As opposed to self-reporting, used in earlier studies, gender guessers scale better, but their accuracy depends on the quantity and quality of data available in one's online profile. In this paper we evaluate the applicability of different gender guessing approaches on several datasets derived from Stack Overflow. Our results suggest that the approaches combining different data sources perform the best.
  • ... Online communities can exist only via software, and several research groups have investigated gender diversity and its effects on online communities [29,39,40,41,44]. For example, Vasilescu et al. found that diversity within OSS communities, while limited, helped strengthen codebases [41]. ...
    ... Mendez et al. found that gender biases in OSS tools and infrastructure can impact OSS newcomer success [26]. Terrell et al. found that, among new contributors (non-core members/outsiders), men's and women's pull request acceptance rate was similar when their profiles are genderneutral but gender-biased when gender could be identified [39]. Such inclusivity bugs are problematic for both an organization's community and its productivity, as research across multiple fields has repeatedly shown. ...
    Preprint
    Although the need for gender-inclusivity in software itself is gaining attention among both SE researchers and SE practitioners, and methods have been published to help, little has been reported on how to make such methods work in real-world settings. For example, how do busy software practitioners use such methods in low-cost ways? How do they endeavor to maximize benefits from using them? How do they avoid the controversies that can arise in talking about gender? To find out how teams were handling these and similar questions, we turned to 10 real-world software teams. We present these teams experiences "in the trenches," in the form of 12 practices and 3 potential pitfalls, so as to provide their insights to other real-world software teams trying to engineer gender-inclusivity into their software products.
  • ... Cyberspace fits women only when male counterparts ignore the female sex of coder. A large study on gender bias in Open Source states that when a woman's gender is identifiable, the community rejected them more often [31]. ...
    Conference Paper
    Full-text available
    The inclusion of women in IT is a challenge for future economy. Although women compose half of population, majority of designers are men which influence the methodology and goals on technological products. Male-dominated environments entail handicaps to women designers who usually adopt a male perspective in working with technologies. This paper focus on historical and present examples to explain why technology should adopt a gender perspective that influence contents and designs in IT. Moreover, we examine how unfriendly environments emphasizes impact on people and process of creation of technological innovation.
  • ... One tool developed by Vasilescu et al. is GenderCompter, a publicly available tool to determine the gender of an individual based on their name and country of origin [2]. To compare the predicted gender from this GenderComputer tool I will use Terrell et al's method of obtaining self-reported gender validate with a data set of emails to confirm genders of individuals [7]. ...
    Conference Paper
    Full-text available
    Investigating gender participation in socio-technical ecosystems has been a popular challenge recently. However, tackling this feat requires an understanding of community features and participation to appropriately serve users. In this work, I describe work I've done to understand the Stack Overflow community for females and propose research to determine gender differences in communication on the site using the Bechdel test. To evaluate this test I explain how I adapt the three criteria of the Bechdel test to analyze Stack Overflow interactions.
  • ... Previous findings vary, but many show that femaleauthored papers are accepted more often or rated higher under DBR (Snodgrass 2006;Budden et al., 2008). The trend is similar in other areas, for example, a recent (unpublished) study found that female-authored code had higher acceptance rates in collaborative software projects when their gender was not identifiable (Terrell et al., 2016). ...
    Article
    The impact of introducing double-blind reviewing in the most recent Evolution of Language conference is assessed. The ranking of papers is compared between EvoLang 11 (double-blind review) and EvoLang 9 and 10 (single-blind review). Main effects were found for first author gender by conference. The results mirror some findings in the literature on the effects of double-blind review, suggesting that it helps reduce a bias against female authors.
  • ... However, in open-sourced contributions to science problems, women's contributions are accepted more often only if the contributor's gender is not identifiable. If it is identifiable, women's contributions are rejected more often (Kennedy, 2016;Terrell et al., 2016). ...
    Article
    Full-text available
    Creativity is currently being redefined in more inclusive and accurate ways. This article examines old and new ways of viewing creativity, focusing especially on how creativity has been considered a male preserve and the need for a more inclusive definition that includes areas such as “everyday creativity.” It places definitions of creativity in their social and historical context, showing how a society’s orientation to a partnership model or a dominator model affects what and who is considered creative. It proposes an un-gendered definition of creativity; highlights the need for this broader definition to meet the enormous contemporary challenges we face; and distinguishes between innovativeness and creativity.
  • Conference Paper
    Careers in computing seem to be well placed to allow gender parity. The tools of the trade don't require any of the most common gender stereotypes. And yet, talented, educated women avoid going into the field - why? The preliminary research reported in this paper focuses specifically on computer programming, since coding is an area that has shown a strong statistical bias towards males with up to 92% of programmers being male. This paper aims to uncover and examine any differences in the coding abilities and approach of males and females.
  • Chapter
    In this chapter, I seek to go beyond contemporary theories of information privacy by subjective the standard information flow models to analysis from the perspective of justice. I examine two perspectives. At the least, one can see privacy as connected to justice instrumentally, that is, privacy is valuable not as a requirement of justice directly but because it is a useful means of justice. This is, I argue, hardly adequate as an entire theory of information justice but it is too easily given short shrift in discussions of privacy (especially by the wealthiest Silicon Valley titans who can protect their interests more directly). A more robust approach looks to theories of distributive justice. Theories of distribution that focus on the distributive process can address two significant weaknesses in information flow models of privacy, weak conceptions of informed consent and the inability to address the original acquisition of information. Pattern theories of distributive justice shift the focus from distributing information to distributing privacy rights, and provide significant insight into what it means to have rights to be left alone or forgotten. Each of these theories makes useful contributions to our understanding or privacy. But they are not wholly adequate to the task; for this, one needs to understand justice structurally as well as distributively.
    • S Arun
    • T Arun
    S. Arun, T. Arun, Journal of International Development 14, 39 (2002).
    • V A Lagesen
    V. A. Lagesen, Science, technology & human values 33, 5 (2008).
    • B Vasilescu
    • A Capiluppi
    • A Serebrenik
    B. Vasilescu, A. Capiluppi, A. Serebrenik, Interacting with Computers 26, 488 (2014).
  • Article
    Full-text available
    Across three decades in both Britain and the United States, surveys indicate that women must work harder than men do. Using data from the 1997 Skills Survey of the Employed British Workforce (U.K.) and the 1997 National Study of the Changing Workforce (U.S.), the authors investigate two possible explanations for this gap in reports of required effort: gender differences in job characteristics and family responsibilities. In multivariate ordered logistic regressions, extensive measures of job characteristics do not explain the difference between women and men. Family obligations, as well, account for little or none of the gap. The authors argue that the association between gender and reported required work effort is best interpreted as reflecting stricter performance standards imposed on women, even when women and men hold the same jobs. The authors discuss alternative interpretations and implications for research.
  • Article
    An experiment with 243 young communication scholars tested hypotheses derived from role congruity theory regarding impacts of author gender and gender typing of research topics on perceived quality of scientific publications and collaboration interest. Participants rated conference abstracts ostensibly authored by females or males, with author associations rotated. The abstracts fell into research areas perceived as gender-typed or gender-neutral to ascertain impacts from gender typing of topics. Publications from male authors were associated with greater scientific quality, in particular if the topic was male-typed. Collaboration interest was highest for male authors working on male-typed topics. Respondent sex did not influence these patterns.
  • Article
    While open source software development promises a fairer, more democratic model of software production often compared to a gift economy, it also is far more male dominated than other forms of software production. The specific ways F/LOSS instantiates notions of openness in everyday practice exacerbates the exclusion of women. ‘Openness’ is a complex construct that affects more than intellectual property arrangements. It weaves together ideas about authorship, agency, and the circumstances under which knowledge and code can and cannot be exchanged. While open source developers believe technology is orthogonal to the social, notions of openness tie the social to the technical by separating persons from one another and relieving them of obligations that might be created in the course of other forms of gift exchange. In doing so, men monopolize code authorship and simultaneously de-legitimize the kinds of social ties necessary to build mechanisms for women’s inclusion.
  • Article
    There are multiple views in human resource management and organizational behavior concerning gender differences in measures of job performance. Some researchers suggest that males generally are evaluated higher than females across a variety of situations that include job performance measurement. At the same time, the presence of specific status cues in expectation states theory (EST; similar to the concept of individuating information) suggests that measures of job performance will be more similar than different for males and females. Previous analyses are unclear in their results for the measurement of the construct of job performance because they have included, and/or focused on, additional constructs (e.g., hiring suitability, leadership performance aggregated with leadership satisfaction) or have used student samples in lab experiments. The authors of this article conducted a meta-analysis of job performance measures from field studies. They found that females generally scored slightly higher than males (mean d = −.11, 80% credibility interval of −.33 to .12). Other analyses suggested that, although job performance ratings favored females, ratings of promotion potential were higher for males. Thus, ratings of promotability may deserve further attention as a potential source of differential promotion rates. These findings and processes are discussed within the context of EST.
  • Article
    Full-text available
    The authors conducted a meta-analysis of 150 studies in which the risk-taking tendencies of male and female participants were compared. Studies were coded with respect to type of task (e.g., self-reported behaviors vs. observed behaviors), task content (e.g., smoking vs. sex), and 5 age levels. Results showed that the average effects for 14 out of 16 types of risk taking were significantly larger than 0 (indicating greater risk taking in male participants) and that nearly half of the effects were greater than .20. However, certain topics (e.g., intellectual risk taking and physical skills) produced larger gender differences than others (e.g., smoking). In addition, the authors found that (a) there were significant shifts in the size of the gender gap between successive age levels, and (b) the gender gap seems to be growing smaller over time. The discussion focuses on the meaning of the results for theories of risk taking and the need for additional studies to clarify age trends. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
  • Article
    Full-text available
    This study meta-analytically tested hypotheses concerning factors that affect sex discrimination in simulated employment contexts. These hypotheses, derived from the social psychological literature on stereotyping, predicted that salience of applicant sex, job sex-type, sex of rater, and amount of job-relevant information would affect discrimination against female and male applicants. Generally, the hypotheses concerning job sex-type and job-relevant information were supported. Female and male applicants received lower ratings when being considered for an opposite-sex-type job, and the difference between ratings of males and females decreased as more job-relevant information was provided. However, ratings of males and females did not differ as hypothesized in regard to salience of sex and rater sex. The research and practice implications of these results are discussed.
  • Article
    College-educated women are less than half as likely as men to be employed in science and engineering (S&E); and if they are, earn about 20 percent less. Using data from the 1993 National Survey of College Graduates, we estimate jointly, determinants of S&E employment and earnings in both S&E and non-S&E jobs. Taking account of gender differences in education (including S&E degrees), work experience and occupational characteristics, we can explain 60 percent of the gender differential in S&E employment and up to two-thirds of the earnings differential in S&E jobs. We find some evidence of gender earnings discrimination in S&E jobs, but less of it than in non-S&E jobs. We also show that the likelihood a worker selects S&E employment depends on her expected pay differential between S&E and non-S&E jobs, as well as on expected gender earnings discrimination in both S&E and non-S&E labor markets.