Content uploaded by Carolina Baptista Menezes
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Carolina Baptista Menezes on Oct 01, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa
Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0102-3772e322216
e322216
ARTIGO ORIGINAL
Brief Meditation and the Interaction between Emotional Interference and Anxiety1
Carolina Baptista Menezes2
Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina
Mirtes G. Pereira
Izabela Mocaiber
Universidade Federal Fluminense
Lisiane Bizarro
Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul
ABSTRACT - This pilot study assessed the effects of a ve day focused meditation training on the interplay between emotional
interference and anxiety in a non clinical sample randomized into two groups (experimental=13; control=18). Emotional
interference was indexed comparing the reaction times in an attention span task with negative or neutral distracting images.
Anxiety experienced during the task was also assessed through self-report. Only in the control group higher anxiety levels
interacted with greater emotional interference and a worse evaluation of valence and arousal of emotional images. These
preliminary ndings suggest that meditation may help modulating anxiety effects on bias to negative stimuli, and that even a
short training may facilitate self-regulatory processes.
Keywords: meditation, emotional interference, anxiety, self-regulatory processes
Meditação breve e a interação entre interferência emocional e ansiedade
RESUMO - Este estudo piloto avaliou os efeitos de cinco dias de meditação focada na relação entre interferência emocional e
ansiedade em amostra não clínica randomizada em dois grupos (experimental=13; controle=18). A interferência emocional foi
indexada comparando os tempos de reação em uma tarefa de atenção com imagens distratoras negativas ou neutras. Também
foi avaliado o autorrelato da ansiedade durante a tarefa. Apenas no grupo controle uma maior ansiedade interagiu com uma
maior interferência emocional e uma pior avaliação de valência e alerta (arousal) das imagens emocionais. Esses achados
preliminares sugerem que a meditação pode ajudar a modular o efeito da ansiedade no viés para estímulos negativos, e que
mesmo um treino breve pode facilitar processos autorregulatórios.
Palavras-chave: meditação, interferência emocional, ansiedade, processos autorregulatórios
1 Acknowledgment: Este estudo contou com o financiamento do
Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa e Desenvolvimento Tecnológico
(CNPq), FAPERJ e CAPES.
2 Endereço para correspondência: Psicologia, Universidade Federal de
Pelotas, Avenida Duque de Caxias, n. 250, Fragata, Pelotas, RS, Brasil.
CEP: 96.030-001. E-mail: para menezescarolina@hotmail.com
The increase in research on sitting and silent meditation
has been generally accompanied by positive psychological
results, such as reduced distress and anxiety symptoms, as
well as improved well-being and regulatory skills (Chiesa,
Serretti, & Jakobsen, 2013; Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson, 2007).
Naturally, one question that arises relates to how much
practice is necessary for these outcomes to take effect and,
accordingly, different durations of meditation practice have
been tested (Carmody & Baer, 2009).
There is some evidence that interventions as short as a few
days can already promote some expected changes. One such
case comes from a study investigating a ve days program
called Integrative Mind Body Training (IBMT), in which
practice was done for 20 minutes daily (Tang et al., 2007).
Results showed a signicant improvement in executive
attention, as measured by the Attention Network Test,
compared to a control group that did progressive relaxation
for the same period (Tang et al., 2007).
Tang et al. (2007) proposed that IBMT superiority over
relaxation may be due to the combination of techniques that
characterize the program, such as breathing, mental imagery,
relaxation and meditation. However, positive results were
also found with interventions using an exclusive meditation
technique. For instance, one study that trained people for 4
days using a focused meditation practice for 20 minutes daily
- compared to a group that trained attention by listening to
an audio book - found signicant improvements in sustained
attention and mindfulness scores only in meditators (Zeidan,
Johnson, Diamond, David, & Goolkasian, 2010a).
Hence, the formal practice of meditation seems to account
for attentional improvements and, importantly, evidence
also extends to the emotional domain. After attending these
interventions, participants signicantly improved self-reports
of mood, state anxiety, and decreased cigarette craving (Tang
et al., 2007; Tang, Tang, & Posner, 2013; Zeidan et al., 2010a;
Zeidan, Johnson, Gordon, & Goolkasian, 2010b). In addition,
after an experimental induction of stress (Tang et al., 2007),
and pain (Liu, Wang, Chang, Chen, & Si, 2013), individuals
who had undertaken a brief meditation training were better
at reducing cortisol levels that had risen with the stressful
situation, and at improving tolerance and distress during
the pain experience, respectively. Also, similar amount of
practice - 20 minutes for 7 days - helped participants decrease
2Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
CB Menezes et al.
their evaluation of negative valenced pictures, which was
accompanied by a reduced activity in the left amygdala
(Taylor et al., 2011).
It is discussed that the combination of an attentive and
a relaxed mind-body state is a key feature of meditation,
likely responsible for the positive results observed (Lutz
et al., 2007; Tang & Posner, 2009; Wallace, 2008). This
assumption is corroborated by the combination of results so
far mentioned, as well as by the positive correlation found
between indexes of central and autonomic nervous system
regulation in participants of the IBMT intervention (Tang et
al., 2009). Specically, the pre-post theta wave increase in
frontal midline anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) positively
correlated with high frequency heart rate variability (Tang
et al., 2009).
This nding is in line with the idea that ACC is involved
in a form of attention that helps regulating cognitive and
emotional processing (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000), and
whose function mediates parasympathetic activity (Critchley
et al., 2003). Importantly, the correlation reported by Tang
et al. (2009) not only elucidates that meditation generates
greater balance between attention and relaxation, but also
demonstrates that since an early stage of practice this
combination may take effect, given that participants had
undergone a ve days training.
Noteworthy, the assumption that this balance may
constitute the mechanism that enables meditation to promote
positive psychological functioning seems to be supported by
ndings from the eld of emotion regulation. Evidence shows
that the use of cognitive strategies to regulate emotions, such
as attention allocation, can be undermined by high levels
of anxiety (Berggren & Derakshan, 2013; Mocaiber et al.,
2009; Sheppes, Luria, Fukuda, & Gross, 2013), in line with
results showing that affective states may be as important as
cognitive skills for the regulation of emotions (Kanske &
Kotz, 2012).
The Present Work
The main objective of this research was to investigate the
effects of a ve day training in focused meditation (FM) on
the interplay between emotional interference and anxiety in
a non clinical sample of undergraduates. For this purpose,
we assessed the interactions between interference indexes
obtained through reaction times in a behavioral task, and
the subjective ratings of experienced anxiety during the task.
The behavioral paradigm (Erthal et al., 2005) consisted of
a negative arousing or neutral picture displayed in the center
of the screen with a peripheral bar on each side of the picture.
Subjects should look at the center, but ignore the image,
and answer if the bars had the same orientation or not. The
type of image (negative or neutral) and orientation (same or
different) were randomly and equally distributed across trials.
Also, in order to guarantee the attentional engagement in the
relevant task, bars orientation could be presented in blocks
with either a 90° or a 6° inclination difference.
Based on ndings that even a short meditation training
can benefit the practitioner on attention and emotional
outcomes (Tang et al., 2007; Zeidan et al., 2010a), and that
anxiety can undermine the cognitive regulation of emotions
(Sheppes et al., 2013), we expected that for the focused
meditation training emotion interference would not change
according to anxiety levels.
Method
Participants
College students from the Universidade Federal do Rio
Grande do Sul (UFRGS), southern Brazil, were invited
to take part in the study through email and posters spread
throughout three campuses. One hundred and twenty nine
(129) students volunteered. Participants were excluded if
they were not between 20-40 years old, did not have normal
or corrected sight, reported any psychiatric or neurologic
disorder, were taking any psychoactive medication, were
undergoing psychotherapy treatment, and had had previous
experience with meditation or yoga.
Initially, 46 eligible participants were equally and
randomly assigned to one of two groups (focused meditation-
FM and wait-list control-WLC). The nal sample comprised
31 participants [FM: N=13, 50,5% female, 100% single,
Mage=24.4 years (SD=4.27); WLC: N=18, 67% female, 80%
single, Mage=24.4(SD=4.9)]. These variables did not differ
between groups (p = > .05), and there was no signicant
difference in attrition rates [χ²(1)=3.37, p=.09].
It should be noted that the study was carried out during
the winter holiday period and on the week the intervention
was delivered it rained very much during the ve days -
which is usual at this time of the year in this region. Hence,
we believe that the bad weather conditions inuenced the
higher drop outs in the meditation group, although we did not
assess this information directly. In line with this assumption,
after the posttest assessment, the 18 control participants
who had remained in the control group underwent the ve
days training without any further drop outs. The study was
approved by the Research Ethic Committee from the Institute
of Psychology (UFRGS), under the registration number
25000.089325/2006-58, and all participants gave informed
consent prior to assessment.
Assessment
Screening tools
Sociodemographic Questionnaire. This questionnaire
was created for the present study in order to investigate
sociodemographic and exclusion criteria variables.
Self-Report Questionnaire - SRQ (Harding et al., 1980).
The SRQ consists of 23 questions that investigate minor and
psychotic psychiatric symptoms through yes/no answers. The
validated Brazilian version with sensitivity and specicity
coefcients of 83% and 80%, respectively, was used (Mari
& Willians, 1986). The cut point for female and male was 7
and 6 positive answers, respectively.
3Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
Meditation, Emotional Interference, Anxiety, Self-Regulatory Processes
Pretest and Posttest
Discrimination Task - DT (Erthal et al., 2005). Figure
1 illustrates the trial structure, as well as the task rationale.
Each trial initiated with a xation cross, shown for 1.500 ms.
Next, a central picture (9º X 12º) and two peripheral bars (0.3º
X 3.0º) were presented for 200 ms. The bars were at 9º to
the right and left of the center of the picture. A whole-screen
checkerboard mask was then shown, remaining on the screen
until the subject responded or for 2.000 ms, which was the
response deadline. The subjects were instructed to ignore the
task-irrelevant central images and to respond as quickly and
as accurately as possible whether or not the orientations of the
peripheral bars were the same. Keypresses (with the right or
left index nger) corresponding to same/different orientations
(“q” or “p”) were counterbalanced across subjects.
Two classes of images were employed: “neutral”
and “emotional/unpleasant”. Neutral images consisted
of photographs of parts of the body or neutral faces, and
unpleasant images consisted of photographs of mutilated
bodies. We chose mutilated bodies because these are
considered to be a very impacting category of emotional
stimuli, likely to cause interference. Indeed, it has been
already demonstrated that these images are efcient in
generating an interference effect in the same paradigm used
in the present study (Erthal et al., 2005).
One hundred and twenty different images, 60 neutral
and 60 unpleasant were utilized. A different set of pictures
was used in the pretest and posttest sessions and in each
session pictures were repeated once. Forty-two images (14
neutral and 28 unpleasant) were taken from the International
Affective Picture System (IAPS) developed by Lang and
colleagues (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2005), and the
remaining ones were obtained from the Internet.
For the internet images, the protocol developed by
Lang and colleagues (Bradley & Lang, 1994) was used for
assessing valence and arousal (N = 20, Mage = 22.3 years,
SD = 1.8). Overall, images in the neutral category had mean
valence ratings of 5.0 and mean arousal ratings of 3.3; images
in the unpleasant category had mean valence ratings of 2.2
and mean arousal ratings of 6.4.
The experimental session started with three training
blocks containing 20 trials each, which were followed
by three regular blocks of trials (80 trials each). The
order of neutral and unpleasant images within a block
was randomized. During training blocks, all images were
photographs of objects, such as tools and furniture. During
each experimental block, the type of bar-orientation was
fixed, and the two different orientations were obtained
by manipulating the angular difference on nonmatching
trials (90º for one block and 6º for two blocks). Each block
contained the same number of match and nonmatching trials
and the same number of neutral and unpleasant images.
Valence and arousal levels for emotional and neutral
images presented in each block type were matched to
avoid differences in emotionality between blocks. During
the training blocks, participants received feedback, which
indicated anticipatory responses (reaction times - RT - less
than 100 ms), slow responses (RT greater than 2.000 ms), as
well as whether an incorrect key was pressed; during training,
the RT was also indicated on the screen after each trial.
Experimental blocks, which followed the training blocks,
lasted approximately ve min each, and their order was
randomized across subjects. The subjects sat approximately
60 cm from the display and the stimuli were presented with
the software E-Prime.
Anxiety rating. An analog scale was used comprising
one question about how anxious participants felt during the
task. Answers were given on a 10-point scale (0 = not at all,
10 = very much).
Picture ratings. Participants viewed the pictures that had
been previously presented in the Discrimination Task in order
to assess their valence and arousal. In total, 4 blocks were
presented: 20 negative pictures taken from the 90º condition,
20 neutral pictures taken from the 90º condition, 20 negative
pictures taken from the 6º condition, and 20 neutral pictures
taken from the 6º condition. For the 6º condition, because
there were two blocks during the behavioral task, the 20
negative and 20 neutral pictures were randomly selected
from both blocks. For the 4 blocks, each image was displayed
for 1 sec and in the end participants had 15 sec. to rate the
whole block using the paper-and-pencil version of the Self-
Assessment Manikin (Bradley & Lang, 1994). For both
valence and arousal subjects rated from 1 (very unpleasant
or very relaxing, respectively) to 9 (very pleasant or very
alerting, respectively). Hence, the lower the valence score,
the greater the unpleasantness, whereas the higher the arousal
score, the greater the alertness.
Figure 1. Experimental design: Across three blocks, in each
trial a centered xation cross was presented for 1.500 ms,
followed by a central picture with two peripheral bars presented
simultaneously (for 200 ms) to the right and left of xation. A
checkerboard-like mask was then presented, which remained on
the screen until the response was made or until 2.000 ms had
elapsed. Subjects were instructed to ignore the central picture
and attend to the peripheral bars, responding with a keypress
as quickly and accurately as possible whether the bars were
in the same or a different orientation. Bars' orientation could
be presented with either a 90° or a 6° inclination difference.
4Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
CB Menezes et al.
Design and Procedure
This study comprised a pilot randomized controlled trial
that relied on a factorial repeated measure design. Concerning
procedure, after advertisement, volunteers interested in taking
part in the study were sent the screening questionnaires
online. Those eligible to participate were randomly allocated
to one of the two groups. An email was sent to inform
participants the group to which they had been allocated and
requesting them to schedule a visit to the laboratory for the
pretest session. Two assessments lasting approximately forty
minutes, one at pretest and another at posttest (during the two
weeks prior and after training, respectively), were carried out
at the Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Neuroscience,
and Behavior, at the Institute of Psychology, UFRGS.
The following sequence of measures was used for both
assessments: DT, Anxiety Rating, and Picture Ratings. FM
training included ve daily meetings, each lasting ninety
minutes. WLC participants did not take part in any activity
between testing sessions, but did receive the meditation
training after nal testing. Trainings were conducted by
the rst author, a psychologist trained on yoga, meditation
and mindfulness practices, and with group experience.
Meetings always started with a brief talk about participants’
doubts, difculties, or experiences during the rest of the day
following the previous meeting, followed by instructions for
practice, breathing exercises, formal meditation practice, and
again a brief talk about the experience with that particular
meeting.
In the two first meetings, formal practice lasted 15
and 20 minutes, respectively. For the following meetings,
practices lasted 30 minutes. Participants could either sit cross-
legged on a mat, or on a chair with their feet on the ground.
Because everyone was a beginner, they were instructed to
pay attention to the breath, trying to slightly prolong the
exhalation. Also, in order to characterize focused meditation,
and to try to guarantee they could maintain their focus to the
meditation process and to the present moment, participants
were instructed to count every exhalation (other types of
focusing, such as the use of mantras, were not used in order to
avoid any direct links to a specic philosophical or religious
tradition). For the three rst meetings, counting consisted
of cycles of 1 to 10, and for the next two days, participants
counted backwards from 100 to 1 (always one number per
exhalation). Participants were not required to carry out extra
practice outside the meetings.
Data Analysis
For DT analyses, all anticipatory and slow responses
(< 100 ms and > 2.000 ms, respectively) were excluded;
eliminated trials were infrequent at pre and posttest (0.70%
and 0.78% of the trials, respectively). Interference produced
by the emotional pictures was analyzed relying on reaction
times (RT) as dependent variables, and according to the
following procedure: we calculated the RT means for
each type of image and then created an index of emotion
interference subtracting emotional from neutral values for
each bar orientation, generating two factors (interference in
the 90º condition x interference in the 6º condition). The more
positive the value, the greater the emotional interference.
Means for anxiety and picture ratings were also analyzed
based on two factors (anxiety: high x low; valence: emotional
x neutral; arousal: emotional x neutral). At pretest, a factorial
general linear model (GLM) was used to assess the interaction
between emotion interference (90º x 6º) and anxiety (high x
low) as within factors, and group (FM x WLC) as a between-
subjects factor. Valence and arousal were separately analyzed
for assessing their interaction with anxiety, considering
type of images (emotional x neutral) and anxiety (high x
low) as within factors, and group (FM x WLC) as between
factor. At posttest, GLMs were carried out with time as an
additional factor (T1 x T2). ANOVAs, polynomial contrasts,
and pairwise comparisons using t-test were applied when
appropriate. For all analyses, the Statistical Package for the
Social Sciences (SPSS 20.0) was used, and the alpha level
for statistical signicance was p = .05.
Results
Pretest
Discrimination task: Emotion interference. Emotion
interference did not differ according to bar orientation [F(1,
44) = 2.72, p = .11], and did not interact with group [F(2,
44) = .82, p = .37].
Discrimination task: Anxiety x emotion interference.
Ratings of anxiety did not interact with interference [F(1, 42)
= .22, p = .88], nor group [F(1, 42) = 1.06, p = .31].
Picture ratings. There was a main effect of valence
[F(1,44) = 465.45, p < .001; emotional images were more
unpleasant (M = 1.4, SD = .10) than neutral images (M = 5.8,
SD = .17)], and a main effect of arousal [F(1,44) = 249.8, p
< .001; emotional images were more arousing (M = 8.2, SD
= .15) than neutral images (M = 3.4, SD = .26)]. There were
no group interactions for valence [F(1,44) = .11, p = .73],
nor arousal [F(1,44) = .92, p = .34].
Anxiety x picture ratings. There were no signicant
interactions between valence x anxiety x group [F(1,42) =
.98, p = .32], nor arousal x anxiety x group [F(1,42) = 2.6,
p = .12].
Posttest
Socio-demographic data, as well as pretest measures
were compared between drop-outs (participants who did
not complete the study because they stopped attending the
meetings) and completers (participants who completed
training and both testing sessions), and no significant
differences were found (p = > .05).
Discrimination task: Emotion interference. There were
no interference x time x group interactions [F(1, 29) = 2.54,
p = .13] for the RT outcome.
Discrimination task: Anxiety x emotion interference.
Total interference signicantly differed according to anxiety
ratings, time and group [F(1, 27) = 4.63, p = .04]. In the
5Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
Meditation, Emotional Interference, Anxiety, Self-Regulatory Processes
meditation group total emotional interference was null at
posttest and did not differ between participants reporting low
(M = .85, SD = 13.17) or high (M = .45, SD = 18.7) anxiety
during the task [t(11) = .02, p = .98], but in the control group
total emotional interference was signicantly higher in
participants reporting high anxiety (M = 33.07, SD = 11.41),
compared to low anxiety (M = -33.08, SD = 16.13) [t(16) =
-3.15, p = .006] (Fig. 2).
Picture ratings. Ratings of emotional images remained
more unpleasant [F(1,29) = 519.3, p < .001], and more
arousing [F(1,29) = 295.7, p < .001] compared to neutral
Figure 2. Mean values (ms) representing total emotional interference
- for both bar orientation conditions - according to levels of anxiety
experienced during the task for each group at posttest. FM= focused
meditation; WLC = wait list control. A GLM for repeated measures
indicated that at posttest total emotional interference did not differ
between anxiety levels in the FM group, but it was signicantly
greater in control participants reporting higher anxiety. p < .05
images across groups. There were no signicant interactions
between valence x time x group [F(1,29) = .02, p < .87], nor
arousal x time x group [F(1,29) = .01, p < .89].
Anxiety x picture ratings. There was a significant
interaction between valence x anxiety x time x group [F(1,27)
= 7.17, p = .01]: at posttest. Meditators who reported either
low or high anxiety during the task did not differ on valence
ratings for emotional [t(11) = .08, p = .93], or neutral [t(11)
= -1.16, p = .27] images; in the control group, emotional
images were rated as signicantly more unpleasant among
participants reporting high anxiety, compared to low anxiety
[t(16) = 2.5, p = .02], but no differences were observed for
neutral images [t(16) = -.68, p = .50] (g. 3A).
The same interaction pattern was observed for arousal
[F(1,27) = 6.28, p = .01]: at posttest, meditators who reported
either low or high anxiety during the task did not differ on
arousal ratings for emotional [t(11) = -.73, p = .47], or neutral
[t(11) = -.01, p = .98] images, whereas in the control group
emotional images were rated as signicantly more arousing
among participants reporting high anxiety, relative to low
anxiety [t(16) = -3.05, p = .007], but no differences were
observed for neutral images [t(16) = .00, p = 1.0] (g. 3B).
Figure 3A. Mean values representing valence ratings according
to levels of anxiety experienced during the task for each type of
image, in each group, at posttest. Standard errors are represented by
the error bars. FM= focused meditation; WLC = wait list control.
A GLM for repeated measures revealed that at posttest only in the
WLC group emotional images were rated as signicantly more
unpleasant than neutral ones. 3B. Mean values representing arousal
ratings according to levels of anxiety experienced during the task
for each type of image, in each group, at posttest. Standard errors
are represented by the error bars. FM= focused meditation; WLC
= wait list control. A GLM for repeated measures yielded that at
posttest only in the WLC group emotional images were rated as
signicantly more arousing than neutral ones at posttest. * p < .05
Discussion
The present pilot study investigated whether a brief
training in focused meditation, comprised of ve consecutive
daily practices, would influence the interplay between
emotion interference and anxiety in a healthy sample of
undergraduates. Emotion interference was indexed by
reaction times obtained from a behavioral task, and anxiety
experienced during the task was measured through self-
reported ratings. Results showed that groups signicantly
differed on emotion interference according to anxiety level.
After meditating on average twenty minutes for ve
consecutive days, emotion interference was null for
6Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
CB Menezes et al.
meditators reporting either low or high anxiety at posttest.
However, for the wait list control group, emotion interference
signicantly differed according to anxiety levels: interference
was greater among control participants reporting high anxiety
experienced during the task. This result was corroborated by
the nding that also only in the control group participants
reporting high anxiety during the task rated emotional images
as signicantly more unpleasant and more arousing.
From a psychological perspective, evidence shows
that higher levels of anxiety can inuence bias to negative
emotions (Bishop, 2007), undermine the effectiveness of
cognitive regulation of negative emotion (Mocaiber et al.,
2009; Sheppes et al., 2013), and interfere in the performance
of conict resolution for negative emotional stimuli (Kanske
& Kotz, 2012). More recently, it has been demonstrated that
anxiety is particularly impairing in the context of disengaging
attention from negative stimuli (Sheppes et al., 2013). Even
though our task did not test the disengagement hypothesis
directly, we believe this rationale might explain our results,
given that in our task the participants’ gaze was directed to
the center of the images, even though they were instructed
to orient their covert attention to the peripheral bars.
Hence, whereas in the control group higher anxiety
levels may have likely impaired the disengagement from the
distracting negative stimuli presented during the attentional
task, meditators seem to have been better able to modulate
this interaction. Also, the impairing inuence of anxiety on
the disengagement from negative stimuli might help explain
why control participants reporting higher anxiety were more
responsive to the unpleasantness and arousal of emotional
images.
On one hand, from the standpoint of emotion regulation,
the presence of better attentional control seems capable of
attenuating the inuence that high anxiety can exert on bias
to negative stimuli (Derryberry & Reed, 2002; Morawetz,
Baudewig, Treue, & Dechent, 2010). However, on the other
hand, anxiety has the potential of interfering with attention
allocation in the context of emotional stimuli (Oliveira,
David, Volchan, & Pereira, 2013; Sheppes et al., 2013).
Thus, in line with some propositions about sitting and
silent meditation, either focused or open monitoring (Lutz,
Slagter, Dunne, & Davidson, 2008; Tang & Posner, 2009;
Zeidan et al., 2010a), we suggest that in spite of being
derived from a short term intervention, our ndings represent
outcomes that may be related to the skills that the practice of
meditation aims to foster. In particular, improved attention
regulation, a more relaxed basal state, and the balance
between these skills (Lutz et al., 2007; Tang & Posner, 2009;
Tang et al., 2009; Wallace, 2008).
Accordingly, it was observed that when novices practice
ten minutes of focused meditation an attention network
consisting of the anterior cingulate, insula and frontal–
parietal regions is signicantly more recruited compared to
a mind wandering condition (Dickenson, Berkman, Arch,
& Lieberman, 2012). Also, one study assessing the effects
of a brief four days meditation training demonstrated that
meditators presented a signicant improvement in sustained
attention as measured by working memory tasks, as well as
reduction in state anxiety, which the authors hypothesized
to be likely interrelated (Zeidan et al., 2010a). Moreover, in
support of these results, after ve days of IBMT training the
increased activation in theta wave in the anterior cingulate
cortex positively correlated with parasympathetic activity
(Tang et al., 2009).
The nding that meditation has the potential to
modulate the interaction between anxiety and emotional
interference - perhaps through fostering a greater balance
between attention regulation and relaxation - may have
important implications for a more positive psychological
functioning. For instance, in line with our results on valence,
one study found that individuals who practiced meditation
for twenty minutes during seven days were less reactive to
negative stimuli, according to their subjective evaluation of
negative valenced pictures, as well as to the neural correlate
of reduced activity in the left amygdala (Taylor et al., 2011).
Another example of implication relates to the nding
that a brief meditation training was able to reduce cigarette
craving (Tang et al., 2013). We suggest this reduction
might have resulted from the meditators’ ability to better
control the interference of cues associated with the drug,
and to reduce attentional bias to these stimuli, which are
processes known to normally lead to craving or to the drug
use behavior (Peuker, Lopes, Menezes, Cunha, & Bizarro,
2013). Likewise, this potential modulation may help reduce
the hazardous interference and impact of stressful stimuli,
in line with ndings that participants of brief meditation
trainings, compared to controls, signicantly reduced cortisol
levels following an experimental stress induction (Tang et
al., 2007), and signicantly improved tolerance and the
experience of distress during an experimental induction of
pain (Liu et al., 2013).
Importantly, the present study shows preliminary results
corroborating the idea that some meditation benets may
take effect since an early stage of practice (Tang et al., 2007;
Tang et al., 2009; Tang et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2011; Zeidan
et al., 2010a, 2010b). Nevertheless, it should be noted that
comparing novice and expert practitioners yields distinct
effects. For instance, more reduced activation of some
default mode network regions of the brain in the context of
emotional appraisal was observed in long term meditators,
indicating less self referential processes and strengthened
present moment awareness (Taylor et al., 2012). Also, more
systematic comparisons of different types of meditation
trainings are necessary to conrm whether the observed
effects comprise core processes of all kinds of practices, or
whether it is possible to disentangle particular effects for
distinct meditation techniques. Data obtained from some of
the cited studies derived from different meditation trainings,
such as IBMT (Tang et al., 2007; Tang et al., 2009; Tang et
al., 2013), which combines meditation, relaxation and guided
imagery, as well as mindfulness (Taylor et al., 2011), which
implies a more broad-monitoring awareness, in opposition
to the exercise of strictly focusing attention on one object
(Lutz et al., 2008), such as in the focused meditation training
taught in the present study.
Finally, our findings contribute to the discussions
regarding the mechanisms that underlie and foster emotion
regulation skills (Bishop, 2007; Derryberry & Reed, 2002;
Kanske & Kotz, 2012; Mocaiber et al., 2009; Oliveira et
al., 2013; Sheppes et al., 2013), and support the proposition
7Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
Meditation, Emotional Interference, Anxiety, Self-Regulatory Processes
that meditation may be a valuable strategy for the study of
regulatory processes (Menezes, Pereira, & Bizarro, 2012).
However, our results should be extended and complemented
by larger scale trials, as well as by the usage of more
ecologically valid measures. In addition, because the present
study investigated a healthy sample, the question of whether
such a short-term training would benet clinical populations
remains unanswered.
Conclusion
Despite some limitations, such as sample size, high rate
of drop-outs, no power calculation, lack of an active control
group, and no follow up assessment, this pilot study suggests
that a ve day training in focused meditation may be able to
foster emotion regulation skills by modulating the interaction
between emotion interference and anxiety.
It is hypothesized that this effect may result from an
improved attention regulation and a more relaxed basal
state, as well as from their interplay. The present ndings
support the idea that short term trainings may produce some
psychological benets since an early stage of practice. The
results also suggest that this is a relevant nding, because
early effects might function as an important reinforcement
for a more regular and long term practice.
References
Berggren, N., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control
decits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you
don’t. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 440–446. doi:10.1016/j.
biopsycho.2012.03.007
Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety:
An integrative account. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(7),
307–316. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The Self-
Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional
inuences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 4(6), 215–222. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mindfulness-
based stress reduction program need to be? A review of class
contact hours and effect sizes for psychological distress.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 627–638. doi:10.1002/
jclp.20555
Chiesa, A., Serretti, A., & Jakobsen, J. C. (2013). Mindfulness:
Top-down or bottom-up emotion regulation strategy?
Clinical Psychology Review, 33(1), 82–96. doi:10.1016/j.
cpr.2012.10.006
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., Josephs, O., O’Doherty, J., Zanini,
S., Dewar, B. K., Cipolotti, L., Shallice, T., & Dolan, R.
J. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control:
Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain: A
Journal of Neurology, 126(Pt 10), 2139–2152. doi:10.1093/
brain/awg216
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional
biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.111.2.225
Dickenson, J., Berkman, E. T., Arch, J., & Lieberman, M. D.
(2012). Neural correlates of focused attention during a
brief mindfulness induction. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 8(1), 40-47. doi:10.1093/scan/nss030
Erthal, F., Oliveira, L., Mocaiber, I., Pereira, M. G., Machado-
Pinheiro, W., Volchan, E., & Pessoa, L. (2005). Load-dependent
modulation of affective picture processing. Cognitive,
Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 388–395.
Harding, T. W., Arango, M. V., Baltazar, J., Climent, C. E., Ibrahim,
H. H. A., Ladrido-Ignacio, L., ... Wig, N. N. (1980). Mental
Disorders in primary health care: A study of their frequency
and diagnosis in four development contries. Psychological
Medicine, 10, 231–241. doi:10.1017/S0033291700043993
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2012). Effortful control, depression, and
anxiety correlate with the inuence of emotion on executive
attentional control. Biological Psychology, 91(1), 88–95.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.04.007
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International
affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures
and instruction manual (Technical Report A-6). Gainesville,
FL: University of Florida.
Liu, X., Wang, S., Chang, S., Chen, W., & Si, M. (2013). Effect
of brief mindfulness intervention on tolerance and distress of
pain induced by cold-pressor task. Stress and Health: Journal
of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress,
29(3), 199–204. doi:10.1002/smi.2446
Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the
neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P. Zelazo,
M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Orgs.), Cambridge handbook
of consciousness (p. 499–554). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008).
Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169. doi:10.1016/j.
tics.2008.01.005
Mari, J., & Willians, P. A. (1986). A validity study of a psychiatric
screening questionnaire (SRQ-20) in primary care in the city
of São Paulo. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 23–26.
Menezes, C. B., de Paula Couto, M. C., Buratto, L. G., Erthal, F.,
Pereira, M. G., & Bizarro, L. (2013). The improvement of
emotion and attention regulation after a 6-week training of
focused meditation: A randomized controlled trial. Evidence-
Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine: eCAM, 2013,
984678. doi:10.1155/2013/984678
Menezes, C. B., Pereira, M. G., & Bizarro, L. (2012). Sitting and
silent meditation as a strategy to study emotion regulation.
Psychology & Neuroscience, 5(1), 27 – 36. doi:10.3922/p&n.
v5i1.225
Mocaiber, I., Pereira, M. G., Erthal, F. S., Figueira, I., Machado-
Pinheiro, V., Cagy, M., ... Oliveira, L. (2009). Regulation
of negative emotions in high trait anxious individuals: An
ERP study. Psychology & Neuroscience, 2(2), 211–217.
doi:10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.014
8Psic.: Teor. e Pesq., Brasília, Abr-Jun 2016, Vol. 32 n. 2, pp. 1-8
CB Menezes et al.
Morawetz, C., Baudewig, J., Treue, S., & Dechent, P. (2010).
Diverting attention suppresses human amygdala responses
to faces. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(226), 226.
doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00226
Oliveira, L., David, I. A., Volchan, E., & Pereira, M. G. (2013).
Emotion and attention interaction: a trade-off between stimuli
relevance, motivation and individual differences. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 7, 364. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00364
Peuker, A. C., Lopes, F. M., Menezes, C. B., Cunha, S. M., &
Bizarro, L. (2013). Implicit processes and drug addiction:
Theory, assessment and future directions. Psicologia:
Teoria e Pesquisa, 29(1), 07–14. doi:10.1590/S0102-
37722013000100002
Sheppes, G., Luria, R., Fukuda, K., & Gross, J. J. (2013). There’s
more to anxiety than meets the eye: Isolating threat-related
attentional engagement and disengagement biases. Emotion,
13(3), 520–528. doi:10.1037/a0031236
Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S., & Fan, M.
(2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is
altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 106(22): 8865-8870. Recovered from
http://www.pnas.org/content/106/22/8865.full?sid=81c57e1c-
1c7a-4ce0-8c73-bdd9b7914538
Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., Yu, Q., Sui, D.,
Rothbart, M. K., Fan, M., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term
meditation training improves attention and self-regulation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43),
17152–17156.
Tang, Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention
state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222–227.
Tang, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2013). Brief meditation training
induces smoking reduction. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(34),
13971–13975. doi:10.1073/pnas.1311887110
Taylor, V. A., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, E.,
Roffe-Vidal, S., Courtemanche, J., Lavarenne, A. S., &
Beauregard, M. (2011). Impact of mindfulness on the neural
responses to emotional pictures in experienced and beginner
meditators. NeuroImage, 57, 1524–1533. doi:10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2011.06.001
Taylor, V. A., Daneault, V., Grant, J., Scavone, G., Breton, E., Roffe-
Vidal, S., ... Beauregard, M. (2012). Impact of Meditation
Training on the Default Mode Network During a Restful
State. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 4-14
doi:10.1093/scan/nsr087
Wallace, B. A. (2008). A revolução da atenção: Revelando o poder
da mente focada. Petrópolis: Vozes.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian,
P. (2010a). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition:
Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and
Cognition, 19(2), 597–605. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Gordon, N. S., & Goolkasian, P. (2010b).
Effects of brief and sham mindfulness meditation on mood
and cardiovascular variables. The Journal of Alternative and
Complementary Medicine, 16(8), 867–873. doi:10.1089/
acm.2009.0321
Recebido em 18.11.2014
Primeira decisão editorial em 13.04.2015
Versão nal em 13.04.2015
Aceito em 08.05.2015 n